ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

This research examined whether parents' and children's perceptions of one another have reciprocal self-fulfilling effects on each other's behavior. Parents and their adolescent children completed self-report surveys and engaged in dyadic videotaped interaction tasks. The surveys assessed mothers', fathers', and children's perceptions of their own and the other's hostility and warmth. Observers coded the videotaped interactions to assess the actual hostility and warmth exhibited by parents and children. Data from 658 mother-child dyads were consistent with the conclusion that children had a self-fulfilling effect on their mothers' hostility but that mothers did not have a reciprocal self-fulfilling effect on their children's hostility. No other self-fulfilling prophecy effects emerged. Findings are discussed in terms of family relations and the differential power of negative versus positive self-fulfilling prophecies.
The Self-Fulfilling Prophecy as an Intrafamily Dynamic
Stephanie Madon, Max Guyll, and Richard L. Spoth
Iowa State University
This research examined whether parents’ and children’s perceptions of one another have
reciprocal self-fulfilling effects on each other’s behavior. Parents and their adolescent
children completed self-report surveys and engaged in dyadic videotaped interaction tasks.
The surveys assessed mothers’, fathers’, and children’s perceptions of their own and the
other’s hostility and warmth. Observers coded the videotaped interactions to assess the actual
hostility and warmth exhibited by parents and children. Data from 658 mother–child dyads
were consistent with the conclusion that children had a self-fulfilling effect on their mothers’
hostility but that mothers did not have a reciprocal self-fulfilling effect on their children’s
hostility. No other self-fulfilling prophecy effects emerged. Findings are discussed in terms
of family relations and the differential power of negative versus positive self-fulfilling prophecies.
keywords: self-fulfilling prophecies, hostility, family relations, interpersonal interaction, dyads
Enduring dyadic relationships emerge and develop as a
result of recurring interactions. As a result of having expe-
rienced numerous interactions in the past, individuals in an
ongoing relationship form a relatively stable set of percep-
tions of the relationship, such as whether it tends to be
characterized by hostility versus warmth, distance versus
closeness, or competition versus cooperation (Hinde &
Stevenson-Hinde, 1987). These perceptions imply expecta-
tions for future interactions, including expectations for
one’s own behavior, as well as expectations for the other
person’s behavior (Hinde, 1979; Lollis, 2003). Relation-
ships and the interpersonal perceptions and expectations
they entail are important because they provide a context
whereby one can readily interpret and assign meaning to the
events that transpire in the course of each new interaction
that occurs. As such, perceptions and expectations have the
potential to actively shape one’s behavior during an inter-
action such that it can alter the interaction itself (Lollis,
2003). If such a process were to occur reliably, then it is
conceivable that the perceptions and expectations that peo-
ple hold for one another could eventually change their
relationship. One interpersonal process through which such
change might occur is the self-fulfilling prophecy.
Self-Fulfilling Prophecies
A self-fulfilling prophecy is an erroneous belief that leads
to its own fulfillment (Merton, 1948). A self-fulfilling
prophecy consists of a sequence of three steps. First, one
person (the perceiver) must develop an inaccurate belief
about another person (the target). Second, the perceiver
must treat the target in a manner that is consistent with the
inaccurate belief. Third, the target must respond to the
perceiver’s treatment by confirming the originally inaccu-
rate belief. Numerous experimental and naturalistic studies
have demonstrated the existence of self-fulfilling prophecies
(for reviews, see Jussim, Eccles, & Madon, 1996; Snyder &
Stukas, 1999). However, this research has focused exclu-
sively on unilateral effects, investigating how the inaccurate
beliefs of one person shape the behavioral outcomes of a
second person and not considering how the second person’s
inaccurate beliefs might simultaneously shape the behav-
ioral outcomes of the first person. Although a unilateral
focus is appropriate for studies in which the outcome of
interest is only relevant to the target (e.g., children’s aca-
demic achievement), outcomes that are relevant to both
perceivers and targets are more appropriately studied with
an analytic approach that assesses bidirectional effects. For
example, both parents and children may hold inaccurate
perceptions of the others’ negative or positive affective
behaviors, and both may be affected by the others’ percep-
tion. In cases such as this, individuals are simultaneously
both perceiver and target. A unilateral approach to studying
such relations is not appropriate because it has the potential
to produce misleading results, such as overestimating the
self-fulfilling prophecy effect that one person has on anoth-
er’s behavior.
