ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

During speech acquisition, children form quick and rough hypotheses about the meaning of a new word after only a single exposure—a process dubbed “fast mapping.” Here we provide evidence that a border collie, Rico, is able to fast map. Rico knew the labels of over 200 different items. He inferred the names of novel items by exclusion learning and correctly retrieved those items right away as well as 4 weeks after the initial exposure. Fast mapping thus appears to be mediated by general learning and memory mechanisms also found in other animals and not by a language acquisition device that is special to humans.
DOI: 10.1126/science.1097859
, 1682 (2004); 304Science
et al.Juliane Kaminski,
"Fast Mapping"
Word Learning in a Domestic Dog: Evidence for
www.sciencemag.org (this information is current as of October 22, 2008 ):
The following resources related to this article are available online at
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/304/5677/1682
version of this article at:
including high-resolution figures, can be found in the onlineUpdated information and services,
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/304/5677/1682/DC1
can be found at: Supporting Online Material
found at:
can berelated to this articleA list of selected additional articles on the Science Web sites
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/304/5677/1682#related-content
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/304/5677/1682#otherarticles
, 3 of which can be accessed for free: cites 14 articlesThis article
44 article(s) on the ISI Web of Science. cited byThis article has been
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/304/5677/1682#otherarticles
2 articles hosted by HighWire Press; see: cited byThis article has been
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/psychology
Psychology
: subject collectionsThis article appears in the following
http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl
in whole or in part can be found at: this article
permission to reproduce of this article or about obtaining reprintsInformation about obtaining
registered trademark of AAAS.
is aScience2004 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title
CopyrightAmerican Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005.
(print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by theScience
on October 22, 2008 www.sciencemag.orgDownloaded from
steroids in asthma may be mediated, in part, by
their ability to increase pH and decrease AM-
Case bioactivity. In combination with studies
highlighting the expression of the chitinase-like
proteins, YM-1 and YM-2 (18) and AMCase
(19) in animal models, these studies emphasize
the potential importance of chitinases as mediators
of Th2 responses. The present studies also suggest
that AMCase is a potential therapeutic target that
can be manipulated to control asthma and other
forms of IL-13– or Th2-mediated pathology.
References and Notes
1. A. Ray, L. Cohn, J. Clin. Invest. 104, 985 (1999).
2. P. G. Holt, Toxicol. Lett. 86, 205 (1996).
3. P. G. Holt, Lancet 356, 1699 (2000).
4. R. G. Boot et al., J. Biol. Chem. 276, 6770 (2001).
5. M. Shahabuddin, T. Toyoshima, M. Aikawa, D. C.
Kaslow, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 90, 4266 (1993).
6. M. Shahabuddin, J. M. Vinetz, EXS 87, 223 (1999).
7. Y. Shibata, L. A. Foster, J. F. Bradfield, Q. N. Myrvik,
J. Immunol. 164, 1314 (2000).
8. J. T. Dessens et al., Infect. Immun. 69, 4041 (2001).
9. A. Herrera-Estrella, I. Chet, EXS 87, 171 (1999).
10. S. R. Palli, A. Retnakaran, EXS 87, 85 (1999).
11. Materials and methods and selected data are avail-
able as supporting material on Science Online.
12. K. D. Spindler, M. Spindler-Barth, S. Sakuda, Arch.
Insect Biochem. Physiol. 36, 223 (1997).
13. K. D. Spindler, M. Spindler-Barth, EXS 87, 201 (1999).
14. P. G. Seferian, M. L. Martinez, Vaccine 19, 661 (2000).
15. P. Strong, H. Clark, K. Reid, Clin. Exp. Allergy 32, 1794
(2002).
16. D. T. Umetsu, J. J. McIntire, O. Akbari, C. Macaubas,
R. H. DeKruyff, Nature Immunol. 3, 715 (2002).
17. J. F. Hunt et al., Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 161,
694 (2000).
