ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

We examined whether the frequent casual reports of people resembling their pets are accurate by having observers attempt to match dogs with their owners. We further explored whether any ability of observers to make such matches is due to people selecting dogs who resemble them, in which case the resemblance should be greater for predictable purebreds than for nonpurebreds, or is due to convergence, in which case the resemblance should grow with duration of ownership. Forty-five dogs and their owners were photographed separately, and judges were shown one owner, that owner's dog, and one other dog, with the task of picking out the true match. The results were consistent with a selection account: Observers were able to match only purebred dogs with their owners, and there was no relation between the ability to pair a person with his or her pet and the time they had cohabited. The ability to match people and pets did not seem to rely on any simple trait matching (e.g., size or hairiness). The results suggest that when people pick a pet, they seek one that, at some level, resembles them, and when they get a purebred, they get what they want.
Research Report
Do Dogs Resemble Their
Owners?
Michael M. Roy and Nicholas J.S. Christenfeld
University of California, San Diego
ABSTRACT—We examined whether the frequent casual reports of
people resembling their pets are accurate by having observers
attempt to match dogs with their owners. We further explored
whether any ability of observers to make such matches is due to
people selecting dogs who resemble them, in which case the re-
semblance should be greater for predictable purebreds than for
nonpurebreds, or is due to convergence, in which case the re-
semblance should grow with duration of ownership. Forty-five
dogs and their owners were photographed separately, and
judges were shown one owner, that owner’s dog, and one other
dog, with the task of picking out the true match. The results
were consistent with a selection account: Observers were able to
match only purebred dogs with their owners, and there was no
relation between the ability to pair a person with his or her pet
and the time they had cohabited. The ability to match people
and pets did not seem to rely on any simple trait matching (e.g.,
size or hairiness). The results suggest that when people pick a
pet, they seek one that, at some level, resembles them, and when
they get a purebred, they get what they want.
It has been asserted, by children’s-book illustrators, at dog shows, and
by strangers passing on the street, that people often bear a striking
resemblance to their pets. We examined whether such observations
are empirically warranted by testing whether people can match dogs
with their owners.
The fact that people remark on the resemblance between people
and their dogs is not in itself evidence that such a similarity generally
exists, as people may attend to the few cases in which there is some
overlap in appearance. People may also take advantage of the fact that
they know which people and pets are paired, given that they are often
connected by a leash, and may then search, post hoc, for features that
match.
There are two basic mechanisms that could create an actual sim-
ilarity between people and their dogs. People might select dogs
who are like them, or the features of people and their dogs might
converge over time. There is some evidence that the facial appear-
ance of married couples converges over time (Zajonc, Adelmann,
Murphy, & Niedenthal, 1987); judges have rated couples physically
more similar after 25 years of marriage than as newlyweds. If a sim-
ilar process were to occur with people and their pets, then one might
find that the ability to match them grows with the length of owner-
ship. In contrast, if owners resemble their dogs because of selection
rather than convergence, then there should be no correlation between
the degree of resemblance and the time spent together. However,
there ought to be, in this case, a greater similarity between people
and their purebred dogs than between people and their non-
purebreds. A nonpurebred puppy’s final appearance is un-
predictable, and so the resemblance between owner and dog should
be confined to the much more predictable purebreds. It is also likely
that the acquisition of a purebred dog involves more foresight and
deliberation than the adoption of a nonpurebred, and so should better
reflect the owner’s desires.
There is some evidence that people’s taste in animals is asso-
ciated with their own appearance: Coren (1999) found that women
with long hair gave higher ratings of attractiveness, friendliness,
loyalty, and intelligence to dogs with floppy, rather than pricked,
ears. There is also evidence that people have at least rudimentary
stereotypes of the type of pet that a person might own (Budge,
Spicer, St. George, & Jones, 1997). In pairing pictures of humans
and a variety of animals, judges were more likely to match cats and
small dogs with women and large dogs with males, though the re-
searchers did not examine whether this stereotype reflects actual
ownership patterns.