Bilateral Model of Parent–Child Relations
The likelihood of bidirectional influence has long been
recognized by both the self-fulfilling prophecy and parent–
child relations literatures (e.g., Bell, 1971; Snyder & Stukas,
1999). For example, whereas historical perspectives por-
trayed children as passive recipients of the effects of more
Stephanie Madon, Department of Psychology, Iowa State Uni-
versity; Max Guyll and Richard L. Spoth, Partnerships in Preven-
tion Science, Iowa State University.
We thank Kenneth Bollen for his thoughtful and thorough re-
sponses to our questions regarding the calculation of specific effects.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Stephanie Madon, Department of Psychology, Iowa State Univer-
sity, Research Park Building 2, 2625 North Loop Drive, Suite 500,
Ames, IA 50010-8296. E-mail: madon@iastate.edu
Journal of Family Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association
2004, Vol. 18, No. 3, 459469 0893-3200/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.3.459
459
powerful parents, the bilateral model (Kuczynski, 2003)
proposes that (a) bidirectional effects exist, (b) both parents
and children are willful agents of inuence, and (c) children
have resources whereby they can inuence their parents,
even though the parents ultimately have more power in the
relationship. Despite this trend in theoretical perspectives,
there has been little in the way of empirical analysis that has
directly examined reciprocal effects within specic interac-
tions. Therefore, the current study investigates relations
between parents and their children during a specic inter-
action using the bilateral model as the guiding framework.
Overview of the Current Investigation and
Hypotheses
This research examines whether parents and childrens
perceptions of one another have reciprocal self-fullling
prophecy effects on the others behavior during mother
child and fatherchild interactions. This issue is examined
within the framework of the bilateral model of parentchild
relations in that reciprocal effects between parent and child
are included in the model. To the best of our knowledge, no
previous study has examined reciprocal self-fullling
prophecies. The outcome variables of interest are the
observer-rated hostility and warmth that parents and chil-
dren express to one another during their interactions. The
importance of examining how self-fullling prophecies
might inuence the affective quality of parentchild inter-
actions is demonstrated by the link between parentchild
affect and important child outcomes. For example, negative
parentchild relationships reduce parentchild closeness,
inhibit the childs attachment to the parent, and impede the
childs internalization of parental valuesfactors that in-
crease childrens risk of developing problem behaviors
(Catalano & Hawkins, 1996).
Method
Participants
Participants were 658 mothers, 576 fathers, and 680 children
who were participating in a larger study.
1
At the time of the study,
children were in early adolescence (M 12.3 years, SD 0.5
years), all in the 7th grade; the sample included 313 (46%) girls
and 367 (54%) boys. Only one child in each family provided data.
Mothers averaged 39 years of age (SD 5.5 years). Fathers
averaged 41 years of age (SD 6.1 years). Mothers and fathers
averaged between 1 and 2 years of post-secondary education. The
median annual income of participating families was $40,000 (M
$43,134, SD $25,136). Characteristic of the geographic area,
more than 98% of parents and children identied their ethnicity as
White.
Procedure
Baseline assessments. Baseline assessments were conducted
in the familys home. Parents and the target child independently
completed questionnaires in separate parts of the residence. Family
members were reminded that their responses would be kept con-
dential and would not be communicated to other family members.
Questionnaire completion required approximately 70 min.
Videotaped interactions. During the in-home visit, parents
and the target child were also videotaped during structured dis-
cussion tasks. The discussion tasks began as soon as project staff
had prepared for the videotaping and occurred both before and
after questionnaire completion. For dual-parent families, the vid-
eotaping included two dyadic interactions (i.e., motherchild and
fatherchild) and one triadic interaction (i.e., motherfather
child). Either the mother or the father was randomly selected for
the rst dyadic interaction, during which time they discussed a
series of questions concerning typical aspects of family life (e.g.,
household chores) for 15 min. Families then engaged in the triadic
interaction for 12 min, during which time both parents and their
child discussed issues of disagreement (e.g., curfews). The third
discussion task followed the same format as the rst, with the
exception that the child interacted with the parent who had not
participated in the rst dyadic discussion task.
