ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Effects of Alcohol and Other Drugs on Driver Performance

Taylor & Francis
Traffic Injury Prevention
Authors:

Abstract

In the past century we have learned that driving performance is impaired by alcohol even in low dosage, and that many other drugs are also linked to impairment. This paper is a summary of some of the more relevant studies in the past fifty years – an overview of our knowledge and unanswered questions. There is no evidence of a threshold blood alcohol (BAC) below which impairment does not occur, and there is no defined category of drivers who will not be impaired by alcohol. Alcohol increases not only the probability of collision, but also the probability of poor clinical outcome for injuries sustained when impaired by alcohol. This review samples the results of the myriad studies that have been performed during the last half century as experiments have moved from examination of simple sensory, perceptual and motor behaviours to more complex measures of cognitive functioning such as divided attention and mental workload. These more sophisticated studies show that significant impairment occurs at very low BACs (<0.02 gm/100 ml). However, much remains to be determined regarding the more emotional aspects of behaviour, such as judgment, aggression and risk taking. Considering that the majority of alcohol related accidents occur at night, there is a need for increased examination on the role of fatigue, circadian cycles and sleep loss. The study of the effects of drugs other than alcohol is more complex because of the number of substances of potential interest, the difficulties estimating drug levels and the complexity of the drug/subject interactions. The drugs of current concern are marijuana, the benzodiazepines, other psychoactive medications, the stimulants and the narcotics. No one test or group of tests currently meets the need for detecting and documenting impairment, either in the laboratory or at the roadside.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=gcpi20
Download by: [Inst St Vincents] Date: 17 November 2016, At: 15:37
Traffic Injury Prevention
ISSN: 1538-9588 (Print) 1538-957X (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gcpi20
Effects of Alcohol and Other Drugs on Driver
Performance
E. J.D. OGDEN & H. MOSKOWITZ
To cite this article: E. J.D. OGDEN & H. MOSKOWITZ (2004) Effects of Alcohol and
Other Drugs on Driver Performance, Traffic Injury Prevention, 5:3, 185-198, DOI:
10.1080/15389580490465201
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15389580490465201
Published online: 11 Aug 2010.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 970
View related articles
Citing articles: 90 View citing articles
Traffic Injury Prevention, 5:185–198, 2004
Copyright C
2004 Taylor & Francis Inc.
ISSN: 1538-9588 print / 1538-957X online
DOI: 10.1080/15389580490465201
Effects of Alcohol and Other Drugs on Driver
Performance
E. J. D. OGDEN
Centre for Drugs and Driving, Swinburne University of Technology, Australia
H. MOSKOWITZ
Southern California Research Institute, Encino, California, USA
In the past century we have learned that driving performance is impaired by alcohol even in low dosage, and that many other
drugs are also linked to impairment. This article is a summary of some of the more relevant studies in the past fifty years—an
overview of our knowledge and unanswered questions.
There is no evidence of a threshold blood alcohol concentration (BAC) below which impairment does not occur, and
there is no defined category of drivers who will not be impaired by alcohol. Alcohol increases not only the probability of
collision, but also the probability of poor clinical outcome for injuries sustained when impaired by alcohol. This article
samples the results of the myriad studies that have been performed during the last half century as experiments have moved
from examination of simple sensory, perceptual, and motor behaviors to more complex measures of cognitive functioning,
such as divided attention and mental workload. These more sophisticated studies show that significant impairment occurs at
very low BACs (<0.02 gm/100 ml).
However, much remains to be determined regarding the more emotional aspects of behavior, such as judgment, aggression,
and risk taking. Considering that the majority of alcohol-related accidents occur at night, there is a need for increased
examination on the role of fatigue, circadian cycles, and sleep loss.
The study of the effects of drugs other than alcohol is more complex because of the number of substances of potential
interest, the difficulties estimating drug levels and the complexity of the drug/subject interactions. The drugs of current
concern are marijuana, the benzodiazepines, other psychoactive medications, the stimulants, and the narcotics. No single
test or group of tests currently meets the need for detecting and documenting impairment, either in the laboratory or at the
roadside.
Keywords Alcohol Impairment; Drug Impairment; Alcohol; Drugs; Driver Performance; Traffic Safety; Driving Skills;
Impairment Tests
Few people in developed countries can imagine life without
their car. While most drivers would know the economic cost of
their vehicle, how many of them stop to count the social cost, or
to consider the associated morbidity and mortality? Collisions
involving vehicles have left countless people crippled, maimed,
or bereaved. Road trauma is the most common cause of prema-
ture death among young adults in most countries not embroiled
in war. Although transport safety continues to be improved by
better engineering of roads and vehicles, the most prominent
causal factor in collisions remains the human element.
Transport mishaps related to alcohol impairment are not new.
It was common knowledge in the pre-industrial age that drunk-
Received 4 February 2004; accepted 7 April 2004.
Address correspondence to H. Moskowitz, Southern California Research
Institute, 4138 Royal Crest Place, Encino, CA 91436. E-mail: herbmosk@ucla.
edu
ards were at increased risk when riding horses or driving horse-
drawn vehicles, but to a limited extent a good horse could share
the responsibility for safe transport with an incompetent driver,
compensate for human impairment, and plod homeward in rel-
ative safety. The advent of the horseless carriage brought the
era of forgiving transportation to an end. The consequences of
combining recreational drug usage and transportation became
more serious.
One of the most remarkable features about vehicle driving
is that a very large fraction of the human race can master the
basic concepts and acquire rudimentary skill at the task relatively
quickly and without expending large amounts of time or energy
(Evans, 1991). Yet the complexity of the man-machine-traffic
interaction can make the tasks of driving complex and hazardous.
Although the increased injury toll associated with motor
transportation became apparent soon after the beginning of the
twentieth century, there was only limited research on driving
185
186 E. J. D. OGDEN AND H. MOSKOWITZ
behavior in general, or driving under inuence of drugs or alcohol
in particular. It was perhaps the massive increase in private mo-
tor vehicle ownership after World War II (WWII) that led to
the increasing research concern with the human costs of mass
private transportation. Prior to the 1950s, for example, although
there were frequent prevalence studies on alcohol in driving ac-
cidents, there was only one primitive case-controlled study. The
1950s saw the beginning of a major spurt in epidemiological
studies of driving accidents and the inuence of alcohol.
The growth in motor vehicle usage was matched by growth
in research into the behavioral effects of alcohol. A series of
large case-controlled epidemiological studies after WWII pro-
vided the evidence for the close association between increased
BAC and increased accident, injury, and fatality rates. These pio-
neering studies included Lucas et al. (1955) in Toronto, Canada,
Vamosi (1960), in Bratislava, Czechoslovakia, McCarroll and
Haddon (1962) in New York City, and Borkenstein et al. (1964)
in Grand Rapids, Michigan. These studies were highly inuen-
tial in making the public aware of the real accident frequency
associated with alcohol.
At the same time the nature of the impairment that under-
lies the increased crash rate was subjected to research at several
levels: studies of the individual components of the driving task,
studies of simulated driving, and studies of on-road driving. This
article has its major focus on alcohol because alcohol is associ-
ated with as many fatal collisions as all other drugs combined.
For instance, Drummer et al. (2004) describe the toxicological
analysis of 3,398 collisions in which the driver died. Of these,
1,704 cases (50.1%) screened negative for alcohol and drugs.
Alcohol was present in 29% of cases (n =990), drugs were
present in 27% of cases (n =907).
Unfortunately, there have been only a few studies that have
examined the nature of the driving impairment from an epi-
demiological perspective. This is partly because of the difculty
in assigning a particular driving decit to an associated behav-
ioral failure, since the same driving error could be due to many
behaviors.
Police descriptions of crashes are typically assigned to the
cause of current interest. For example, Moskowitz (2004) exam-
ined a series of crashes at T-intersections where drivers drove
through into the wooded area beyond the junction where the
road no longer existed. From the 1950s through the 1980s the
majority of police reports characterized this as loss of control.
Nowadays inattentionhas become a favorite explanation, al-
though there is no evidence that driver behavior has changed.
There have only been a few multi-disciplinary accident investi-
gation studies with professional human-factors personnel aware
of research in cognitive psychology. More studies of this type
would help enlighten the epidemiological record.
Moskowitz and Robinson (1988) and Moskowitz and
Fiorentino (2000) have extensively reviewed the experimental
literature on alcohol and driving. Jones et al. (2003) have re-
viewed the literature on drugs. This article does not purport to
be a comprehensive literature review, so much as an overview
of the eld and the directions research has taken.
ALCOHOL
Alcohol is a drug whose main acute effect is on the cen-
tral nervous system. Unlike anesthetic agents that depress all
brain functions, the effects of alcohol are rst manifest in the
brain centers involved in highly integrated functions, such as
skilled performance. The analysis of sensory information, the
control of intricate movement patterns and short-term memory
are especially sensitive to alcohol. The effects on human skills
and performance commence at the lowest measurable BACs
and increase in a roughly dose-related manner (Moskowitz &
Robinson, 1988).
There is no evidence of a threshold effect for alcohol because
some impairment of performance occurs at the lowest levels that
can be measured; nor is there a level at which a sudden transition
from unimpaired to impaired can be expected: whatever the level
of BAC examined, at least some skills can be demonstrated to be
signicantly impaired. The effects of alcohol are dependent both
on the quantity consumed and the nature of the performance re-
quired. There is no evidence that low BACs improve any human
skill (Moskowitz, 1985).
The variation in individual performance at BACs below
0.10% is sufciently broad that uncertainties must attach to any
prediction of the precise effects of a given quantity of alco-
hol on an individual. All individuals are impaired at any level
of alcohol and the impairment increases as the BAC increases.
Individual psychomotor abilities vary at the no-alcohol base-
line, so that viewing an individual behavior at low BAC can
give only limited information about the BAC. Conversely, as
impairment can be demonstrated at all levels of BAC there is
general agreement that the skills relating to driving can be pre-
sumed to be adversely affected below 0.10% (the limit in many
jurisdictions) and many of the skills related to driving are sig-
nicantly impaired below 0.05% (Dunbar et al., 1987; Mitchell,
1985; Moskowitz, 1985; Starmer et al., 1988). Moskowitz and
Robinson (1988) concluded: The legislature is free to pro-
hibit driving at any B.A.C., since such a limit would not con-
tradict the scientic data demonstrating no lower limit to
impairment.
Subjective Measures of Impairment
For the purpose of this article, impairment has been dened as the
occurrence of a change for the worse in the performance required
for safe driving. Impairment is not the same as intoxication.
Dictionaries give multiple denitions of intoxication using terms
like drunk,”“inebriation,and stupefaction,which are not
scientic concepts and have no relevance to the driving task.
When the layman thinks of impairment, he probably envis-
ages the obvious signs of lack of judgment, poor self-control,
and loss of gross motor skills. Widmark (1981) reported on
the experience in Sweden of testing drivers arrested on sus-
picion of being under the inuence of alcohol. Swedish law
required these drivers to be examined by physicians at a po-
lice station using a standard seven-item behavioral test battery.
At 0.15 g/100 ml physicians assessed only 50% of the arrested
ALCOHOL EFFECTS ON DRIVERS 187
drivers as being under the inuence and only assessed every-
one over 0.26 g/100 ml as intoxicated. Perper (1986) describes
87 patients entering an alcohol treatment program with positive
BACs. Twenty-four percent of the subjects over 0.20 g/100 ml
showed no evidence of intoxication and 26% of those over 0.30 g/
100 ml passed many behavioral tests. Physicians fared little bet-
ter when grading patients in an emergency room. Those patients
evaluated as being sober were found to have a mean BAC of
0.272 g/100 ml. One patient declared not to be intoxicated had
a BAC of 0.54 g/100 ml (Urso, 1981).
The probability of a police ofcer detecting drivers on the
road with a BAC above the legal limit is estimated to less than 1%
(Borkenstein et al., 1963). Wells (1997) examined the ability of
police to detect impairment at the roadside. When police cleared
drivers from a sobriety checkpoint as not under the inuence,
researchers requested breath samples for condential analysis.