To examine whether people do look like their pets, and to explore
the mechanism underlying any such resemblance, we asked judges to
match photographs of owners with their dogs, both purebreds and
nonpurebreds.
METHOD
Pictures of 45 dogs and, separately, their owners were taken at three
dog parks. Fifteen such pairs were photographed in each location.
Owners were approached at random and asked if they would be willing
to help us with a psychology experiment examining the relation be-
tween owners and their dogs. The pictures were taken so that the
background was different for dog and owner. This ensured that raters
would not be able to match dog and owner by simply comparing the
backgrounds in the photographs. Owners were photographed from the
waist up, facing forward, wearing whatever clothes they had chosen for
Address correspondence to Nicholas J.S. Christenfeld, Department
of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr.,
La Jolla, CA 92093-0109; e-mail: nchristenfeld@ucsd.edu.
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Volume 15—Number 5 361Copyright r2004 American Psychological Society
going to the park and whatever facial expression they chose for the
picture. Dogs were photographed facing forward, with the whole dog
visible; they made whatever facial expression they chose, exhibiting
rather more lolling tongues than the owners.
Owners (24 women and 21 men, with a mean age of 36 years) were
asked to indicate the breed of their dog and how long they had owned
the dog. Because of an error in procedure, data on length of ownership
were available from only two of the parks (n530). Overall, there were
25 purebred dogs (representing 15 different breeds) and 20 non-
purebreds (mean age 53.7 years, range 54 months to 12.5 years).
Owners had been with their dogs for an average of 2.9 years, with 76%
of the owners acquiring their dog in its 1st year. There was no sig-
nificant difference between purebred and nonpurebred owners in
length of ownership or age of the dogs at adoption.
Each set of 15 pictures was viewed by 28 naive undergraduate
judges who were participating for course credit. We constructed
triads of pictures, each consisting of one owner, that owner’s dog, and
one other dog photographed at the same park. Each judge was shown
the 15 owners from one dog park, one at a time, and instructed to
identify which of the two possible dogs belonged to each person. In a
Latin square design, each of the 14 incorrect dogs served as a foil for
each dog, with the order of presentation randomized. Thus, within
each set of 15 pairs, each owner-and-dog pair was presented with
every other dog photographed at that park. A dog was regarded as
resembling its owner if a majority of judges (i.e., more than 14)
matched the pair.
RESULTS
There was no evidence of any resemblance between nonpurebreds
and their owners; of the 20 such dogs, there were 7 matches, 4 ties,
and 9 misses, w
2
(2, N520) 50.64, n.s., r-equivalent 5.14 (Ro-
senthal & Rubin, in press). However, purebreds could be matched
with their owners; of the 25 purebreds, there were 16 matches, 0
ties, and 9 misses, w
2
(2, N525) 56.75, p<.05, r-equivalent5.37.
The difference between the matchability of nonpurebreds and
purebreds was significant, w
2
(2, N545) 57.03, p<.05, f5.40
(Cohen, 1969).
The results were consistent with the notion that the ability to match
is due to selection rather than convergence, as there was no correla-
tion between how long a dog and owner had been together and the
number of judges who correctly picked that dog and owner as be-
longing together (r5.12, n.s., over the 30 pairs for whom this in-
formation was available).
Further analyses explored the possible origin of the resemblance
between purebred dogs and their owners. Three undergraduates
naive to the purpose of the experiment rated several features of the
owners and purebred dogs independently, and we then correlated the
ratings of dogs and their owners on these dimensions. If the judges
hadmatchedpairsonthebasisofthese characteristics, then we
would find significant correlations. The three raters judged hairiness,
size, sharpness of features, attractiveness, perceived friendliness,
and perceived energy level using 6-point Likert-type scales (run-
ning, e.g., from 1, not hairy,to6,very hairy). The reliability of the
ratings on each dimension was examined factor analytically, allowing
us to determine how each individual’s ratings loaded on the latent
concept (Schuster & Smith, 2002). All dimensions displayed ade-
quate reliability for both human and canine photographs: Using
principal components extraction with varimax rotation, we obtained
only one factor for each attribute; eigenvalues were all greater than
1.7, and the factors accounted for between 58 and 88% of the
variability.