2
Measures
This investigation focuses on measures that pertain to the qual-
ity of the parentchild relationship (assessed by self-report) and to
parentchild dyadic interaction behaviors (assessed by third-party
observation). These measures are described below.
Perceptions of hostility and warmth. Mothers, fathers, and
children individually responded to questions that assessed their
perceptions of their own and the others typical hostility and
warmth. To assess parents perceptions of the hostility that they
typically show toward their children, and childrens perceptions of
the hostility that their parents typically show toward them, each
parent and child reported: how often the mother (father) loses her
(his) temper and yells at the child, and how often in the past month
the mother (father) got angry at the child, shouted or yelled at the
child, and insulted or swore at the child. To assess childrens
perceptions of the hostility that they typically show toward their
parents, and parentsperceptions of the hostility that their children
typically show toward them, each parent and child reported how
often during the past month the child: got angry at the mother
(father), shouted or yelled at the mother (father), and insulted or
swore at the mother (father).
To assess parents perceptions of the warmth that they typically
show toward their children, and childrens perceptions of the
warmth that their parents typically show toward them, each parent
and child reported how often during the past month the mother
(father): let the child know that she (he) cares about her or him,
acted loving and affectionate toward the child, and let the child
know that she (he) appreciates her or him. To assess childrens
perceptions of the warmth they typically show toward their par-
ents, and parents perceptions of the warmth their children typi-
cally show toward them, each parent and child reported how often
during the past month the child: let the mother (father) know that
1
The larger study included a control condition plus two inter-
ventions designed to prevent adolescent problem behaviors: the
Life Skills Training program (LST; Spoth, Redmond, Trudeau, &
Shin, 2002), and the Strengthening Families Program: For Parents
and Youth 1014 (Molgaard, Kumpfer, & Fleming, 1997) in
combination with the LST. Because the current study includes only
data collected prior to implementation of the interventions, the
interventions could not have affected the results of this investiga-
tion and, therefore, are not further discussed.
2
Single-parent families engaged in two parentchild interac-
tions. The rst focused on aspects of family life. The second
focused on issues of disagreements.
460 MADON, GUYLL, AND SPOTH
she or he cares about her (him), acted loving and affectionate
toward the mother (father), and let the mother (father) know that
she or he appreciates her (him).
All items were assessed on a 7-point response scale ranging
from 1 (always)to7(never) except for the items assessing per-
ceptions of parents tendency to lose their temper and yell at the
child, which was assessed on a 5-point response scale ranging from
1(almost always)to5(almost never). Responses to these items
were rescaled into a 7-point response scale in order to combine
them with the other hostility items. All items were reverse scored
so that higher values reected greater perceptions of hostility and
warmth. Tables 1 and 2 present means, variances, and internal
consistencies for each scale.
Interaction behaviors. Interaction behaviors exhibited during
the motherchild and fatherchild dyadic discussion tasks were
coded for hostility and warmth. Trained coders used the Iowa
Family Interaction Rating Scales (IFIRS; Melby & Conger, 2001),
which has been shown to be both reliable and valid (e.g., Melby,
Conger, & Puspitawati, 1999). The trained coders provided global
assessments of the hostility and warmth displayed by mother and
child and father and child on a 9-point rating scale ranging from 1
(not at all characteristic)to9(mainly characteristic). Each vid-
eotape required approximately 3 hr to code. To assess interrater
reliability, approximately 25% of the videotapes were randomly
selected to be coded by a second coder. As detailed in Tables 1 and
2, intraclass correlation coefcients (ICCs) indicated good inter-
rater agreement for the observer ratings of the parentchild dyads
(.61 all ICCs .80).
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Table 1 presents the correlations and covariances among
the hostility-related variables included in the analysis, as
well as the mean and variance for each variable. Table 2
presents parallel information for the warmth-related data.
Overview of Analyses
An observed variable path-analytic approach was taken
utilizing LISREL 8.30 (Jo¨reskog & So¨rbom, 1999). Figure
1 presents the path model that was used to perform four
analyses of data acquired from the self-report questionnaires
and the observer ratings of parentchild dyadic interactions.