18. D. C. Webb, A. N. McKenzie, P. S. Foster, J. Biol. Chem.
276, 41969 (2001).
19. N. Zimmermann et al., J. Clin. Invest. 111, 1863
(2003).
20. This work was supported by grants from NIH (R01-
HL-61904, R01-HL-64242, R01-HL-66571, and P50-
HL-56389) to J.A.E. and grants from NIH and Amer-
ican Lung Association (R01-HL-074095 and Research
Grant, respectively) to Z.Z. We thank K. Bertier and S.
Ardito for their excellent secretarial assistance and S.
Chen for her technical assistance.
Supporting Online Material
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/304/5677/1678/
DC1
Materials and Methods
Figs. S1 to S11
Table S1
5 January 2004; accepted 5 May 2004
Word Learning in a Domestic
Dog: Evidence for “Fast Mapping”
Juliane Kaminski, Josep Call, Julia Fischer*
During speech acquisition, children form quick and rough hypotheses about
the meaning of a new word after only a single exposure—a process dubbed
“fast mapping.” Here we provide evidence that a border collie, Rico, is able
to fast map. Rico knew the labels of over 200 different items. He inferred
the names of novel items by exclusion learning and correctly retrieved those
items right away as well as 4 weeks after the initial exposure. Fast mapping
thus appears to be mediated by general learning and memory mechanisms
also found in other animals and not by a language acquisition device that
is special to humans.
The rate at which most toddlers acquire their
vocabulary is astounding: From about 2 years
of age, typical English-speaking children in-
corporate about 10 new words per day into
their vocabulary until they reach an average
vocabulary size of 60,000 words by the time
they graduate from high school (1). Several
studies have shown that children have a set of
operating principles that guide the task of
word learning (24 ). However, it remains a
matter of debate which of these principles are
unique to language learning and which are
more general cognitive abilities that may be
shared with other living creatures. We inves-
tigated the outer limits of a domestic dog’s
“word learning”; that is, his ability to acquire
the relation between a word and the object
that this word refers to (the referent). By
studying his retrieval behavior with famil-
iar and novel items, we specifically tested
whether he would be able to infer the ref-
erent of a new word by exclusion learning:
that is, to “fast map” (57 ) and retain this
knowledge over time.
The study animal, Rico, is a border collie
and was born in December 1994. He lives as
a pet with his owners and was reported by
them to know the labels of over 200 items,
mostly children’s toys and balls, which he
correctly retrieved upon request. Rico was
first introduced to fetching items when he
was 10 months of age, when his owners
placed three different items in different loca-
tions around the flat and asked the dog for
one of these items. Rico was rewarded with
food or play if he fetched the correct object.
He was gradually familiarized with an in-
creasing number of items. Typically, the
owners introduced new items by presenting
them and saying their name two or three
times. Rico then got the chance to play with
the new item, and it was subsequently inte-
grated into the collection of other items.
However, it remained unclear whether a
“Clever Hans” (8) effect might account for
his performance. The first experiment was
therefore designed to assess Rico’s ability to
correctly retrieve his various items under
controlled conditions. We randomly assigned
the 200 items he was reportedly familiar with
to 20 sets of 10 different items each. While
the owner waited with the dog in a separate
room, the experimenter arranged a set of
items in the experimental room and then
joined the owner and the dog. Next, the ex-
perimenter instructed the owner to request the
dog to bring two randomly chosen items (one
after the other) from the adjacent room (9).
While Rico searched for the requested item,
he could not see the owner or the experiment-
er. He retrieved a total of 37 out of 40 items
correctly (binomial test, P 0.001). This
experiment showed that Rico indeed knew
the labels of these items. One may raise the
objection that the words may in fact consti-
tute one-word propositions, such as “fetch-
the-sock.” However, anecdotal evidence sug-
gests that he indeed understands that the
words refer to the objects. For instance, he
can be instructed to put an item into a box or
to bring it to a certain person. More system-
atic testing will be needed to specify his
understanding of entire phrases. In any case,
the number of labeled objects is substan-
tially larger than those reported in previous
studies with dogs, where subjects were test-
ed with only three to five objects (10, 11).