For none of the rated characteristics, however, was the correlation
between person and pet significant, although there was a suggestion of
a trend for people and their purebred pets to be rated similarly on
friendliness (r5.31, p5.13). To examine whether any rated similarity
would emerge when all the variables were considered simultaneously,
we performed exploratory canonical correlation analyses. No sig-
nificant relation between person and pet was found for any combi-
nation of the attributes, approximate Pillai’s F(64, 128)50.81, n.s. We
also examined whether, as suggested by the findings of Budge et al.
(1997), the judges’ accuracy with purebreds could be based on dif-
ferences between the pets of men and women. Dogs with male and
female owners did not differ on any of the rated characteristics, all
ts(23) <1.1, n.s. Finally, in light of the findings of Coren (1999), we
examined if the hairstyle of owners (long or short) was related to the
ear style (lop or pricked) of their dogs. There was no association for
female owners (f5.03, n.s.) or female and male owners combined
(f5.19, n.s.).
DISCUSSION
The results suggest that when people pick a pet, they seek one that, at
some level, resembles them, and when they get a purebred, they get
what they want. The resemblance between dogs and their owners was
confined to purebreds, and there was no correlation between length of
ownership and similarity. Both findings are consistent with a selection,
but not a convergence, account of the phenomenon.
The results do not reveal at what level the resemblance between
person and pet exists. It could be at the level of physical attributes,
with owners of wolfhounds being tall and elegant, or at a stylistic level,
with retriever owners being warm and friendly. There are personality
differences between owners of various sorts of pets (Podberscek &
Gosling, 2000), and our judges could have used what they knew about
temperament differences between breeds to help them make matches.
It does not, however, appear that they used any single obvious char-
acteristic to make the matches, because we found no significant cor-
relations between dogs and owners on the six dimensions we
examined. That is, the judges did not make correct matches by simply
matching hairy people with hairy dogs, or big people with big dogs.
There was some suggestion that people and pets were similar in ap-
parent friendliness, but the effect was of modest size, and not statis-
tically significant. It may be that the judges used some other more
subtle trait, or based their judgments on a more configural analysis of
the animals. We also cannot know from these data if people can tell
whether a particular person is an owner of a dog, as opposed to, say, a
weasel. However, it does appear that, as in the case of selecting a
spouse (Berscheid & Reis, 1998), people want a creature like them-
selves.
REFERENCES
Berscheid, E., & Reis, H.T. (1998). Attraction and close relationships. In D.T.
Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psy-
chology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 193–281). New York: McGraw-Hill.
362 Volume 15—Number 5
Dogs Resemble Owners
Budge, R.C., Spicer, J., St. George, R., & Jones, B.R. (1997). Compatibility
stereotypes of people and pets: A photograph matching study. Anthrozoo
¨s,
10, 37–46.
Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Coren, S. (1999). Do people look like their dogs? Anthrozoo
¨s,12, 111–114.
Podberscek, A.L., & Gosling, S.D. (2000). Personality research on pets and
their owners: Conceptual issues and review. In A.L. Podberscek, E.S.
Paul, & J.A. Serpell (Eds.), Companion animals and us (pp. 143–167).
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D.B. (in press). r-equivalent: A simple effect size
indicator. Psychological Methods.
Schuster, C., & Smith, D.A. (2002). Indexing systematic rater agreement with a
latent-class model. Psychological Methods,7, 384–395.
Zajonc, R.B., Adelmann, P.K., Murphy, S.T., & Niedenthal, P.M. (1987).