The four analyses correspond to the motherchild hostility
and warmth data and to the fatherchild hostility and
warmth data. Because the analysis estimates each path co-
efcients asymptotic standard error, the path coefcient is
divided by its standard error to yield a z-test for statistical
signicance (Bollen, 1989). The following sections explain
how the models effects pertain to the issues addressed by
this investigation.
3
Predictive accuracy versus self-fullling inuence.
There are two nonmutually exclusive reasons why a per-
ceivers perception might predict a targets behavior, one
relating to predictive accuracy and one relating to self-
fullling inuence. Because a perception can be both par-
3
As shown on the left-hand side of Figure 1, the model allowed
for intercorrelations among parents and childrens self-reports of
both their own and the others typical behavior. However, these
relationships are of only secondary interest vis a´ vis the issues
examined in the current investigation. Therefore, we devote text
neither to their meaning (which in any case is straightforward) nor
to their corresponding results (which are provided in Figure 1).
Table 1
Descriptive Data for Self- and Observer Reports Relating to Interpersonal Hostility: Covariances, Correlations, Means,
Variances, and Reliability
Variable 123456
Motherchild dyads (N 658)
1. C typical hostility to M (C) (0.86) 0.52 0.66 0.39 0.31 0.31
2. C typical hostility to M (M) 0.45 (0.88) 0.45 0.70 0.26 0.32
3. M typical hostility to C (C) 0.62 0.43 (1.04) 0.46 0.28 0.29
4. M typical hostility to C (M) 0.32 0.59 0.42 (0.80) 0.16 0.31
5. C actual hostility to M (O) 0.52 0.45 0.53 0.26 (3.30) 0.46
6. M actual hostility to C (O) 0.53 0.55 0.55 0.52 1.56 (3.44)
M 2.11 2.30 2.42 2.80 2.22 2.68
a
.69 .76 .80 .83 .73 .61
Fatherchild dyads (N 576)
1. C typical hostility to F (C) (0.63) 0.24 0.54 0.18 0.32 0.32
2. C typical hostility to F (F) 0.39 (0.62) 0.25 0.37 0.23 0.37
3. F typical hostility to C (C) 0.67 0.31 (1.04) 0.36 0.26 0.44
4. F typical hostility to C (F) 0.28 0.58 0.43 (0.67) 0.08 0.39
5. C actual hostility to F (O) 0.25 0.18 0.16 0.06 (2.66) 1.29
6. F actual hostility to C (O) 0.23 0.27 0.25 0.27 0.45 (3.10)
M 1.81 2.03 2.22 2.58 1.91 2.27
a
.61 .76 .80 .83 .80 .63
Note. C child; M mother; O observer; F father. Variances appear in parentheses on the diagonal, and correlations and
covariances appear above and below the diagonal, respectively. (C), (M), and (F) denote variables based on child, mother, and father
self-report, respectively. O denotes variables based on observer ratings of actual behaviors exhibited during parentchild dyadic
interactions.
a
Values represent internal consistency scores as assessed by Cronbachs
for questionnaire scales. Values for observer ratings (O)
represent interobserver reliabilities as assessed by intraclass correlation coefcients.
461SELF-FULFILLING PROPHECIES
tially accurate and partially inaccurate, the magnitude of the
total bivariate relationship between the perceivers percep-
tion and a targets behavior can include a combination of
both predictive accuracy and self-fullling inuence
(Jussim, 1991). Therefore, when testing for self-fullling
prophecy effects, one must attempt to control for predictive
accuracy.
Accuracy is the extent to which a perceivers perception
accurately and directly predicts a targets behavior without
causing the behavior. The model shown in Figure 1 controls
for accuracy by including the direct effects of child percep-
tions of child behavior on child behavior (Path a), parent
perceptions of child behavior on child behavior (Path b),
child perceptions of parent behavior on parent behavior
(Path e), and parent perceptions of parent behavior on parent
behavior (Path f). To the degree that either parent or child
perceptions can directly predict either parent or child be-
havior, that portion of the variance in actual behavior will be
attributed to accuracy and will, therefore, be unavailable for
providing support for the existence of a self-fullling proph-
ecy (see Jussim et al., 1996).