Eighty-seven percent of drivers between 0.05 and 0.079 g/100 ml
were not arrested, 62% of those between 0.080.99 g/100 ml;
64% of those between 0.10 and 0.119 g/100 ml; and, 62% of
those at or above 0.12 g/100 ml were not arrested. Whether
contemporary education of police has improved this detection
rate has not been tested.
The gross intoxication that the layman associates with being
drunkbears no relationship to the impairment that is signi-
cant for road safety.
PERFORMANCE MEASURES
Skills Related to Driving
The 1988 review by Moskowitz and Robinson of the effects
of low levels of alcohol on driving-related behavior summa-
rized studies on reaction time, tracking, concentrated attention,
divided attention, information processing, visual functions, per-
ception, psychomotor skills, and performance in simulators and
on the road.
They obtained more than 500 studies from a sampling of
the literature. From these 500 studies, 177 were selected for
incorporation in the report because they met their criteria of
adequate statistical analysis, sufcient information that the BAC
at the time of behavioral testing could be determined, and the
presence of placebo treatment. Of the 177 studies 158 reported
impairment of one or more behavioral skills at one or more BACs
ranging from 0.01g/100 ml to 0.10 g/100 ml and above. Only
19 studies failed to report impairment at the levels examined.
A more recent review of the literature summarized 112 stud-
ies published from 1981 to 1997 and screened using similar
criteria (Moskowitz & Fiorentino, 2000). In this review 27% of
the studies reported nding impairment by .039 g/100 ml, 47%
by 0.049 g/100 ml and 92% by .079 g/100 ml. The greater sensi-
tivity of the later studies may be accounted for by improvements
in methodology, better instrumentation, and more frequent ex-
amination of multiple BACs. Some studies reported impairment
at BACs less than 0.01 g/100 ml. The studies showing greatest
sensitivity to alcohol impairment were on-the-road and simula-
tor studies of driving, divided attention tasks, and measures of
drowsiness. The least sensitive tests were simple reaction time
and studies of critical icker fusion. In between were tasks such
as vigilance, tracking, perception, visual functions, and cogni-
tive tasks. There is strong evidence that some driving-related
skills were impaired with any departure from zero BAC. By
0.05 g/100 ml the majority of studies reported impairment of
some skill by alcohol.
Reaction Time
Although reaction time is adversely affected by alcohol, the level
at which signicant effects are noted depends on the complexity
of the reaction demanded and the complexity of the stimulus.
Some studies have demonstrated deterioration at levels as low
as 0.02%, but a level of 0.07% is needed to produce signicant
decit with common tasks (Starmer, 1989). The nature of the
stimulus and the reaction required are complicating factors in
interpreting the reported results. In a complex task such as driv-
ing at night deterioration of reaction time is observed at low
levels. Tiredness is an important factor in increasing reaction
time (Cortsen, 1982).
Simple reaction time is the only experimental variable that
has failed to consistently and overwhelmingly demonstrate im-
pairment by alcohol (Moskowitz et al., 2000). No systematic
study of the variation in results from reaction time experiments
has been undertaken to explain this variability. However, reac-
tion time experiments involving complex situations tend to show
more impairment at lower levels than simpler experiments with
fewer demands. Since the driving task is intrinsically a complex
task, studies on simple reaction time under the inuence of al-
cohol appear less relevant than studies that are more analogous
to the complexity of real driving.
Tracking
The ability to follow a complex path under the inuence of
alcohol was one of the earliest performance measures studied.
Tracking is analogous to car control because the subject uses a
control device, such as a steering wheel, to follow a target that
moves on a screen or (in actual driving) to follow the contours
of the road. Tracking is the essence of what most people have in
mind when they conceive of car control.
Thirty years ago, Moskowitz (1973) reviewed tracking under
the inuence of alcohol and concluded that there was variability
in alcohol effects depending on whether the tracking task was
compensatory or pursuit. A compensatory tracking task has the
operator observing only the difference between the desired po-
sition and the actual position of a controlled element and acting
to reduce the error. A pursuit-tracking task has the observer at-
tempting to follow a moving target on a course. Compensatory
tracking tasks performed alone have generally failed to nd al-
cohol impairment, except at very high BACs.
There is marked impairment of tracking at quite low alco-
hol levels if tracking is not the only task. The Moskowitz and
Robinson report examined 28 tracking studies, where the track-
ing was accompanied by some additional task, and found that
188 E. J. D. OGDEN AND H. MOSKOWITZ
the BAC at which impairment occurs is as low as 0.02 g/100 ml
(Moskowitz & Robinson, 1988).
Vigilance
Concentration is not particularly sensitive to alcohol and no ef-
fects are demonstrated below 0.05%. However, when the task
also involves speed and accuracy, such as clerical tasks, impair-
ment can be demonstrated in the range of BAC from 0.005% to
0.009% (Nash, 1962). The most consistent nding is an increase
in error rates (Starmer, 1989). Low doses of alcohol interfere
with learning and adaptation to unfamiliar tasks (Ogden et al.,
1995).
Divided Attention Tasks
Any experiment that requires subjects to do more than one thing
at a time is highly sensitive to drug effects (Moskowitz, 1984).
Deterioration is detected on some tests at levels below 0.02% and
many studies show deterioration below 0.05%. Small quantities
of alcohol impair the ability to perform a secondary task while
driving, long before the effect on the mechanics of driving are
demonstrable (Brown, 1970).
It has been suggested that one of the reasons for this deterio-
ration in performance is that the alcohol-affected brain processes
information more slowly (Moskowitz & Austin, 1983). Since the
mental workload required to divide attention is a component of
nearly all studies, the challenge is to isolate the effects of alcohol
on divided attention from the effects on the constituent compo-
nents of the task. A study by Moskowitz, Burns, and Williams
(1985) demonstrated that divided attention performance was im-
paired for all subjects at a BAC of 0.015 g/100 ml.
Visual Functions
Vision is peculiarly sensitive to sedatives including alcohol caus-
ing abnormal eye movements, difculty in accurate eye track-
ing of moving objects, impaired color discrimination, tunnel vi-
sion, and even temporary blindness (Colson, 1940; Grant, 1974;
Thompson-Crawford & Slater, 1971; Wallgren & Barry, 1970;
Wilkinson et al., 1974).
Alcohol may impede recovery from glare and impair visual
acuity, although the studies have produced conicting results.
Moskowitz, Wilkinson, and Burg (1993) reviewed 112 studies
of visual performance under alcohol, and suggested that the dis-
crepancy in ndings resulted from experimental techniques that
confounded alcohol effects on glare and acuity with concomitant
presence of other visual functions, such as search behavior, that
have also been demonstrated to be impaired by alcohol. Alco-
hol effects on visual performance are most marked for moving
objects or when there is a simultaneous demand to process other
information (Adams et al., 1978; Zeidman et al., 1980).
Eye movement control is the most sensitive of the various
components of eye function and is affected at very low BACs. A
BAC of 0.04% is enough to induce nystagmus (Aschan
et al., 1956; Grant, 1974; Katoh, 1988; Stapleton et al., 1986).
Moskowitz and Robinson (1988) reviewed 28 studies of opto-
metric vision tasks such as saccade velocity, nystagmus, track-
ing, and acuity, and reported impairment at low BACs but esti-
mated that the magnitude of the impairments were unlikely to be
important for the driving tasks. More cognitive visual functions
exhibit impairment at even lower BACs and are more likely to
inuence driving.
Alcohol changes the way that the subject uses vision. Belt
(1969) used eye movement recordings in drivers on the road
to demonstrate that BACs as low as 0.04 g/100 ml produced
changes in the distribution of eye xation. Similar results have
been found by other investigators including a form of tunnel
vision with fewer visual excursions to the periphery and a shift
in the distribution and duration of eye xation (Buikhuisen &
Jongman, 1972; Moskowitz et al., 1976).
It seems that alcohol slows processing of visual information
requiring longer time spent xed on an object in order to perceive
its nature. One consequence of this slower visual processing is
that fewer xations are possible in any given time, which in
turn means that fewer things can be seen. Drivers are literally
looking less, because each look takes longer under the inuence
of alcohol (Moskowitz et al., 1976). Alcohol-affected drivers are
unable to discern the meaning of road signs until they are closer
to the sign compared to driving when unimpaired (Davis W.,
1998). This difculty is even more evident with poor lighting
(Hicks, 1976).
Driving Skills
The actual skills required for driving have been studied in sim-
ulators, on the road in instrumented cars and as a cause of epi-
demic disease. The complexity of the task and the number of
variables to be considered in safe drivingmake simple mod-
els impossible.
Driving simulators have been used because of the inherent
safety advantage of simulated driving. The more complex the
driving challenge, the lower the BAC at which errors occur.
Steering errors are noticed at an alcohol concentration of 0.03%
and collision frequencies rise. Subjects tend to ignore rules and
instructions before reaching 0.05%. Subjects are more sluggish
to correct positional errors and steering control responsiveness
deteriorates after low to moderate doses of alcohol (Starmer,
1989). Drinking experience does not make any difference to
driving ability (Laurell et al., 1990). Prior driving skill does not
reduce impairment (Beirness & Vogel-Sprott, 1982).
Closed driving course assessments are also used as a model
of on-the-road driving tasks. In common with the results of sim-
ulator testing, the more complex the task required when actually
driving, the greater the decit produced by alcohol. Increasing
blood alcohol levels result in progressive impairment of driv-
ing performance. This impairment is clearly demonstrated in
non-competitive drivers at 0.05% and in competition drivers
at 0.08% (Starmer, 1989). The alcohol-impaired driver may use
past experience and learning to cope with normal routine driving
demands, but cannot do so in an emergency situation (Lovibond
& Bird, 1971).
ALCOHOL EFFECTS ON DRIVERS 189
EPIDEMIOLOGY OF VEHICULAR ACCIDENTS
Driving Survey Data
One way of studying the drinking driver is to study those people
apprehended for driving offenses. Such surveys cannot provide
information about the general population because they are, by
denition, studies of a selected subgroup. Excluding random
breath-testing,police ofcers do not apprehend drivers ran-
domly and police are not randomly distributed in time or place.
Police patrol known trouble spotsand select individuals be-
cause of their driving style, vehicle, or other attributes. The re-
sults of such studies are as much a measure of policing biases
as of driver characteristics.
Post Accident Surveys
Post-accident studies have similar limitations except that the
driver is selected by involvement in a collision. Several con-
trolled epidemiological studies have been performed.
Borkenstein et al. (1964) performed an inuential study in Grand
Rapids, Michigan. They compared breath alcohol levels in
roughly 6,000 crash-involved drivers with 7,600 control drivers
who had not crashed. The probability of involvement in a col-
lision was determined for each BAC by comparing the relative
number of collision-involved drivers at each BAC in the crash
group with the relative number of non-collision-involved drivers
at the same BAC in the control group.
The Grand Rapids study indicated that the probability of caus-
ing an accident was a sharply rising exponential function of the
drivers blood alcohol concentration. At 0.10 g/100 ml there was
a roughly six-fold increase in crashes compared with the crash
rate for drivers with no alcohol. At 0.15 g/100 ml the odds ratio
was 25 to 1. Young drivers (16 and 17 years) had a ve-fold
increase in crashes with BACs below 0.04 g/100 ml. At every
blood alcohol concentration, drivers under 21 years and over
70 years of age had greater crash rate than drivers age 25 to
45 years.
The original Grand Rapids report created some confusion
with its J-shaped curve. It not only showed no increase in overall
crashes for alcohol levels below 0.04 g/100 ml, it even suggested
that drivers might do better with low levels of alcohol rather than
none!
Many researchers have since argued that the Grand Rapids
study failed to compensate for factors other than alcohol that
inuence crash rate. For valid comparison, the control group
should have shared the characteristics that inuenced outcome.