Rico’s “vocabulary size” is comparable to
that of language-trained apes, dolphins, sea
lions, and parrots (12).
To assess Rico’s ability to fast map, we
placed a novel item together with seven fa-
miliar items in an adjacent room (total n 8
items requested in 8 trials). In this so-called
identification task, we conducted a total of 10
sessions in which we introduced 10 novel
items. In the first trial of a session, the owner
always asked Rico to bring a familiar item,
and in the second or third trial asked him to
bring an item using the novel name (9). After
the completion of a session, Rico was al-
lowed to take a break before another session
commenced. Rico retrieved the novel item
from the first session on and was overall
correct in 7 out of 10 sessions (binomial test,
P 0.001). Apparently, he was able to link
the novel word to the novel item based on
exclusion learning, either because he knew
that the familiar items already had names or
because they were not novel. Four weeks
after the initial and sole exposure, we as-
Department of Developmental and Comparative Psy-
chology, Max-Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthro-
pology, Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig, Germany.
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-
mail: fischer@eva.mpg.de
R EPORTS
11 JUNE 2004 VOL 304 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org1682
on October 22, 2008 www.sciencemag.orgDownloaded from
sessed Ricos retention of the relation be-
tween the novel word and the novel item. In
this retention task, we only used those objects
that Rico had successfully retrieved in the
identification task. In between the identifica-
tion and the retention task, he had no access
to the target items. We placed a target item
together with four completely novel and four
familiar items in a room (total n 9 items)
and asked him first to bring a familiar item
and subsequently (in the second or third trial)
to bring the target item. Four weeks after the
identification task, he correctly retrieved the
target item in 3 out of 6 sessions (P 0.1).
This retrieval rate is comparable to the per-
formance of 3-year-old toddlers (1, 5). In the
other cases, he brought one of the unfamiliar
items and never one of the familiar items. We
then replicated the experiment, first conducting
a new set of identification tasks using novel
items. When he was tested for retention of the
novel word-object combinations 10 min after
the identification task, he correctly retrieved the
target item in 4 out of 6 trials (P 0.02).
These experiments demonstrate that Rico
reliably associates arbitrary acoustic patterns
(human words) with specific items in his
environment. Apparently, Ricos extensive
experience with acquiring the names of ob-
jects allowed him to establish the rule that
things can have names. Consequently, he was
able to deduce the referent of a new word on
the basis of the principle of exclusion when
presented with a novel item along with a set
of familiar items (13, 14 ). This corresponds
to the acquisition of the novel-name
nameless category principle (3) and also to
the avoidance of lexical overlap described in
children (4 ). Moreover, Rico was able to
store this knowledge about the link between
word and object in memory, because he was
able to correctly retrieve the target item from
a set of novel and familiar items both imme-
diately after introduction of the novel word-
object combination and 4 weeks later.
It remains a matter for further empirical
investigation whether Ricos accomplish-
ments are based on an exceptional mind or
a result of his extensive exposure to many
word-object combinations. Undoubtedly,
he is a highly motivated dog, and some of
his talent may be accounted for by the fact
that border collies are working dogs (15).