Self-fullling inuence, by contrast, is the extent to
which a perceivers perception indirectly predicts a targets
behavior because the perception rst inuences perceiver
behavior which, in turn, inuences target behavior. That is,
the effect of the perceivers perception on the targets be-
havior cannot be direct, but must be indirect and mediated
solely by the perceivers behavior. For example, in the
current model, a child-based self-fullling prophecy effect
on a parent is associated with the indirect effect of com-
pound Path cg, and a parent-based self-fullling prophecy
effect is associated with the indirect effect of compound
Path dh. As noted above, only the portion of a targets
behavior that cannot be accounted for by accuracy (i.e., the
direct effects of both interactantsperceptions of the targets
behavior) is available for providing support for a self-
fullling prophecy. In addition, only the portion of the
perceivers behavior that cannot be attributed to accuracy
(i.e., the direct effects of both interactants perceptions of
the perceivers behavior) is available for mediating the
indirect effect of the perceivers perception on the targets
behavior and thereby providing support for a self-fullling
prophecy.
Reciprocal effects. Paths g and h reect the reciprocal
effects anticipated in the interaction, in which one persons
behavior in the interaction affects the other persons behav-
ior. The self-fullling prophecy effects are transmitted by
compound paths that include these reciprocal effects.
4
Hostile Behavior
Model t. The model presented in Figure 1 provided a
good t to the hostility-relevant data for both motherchild,
4
Calculating reciprocal effects between parent and child behav-
iors requires summing across an innite series of terms to calculate
each self-fullling prophecy effect. For example, the child-based
effect is [(c g)] [(c g) (h g)] [(c g) (h g)
2
]
[(c g) (h g)
3
] (etc.).
Table 2
Descriptive Data for Self- and Observer Reports Relating to Interpersonal Warmth: Covariances, Correlations, Means,
Variances, and Reliability
Variable 123456
Motherchild dyads (N 658)
1. C typical warmth to M (C) (1.79) 0.47 0.70 0.39 0.24 0.17
2. C typical warmth to M (M) 0.80 (1.62) 0.39 0.72 0.27 0.20
3. M typical warmth to C (C) 1.21 0.65 (1.66) 0.40 0.17 0.19
4. M typical warmth to C (M) 0.51 0.90 0.50 (0.96) 0.20 0.25
5. C actual warmth to M (O) 0.46 0.49 0.32 0.28 (2.07) 0.18
6. M actual warmth to C (O) 0.42 0.45 0.45 0.43 0.47 (3.28)
M 5.31 4.91 5.51 5.60 2.15 6.10
a
.87 .89 .87 .89 .67 .78
Fatherchild dyads (N 576)
1. C typical warmth to F (C) (2.26) 0.81 1.78 0.52 0.37 0.26
2. C typical warmth to F (F) 0.42 (1.67) 0.72 1.04 0.31 0.49
3. F typical warmth to C (C) 0.86 0.40 (1.91) 0.50 0.36 0.28
4. F typical warmth to C (F) 0.32 0.74 0.33 (1.20) 0.22 0.54
5. C actual warmth to F (O) 0.17 0.17 0.18 0.14 (2.07) 0.58
6. F actual warmth to C (O) 0.09 0.20 0.11 0.25 0.21 (3.75)
M 5.20 4.48 5.35 4.92 2.06 5.65
a
.91 .88 .90 .90 .61 .78
Note. C child; M mother; O observer; F father. Variances appear in parentheses on the diagonal, and correlations and
covariances appear above and below the diagonal, respectively. (C), (M), and (F) denote variables based on child, mother, and father
self-report, respectively. O denotes variables based on observer ratings of actual behaviors exhibited during parentchild dyadic
interactions.
a
Values represent internal consistency scores as assessed by Cronbachs
for questionnaire scales. Values for observer ratings (O)
represent interobserver reliabilities as assessed by intraclass correlation coefcients.
462 MADON, GUYLL, AND SPOTH
Figure 1. Path model and results relating parent and child self- and other-reports to observer ratings of interpersonal hostility and warmth
actually exhibited during an interaction. Results pertaining to hostility for motherchild dyads (N 658) and fatherchild dyads (N
576) are identied by MH and FH, respectively. Likewise, results pertaining to warmth for motherchild and fatherchild dyads are
identied by MW and FW, respectively. * p .05. ** p .01.