The Grand Rapids study was biased by a zero BAC group with
a greater proportion of both younger and older drivers than the
crashgroup. Both younger and older drivers have higher crash
rates with no alcohol present than drivers aged 25 to 55 years.
Other variables that affect crash rates that were not equally dis-
tributed among the various groups in the study include educa-
tional level, number of miles driven, occupation, and frequency
of drinking. Determining the relationship between BAC and
crash probability requires controlling for these other variables.
The Grand Rapids data has subsequently been analysed by
several groups using more sophisticated statistical methods re-
vealing some apparent paradoxes (Allsop, 1966; Hurst, 1973).
Daily drinkers had the lowest accident rate compared to weekly,
monthly, or yearly drinkers. The youngest and oldest drivers,
who tend not to drink daily, have higher crash rates than 25
55-year-olds who might be drinking daily. Once the variable
drinking frequencyis controlled, the probability of involve-
ment in collision increases with any departure from zero BAC
and the rate of increase is greatest for the least frequent drinkers.
The curve loses its J shape.
The US Department of Transportation sponsored another epi-
demiological study of alcohol and crash probability, collecting
data from drivers involved in crashes in Long Beach, California,
and Fort Lauderdale Florida for more than a 12-month period
(Moskowitz et al., 2000, 2002). This study represented an im-
provement over prior study designs by sampling control drivers
at the same site, time, and direction of travel as the original crash
drivers. Two control drivers were obtained for each crash driver
one week after the collision. Extensive efforts were made by the
police to capture as many hit-run drivers as possible. BAC esti-
mates were obtained from drivers who refused to participate by
passive breath sampling techniques. These factors proved im-
portant since more than 69% of the apprehended hit-run-drivers
had positive BACs, typically in the higher ranges. Almost 50%
of the crash drivers who refused to participate had positive BACs
in contrast to fewer than 16% of the control drivers who refused
to participate.
Had it not been for the apprehension of some 20% of the
hit-run drivers and the use of the passive alcohol sensors to de-
termine alcohol presence in drivers who refused to participate,
it was estimated that more than 46% of all drivers involved in
crashes who had positive blood alcohol would not have been
detected. The study used logistic regression analysis to adjust
the control and crash samples for variation in age, sex, drink-
ing practice, and other variables. This improved analysis of the
probability of crash involvement as a function of BAC indicated
that crash probability increased for alcohol involved drivers at
all levels from 0.01 g/100 ml, and the probabilities for crash
involvement were considerably greater than in any other prior
study.
Single-Vehicle Collisions
The Grand Rapids study reported that the probability, of a sin-
gle vehicle collision at various BACs was greater than that of
a multiple-vehicle collision. That nding was supported by re-
ports in the 1950s and 1960s that roughly 70% of fatally injured
drivers in single-vehicle collisions had alcohol present (NHTSA,
1997).
Zador (1991) argues that single-vehicle crashes provide the
only true measure of the contribution of alcohol to increasing
the rate of crash involvement. Non-impaired drivers may be
able to compensate for the impairment of other drivers and so
avoid becoming involved in collisions. When Zador examined
190 E. J. D. OGDEN AND H. MOSKOWITZ
the probability of fatal single-vehicle crashes involving alcohol
as a function of driver age and sex using data from the Na-
tional Highway Trafc Safety Administration, he determined
that 0.020.04 g/100 ml BAC increased fatal crash involve-
ment by 40%; BACs between 0.05 and 0.09 g/100 ml increased
fatal crash involvement by 1,100%; BACs between 0.10 and
0.14 g/100 ml increased fatal collision probability by 4,800%;
and at levels of 0.15 g/100 ml or higher, the fatal collision rates
increased by 38,000%. Thus the role of alcohol in fatal crashes
is even more signicant than the Grand Rapids report suggested
for all collisions.
Stein (1990) looked at drivers at 0.10% or above and found
their chances of being involved in a fatal collision were 100 times
greater than the sober driver regardless of time of day. However
the sober drivers chances of being in a fatal collision with such
a driver rose dramatically between 1 A.M. and 3 A.M. because of
the increased concentration of drunk drivers in the early hours
of the morning.
RATE OF ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION
The rate at which consumption of alcohol has been under-
taken inuences the outcome. Moskowitz and Burns (1976)
studied four groups of subjects who drank alcohol at different
rates to achieve a peak BAC of 0.10 g/100 ml, and a control
group with a placebo beverage. The 40 subjects were tested on
a performance battery that included measures of information
processing, motor control, hand steadiness, and body sway. The
duration over which they drank ranged from 15 minutes to four
hours. The group that consumed the greatest amount of alcohol
was the group that took the longest period of time to achieve
0.10 g/100 ml, since they were eliminating alcohol as they were
consuming it. Nevertheless, the most impaired individuals were
in the group that drank the fastest, even though they drank the
least total amount, and the least impaired were in the group that
drank the greatest amount at the slowest rate.
ALCOHOL AND FATIGUE
Sleep disorders have become a recognized medical speciality,
and the last decade has seen increased interest in the effects
of alcohol on sleepiness (Lyznicki, Doege, Davis, & Williams,
1998), particularly the effects after the BAC has dropped to zero.
Roehrs et al. (1994) compared subjects given sufcient alco-
hol to produce a peak level of 0.06 g/100 ml at 7.30 A.M., alcohol
sufcient to reach a peak at 0.04 g/100 ml given at 10.30 A.M.,or
placebo. By 3:30 P.M., all subjects were at zero BAC. Subjects
were tested for sleep latency (time to fall asleep) at two-hour
intervals from 9:30 A.M. until 9:30 P.M . Subjects displayed a
shortened time to fall asleep throughout the entire period when
alcohol was present and even when the BAC had dropped to
zero, compared with the placebo treatment day.
Taking a nap normally combats fatigue and increases the time
required to fall asleep. A dose of alcohol that produces a peak
level of 0.04 g/100 ml counteracts the effect of the nap (Roehrs
et al., 1989).
These laboratory studies are relevant to real-life driving sit-
uations. In New Mexico there was an increase in the num-
ber and proportion of alcohol-related trafc crashes during the
seven days following the change to and from daylight-saving,
compared with the week before and the second week after the
changes (Hicks et al., 1998).
HANGOVER
The after effects of alcohol intoxication have been shown
to persist after the BAC has fallen to zero. Many people with
a hangover report feeling unwell and impaired. Measurable ef-
fects of hangover include hormonal changes, depression of brain
activity, difculty with judgment of space-time relationships, ir-
ritability, and poor concentration.
The degree of hangoveris not easily measured, but impair-
ment has been demonstrated in driving simulators, ight simu-
lators, skiing, and administrative tasks. The impairment persists
for at least three hours after all the alcohol has been metabolized
(Collins & Chiles, 1980; Delin & Lee, 1992; Lemon et al., 1993;
Yesavage et al., 1986; York & Regan, 1988).
ALCOHOL AND AGGRESSION
The last decade has seen increasing concern about aggressive
driving and the phenomenon the media call road rage.There
is no hard epidemiological evidence linking aggressive driving
and alcohol consumption, but there is extensive laboratory ev-
idence showing increased aggressive behavior under alcohol.
Bushman and Cooper performed a meta-analysis of thirty ex-
perimental studies and concluded that the evidence supported
the conclusion that alcohol causes aggression in male social
drinkers(Bushman & Cooper, 1990).
There is a vast literature in sociology, criminology, and psy-
chology linking alcohol to acts of violence, as well as the indi-
vidual characteristics that may predispose some individuals to
violence and the situations that promote its expression (Brain,
1986). It has been suggested that alcohol reduces inhibition and
unmasks underlying aggressive tendencies. The extent to which
this is reected in crash statistics is not yet known.
ALCOHOL AND DEGREE OF INJURY
Alcohol not only reduces performance and affects behavior,
but the use of alcoholic beverages predisposes to more severe
and extensive injury than would be experienced by non-drinkers
given impact of the same severity(Committee on Trauma Re-
search, 1985). There is an extensive trauma literature on this
subject, which is beyond the scope of this article.
Animals subjected to a standardized force with and without
alcohol demonstrate increased trauma with alcohol (Brodner
et al., 1981). Humans experiencing trauma have altered hor-
monal responses when alcohol is present that may inuence
outcome (Woolf et al., 1990), and alcohol affected trauma vic-
tims are likely to have sustained more injuries (Fabbri et al.,
2001).
ALCOHOL EFFECTS ON DRIVERS 191
Waller et al. (1986) examined over a million collision reports
in North Carolina from 1979 to 1983. When they controlled for a
wide variety of factors such as crash severity, type and weight of
vehicle, speed, driver age and sex, and seatbelt use, they found
that the presence of alcohol increased the probability of being
killed in an collision 225% over that of a matched non-alcohol
involved driver.
Evans and Frick (1993) used fatal crash data for two-car
crashes where at least one driver was killed and controlled for
factors such as relative weight and impact areas. They deter-
mined that the presence of a BAC of 0.10 g/100 ml roughly
doubled the risk of death from a given impact and a BAC of
0.25 g/100 ml tripled the probability of death.
DRUGS OTHER THAN ALCOHOL
Epidemiology of Drugs and Crashes
While alcohol remains the dominant drug causing impairment
of driving performance, other drugs, especially in combina-
tion with alcohol, increase collision risk. Reviewing the his-
tory of 43,000 outpatients, Skegg et al. (1979) found that the
53 crash-involved drivers in that sample were 4.9 times more
likely than their matched controls to have been using a tranquil-
lizer. The relative risk of a driver being killed in a trafc crash
(assessed by odds ratio analysis) shows a signicant increase for
drivers consuming alcohol alone, alcohol with other psychoac-
tive drugs, combinations of psychoactive drugs, and cannabis
(Alvarez et al., 1992a, 1992b; Alvarez et al., 1997; Drummer &
Gerostamoulos, 1998; Drummer et al., 1998).
Impairment can be predicted from known or expected effects
of medication on:
Alertness (e.g., sedation, stimulation)
Vision (e.g., visual blurring, delayed recovery from glare)
Function (e.g., impaired coordination or movement)
Performance (e.g., impaired performance on skills testing)
Psycho-social (e.g., changes in behaviour, risk taking)
Cognition (e.g., changes in processing information)
This information is available from the pharmacology of certain
substances, reports of adverse drug reactions, epidemiological
data, and specic testing (Ogden & Brous, 1999).
Major Problems in Interpreting Data on Drugs and Driving
There are many major problem areas that need to be considered
when attempting to show the correlation between drug consump-
tion and road trauma.
Proof That the Drug Has Been Consumed. Proof of drug
consumption requires analysis of a body uid to identify the
drug. There is a large number of potential drugs that could be
screened, and many of the drugs of interest may only be present
in minute quantities while having signicant effects.
Could the Amount of Drug Detected Produce Impairment?
The fact that a substance is found does not mean that it caused
impairment. It is necessary to ask a series of questions: Does
this substance cause impairment of human skills? If so, is such
impairment universal or idiosyncratic? Does the impairment oc-
cur in normal dosages or only when the drugs is used in excess?
The presence of a drug may not necessarily mean the driver is
impaired (Maki & Linn¨oila, 1976). There is considerable in-
formation on the clinical use of some drugs and on the normal
levels expected and on what constitutes a toxicconcentra-
tion (Baselt et al., 1975; Uges, 2004). The clinical concepts
of therapeuticand toxicdo not necessarily correlate with
impairment. Some individuals will be impaired with levels of
a drug normally considered therapeutic (e.g., sedatives), while
dangerously toxic levels of other drugs may have no effect on
driving skills (e.g., paracetamol) (Pearl et al., 1989). There is no
critical level of most drugs above which impairment is present
or below which no impairment can be demonstrated (Starmer
et al., 1988).
While we are interested in the behavioral effects of drugs,
presumably due to activity at some site in the brain, we are
limited to taking samples from peripheral sites in the body. The
drug levels in blood, urine, saliva, hair, etc., may be
1. quite different from that in the CNS and
2. not well correlated over time as levels change at the central
and peripheral sites.