Convergence in the physical appearance of spouses. Motivation and
Emotion,11, 335–346.
(RECEIVED 4/3/03; REVISION ACCEPTED 6/4/03)
Volume 15—Number 5 363
Michael M. Roy and Nicholas J.S. Christenfeld
... Una serie de estudios ha señalado que la elección del animal de compañía puede estar mediada por los valores culturales del entorno. Por ejemplo, diversos estudios de emparejamiento de fotografías demuestran que es posible detectar qué perros conviven con qué humanos con una probabilidad superior al azar (Nakajima et al., 2013;Payne et al., 2005;Roy et al., 2004). Esto ocurre porque se eligen individuos que representan -dentro del contexto-ciertos valores identitarios, como la afabilidad o la agresividad. ...
Poster
Full-text available
DOG PEOPLE, CAT PEOPLE. IS THERE A RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PERSONALITY AND THE CHOICE OF COMPANION ANIMALS? Introduction and objectives. Previous studies have described a relationship between personality characteristics and preferences for dogs and cats as companion animals. This work seeks to check if there are differences between "dog people", "cat people" and people who identify with both species. Method. Two studies were conducted. The first included N=460 Spanish adults (87% women, M=21.0 years; SD=4.2 years) who answered two self-labeling questions (I consider myself a dog/cat person) to be rated on an agreement scale (1-strongly disagree, 7-strongly agree), and the Ten Item Personality Index, a short measure of personality. For the second study, the self-labeling questions were recoded in a dichotomous manner generating three comparison groups: "dog person", "cat person", and "both animals" (N=137, 33, and 159 respectively). Results. Regression analyses showed that agreeableness (beta=.213***) and openness (beta=.109*) were directly related to preference for dogs, while no personality variable was significantly related to preference for cats. Comparison of means showed statistical similarity between people labeled as "dog people" and "of both animals". Both outperformed "cat people" in agreeableness and openness (*p<.05) with moderate to large effect sizes (d=.62-.80). The differences in the extroversion variable did not reach statistical significance, but they did have appreciable effect size when comparing people of dogs and cats (d = .33). Discussion. These findings offer unprecedented information in a Spanish sample. The results coincide with the previous literature in pointing out that the greater agreeableness and extroversion among dog people; The openness was also higher in this group, contrary to what is described in the research carried out in other countries. It is necessary to analyze the interaction between personality and culture to improve our understanding of human-animal interaction.
... Owners tend to think their dog resembles them in personality (Turcsán, Range, Virányi, Miklósi, & Kubinyi, 2012). Several studies have reported that dogs also tend to physically resemble their owners, such that photos of owners and their pets can be paired at above-chance levels (Levine, 2005;Nakajima, Yamamoto, & Yoshimoto, 2009;Roy & Christenfeld, 2004, 2005. Overall, these aspects of canine partner choice are similar to human partner choice: Physical appearance, personality, and behavior matter strongly in both. ...
Article
Humans often say they prefer certain attributes and trait levels and yet choose options inconsistent with those preferences, a phenomenon known as the stated–revealed preference gap. In this article, we compare preferences and choices in the decision to adopt a dog, a social-choice problem that is largely one-sided. We used existing and newly gathered field data about the dog adoption process to study how people make their choices of companion animals and how those choices can be improved. We found that in the real-world choice of dogs within an animal shelter, individuals generally showed a large amount of overlap between their stated preferences and their ratings of the traits of their chosen dog. However, there was little relationship between an adopter’s perceptions of their chosen dog’s behavioral traits and third-party in-shelter behavior evaluations of the same dogs, suggesting that it is difficult to predict which dogs will satisfy an adopter’s preferences. We also tested which commonly collected factors impact how quickly dogs are adopted from animal shelters. Overall, this work provides insight into the process of combining experimentally collected data and big data to elucidate choice behavior. Full-text publicly available to read without a subscription via Springer at: https://bit.ly/2QSNdQ4
Article
Full-text available
Studies have shown that possessions such as cars and dogs resemble their owners, and products such as dolls resemble their makers even when students make them. We conducted three experiments to examine which part of the dolls resembled their makers. The results demonstrated that people match dolls to their makers when their eye regions were masked (Experiment 1), and the matching is possible even with the back views of the dolls (Experiment 2). These results may indicate people match dolls to their makers based on resemblances other than faces. Experiment 3 demonstrated that no effect of resemblance in personality traits was observed when dolls’ faces were visible. However, the resemblance of personality traits assumed by the dolls and their makers play an important role in the matching judgment when dolls’ faces were invisible (because of back views).