More generally, dogs appear to have been
evolutionarily selected for attending to the
communicative intentions of humans (16
18). Nevertheless, we assume that Ricos
performance can be decomposed into a set
of simpler mechanisms. These consist of (i)
his acquisition of the principle that objects
have labels; (ii) a general learning mecha-
nism, namely learning by exclusion (emer-
gent matching) (19); and (iii) the ability to
store that knowledge in memory. There-
fore, our results strongly support the view
that a seemingly complex linguistic skill
previously described only in human chil-
dren may be mediated by simpler cognitive
building blocks that are also present in
another species. Whether Ricos ability to
form a link between a label and an object is
homologous to childrens knowledge about
the names of things remains a matter for
further investigation. Clearly, from early
on, toddlers have a much broader knowl-
edge than Rico about the meaning of words,
and they can distinguish between different
functions of words such as verbs, adjec-
tives, and proper nouns. Moreover, children
are able to use their newly acquired knowl-
edge productively; that is, they are able to
say the words whose meaning they have
identified through fast mapping. Nonethe-
less, our findings corroborate the assump-
tion that listenersability to attach meaning
to specific sounds evolved much earlier
than, and independently from, a flexible
production of specific sound patterns (20).
That is, some of the perceptual and cogni-
tive mechanisms that may mediate the com-
prehension of speech were already in place
before early humans began to talk (21, 22).
References and Notes
1. P. Bloom, How Children Learn the Meanings of Words
(MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2000).
2. D. A. Baldwin, Dev. Psychol. 29, 832 (1993).
3. C. B. Mervis, J. Bertrand, Child Dev. 65, 1662 (1994).
4. M. Tomasello, Constructing a Language (Harvard
Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 2003).
5. L. Markson, P. Bloom, Nature 385, 813 (1997).
6. S. Carey, E. Bartlett, Child Lang. Dev. 15, 29 (1978).
7. T. H. Heibeck, E. M. Markman, Child Dev. 58, 1021
(1987).
8. O. Pfungst, Clever Hans: The Horse of Mr. von Osten
(1911), published in Classics in Psychology, 1855-
1914: A Collection of Key Works (Thoemmes, Bristol,
UK, 1998).
9. Further details on materials and methods and a video
clip demonstrating Rico’s performance during a trial
are available on Science Online.
10. C. J. Warden, L. H. Warner, Q. Rev. Biol. 3, 1 (1928).
11. C. A. Young, Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 32, 75 (1991).
12. H. W. L. Miles, S. E. Harper, in Hominid Culture in
Primate Perspective, D. Quiatt, J. Itani, Eds. (Univer-
sity Press of Colorado, Niwot, CO, 1994).
13. R. J. Schusterman, K. Krieger, Psychol. Rec. 34,3
(1984).
14. L. M. Herman, D. G. Richards, J. P. Wolz, Cognition 16,
129 (1984).
15. R. Coppinger, R. Schneider, in The Domestic Dog: its
Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People,J.
Serpell, Ed. (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1995).
16. B. Hare, M. Brown, C. Williamson, M. Tomasello,
Science 298, 1634 (2002).
17. J. Call, J. Bra¨uer, J. Kaminski, M. Tomasello, J. Comp.
Psychol. 117, 257 (2003).
18. A. Miklosi et al., Curr. Biol. 13, 763 (2003).
19. K. M. Wilkinson, W. V. Dube, W. J. McIlvane, Psychol.
Rec. 48, 407 (1998).
20. J. Fischer, Anim. Behav. 67, 655 (2004).
21. M. D. Hauser, N. Chomsky, W. T. Fitch, Science 298,
1569 (2002).
22. T. J. Bergmann, J. C. Beehner, D. L. Cheney, R. M.
Seyfarth, Science 302, 1234 (2003).
23. We thank S. Baus and her family for allowing us to
study Rico and for her enthusiastic participation in
this study. R. Mundry gave statistical advice, and C.
Teufel, M. Hauser, and three anonymous reviewers
provided valuable comments on the manuscript.
Funding by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft to
J.F. (grant Fi707/4) is gratefully acknowledged.