Could This Amount of Drug Have Contributed to the Crash?
There are a number of individuals whose behavior and function-
ing is considerably improved by prescription medications, and
without which they would not be t to hold a drivers licence, such
as anti-convulsants for epilepsy. Withdrawal of such drugs may
produce a considerable deterioration in driving performance.
The alcohol literature has relied heavily on the utility of mea-
suring the blood alcohol concentration. Alcohol is a relatively
easy drug to study: it is taken in large quantities; it is water-
soluble; the concentration is easy to measure; and impairment
is effectively dose-related. This paradigm does not translate to
other drugs that may be impairing in miniscule doses; be protein
bound or sequestered into fat; be hard to quantify; and the blood
levels may have no correlation with impairment.
THC is a good example of the problems understanding the
relationship between drug usage and impairment. When mari-
juana is smoked, THC in the inhaled smoke is absorbed within
seconds. Peak blood levels appear about the time smoking is
nished. The cannabinoids are rapidly distributed into fat and
blood levels fall within minutes. Maximum impairment is ob-
served an hour after smoking, when THC levels are about 5 to
10% of the peak (Tzambasis, 2001). The half-life of THC is
estimated to be as long as 10 days and metabolic products can
be found for several weeks after exposure. The presence of THC
metabolites is evidence of drug exposure and not of impairment.
The work of Terhune in the United States (Terhune et al.,
1992) and Drummers group in Australia (Drummer &
Gerostamoulos, 1998; Drummer et al., 1998; Robertson &
Drummer, 1994) has examined the culpability of fatally injured
192 E. J. D. OGDEN AND H. MOSKOWITZ
Table I Risk of culpability for fatal collision
Culpability
Drug(s) Percentage of cases odds ratio
Alcohol alone 24 9.1
Alcohol plus drugs 9 11
Psychoactive drugs 2 3.4
Drug combinations 3 4.6
THC >5 ng/ml 1 3.0
Benzodiazapines 4 2.4
All psychotropics 13 1.5
Stimulants 3 1.4
drivers. They have each used the odds ratio to indicate the rel-
ative importance of various drugs in fatal collision causation.
Drummer summarized this work in Table I (Drummer, 2002).
These results must be taken cautiously, because epidemio-
logical studies to evaluate the role of drugs such as cannabis in
live drivers are fraught with difculties. First, the rate at which
subjects agree to participate in providing body uid samples for
drug testing is far below that found in alcohol studies. The re-
sults of studies where voluntary participation rates are only in
the 80% range may suffer considerable bias.
Second, relative risks for death may be very different from the
risk of injury or non-injury collision. Studies of fatal collision
may not be comparable with studies of injured or non-injured
drivers.
Third, the probability of crash involvement is also a function
of non-drug factors including geographic area, trafc conditions,
vehicle characteristics, and the individual characteristics of the
driver. Few studies have obtained data that would permit the
separation of the possible effects of a drug in collision causation
from all the other factors that determined the event.
For example, in the United States the National Highway Traf-
c Safety Administration estimates that alcohol is present in 8%
of all motor collisions. Even if one assumes that alcohol was the
sole cause of all of those crashes, it leaves 92% of the crashes as
due to other causes. How do we determine the degree to which
those other causes are also present in alcohol-related crashes?
How are the contributions of the various factors to be separated
or proportioned? The difculty of performing epidemiological
studies even with respect to cannabis, the most frequently stud-
ied drug other than alcohol, can be seen in two recent reviews
of the literature that reached differing conclusions. Bates and
Blakely, (1999) concluded that .. . there is no evidence that
consumption of cannabis alone increases the risk of culpability
for trafc crash fatalities or injuries for which hospitalization
occurs, and may reduce those risks.Ramaekers et al. (2004)
concluded that drivers who had recently used cannabis were
about three to seven times more likely to be responsible for
their crash as compared to drivers that had not used drugs or
alcohol.
Impairment Tests
Given the difculty of obtaining appropriate biological samples,
getting timely analysis, and interpreting the result, law enforce-
ment measures aimed at drug-impaired driving have been de-
pendent on behavioral tests to demonstrate impairment (Ogden,
1995). There is worldwide interest in roadside testing for drugs
and the establishment of per se denitions of impairment.
The probability of being arrested for driving while impaired
by alcohol was estimated to be extremely low with a risk es-
timated at 0.001 per trip made while intoxicated. This led the
US Department of Transportation to commission work during
the late 1970s to develop a standardised eld sobriety test bat-
tery that would facilitate the accurate recognition of intoxicated
drivers in the eld.
Burns and Moskowitz carried out two large research projects
involving over 500 subjects (Burns & Moskowitz, 1977; Tharp
et al., 1981). Police ofcers tested laboratory subjects who had
consumed alcohol to simulate intoxicated drivers. They identi-
ed three tests (Horizontal Gaze Nystagmus, Walk and Turn,
One-Leg Stand) that reliably identied alcohol intoxication (de-
ned BAC >0.10%). This battery of three relatively straight-
forward tests was recommended for adoption by police as a
roadside screen for sobriety, The three tests became collectively
known as the Standardised Field Sobriety Test (SFST).
In the laboratory studies, police ofcersestimates of BAC
differed from the measured concentration by an average 0.03%.
They were able to correctly classify subjects above or below
0.10 g/100 ml, 81% of the time.
Anderson et al. (1983) found that different police departments
had predictive accuracy between 76% and 96%, suggesting that
reliability may be related to training and careful adherence to
protocol rather than inherent test validity. Drugs other than al-
cohol may have contributed to the apparent over-classication.
At the roadside, No decisionis not an option for operational
police (Burns, 1991): a decision must always be made. It seems
natural that police will err on the side of caution when making
roadside assessments: better to make an incorrect release than
make an incorrect arrest. It is apparent that the arrestcrite-
rion is lower in the laboratory. The penalties for mistakes in a
laboratory setting are, of course, fairly trivial when compared
to a real world setting(Burns & Anderson, 1995). A Finnish
study with more than 5,000 subjects found that observations of
nystagmus combined with tests of balance and walking was the
best screening tool for alcohol (Pentilla et al., 1974).
Evidence of drug impairment traditionally relied on physi-
cians with the appropriate experience and interest. The Los An-
geles Police Department pioneered the training of police ofcers
to perform these eld evaluations and give expert evidence on
drug effects. The program allowed prosecution for drug impaired
driving in three discrete steps:
1. The arresting ofcer establishes impairment and calls for a
Drug Recognition Expert.
2. The drug recognition ofcer establishes that the impairment
is likely to be due to a drug in a particular class.
3. The laboratory conrms that a drug in that class is present in
blood or urine.
ALCOHOL EFFECTS ON DRIVERS 193
Enthusiasm for the abilities of the police trained Drug Recogni-
tion Expert (DRE) performance should be restrained. There are
no scientic studies utilizing adequate blind controls to examine
the ability of DRE trained ofcers to correctly identify an indi-
vidual as drug impaired, and/or identify the drug classication.
The studies performed have been primarily eld observational
studies. It is interesting to note that when one examines the
ability of police ofcers to detect and identify the presence of
alcohol beverage on an individuals breath (with adequate con-
trols) how much the results vary from commonly held police
opinions as to their ability to detect whether a person has been
drinking (Moskowitz et al., 1999).
The DRE program has been rened and renamed Drug Eval-
uation and Classication (DEC). SFST procedures remain at
the core of this process with the addition of some physiological
data (pupil reaction, pulse, blood pressure, nger-nose test) to
aid drug classication.
No battery has been standardized against the broad range of
drugs that are implicated in driving impairment. Tzambasis has
shown that observation of head movements and jerks improves
the discrimination of SFST for impairment due to marijuana
(Tzambasis, 2001; Tzambazis et al., 2000, 2001), but similar
work has not yet been done with other substances.
SPECIFIC DRUGS
There are many ways to look at the literature on drugs and
driving. The alcohol review was arranged according to experi-
mental methodology and performance variables. A similar ap-
proach to other substances would give a coherent overview of
the research methodologies, but make it difcult for the reader
to appreciate the specic effects of individual substances. We
have chosen to present the data by drug because of the practical
implications, acknowledging that this is done at the expense of
presenting a somewhat disjointed overview.
Marijuana
Marijuana is the common term given to the leaves of the plant
cannabis sativa. It has been used for centuries in various parts of
the world, and has become a popular recreational drug through-
out the western world.
Of the many chemical compounds in its leaves, delta-9
tetrahydrocannabinol (9 THC or THC) has been identied as
the major psychoactive component. THC has signicant effects
on the human brain in tiny concentrations both at the time of
consumption and long term.
The most frequently detected metabolite of THC is 11-nor-
carboxy-delta-9-THC (often called THC-carboxylic acid),
which is inactive. THC-carboxylic acid can be detected in blood,
urine, and other tissues for an extended period. It has a metabolic
half-life of 3340 hours, but its half-life in fatty tissues is sig-
nicantly longer because of its afnity for fat. It can take up to
a month to be eliminated beyond the detection limits of modern
toxicological techniques. The detection of the metabolite in a
biological sample is therefore evidence of the consumption of
cannabis within the last month, but does not enable a more ac-
curate estimate of when it was consumed nor quantication of
the dose consumed.
There is no biological measurement of cannabinoid concen-
tration that allows direct estimate of cannabis-induced impair-
ment of driving skills as exists for alcohol (Chesher et al., 1986).
Tzambasiss (2001) observation that impairment was greatest 70
minutes after smoking when THC levels had fallen to 5 to 10%
of peak level demonstrates this point.
Drivers under the combined inuence of marijuana and al-
cohol have an increased likelihood of initiating a crash and the
combination produces an additional decrement in performance
of driving related tasks (Burns & Moskowitz, 1980; Chesher
et al., 1986; Drummer, 2002; Drummer & Gerostamoulos, 1998;
Drummer et al., 1998; Klonoff, 1983; Perez-Reyes et al., 1988;
Ramaekers et al., 2004).
Early studies on the effects of marijuana on simulated driv-
ing performance established that some driving variables are im-
paired by the consumption of marijuana. In particular subjects
appeared to have delayed or inappropriate reactions, attention
decits, poor speed and distance judgment, and poor hazard per-
ception. People affected by cannabis tend to travel slowly and
avoid risk. It is unclear to what extent this is due to conscious
recognition of impairment rather than distortion of judgement
of time and distance.
More recent studies in more realistic driving simulators show
that marijuana increases the variability of speed control and road
position. Marijuana-affected participants tend to hit obstacles,
miss signs, have delayed responses to the need to change speed
(both braking and accelerating are inappropriate), and drive
more slowly than when unaffected (Drummer, 2002; Drummer
& Gerostamoulos, 1998; Drummer et al., 1998; Tzambasis,
2001; Tzambazis et al., 2000, 2001).
There have been several on-road driving studies examining
the effects of cannabis on driving performance. Results showthat
marijuana results in poor car handling, with drivers exposed to
high doses of marijuana ve times more likely to strike cones
on a driving task than when not affected by the drug (Klonoff,
1974, 1983; McBay & Owens, 1981; Ramaekers et al., 2000).
There does not appear to be a hangover effectof the sort
seen with alcohol and long-acting sedatives (Chait, 1990; Chait
et al., 1985), however there is some prolonged impairment of
skilled performance. Leirer et al. (1991) studied nine experi-
enced licensed pilots performing a simulator ight with numer-
ous response variables before and after smoking a cigarette con-
taining 20 mg of delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC). Marijuana
impaired performance at 0.25, 4, 8, and 24 hours after smoking.
At 24 hours, only one pilot reported awareness of drug effects.
THC levels greater than 5 ng/ml are associated with a three-
fold increase in the risk of being responsible for a fatal colli-
sion (Drummer, 2002). Yet several other epidemiological stud-
ies have failed to nd above baseline fatality rates for use of
cannabis alone (Bates & Blakely, 1999). The combination of
marijuana and alcohol severely impairs performance (NHTSA,
2000).