Article
Full-text available
Many often say that people resemble their pets or that the faces of manga characters and Buddha statues resemble those of their artists. Previous studies demonstrated that participants could match dogs with their owners, suggesting that pets resemble their owners. Other studies also demonstrated that people can match personal belongings, including inanimate objects, to their owners. However, it is unknown whether people tend to make objects that resemble themselves. In this study, we examined whether people tend to make objects that resemble themselves with dolls made of cloth as stimuli. The results demonstrated that people tend to project themselves into dolls, even in the case of amateur college students. The mere exposure effect or the algorithm “self seeks like” may be at play in not only people’s selection of pets but also their making of objects.
Article
Bize benzeyenleri sevmek insanın doğasında vardır. Bebekler bile kendi zevklerini paylaşan bireyleri tercih eder ve zıt görüşlere sahip olanlardan hoşlanmazlar. Ancak çoğulcu toplumumuz farklılıkları kabul etmeyi ve aynı fikirde olmayanlara tahammül etmeyi gerektirir. Bebek araştırmalarındaki bulgular çeşitliliği kabul etmeyi teşvik edecek stratejilerle ilgili bilgi verebilir mi?
Article
Recent technological advances make it possible to create automated, virtual interviewers, called embodied conversational agents (ECAs). We study how an ECA compares to a human interviewer in three experiments. In experiment 1, we show that two theoretically motivated factors-making the ECA facially and vocally similar to the interviewee-result in the ECA performing similarly to or better than human interviewers for six antecedents of disclosure quality. In two additional experiments, we show that employees are on average 21 to 32 percent more likely to disclose violating internal controls to an ECA than to a human, even if the human interviewer has significant interviewing experience. These findings contribute to the ECA design literature by showing that similarity-enhancing features of ECAs increase the antecedents of disclosure. The findings also contribute to the accounting literature by demonstrating that ECA technology can increase the scope of interviewing in accounting without reducing interview quality.
Article
Full-text available
A latent-class model of rater agreement is presented for which 1 of the model parameters can be interpreted as the proportion of systematic agreement. The latent classes of the model emerge from the factorial combination of the "true" category in which a target belongs and the ease with which raters are able to classify targets into the true category. Several constrained cases of the model are described, and the relations to other well-known agreement models and kappa-type summary coefficients are explained. The differential quality of the rating categories can be assessed on the basis of the model fit. The model is illustrated using data from diagnoses of psychiatric disorders and classifications of individuals in a persuasive communication study.
Article
Full-text available
One tenant of folk psychology is that people tend to select or form a preference for pet dogs that have a similar appearance to themselves. A sample of 261 women judged the desirability of four breeds of dogs. Two breeds had lopped ears (English Springer Spaniel, Beagle) and two had pricked ears (Siberian Husky, Basenji). Long hairstyles in women produce a facial framing effect similar to lop ears while short or pulled back hairstyles produce a facial configuration more similar to prick-eared dogs. Consistent with this interpretation, women with long hair tended to prefer the lop-eared dogs while women with the short hairstyles preferred the prick-eared dogs, consistent with the folk belief. These results are interpreted in light of social psychological principles, namely the effects of familiarity and mere exposure on affect and interpersonal attraction.