Supporting Online Material
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/304/5677/1682/
DC1
Materials and Methods
Movie S1
15 March 2004; accepted 26 April 2004
R EPORTS
www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 304 11 JUNE 2004 1683
on October 22, 2008 www.sciencemag.orgDownloaded from
... Our definition of a word differs from that of many scholars who study the communicative abilities of animals. As evidence that animals use words, they cite the communicative abilities of chimpanzees, monkeys, dolphins, dogs, and birds (Savage- Rumbaugh et al., 1993;Hauser et al., 2002;Kaminski et al., 2004;Seyfarth et al., 2005;Pepperberg, 2016). It is important to note that none of those studies defined words. ...
... Studies of comprehension cannot provide a definitive answer to the question of whether animals use words because it is not clear if a subject's response to an experimenter's vocal command is based on the perception of its acoustic properties or its lexical status. That problem arises both in instances of individual commands (e.g., dogs, Kaminski et al., 2004) and in sequences of words (e.g., chimpanzees, Savage- Rumbaugh et al., 1993). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
This collection of papers from world leaders in developmental psychology, neuroscience, music, education and psychiatry consolidates the lifetime work of Emeritus Professor Colwyn Trevarthen, FRSE. Spanning research from the 1960s to the present, Trevarthen’s contributions to science have changed our understanding of infancy, neuroscience, education and musicality. The present collection of papers from these diverse fields describes current issues, principles and perspectives for working practice on the role of intersubjectivity in early human life, its contribution to health and learning, and therefore its role in scientific understanding of the fundamentals of the human mind. By bringing together scholars, scientists, medical and educational practitioners, this book serves as a landmark for the field of intersubjectivity and celebrates Trevarthen’s 93rd birthday.
... The term symbol is often used as a synonym for a sign, which is classically defined by Ferdinand de Saussure as an arbitrary binding between a 'signifier' (for instance a word, a digit, but also a traffic sign, logo, etc.) and a 'signified' (the meaning or content to which the signifier refers) de Saussure, 1995. In that respect, however, many non-human animals, including chimpanzees, macaques, but also dogs, are able to learn hundreds of such indexical relationships, even with arbitrary signs (Kaminski et al., 2004;Livingstone et al., 2010;Matsuzawa, 1985;Premack, 1971). Even bees can learn to associate arbitrary visual shapes to abstract representations such as visual quantities (two or three elements) independently of the density, size, and colour of the elements in the visual display (Howard et al., 2019). ...
... However, they may also merely show that animals may begin to spontaneously reverse new associations once they have received extensive training with bidirectional ones (Kojima, 1984). The bulk of the literature strongly suggests that while animals easily learn indexical associations, especially monkeys and chimpanzees (Diester and Nieder, 2007;Livingstone et al., 2010;Matsuzawa, 1985;Premack, 1971), but also dogs (Fugazza et al., 2021;Kaminski et al., 2004), vocal birds (e.g. Pepperberg, 2009), and even bees (Howard et al., 2019), they exhibit little or no evidence for genuine symbolic processing. ...
Article
Full-text available
The emergence of symbolic thinking has been proposed as a dominant cognitive criterion to distinguish humans from other primates during hominisation. Although the proper definition of a symbol has been the subject of much debate, one of its simplest features is bidirectional attachment: the content is accessible from the symbol, and vice versa. Behavioural observations scattered over the past four decades suggest that this criterion might not be met in non-human primates, as they fail to generalise an association learned in one temporal order (A to B) to the reverse order (B to A). Here, we designed an implicit fMRI test to investigate the neural mechanisms of arbitrary audio–visual and visual–visual pairing in monkeys and humans and probe their spontaneous reversibility. After learning a unidirectional association, humans showed surprise signals when this learned association was violated. Crucially, this effect occurred spontaneously in both learned and reversed directions, within an extended network of high-level brain areas, including, but also going beyond, the language network. In monkeys, by contrast, violations of association effects occurred solely in the learned direction and were largely confined to sensory areas. We propose that a human-specific brain network may have evolved the capacity for reversible symbolic reference.