194 E. J. D. OGDEN AND H. MOSKOWITZ
Anti-Anxiety Drugs
The benzodiazepine group of drugs includes minor tranquil-
lizers, sedatives, anticonvulsants, and hypnotics. Representative
members of the group are diazepam, oxazepam, nitrazepam, and
unitrazepam. These drugs have largely taken the place of the
barbiturates in the treatment of anxiety and insomnia because of
their efcacy and safety even with overdose.
Different members of the class depress the central nervous
system to varying degrees and in qualitatively different ways.
Some are better at relieving pathological anxiety and agitation
and are classied as tranquillizers. Others are more sedating and
hence are used to treat insomnia. Some members of the group
are primarily used in epilepsy as anticonvulsants. There is no
sharp distinction between any of these effects and higher doses
of any of the benzodiazepines may induce sedation and coma.
Berghaus and Grass (1997) reviewed over 500 experimental
studies of driving related tasks. They showed that the serum level
of each of the benzodiazepines studied was related to the degree
of impairment in the laboratory.
The data suggest that there is an increased risk of personal
injury crashes among drivers using anti-anxiety drugs compared
with the rest of the population (Skegg et al., 1979) and this is ex-
acerbated by alcohol (Sepp¨al¨a et al., 1976a). There is a hangover
effect and a small dose of alcohol the following day can potenti-
ate the effect. There is a decrement in tasks requiring vigilance
at low doses and tolerance is only occasionally noted. The oppo-
site effect, exaggerated impairment, has also been documented
(Kolega, 1989).
The benzodiazepine group has been shown to impair driv-
ing skills to a similar degree and in similar ways to alcohol.
Thomas (1998) concluded that the risk of collision was dou-
bled for patients taking benzodiazepines. The impairment and
collision risk are greatest in the rst two weeks of treatment
(de Gier et al., 1981). The ICADTS working group concluded
that patients should be warned not to drive in the rst two weeks
of treatment (Alvarez & de Gier, 2002). However,not all research
has found an association between sedative use and collision risk
(Jick et al., 1981). de Gier (1993) reported that clinically anxious
patients are also poor drivers. Although treatment with benzodi-
azepine tranquillizers will improve clinical anxiety, there is no
improvement in their driving ability.
Psychoactive Medication
The untreated psychiatric patient is a potentially hazardous driver
either because of the underlying illness and the consequent dis-
order of the mind, or of the associated psychomotor impairment.
Once stabilized on medication, patients are better on their medi-
cation and a greater risk without it (Sepp¨al¨a et al., 1976b; Smiley
et al., 1981). There is some question about the generality of this
conclusion (de Gier et al., 1981) as it has been based primarily
on subjective clinical judgment without adequate research back-
ing. There is some interaction with alcohol generally related to
the sedative effects of some psychoactive drugs (Bauer, 1984;
Hindmarch, 1984).
Different members of each class of psychoactive medication
have quite different effects on driving. For instance, the tricyclic
antidepressants are quite impairing of driving skill, while the se-
lective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) antidepressants have
lesser effect on driving and little or no interaction with alcohol
(Pullen, 1999). Starmer and Mascord (1994) have stated that
tricyclic anti-depressants, which are intrinsically sedative in na-
ture and cause driving impairment in normal individuals, will
improve the driving ability of depressed patients.
Stimulants—Amphetamine/Cocaine
There are laboratory studies showing that small doses of stim-
ulants can improve cognitive performance (De Wit et al., 2002;
Wachtel & de Wit, 1999) and improve reaction time (Fleming
et al., 1995; Halliday et al., 1994). On the other hand, am-
phetamines cause decits in divided attention tasks and per-
ception in the peripheral visual elds (Easterbrook, 1955; Mills
et al., 2001).
Amphetamine variants (dexamphetamine, methamphetamine,
and methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, or Ecstasy))
have been implicated in trafc fatalities (Drummer, 1994, 2002;
Drummer & Gerostamoulos, 1998, 1999; Drummer et al., 1998).
A review of the epidemiological evidencein 1987 by Hurst found
little to support such a relationship.
Theres been little laboratory work with driving simulators or
on-the-road performance under amphetamines. However, using
a driving simulator, Silber et al. (2004) found that 0.42 mg/kg
dexamphetamine signicantly impaired overall performance for
daytime but not night-time driving, possibly because the visual
eld is restricted in the night-time simulation and peripheral
cues are less important. During the daytime simulation, drivers
signalled incorrectly and failed to stop at red trafc lights more
frequently.
Laboratory studies are required that replicate the conditions
under which the amphetamines are frequently used. Thus, for ex-
ample, long-distance drivers often take methamphetamine repet-
itively and so the drug should be examined experimentally under
similar conditions. Finally, it is known that methamphetamine
depresses neurotransmitters in the brain for extended periods
over a week or more, even following single-dose treatments.
During that period subjects exhibit depressed behavior that
should be examined for impairment.
Opioid Analgesics
The opiate drugsheroin, methadone, codeine, and related
compoundsare used for pain relief and the suppression of
cough. They have a high addiction potential. Acute sedation
and impairment is observed in a dose-related manner and there
is a deleterious interaction with alcohol, although the effects
are slight compared with alcohol (Chesher, 1989). Methadone
is used for the long-term maintenance therapy of narcotic ad-
dicts. Long-term methadone maintenance is not associated with
an increase in collision risk after the initial stabilization period
ALCOHOL EFFECTS ON DRIVERS 195
(Chesher et al., 1995; Friedel & Berghaus, 1995; Friedrich et al.,
1991; Moskowitz & Robinson, 1985).
Minor Analgesics and Anti-Arthritics
There are few central side effects of the common minor pain-
killers (aspirin, paracetamol) or of the non-steroidal anti-
inammatory agents that are used for the treatment of arthritis.
SUMMARY
Alcohol, the substance most frequently found in crash-
involved drivers, has been extensively examined in experimen-
tal and epidemiological studies. While behavioral areas such as
judgment, emotion, cognition, and sleep requires further work,
the existing large literature describes numerous behaviors im-
paired by alcohol and their role in trafc safety.
Other drugs have been shown to impair human performance,
or have been implicated in epidemiological studies as increasing
the risk of crashes. None have been examined in the same detail
as alcohol over the wide range of possible behaviors. Such ex-
amination would enhance the ability to evaluate the role of these
drugs in trafc. The increasing sophistication of behavioral mea-
sures examining cognitive processes and judgment will assist
researchers in estimating drug safety.
REFERENCES
Adams A, Brown B, Haegerstrom-Portney G, Flom M, Jones R. (1978)
Marijuana, alcohol and combined drug effects on the time course of
glare recovery, Psychopharmacology, Vol. 56, pp. 8186.
Allsop R. (1966) Alcohol and Road Accidents (Road Research Labora-
tory Report No. 6). Road Research Laboratory. Illingworth, England.
Alvarez F, Prada R, Del Rio M. (1992a) Drugs and alcohol consumption
amongst Spanish drivers, Forensic Science International, Vol. 53,
pp. 221225.
Alvarez F, Prada R, Del Rio M. (1992b) Patterns of drug consumption
among Spanish drivers, Therapie, Vol. 47, pp. 6366.
Alvarez F, Sancho M, Vega J, Del Rio M, Rams M, Quepio D. (1997)
Drugs other than alcohol (medicines and illicit drugs) in people in-
volved in fatal road accidents in Spain. In C. Mercier-Guyon (ed.),
Proceedings of the 14th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs
& Trafc Safety, vol. II, pp. 745750. Published by: CERMT, Centr
dEtudes et de Recherches en Medecine du Trac, Annecy, France.
Alvarez J, de Gier J. (2002) Prescribing And Dispensing Guidelines
For Medicinal Drugs Affecting Driving Performance. ICADTS,
Montreal.
Anderson T, Schweitz R, Snyder M. (1983) Field Evaluation of a Be-
havioral Test Battery for DWI, Report DOT HS-806-475. US Depart-
ment of Transportation, Ofce of Driver and Pedestrian Research,
National Highway Trafc Safety Administration, Washington, DC.
Aschan G, Bergstedt M, Goldberg L, Laurell L. (1956) Positional nys-
tagmus in man during and after alcohol intoxication, Quarterly Jour-
nal of Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 17, pp. 381440.
Baselt R, Wright J, Cravey R. (1975) Therapeutic and Toxic Concentra-
tions of More than 100 Toxicologically Signicant Drugs in Blood,
Plasma or Serum: A Tabulation, Clin. Chem., Vol. 21, pp. 4462.
Bates MN, Blakely TA. (1999) Role of cannabis in motor vehicle
crashes. Epidemiol Rev, Vol. 21, pp. 222232.
Bauer, R. (1984) Trafc accidents and minor tranquillisers: A review,
Public Health Reporter, Vol. 99, pp. 572574.
Beirness D, Vogel-Sprott M. (1982) Does prior skill reduce alcohol in-
duced impairment? Journal of Studies in Alcohol, Vol. 43, pp. 1149
1156.
Belt B. (1969) Driver Eye Movement as a Function of Low Alcohol
Concentrations. Driving Research Laboratory, Ohio State University,
Columbus, OH.
Berghaus G, Grass H. (1997) Concentration-effect relationship with
benzodiazepine therapy. In International Council on Alcohol, Drugs
&Trafc Safety. 14th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs &
Trafc Safety. September 2126, 1997. Annecy, France.
Borkenstein R, Crowther RF, Shumate RP, Zeil WW, Zylinan R. (1964)
The Role of the Drinking Driver in Trafc Accidents. Department of
Police Administration, Indiana University, Bloomington IN.
Borkenstein R, Trubitt T, Lease R. (1963) Problems of enforcement and
prosecution. In Alcohol and Trafc Safety, Fox, B. and Handcox,
J. (eds.), pp. 137188. Public Health Service Publications, United
States Government Printing Ofce, Washington, DC.
Brain P. (1986) Alcohol and Aggression. Croom Helm Ltd, Beckenharn,
Kent.
Brodner R, Gilder IV, Jr JC. (1981) Experimental spinal cord trauma:
Potentiation, by Alcohol, Journal of Trauma, Vol. 21, pp. 124
129.
Brown I. (1970) Safer drivers, Brit. J. Hosp. Med., Vol. 4, pp. 441450.
Buikhuisen W, Jongman R. (1972) Trafc Perception Under the Inu-
ence of Alcohol, Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 33,
pp. 800806.
Burns M. (1991) Horizontal Gaze Nystagmus: The Controversy and
the Issues. The DRE, Vol. 3, no. 3.
Burns M, Anderson E. (1995) A Colorado Validation Study of the
Standardized Field Sobriety Test (SFST) Battery. Colorado Dept.
of Transportation.
Burns M, Moskowitz H. (1977) Psychophysical tests for DWI ar-
rest, Report DOT-HS-5-01242. US Department of Transportation,
NHTSA, Washington, DC.
Burns M, Moskowitz H. (1980) Alcohol, Marijuana and Skills Perfor-
mance. In: L. Goldberg (Editor) Proceedings of the 8th International
Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Trafc Safety, Sweden, Vol III,
pp. 954968.
Bushman B, Cooper H. (1990) Effects of alcohol on human aggression:
An integrative research review, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 107,
pp. 341354.
Chait LD. (1990) Subjective and behavioral effects of marijuana
the morning after smoking, Psychopharmacology (Berl), Vol. 100,
pp. 328333.
Chait LD, Fischman MW, Schuster CR. (1985) Hangovereffects the
morning after marijuana smoking, Drug Alcohol Depend, Vol. 15,
pp. 229238.
Chesher G, Dauncey H, Crawford J, Horn K. (1986) The interaction
between alcohol and marijuana, Report CR 40. Federal Ofce of
Road Safety, Canberra.
Chesher G. (1989) Understanding the opioid analgesics and their effects
on skills performance, Alcohol Drugs and Driving, Vol. 5, pp. 111
138.