Article
Full-text available
Theoretical and experimental studies suggest that mating and pair formation are not likely to be random. Assortative mating, characterized as self seeking like, seems to be widely practiced in nature. Experimental evidence for it is strong among humans seeking a mate. Assortative mating increases the probability of finding a genetically similar mate, without fomenting inbreeding, achieving assortative mating without hindering the working of other mate-selection strategies that aim to maximize the search for good genes, optimizing the working of sex in evolutionary terms. Self seeking like seems to be a behavioural inborn trait among humans, and here we present evidence that the same behavioural mechanism seems to be at work when humans choose a pet. We show that in a significant proportion of human–pet pairs, sampled in pet beauty contests, the partners show much higher facial resemblances than can be expected by random pair formation.
Article
Full-text available
This study attempted to determine whether people who live with each other for a long period of time grow physically similar in their facial features. Photographs of couples when they were first married and 25 years later were judged for physical similarity and for the likelihood that they were married. The results showed that there is indeed an increase in apparent similarity after 25 years of cohabitation. Moreover, increase in resemblance was associated with greater reported marital happiness. Among the explanations of this phenomenon that were examined, one based on a theory of emotional efference emerged as promising. This theory proposes that emotional processes produce vascular changes that are, in part, regulated by facial musculature. The facial muscles are said to act as ligatures on veins and arteries, and they thereby are able to divert blood from, or direct blood to, the brain. An implication of the vascular theory of emotional efference is that habitual use of facial musculature may permanently affect the physical features of the face. The implication holds further that two people who live with each other for a longer period of time, by virtue of repeated empathic mimicry, would grow physically similar in their facial features. Kin resemblance, therefore, may not be simply a matter of common genes but also a matter of prolonged social contact. A. L. Dear A. L.: As far as physical appearance is concerned, likes seem to attract. Some experts feel that this resemblance may partly be explained by the fact that couples who've lived together for some time usually eat the same diet and share the same habits. The Joyce Brothers Column April 1985
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this article is to propose a simple effect size estimate (obtained from the sample size, N, and a p value) that can be used (a) in meta-analytic research where only sample sizes and p values have been reported by the original investigator, (b) where no generally accepted effect size estimate exists, or (c) where directly computed effect size estimates are likely to be misleading. This effect size estimate is called r(equivalent) because it equals the sample point-biserial correlation between the treatment indicator and an exactly normally distributed outcome in a two-treatment experiment with N/2 units in each group and the obtained p value. As part of placing r(equivalent) into a broader context, the authors also address limitations of r(equivalent).
Article
Compatibility is an important aspect of the relationship between people and their pets as it is likely to influence both the quality and the longevity of the relationship. Perceived compatibility is also important since the stereotypical views we hold of particular person-pet combinations are likely to affect our pet selections. So far, little research has been carried out in this area. The present study investigated the existence and nature of pet-owner stereotypes with respect to male and female target persons in combination with cats and dogs. A sample of 102 students paired up photographs of ten people with photographs of ten pets and provided reasons for their pairings. Chi square analyses demonstrated significant pairing trends for nine of the ten target persons and thus evidence for pet-owner stereotypes. They also suggest that, as with human stereotypes, sex is a major component, specifically, males were associated with larger dogs and females with smaller dogs and cats. Qualitative analyses of the reasons for selecting pairs revealed a wide range of factors including sex, age, personality attributes and appearance.
Article
A latent-class model of rater agreement is presented for which 1 of the model parameters can be interpreted as the proportion of systematic agreement. The latent classes of the model emerge from the factorial combination of the "true" category in which a target belongs and the ease with which raters are able to classify targets into the true category. Several constrained cases of the model are described, and the relations to other well-known agreement models and kappa-type summary coefficients are explained. The differential quality of the rating categories can be assessed on the basis of the model fit. The model is illustrated using data from diagnoses of psychiatric disorders and classifications of individuals in a persuasive communication study.