... Because of this setup, it is important to note that the cued location was either the left or the right side for all dogs, but never the center, since dogs tend to make center choices in situations when they are uncertain of the correct response (Range et al., 2011). On the other hand, we used three locations because dogs have a tendency to learn easily by exclusion (Kaminski et al., 2004); therefore using only two containers would increase the probability of applying this technique and allow dogs to learn only one command, and all other cues would be associated with the other side. ...
Article
Full-text available
The role of sleep in memory consolidation is a widely discussed but still debated area of research. In light of the fact that memory consolidation during sleep is an evolutionary adaptive function, investigating the same phenomenon in nonhuman model species is highly relevant for its understanding. One such species, which has acquired human-analog sociocognitive skills through convergent evolution, is the domestic dog. Family dogs have surfaced as an outstanding animal model in sleep research, and their learning skills (in a social context) are subject to sleep-dependent memory consolidation. These results, however, are correlational, and the next challenge is to establish causality. In the present study, we aimed to adapt a TMR (targeted memory reactivation) paradigm in dogs and investigate its effect on sleep parameters. Dogs ( N = 16) learned new commands associated with different locations and afterward took part in a sleep polysomnography recording when they were re-exposed to one of the previously learned commands. The results did not indicate a cueing benefit on choice performance. However, there was evidence for a decrease in choice latency after sleep, while the density (occurrence/minute) of fast sleep spindles was also notably higher during TMR recordings than adaptation recordings from the same animals and even compared with a larger reference sample from a previous work. Our study provides empirical evidence that TMR is feasible with family dogs, even during a daytime nap. Furthermore, the present study highlights several methodological and conceptual challenges for future research.
... Some dogs have been shown to understand a large number of words and can competently produce [44] and respond [45] to them appropriately. Although it has long been established that dogs can learn and understand a wide range of human words [46], until recently they have not had the capacity to produce words in return. These technologies provide a means by which animals can directly communicate to their human caregivers. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent discussions in animal welfare have emphasised the importance of animal agency—the ability of animals to make choices and exert control over their environment in a way that aligns with their needs and preferences. In this paper, we discuss the importance of animal agency for welfare and examine how use of some types of animal–computer interaction can enable animals to exercise more agency in captive environments through increased choice and control, cognitive challenge, and social interactions; as well as considering some of the potential limitations of such efforts.
... Dogs are not only able to comprehend human gestures, but they are also capable of learning numerous words. Besides early studies reporting this ability in dogs (Griebel and Oller, 2012;Kaminski et al., 2004;Pilley and Reid, 2011), there has been a recent upsurge in research on object label learning, a skill that only a few "gifted" individuals seem to possess (e.g., Dror et al., 2021Dror et al., , 2023Dror et al., , 2024Fugazza et al., 2022;Fugazza, Andics, et al., 2021;. These dogs can quickly acquire large vocabularies of object labels, which they can recall for at least 2 years (Dror et al., 2024). ...
... Perceptually, dogs exhibit remarkable receptivity to human speech cues [23][24][25][26], can learn extensive word repertoires [27], demonstrate fast mapping [28] and statistical learning abilities [29], and possibly word-elicited mental representations [30]. Moreover, behavioural evidence suggests that humans possess a unique sensitivity to dogs' acoustic signals, as sightless individuals without prior experience with dogs can accurately assess emotional valence from dog vocalisations [31], and humans generally demonstrate greater proficiency in evaluating the emotional valence of dog vocalisations compared to those of other species [32,33]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Within species, vocal and auditory systems presumably coevolved to converge on a critical temporal acoustic structure that can be best produced and perceived. While dogs cannot produce articulated sounds, they respond to speech, raising the question as to whether this heterospecific receptive ability could be shaped by exposure to speech or remains bounded by their own sensorimotor capacity. Using acoustic analyses of dog vocalisations, we show that their main production rhythm is slower than the dominant (syllabic) speech rate, and that human–dog-directed speech falls halfway in between. Comparative exploration of neural (electroencephalography) and behavioural responses to speech reveals that comprehension in dogs relies on a slower speech rhythm tracking (delta) than humans’ (theta), even though dogs are equally sensitive to speech content and prosody. Thus, the dog audio-motor tuning differs from humans’, and we hypothesise that humans may adjust their speech rate to this shared temporal channel as means to improve communication efficacy.