Chesher G, Lemon J, Gomel M, Murphy G. (1995) Are the driving
related skills of clients in a methadone maintenance programme af-
fected by methadone? In CN Koeden and AJ McLean (eds.) Pro-
ceedings of the 13th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs
and Trafc Safety, Adelaide, Vol I, pp. 311320.
196 E. J. D. OGDEN AND H. MOSKOWITZ
Collins WE, Chiles WD. (1980) Laboratory performance during acute
alcohol intoxication and hangover, Hum Factors, Vol. 22, pp. 445
462.
Colson Z. (1940) The effect of alcohol on vision, J. Am. Med Assoc,
Vol. 115, 15251527.
Committee on Trauma Research. (1985) Injury in America. An increas-
ing Public Health Problem. Commission on Life Sciences, N.R.C.,
and the Institute of Medicine ed. National Academy Press, Washing-
ton, DC.
Cortsen M. (1982) Increased viso-motoric reaction time of young tired
drunk drivers, Forensic Science International, Vol. 20, pp. 121125.
Davis W, HRA. (1998) Fatal alcohol related trafc crashes increase
subsequent to changes to and from daylight savings time, Perceptual
and Psychological Skill, Vol. 86, pp. 879882.
de Gier J, Hart B, Nelemans F, Bergman H. (1981) Psychomotor per-
formance and real driving performance of outpatients receiving di-
azepam, Psychopharmacology (Berl), Vol. 73, pp. 340344.
de Gier JJ. (1993) Driving Licences and Known Use of Licit or Illicit
Drugs, Report IHP 93-39. Institute for Human Psychopharmacology,
University of Limburg, The Netherlands.
De Wit H, Enggasser J, Richards J. (2002) Acute administration of
d-amphetamine decreases impulsivity in healthy volunteers, Neu-
ropsychopharmacology, Vol. 27, pp. 813825.
Delin CR, Lee TH. (1992) Drinking and the brain: Current evidence,
Alcohol, Vol. 27, pp. 117126.
Drummer O. (1994) Drugs in drivers killed in Victorian road trafc
accidents. The use of responsibility analysis to investigate the con-
tribution of drugs to fatal accidents., Report 0394. Victorian Institute
of Forensic Pathology and Monash University Department of Foren-
sic Medicine, Melbourne.
Drummer O. (2002) Involvement of Drugs in Accident Causation. Pa-
per presented at the 2nd Australasian Conference on Drug Strategy,
Perth, WA, 2002.
Drummer OH, Gerostamoulos J, Batziris H, Chu M, Caplehorn J,
Robertson MD, et al. (2004) The involvement of drugs in drivers
of motor vehicles killed in Australian road trafc crashes, Accid.
Anal. Prev., Vol. 36(2), pp. 239248.
Drummer O, Gerostamoulos J. (1999) The involvement of drugs in
car drivers killed in Victorian road trafc accidents, Report 0499.
Victorian Institute of Forensic Medicine and Monash University De-
partment of Forensic Medicine, Melbourne.
Drummer O, Gerostamoulos J. (1998) Drugs in Drivers Killed in New
South Wales Road Trafc Accidents; The Use of Responsibility Anal-
ysis to Investigate the Contribution of Drugs to Fatal Accidents. Sec-
ond Report. (19951996), Report 0498. Victorian Institute of Foren-
sic Medicine, Melbourne.
Drummer O, Gerostamoulos J, Batziris H. (1998) Drugs in Drivers
Killed in Victorian Road Trafc Accidents; The Use of Responsibil-
ity Analysis to Investigate the Contribution of Drugs to Fatal Acci-
dents. Second Report. (19941996), Report 0298. Victorian Institute
of Forensic Medicine, Melbourne.
Dunbar J, Pentilla A, Pikkarainen J. (1987) Drinking and driving: choos-
ing legal limits, Brit. Med. J., Vol. 295, pp. 14581460.
Easterbrook JA. (1955) The effect of emotion on cue utilization
and the organisation of behaviour, Psychological Review, Vol. 66,
pp. 183201.
Evans, L. (1991) Trafc Safety and the Driver, Van Norstrand Reinhold,
New York.
Evans L, Frick M. (1993) Alcohols effect on fatality risk from a phys-
ical insult, Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 54.
Fabbri A, Marchesini G, Morselli-Labate AM, Rossi F, Cicognani A,
Dente M, Lervese T, Ruggeri S, Mengozzi U, Vandelli A. (2001)
BACand management of road trauma patients in the emergency de-
partment, J. Trauma, Vol. 50, pp. 521528.
Fleming K, Bigelow LB, Weinberger DR, Goldberg TE. (1995) Neu-
ropsychological effects of amphetamine may correlate with person-
ality characteristics, Psychopharmacology Bulletin, Vol. 31, pp. 357
362.
Friedel B, Berghaus G. (1995) Methadone and driving. In CN Koeden
and AJ McLean (eds.) Proceedings of the 13th International Confer-
ence on Alcohol, Drugs and Trafc Safety, Adelaide, Vol. I, pp. 307
310.
Friedrich G, Joachim H, Strohbeck-K´uhne P, Weigend M. (1991) In-
vestigations in the driving ability of heroin addicts under methadone
treatment. In Proceedings of the Conference on the The Victim and
the Road User, published by McMillan, New Dehli, India.
Grant W. (1974) Toxicology of the Eye: Drugs, Chemicals, Plants,
Venoms. C.C. Thomas, Springeld, IL.
Halliday R, Naylor H, Brandeis D, Callaway E, Yano L, Herzig K.
(1994) The effect of d-amphetamine, clonidine, and yohimbine on
human information processing, Psychophysiology, Vol. 31, pp. 331
337.
Hicks G, Davis W, Hicks R. (1998) Fatal alcohol related trafc crashes
increase subsequent to changes to and from daylight savings time,
Perceptual and Psychological Skills, Vol. 86, pp. 879882.
Hicks J. (1976) An evaluation of the effort of sign brightness on the
sign-reading behavior of alcohol-impaired drivers, Human Factors,
Vol. 18(1), pp. 4552
Hindmarch J. (1984) Effects of psychoactive drugs on car handling
and related psychomotor ability: A review.In Drugs and Driving,
OHanlon J., deGier J. (eds.), pp. 7182. Taylor and Francis, London.
Hurst PM. (1973) Epidemiological aspects of alcohol in driver crashes
and citations, Journal of Safety Research, Vol. 5, pp. 130148.
Hurst PM. (1987) Amphetamines and Driving, Alcohol, Drugs, and
Driving, Vol. 3(1), pp. 1317.
Jick H, Hunter J, Dinan B, Madsen S, Stergachis A. (1981) Sedating
drugs and automobile accidents leading to hospitalization, Am. J.
Public Health, Vol. 71, pp. 13991400.
Jones RK, Shinar D, Walsh JM. (2003) State of Knowledge of Drug-
Impaired Driving, Final Report, National Highway Trafc Safety
Administration Report DOT HS 809 642.
Katoh Z. (1988) Slowing effects of alcohol in voluntary eye movement,
Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine, Vol. 59, pp. 606610.
Klonoff H. (1974) Marijuana and driving in real-life situations. Science,
Vol. 186, pp. 317324.
Klonoff H. (1983) Acute psychological effects of marijuana in man,
including cognitive, psychomotor and perceptual effects on driving.
In Cannabis and health hazards,OBrien, K. F., Katanach, E. (eds.),
pp. 443473. Toronto Addiction Research Foundation.
Kolega H. (1989) Benzodiazepines and vigilance performance. A re-
view. Psychopharmacology, Vol. 98, pp. 145156.
Laurell H, McLean A, Kloeden C. (1990) The effect of BAC on light and
heavy drinkers in a realistic night driving situation. NHMRC Road
Accident Research Unit, The University of Adelaide, Adelaide.
Leirer VO, Yesavage JA, Morrow DG. (1991) Marijuana carry-over
effects on aircraft pilot performance. Aviation, Space, and Environ-
mental Medicine, Vol. 62, pp. 221227.
Lemon J, Chesher G, Fox A, Greeley J, Nabke C. (1993) Investigation
of the hangovereffects of an acute dose of alcohol on psychomotor
performance, Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res., Vol. 17, 665668.
ALCOHOL EFFECTS ON DRIVERS 197
Lovibond S, Bird K. (1971) Danger Levelthe Warwick Farm Project.
In: The Proceedings of the 29th International Congress on Alco-
holism and Drug Dependence, Sydney, 1971.
Lucas GH. (1955) Contribution of alcohol to motor car accidents, Can.
Serv. Med. J., Vol. 11, 892894.
Lucas GH, Kalow W, McColl JD, Grifth BA, Smith HW. (1955).
Quantitative studies of the relationship between alcohol levels and
motor vehicle accidents. Alcohol and Road Trafc, Proceedings of
the Second International Conference on Alcohol and Road Trafc
(1953). Toronto, Canada: Garden City Press Co-operative, pp. 139
142.
Lyznicki JM, Doege TC, Davis RM, Williams MA. (1998). Sleepiness,
driving, and motor vehicle crashes. Council on Scientic Affairs,
American Medical Association, JAMA, Vol. 279, No. 23, pp. 1908
1913.
Maki M, Linn¨oila M. (1976) Trafc Accident Rates among Finnish
Outpatients, Accid. Anal & Prev, Vol. 8, pp. 3944.
McBay AJ, Owens SM. (1981) Marijuana and driving, NIDA Res
Monogr, Vol. 34, pp. 257263.
McCarroll JR, Haddon W, Jr. (1962) A controlled study of fatal automo-
bile accidents in New York City, J. Chronic. Dis., Vol. 15, pp. 811
826.
Mills KC, Spruill SE, Kanne RW, Parkman KM, Zhang Y. (2001) The
inuence of stimulants, sedatives, and fatigue on tunnel vision: Risk
factors for driving and piloting, Human Factors, Vol. 43, pp. 310
327.
Mitchell MC. (1985) Alcohol induced impairment of central nervous
system function: behavioural skills involved in driving, J. Stud. Alc.,
Vol. 10, pp. 109116.
Moskowitz H. (1973) Laboratory studies of the effect of alcohol on
some variables related to road safety. Research, Vol. 5, pp. 185199.
Moskowitz H. (1984) Attention tasks as skills performance measures
of drug effects, British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, Vol. 18,
pp. 51s61s.
Moskowitz H. (2004) Personal communication.
Moskowitz H, Austin G. (1983) A Review of Selected Research Studies
from the Last Decade on the Effects of Alcohol on Human Skills Per-
formance.InA Critical review of the drug/performance literature,
Vol I (L.A. Laundry ed.), published by Associate Consultants, Inc.,
Washington, DC, for the US Army medical Research and Develop-
ment Co., 1979.
Moskowitz H, Blomberg R, Burns M, Fiorentino J, Peck R. (2002)
Methodological issues in epidemiological studies of alcohol crash
risk. In Proceedings of the 2002 Montreal, Canada, International
Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Trafc Safety, Vol. I, pp. 4550.
Moskowitz H, Burns M. (1976) Effects of rate of drinking on hu-
man performance, Journal of Studies on Alcohol,, Vol. 46, pp. 482
485.
Moskowitz H, Burns M, Williams AF. (1985) Skills performance at low
blood alcohol levels, Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 46, No. 6,
pp. 482485.
Moskowitz H, Burns M, Ferguson S. (1999) Police ofcersdetection
of breath odors from alcohol ingestion, Accid Anal Prev, Vol. 31,
pp. 175180.
Moskowitz H, Burns M, Fiorentino D, Smiley A, Zador P. (2000) Driver
Characteristics and Impairment at Various BACs, Report HS 809-
075. National Highway Trafc Safety Administration, Springeld,
VA .
Moskowitz H, Robinson CD. (1985) Methadone maintenance and
tracking performance. In Kaye S., Meier G.W. (eds.), Alcohol, Drugs
and Trafc Safety. (Proceedings of the 9th International Conference,
San Juan, Puerto Rico, 1983.) National Highway Trafc Safety Ad-
ministration, U.S. Department of Transport ation, DOT HS 806814,
9951004.