Article
Full-text available
It is well known that dogs are capable of following human verbal instructions. However, very little is known about the equivalent ability in cats. In this study, we used a switched stimuli task to examine whether cats rapidly form picture-word association, which is a fundamental ability for word learning. We presented cats with two meaningless picture-word combinations, in the habituation phase. Then, on half of the trials we switched the combination (switched condition), but the other half of the trials remained as before (non-switched condition). If cats rapidly form picture-word association, they were expected to look at the monitor for longer in the switched condition, reflecting detection of the change. We used human speech as stimuli in Exp.1, and mechanical sounds (electronic sounds) in Exp.2. Cats expressed detection of the switched combination in Exp.1, where human speech and objects were paired. However, in Exp.2 where non-social sounds and objects were paired, there was no statistical difference between switched and non-switched conditions, although there was a main effect of condition when the data from the two experiments were pooled. These results demonstrate that cats can rapidly form picture-word association. Further research should investigate whether domestication has played a role in this ability.
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has documented infants' ability to follow adults' line-of-regard and pointing gestures by 9–22 mo of age, but it has not been clear whether infants appreciate that such cues are a priviledged source of information about word reference. Study 1 demonstrated that this is understood by at least 19–20 mo: Infants used referential cues to guide a new word–object mapping even though this required that they ignore competition from temporal contiguity. In Study 2, the effects observed in Study 1 were not obtained with actions that served to enhance the salience of an object nonreferentially (e.g., manipulating without looking), suggesting that infants appreciate that actions that accompany labeling are not necessarily referential. Taken together, these indicate that language learning is grounded in a relatively rich understanding of cues to reference, at least from late infancy on. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Researchers from behavior analytic, developmental, and comparative perspectives have all investigated the conditions under which new arbitrary (symbolic) stimulus-stimulus relations are acquired. For example, young children, people with severe mental retardation, and several species of nonhuman mammals all exhibit emergent matching (EM) in the context of a well-established matching-to-sample baseline: When presented with an undefined sample stimulus and a comparison array that includes one undefined comparison and one or more baseline comparisons, participants select the undefined comparison. Further, subsequent testing may show a learning outcome: Exposure to EM trials may result in a new defined relation involving the formerly undefined stimuli. Between 1974 and the late 1980s, emergent matching and learning outcomes were described independently by behavior analytic, child language, and animal cognition researchers. Cross-literature citations were virtually absent, however. More recently, crossdisciplinary citations have begun to appear. This article briefly reviews the history of EM research, emphasizing the independent development of research programs, methods, and terminology in the three disciplines. We then identify several research areas where a multidisciplinary approach may benefit all concerned.
Article
The present investigations were undertaken to compare interspecific communicative abilities of dogs and wolves, which were socialized to humans at comparable levels. The first study demonstrated that socialized wolves were able to locate the place of hidden food indicated by the touching and, to some extent, pointing cues provided by the familiar human experimenter, but their performance remained inferior to that of dogs. In the second study, we have found that, after undergoing training to solve a simple manipulation task, dogs that are faced with an insoluble version of the same problem look/gaze at the human, while socialized wolves do not. Based on these observations, we suggest that the key difference between dog and wolf behavior is the dogs' ability to look at the human's face. Since looking behavior has an important function in initializing and maintaining communicative interaction in human communication systems, we suppose that by positive feedback processes (both evolutionary and ontogenetically) the readiness of dogs to look at the human face has lead to complex forms of dog-human communication that cannot be achieved in wolves even after extended socialization.