Moskowitz H, Robinson C. (1988) Effects of Low Dose Alcohol on
Driving Related Skills. A Review of the Evidence (Technical Report
DOT FT 807 280). National Highway Trafc Safety Administration.
Springeld, VA.
Moskowitz H, WilkinsonC, Albert Burg (1993) The effect of alcohol on
driving related visual performance in Alcohol Drugs & Trafc Safety,
T92, Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Alcohol,
Drugs & Trafc Safety, Vol. 2. Utzelmann H., Berghaus G., Kroj G.
(eds.)Verlog TUV Rheinland, Cologne, 1993.
Moskowitz H, Ziedman K, Sharma S. (1976) Visual search behaviour
while viewing driving scenes under the inuence of alcohol and
marijuana, Human Factors, Vol. 8, pp. 417432.
Nash H. (1962) Alcohol and Caffeine: A Study of Their Physiological
Effects. Charles C. Thomas, Springeld, IL.
NHTSA (2000) National Highway Trafc Safety Administration
(NHTSA) notes. Marijuana and alcohol combined severely im-
pede driving performance, Ann Emerg Med, Vol. 35, pp. 398
399.
Ogden E. (1995) Impairment of driving by drugs. In Inquiry into the
Effects of Drugs (Other than Alcohol) on Road Safety in Victoria.
Parliament of Victoria, Melbourne.
Ogden E, Brous D. (1999) Medicines and Driving: Code of Practice
for Health Care Professionals, Report DFA98/99. VicRoads, Mel-
bourne.
Ogden E, Cairns I, Curry E. (1995) Impairment of Learning by Low
Dose Alcohol. In CN Koeden, AJ McLean (eds.) Proceedings of the
13th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Trafc Safety,
Adelaide, Vol. II, pp. 627632.
Pearl J, Hodder A, Havi B, Starmer G, Tattam B, Vine J, Watson T.
(1989) Drug usage by drivers in New South WalesA new look at
the Drug-impaired driver. In MW Perrine (ed.) Proceedings of the
11th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Trafc Safety,
Chicago, IL, pp. 418423.
Pentilla A, Tenhu M, Kataja M. (1974) Evaluation of Alcohol Intoxica-
tion in Suspected Drunken Driving. Statistical and Research Bureau
of TALJA, Helsinki.
Perez-Reyes M, Hicks R, Bumberry J, Jeffcoat A, Cook C. (1988)
Interaction between marihuana and ethanol: Effects on psychomotor
performance, Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res., Vol. 12, pp. 268276.
Perper I, Twerski A, Wieland JW. (1986) Tolerance a high BACs: A
study of 110 cases and review of the literature, Journal of Forensic
Sciences, Vol. 3(i), pp. 212221.
Pullen R. (1999) [Psychotropic drugs and automobile driving ability of
elderly patients], Versicherungsmedizin, Vol. 51, pp. 7174.
Ramaekers JG, Berghaus G, van Laar M. Drummer OH. (2004) Dose
related risk of motor vehicle crashes after cannabis use, Drug Alcohol
Depend, Vol. 73, pp. 109119.
Ramaekers JG, Robbe HW, OHanlon JF. (2000) Marijuana, alcohol
and actual driving performance, Hum. Psychopharmacol, Vol. 15,
pp. 551558.
Robertson M, Drummer O. (1994) Responsibility analysis: A method-
ology to study the effects of drugs in driving, Accident Analysis and
Prevention, Vol. 26, pp. 243247.
Roehrs T, Clairborue D, Knox M, Roth T. (1994) Residual sedating
effects of ethanol, Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
Vol. 18, pp. 831834.
198 E. J. D. OGDEN AND H. MOSKOWITZ
Roehrs T, Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos A, Zwyghuizen H, Roth T. (1989)
Sedating effects of ethanol after a nap, Alcohol, Drugs and Driving,
Vol. 5, pp. 351356.
Sepp¨al¨a K, Korttila K, Hakkinen S, Linnoila M. (1976a) Residual ef-
fects and skills related to driving after a single oral administration of
diazepam, medazepam or lorazepam, Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol, Vol. 3,
831841.
Sepp¨al¨a T, Saario I, Mattila M. (1976b) Two weekstreatment with
chlorpromazine, thioridazine, sulpiride, or bromazepam: Actions and
interactions with alcohol on psychomotor skills related to driving,
Mod. Probl. Pharmacopsychiatry, Vol. 11, 8590.
Silber B, Papafotiou K, Croft R, Ogden E, Swann P, Stough C. (2004)
The Effects of Dexamphetamine on Driving Performance. (submitted
for publication).
Skegg D, Richards S, Doll R. (1979) Minor tranquillisers and road
accidents, Br Med J, Vol. 1, pp. 917919.
Smiley A, Moskowitz H, Ziedman K. (1981) Driving simulator studies
of marijuana alone and in combination with alcohol. Paper presented
at the 25th Conference of the American Association for Automobile
Medicine.
Stapleton J, Guthrie S, Linnoila M. (1986) Effects of Alcohol and other
Psychotropic Drugs on Eye Movements: Relevance to Trafc Safety,
J. Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 47, pp. 426432.
Starmer G. (1989) Effects of low to moderate doses of ethanol on
human driving performance. In Human Metabolism of Alcohol (Vol-
ume 1): Pharmacokinetics, Medicolegal aspects and General Inter-
est, Crow K., Batt R. (eds.), pp. 101133. CRC Press, Boca Raton,
Florida.
Starmer G, Mascord D. (1994) Drugs and Trafc Safety, Report CR
140. Report Federal Ofce of Road Safety.
Starmer G, Vine J, Watson T. (1988) A Coordinated Approach to
the Drugs and Trafc Safety Problem. In Medicines and Road
Trafc Safety, Burley D., Silverstone T. (eds.) CNS Publishers,
London.
Stein, S. (1990) Risk factors of sober and drunk drivers by time of day,
Alcohol, Drugs and Driving, Vol. 3, pp. 215217.
Terhune K, Ippolito C, Hendricks D, Michalovic J, Bogema S, Santinga
P, Blomberg R, Preusser D. (1992) The incidence and role of drugs in
fatally injured drivers. US Department of Transportation, National.
Tharp V, Burns M, Moskowitz H. (1981) Development of Psychophys-
ical Tests for DWI Arrest, Report Contract DOT-HS-8-01970. US
Department of Transportation, National Highway Trafc Safety Ad-
ministration, Washington, DC.
Thomas R. (1998) Benzodiazepine use and motor vehicle accidents.
Systematic review of reported association, Canadian Family Physi-
cian, Vol. 44, pp. 799808.
Thompson-Crawford T, Slater S. (1971) Eye signs in suspected drink-
ing drivers: Clinical examination and relation to blood alcohol. New
Zealand Medical J., pp. 9296.
Tzambasis K. (2001) An Evaluation of the Efciency of Sobriety Test-
ing to Detect Cannabis and Impaired Driving Ability. In Department
of Neuropsychology. Swinburne University, Melbourne.
Tzambazis K, Stough C, Nathan P. (2000) Detection of Cannabis-
Induced Impairment with Sobriety Testing, Report Technical Report
No: DFA 98/99-10. Vicroads.
Tzambazis K, Stough C, Nathan P. (2001) Detection of Cannabis-
Induced Impairment with Sobriety Testing, Report DFA 98/99-10.
Vicroads.
Uges D. (2004) Therapeutic and Toxic Drug Concentrations List. The
International Association of Forensic Toxicologists.
Urso Gavaler JS, Van Thiel DH. (1981) Blood ethanol levels in sober al-
cohol users seen in an emergency room, Sciences, Vol. 28, pp. 1053
1056.
Vamosi M. (1960). Determination of the amount of alcohol in the blood
of motorists, Trafc Safety Res. Rev., Vol. 4, No. 3, pp. 811.
Wachtel SR, de Wit H. (1999) Subjective and behavioural effects of
repeated d-amphetamine in humans, Behavioural Pharmacology,
Vol. 10, pp. 271281.
Waller P, Stewart R, Hansen A, Stutts J, Popkin C, Rodgman E. (1986)
The potentiating effects of alcohol on driver injury, Journal of the
American Medical Association, Vol. 256, pp. 14611466.
WallgrenH, Barry H. (1970) Actions of Alcohol, Vol I&II, pp. 287292,
Elsevier Publishing, Amsterdam-London-New York.
Wells LG, MA, Foss RD, Ferguson SA, Williams AF. (1997) Drinking
drivers missed at sobriety checkpoints, Journal of Studies on Alcohol,
Vol. 15, pp. 513517.
Widmark E. (1981) Die theoretischen Grundlagen und die praktische
Verwendbarkeit der gerichtlich-medizinischen Alkoholbestimmung.
In Principles and Applications of Medico-legal Alcohol Determina-
tion. Biomed. Publ, Davis, Calif.
Wilkinson I, Kime R, Purnel M. (1974) Alcohol and Human Eye Move-
ment, Brain, Vol. 97, pp. 785792.
Woolf P, Cox C, Kelly M, McDonald J, Harnill R. (1990) Alcohol intox-
ication blunts sympattheto-adrenal activation following brain injury,
Alcoholism Clinical and Experimental Research, Vol. 14, pp. 205
209.
Yesavage JA, Leirer VO. (1986) Hnagover effects on aircraft pilots 14
hours after alcohol ingestion: A preliminary report, Am. J. Psychiatry,
Vol. 143, No. 12, pp. 15461550.
York JL, Regan SG. (1988) After-effects of acute alcohol intoxication,
Alcohol, Vol. 5, pp. 403407.
Zador P. (1991) Alcohol-related relative risk of fatal driver injuries in
relation to driver age and sex, Journal of Studies on Alcoho, Vol. 52,
pp. 302310.
Zeidman K, Moskowitz H, Niemann R. (1980) Effects of alcohol on
drivers visual information processing, Report PB81-172751. Na-
tional Highway Trafc Safety Administration, US Department of
Transportation, Washington, DC.
... However, human (driver)-related factors are the most significant, accounting for about 78% of accidents [9]. Substance use, including alcohol and other psychotropic substances, is a widely recognized considerable risk factor that significantly inflates the risk of RTAs to two-to sixfold, and multiple substance use poses a greater risk [10][11][12][13][14]. Substance use impairs driving by slowing reaction time, blurring vision, fostering false confidence, diminishing concentration, and encouraging aggressive driving [12]. ...
... However, human (driver)-related factors are the most significant, accounting for about 78% of accidents [9]. Substance use, including alcohol and other psychotropic substances, is a widely recognized considerable risk factor that significantly inflates the risk of RTAs to two-to sixfold, and multiple substance use poses a greater risk [10][11][12][13][14]. Substance use impairs driving by slowing reaction time, blurring vision, fostering false confidence, diminishing concentration, and encouraging aggressive driving [12]. In the year 2022, 2.2% of RTAs in India were due to consumption of alcohol and any other drug(s) [4]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Road traffic accidents (RTAs) are a critical public health problem leading to significant morbidity, mortality, and socioeconomic losses. Despite known risk factors like substance use and sleep-related problems, there is limited research on the prevalence of these factors among drivers who met with RTAs. Hence, this study aimed to gain insight into the prevalence of substance use and sleep-related problems among this population attending a trauma center in the northern State of India. Methodology: A cross-sectional study was conducted among 383 driver victims (DVs) who presented to a publicly funded tertiary care hospital's trauma emergency department of the Himalayan State of India following RTAs. The hospital's catchment area is vast and caters to people from both hilly and plain areas. Data were collected for sociodemographic characteristics, clinical parameters, and accident-related factors using a semi-structured proforma. Substance use-related problems were assessed through detailed history evaluation, thorough examinations, structured questionnaires, and body fluid (blood and urine) drug analysis. Sleep-related parameters were evaluated in detail, including excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS), the functional outcome of sleepiness, and the chronotype using structured and validated questionnaires. The nature, site of injuries, and their severity were determined using the Abbreviated Injury Severity (AIS) Scale. Results: Among DVs, 221 (57.7%) tested positive for alcohol; 71 (18.6%) had used other psychotropic substances, with cannabis being the most common among them; and 56 (14.6%) reported using multiple substances. Eighty-three (21.7%) participants had EDS, and 102 (26.6%) experienced fatigue and sleepiness during the accident. The most common type of injuries was fracture and dislocation 206 (53.8%), with the extremities (both upper and lower) being the most common body region (218, 56.9%) involved, along with head traumas in equal proportions. Injuries were predominantly minor, yet a concerning 7.6% of the participants experienced severe trauma. Conclusion: The study highlights the substantial role of substance use and sleep-related problems in RTAs, emphasizing the need for interventions targeting these factors to reduce the burden of RTAs. Policies enforcing stricter substance use regulations and promoting sleep health awareness and sleep assessments for drivers could significantly mitigate RTAs and improve road safety in India.