Book
How do children learn that the word "dog" refers not to all four-legged animals, and not just to Ralph, but to all members of a particular species? How do they learn the meanings of verbs like "think," adjectives like "good," and words for abstract entities such as "mortgage" and "story"? The acquisition of word meaning is one of the fundamental issues in the study of mind. According to Paul Bloom, children learn words through sophisticated cognitive abilities that exist for other purposes. These include the ability to infer others' intentions, the ability to acquire concepts, an appreciation of syntactic structure, and certain general learning and memory abilities. Although other researchers have associated word learning with some of these capacities, Bloom is the first to show how a complete explanation requires all of them. The acquisition of even simple nouns requires rich conceptual, social, and linguistic capacities interacting in complex ways. This book requires no background in psychology or linguistics and is written in a clear, engaging style. Topics include the effects of language on spatial reasoning, the origin of essentialist beliefs, and the young child's understanding of representational art. The book should appeal to general readers interested in language and cognition as well as to researchers in the field. Bradford Books imprint
Article
Two California sea lions ( Zalophus californianus) were trained in a symbolic matching paradigm. Gestural signs produced by movements of the trainer's arms and hands referred to types of objects, modifiers, and actions. Both Ss were eventually trained with 3-sign constructions consisting of Modifier + Object + Action, in that order. Modifiers consisted of size and color attributes or of different locations of the objects. After 24 mo of training, 1 S had a comprehension vocabulary of 20 signs, while the other S achieved a comprehension vocabulary of 16 signs after 20 mo. Semantic comprehension was considered to be demonstrated if the S could follow directions when each sign was contrasted with every other in a series of commands. Currently, the Ss' semantic comprehension consists of 64 and 190 3-sign combinations, respectively. (30 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Few studies have addressed the development of nonhuman primate infants' responses to conspecific vocalizations. Previous studies showed that the appropriate response to alarm, intergroup and long-distance contact calls emerged at about 6 months of age. It remained unclear whether this age constitutes a watershed in terms of infants' sociocognitive development or whether it was due to the types of stimuli used in the experiments. I therefore examined the development of infant Barbary macaque, Macaca sylvanus, responses to maternal calls, under the assumption that recognition of the mother is one of the tasks that infants should master as early as possible. I presented infants of different age categories with short bouts of screams recorded from their mothers or another female of the same social group. Experiments on yearlings confirmed the suitability of the experimental approach: yearlings responded significantly more strongly to maternal calls than to calls from unrelated females. Infants were tested at 4, 10 and 16 weeks of age. In the youngest age group, they failed to respond to the playbacks, whereas from 10 weeks of age on they responded significantly more strongly to maternal calls, suggesting that by this age they recognized their mothers by voice. These results suggest that the developmental trajectories in the domain of comprehension learning may be flexible, in the sense that infant responses may depend on the salience of, and the exposure to, the call type under study. The experiments also show that screams may transmit individual-specific characteristics that are perceptually salient to the listeners.
Article
Domestic canids now have more uses than as pets and hunting partners. For example, humans have found them useful in the detection of drugs, as companions (e.g. those used in therapy for the mentally handicapped), and as aids to the physically handicapped. In this study, three dogs were used as subjects in five separate experimental tasks to test the abilities of these animals for increased usefulness as service dogs for the handicapped. The tasks included retrieving a particular wooden object in a three-choice situation as requested by the handler, locating the same objects at varying heights, retrieving the objects from behind a barrier, retrieving the objects when replaced by plastic ones, and returning to the handler without an object when the requested item was missing from a group of three presented objects.All three subjects were able to complete all five experimental tasks at a correct performance level of 83% or above (range 83–92%). In addition, their performances were comparable to those which have been studied in other (non-human) species. Thus, in this study, the dogs showed that it may be possible for humans to increase the difficulty of tasks required and, hence, the usefulness of domestic dogs as aids to the handicapped.