... Drunk driving is also reported to be responsible for more than 40% of all traffic accidents [1][2][3][4][5]. Drunk driving remains a pervasive and deadly issue worldwide, with significant social and economic consequences [6][7][8][9]. Despite stringent legal frameworks and widespread public awareness campaigns, alcohol-impaired driving continues to result in a substantial number of traffic accidents, injuries, and fatalities [10][11][12][13]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to decrease the number of drunk drivers, a significant social problem. Traditional methods to measure alcohol intake include blood alcohol concentration (BAC) and breath alcohol concentration (BrAC) tests. While BAC testing requires blood samples and is impractical, BrAC testing is commonly used in drunk driving enforcement. In this study, the multiple biological signals of electrocardiogram (ECG), photoplethysmogram (PPG), and electrodermal activity (EDA) were collected non-invasively and with minimal driver restraint in a driving simulator. Data were collected from 10 participants for approximately 10 min at BrAC levels of 0.00%, 0.03%, and 0.08%, which align with the latest Korean drunk driving standards. The collected data underwent frequency filtering and were segmented into 30 s intervals with a 10 s overlap to extract heart rate variability (HRV) and pulse arrival time (PAT). Using more than 10 machine learning algorithms, the classification accuracy reached 88%. The results indicate that it is possible to classify a driver’s level of intoxication using only non-invasive biological signals within a short period of about 30 s, potentially aiding in the prevention of drunk driving.
... Alcohol consumption impairs brain functioning and can affect road safety even at low alcohol concentrations (Ogden & Moskowitz, 2004). Drink-driving is defined as driving under the influence of alcohol. ...
Article
Aims: To assess the prevalence of drink-driving among adults in Germany and subgroups, and potential associations with sociodemographic or drinking characteristics. Methodology: Cross-sectional population survey of 5,153 respondents aged 18+ years in Germany (June–November 2021). We assessed self-reported drink-driving, defined as, in the past month, driving a motorised vehicle within 1 hour of consuming 2 or more alcoholic beverages. Regression models were used to examine sociodemographic and drinking characteristics associations with self-reported drink-driving among adults in Germany who drank alcohol and reported driving a motorised vehicle in the past month. Results: The prevalence of self-reported drink-driving was 4.1 % (95 % CI 3.6–4.7) among adults in Germany, and 5.7 % (95 % CI 5.0–6.5) among adults who were alcohol users and reported driving a motorised vehicle in the past month. Lower odds of drink-driving were found for women vs men (adjusted odds ratio (OR adj ) = 0.63, 95 % CI 0.45–0.88), people with low vs middle income (OR adj = 0.82, 95 % CI 0.72–0.94), people living in rural vs urban areas (OR adj = 0.38, 95 % CI 0.27–0.54), and people consuming low vs medium alcohol levels (OR adj = 0.28, 95 % CI 0.18–0.44). There was no clear association with age or education level. Conclusions: Roughly 4 in 100 adults drank and drove in the past month in Germany. This is a public health issue requiring further study and potentially more targeted interventions.
... Prestasjonsforringelse varierer mye mellom enkeltindivider med samme blodalkoholkonsentrasjon (10,11). Ved innføringen av straffbarhetsgrense for alkoholpåvirkning har man sett bort fra slik variabilitet, som blant annet kan skyldes toleranseutvikling. ...
... Moderate levels of alcohol reduce the speed of temporal visual processing by increasing neuronal latencies across a variety of visual tasks (Khan & Timney, 2007). It has been argued that significant perceptual, motor and cognitive impairments occur even at very low blood alcohol levels (<0.02gm/100ml) (Koelega, 1995;Ogden & Moskowitz, 2004). ...
Preprint
The study aimed to investigate to what extent acute moderate doses of alcohol affect the speed of endogenous versus exogenous attentional shift times. Subjects viewed an array of 10 moving clocks and reported the time a clock indicated when cued. Target clocks were indicated by peripheral or central cues, including conditions of pre-cuing. This allowed assessing shift times when attention was pre-allocated, when peripheral cues triggered exogenous attention shifts, and when central cues triggered endogenous attention shifts. Each subject participated in 2 sessions (alcohol/placebo), whereby the order of drug intake was counterbalanced across subjects, and subjects were blinded to conditions. Confirming previous results, we show that pre-cuing resulted in the fastest shift times, followed by exogenous cuing, with endogenous attentional shifts being slowest. Alcohol increased attentional shift times across all 3 conditions compared to placebo. Thus, the detrimental effects of alcohol on attentional shift times did not depend on the type of attention probed.
... 0.015%), a driver's ability to divide attention between two or more sources of visual information may be impaired. 37 Several reviews and individual studies have concluded there are no thresholds of BAC below which there is no impairment. It is noteworthy that in Sweden and Norway legal limits for blood alcohol while driving are currently among the lowest in the world (i.e. ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
This research report is a unique compilation of research on how alcohol affects the human brain throughout the life course. Overall, it shows that brain health benefits from avoiding alcohol. This applies both to occasional drinking as well as to intensive or regular consumption over time. The report highlights that alcohol affects all brain functions and that it impairs, for example, memory and intellectual ability. In addition, alcohol increases the risk of several brain diseases and injuries, for example fetal alcohol syndrome, accidents, stroke, and dementia. Reducing or avoiding alcohol intake is an important factor in preserving brain health. To get there, alcohol policy measures are required to reduce alcohol’s contribution to neurological, cognitive and psychiatric problems at the population level.
... Alcohol use while driving has been repeatedly emphasised as a main risk factor for RTCs and injuries among motorcycle drivers (Da Silva et al., 2012;Kiwango et al., 2021;Truong & Nguyen, 2019). Acute alcohol consumption can affect driving directly by impairing motor and coordination functions and distorting colour discrimination in red light crossing (Jurgen, 2013;Ogden & Moskowitz, 2004;Woratanarat et al., 2009). Alcohol use has also been shown to indirectly increase risk-taking behaviours such as speeding, non-helmet use and tailgating (Sewell et al., 2009;Staton et al., 2018;Woratanarat et al., 2009), which may significantly increase the crash and injury risk. ...
Article
Full-text available
The effects of alcohol are especially problematic in terms of trauma and injury that is encountered in the emergency departments (EDs) of hospitals globally. Such injuries occur due to different causes such as falls, assaults or motor vehicle accidents and management is often associated with alcohol and potential for other drug use. This review provides an up-to-date perspective on the diagnostic assessment and initial management of alcohol-related trauma in the ED. Alcohol-related injury is common, and a large proportion of trauma patients have blood alcohol concentrations (BACs). These patients are often young adults and males and the injuries can be in the head and neck regions mostly. Performing screening and identification of alcohol use among patients in the ED is essential to ensure appropriate feedback is given immediately. However, there are diagnostic difficulties due to the imitation or masking of injuries by alcohol influence. This means that the immediate agenda has a priority in establishing and responding to emergent situations and acute conditions and far-reaching plans include provisions of brief interventions and linkages to care. There are significant economic consequences of injuries due to alcohol, which can lead to escalation of health care costs. Potential issues for public health focus on the objectives towards campaigns and policies that are relevant to the risks that come along with the use of alcohol. This review also emphasizes the need for multifaceted continuation care approaches to minimize the reunion of alcohol-related injuries and their socioeconomic impacts.
Article
This paper describes a newly developed questionnaire-based survey conducted in a nationwide online survey in Germany that measures the prevalence of driving under the influence of alcohol via self-reports concerning randomly selected trips from 7 days prior. The results analysed using a multivariate regression analysis point to the likelihood of men and younger drivers in particular driving under the influence of alcohol. The proposed survey concept adds a new variant to the set of instruments for recording driving under the influence of alcohol by determining a trip-based prevalence, thus offering new insights into driving under the influence in alcohol of Germany.
Article
Behavioral sensitization is thought to be an important determinant of drug-taking and drug-seeking behaviors. Although there is abundant research characterizing behavioral sensitization in animals, there is little evidence for this phenomenon in humans. The aim of the present study was to determine if repeated oral d-amphetamine administration enhances self-reported mood and other behavioral indices of d-amphetamine effects in humans. Sixteen healthy volunteers, crith no prior stimulant use, received two doses of d-amphetamine (20 mg) and two doses of placebo, in alternating order, on 4 consecutive days, under double-blind conditions. Mood and behavioral effects were measured using standard self-report questionnaires. Heart rate, blood pressure, psychomotor performance, and tapping speed were also monitored. d-Amphetamine elicited prototypical increases on several measures including self-reported drug effects, mood, and physiological responses. However, except for a slight reduction in 'feel drug' scores during the first hour of the second d-amphetamine session, the majority of effects were not altered on the second session. These results indicate that the subjective effects of d-amphetamine display only an apparent mild tolerance after a single exposure 48 h earlier. (C) 1999 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Chapter
This chapter aims to provide practice-oriented information for prescribing physicians and dispensing pharmacists who want to provide their patients with adequate advice based on a clear understanding of the risks of accident involvement under different treatment conditions. Specific attention will be given to the application of a graded-level warning system based on categorization systems for psychotropic medicines that have been introduced,, and sometimes legally implemented, in several European countries. This warning system allows physicians and pharmacists to select the least impairing medicines within a therapeutic class. Advice for the patient based on three categories has been described in clear instructions, allowing the patient to make the right decision. For the most frequently used drug classes (antihistamines, antidepressants, hypnotics and tranquillizers) information will be provided on drugs with little or no impairment within the respective classes, and risk factors (e.g. liver and./or renal dysfunction, drug-drug interactions) that might increase impairing effects. If drugs with little or no impairment can not be prescribed, specific patient information will focus on recognizing signs of impaired performance.
Article
Blood alcohol concentrations (BACs) over 0.04% are definitely associated with an increased accident rate. The probability of accident involvement increases rapidly at BACs over 0.08%, and becomes extremely high at BACs above 0.15%. When drivers with BACs over 0.08% have accidents, they tend to have more single vehicle accidents, more severe (in terms of injury and damage) accidents and more expensive accidents than sober drivers. BACs of 0.04% and below apparently are not inconsistent with traffic safety. Many factors other than alcohol are related to the probability of accident involvement. The driver classes with the worst accident experience, in addition to the alcoholically impaired, are the young or very old, the inexperienced and those with less formal education. Persons with the most education, those with better jobs and the middle aged, have better than average accident experience. The effects of alcohol are consistent within the various socioeconomic classes considered. High BACs are always associated with bad accident experience. At the higher BACs, the difference in the accident potential between the various classes of drivers is unimportant. An important aspect of the applied survey technique is that it is adaptable to assessing the effect of various policies directed at the drinking driver. Drinking and driving is clearly associated with the frequent use, or abuse, of alcohol. Many drivers with BACs overestimate the number of drinks that it is safe to have before driving. The tendency to drive after drinking is related significantly to the socioeconomic categories appearing most frequently in the drinking driver class.