463SELF-FULFILLING PROPHECIES
2
(1, 658) .04, p .84, and fatherchild,
2
(1, 576)
2.02, p .16, dyads.
Accuracy-based prediction of hostile behaviors. Figure
1 presents standardized effects and p levels for Paths a, b, e,
and f, which account for the effects of accuracy in predict-
ing hostile behavior for both motherchild (MH) and
fatherchild (FH) dyads. These results reveal that the child
and both parents were accurate in predicting child hostility
(Paths a and b). Results further indicated that whereas both
parents were accurate in predicting their own hostility (Path
f), child perceptions were not accurate predictors of parent
hostility (Path e).
Expectancy-based effects on hostile behaviors. Within
the context of the analytic model, the potential for one
persons behavior to be affected by her or his inaccurate
perception about the other person is represented by Paths c
and d in Figure 1. As reported in the gure, the only
expectancy-based effect was that childrens perception of
their mothers typical hostility was positively associated
with childrens hostility during the interaction (Path c). No
such effect was observed for mothers, nor for either children
or fathers in the fatherchild dyads.
Reciprocal effects of parent and child hostile behaviors.
The possibility that parent and child could inuence each
others behavior during the interaction is represented by
Paths g and h. As reported in Figure 1, ndings reveal that
greater child hostility in the interaction predicted both
greater mother hostility and greater father hostility (Path g).
Hostility exhibited by the parent did not predict child hos-
tility (Path h).
Self-fullling effects of parent and child perceptions of
hostility. The ndings reported above indicate that chil-
drens perception of their mothers typical hostility was
positively associated with childrens hostility during the
interaction, which, in turn, was positively associated with
mothers hostility during the interaction. These ndings
appear to suggest that childrens perceptions inuenced
mothers hostile behaviors via a self-fullling prophecy.
However, the fact that both of the constituent direct effects
(i.e. Paths c and g) are signicant does not necessarily mean
that the corresponding indirect effect is also signicant
(Bollen, 1987). Moreover, in order to test the self-fullling
prophecy hypothesis, it is necessary to test the specic
indirect effect of childrens perception on their mothers
hostile behavior that is specically conveyed through the
childrens behavior (i.e., solely via Path c, and not via Path
e).
5
To calculate the magnitude and signicance of this
specic indirect effect, we used Bollens (1987) procedure,
which makes use of the delta method (Sobel, 1986) to
generate the necessary standard errors. The interested reader
may obtain excellent treatments of these topics from the
citations provided.
Results from these analyses indicated that childrens per-
ceptions of their mothers typical hostility affected how
hostile they themselves behaved during the interaction,
which, in turn, affected how hostile their mothers actually
behaved during the interaction,
.11, z 2.58, p .01.
This pattern is consistent with a self-fullling prophecy and
occurred in the context of a model that attempted to control
for accuracy. That is, the model controlled for that portion
of mothers hostility that could be predicted by the ability of
both childrens and mothers perceptions to directly predict
mothers hostility (Paths e and f).
We also tested for a self-fullling prophecy originating in
mothers perceptions of childrens hostility, but analyses
yielded no support for such an effect (z 1). Results for
fatherchild dyads did not support the idea that fathers or
children conveyed self-fullling prophecy effects on the
others behavior, (zs 1).
Warmth Behaviors
Model t. The model presented in Figure 1 provided a
good t to the warmth-relevant data for both motherchild
dyads,
2
(1, N 658) .08, p .77, and fatherchild
dyads,
2
(1, N 576) 0.25, p .61.
Accuracy-based prediction of warmth behaviors. For
the motherchild dyads, ndings indicated that childrens
and mothers perceptions of childrens warmth exhibited a
measure of accuracy (Paths a and b), as did both childrens
and mothers perceptions of mothers warmth (Paths e and
f). For the fatherchild dyads, only fathers perception of
fathers warmth evidenced any accuracy, insomuch as it
predicted fathers actual warmth during the interaction
(Path f).
Expectancy-based effects on warmth behaviors. Results
provided no support for the idea that either mothers, fa-
thers, or childrens perceptions of the other persons
warmth affected their own warmth during the interaction
(Paths c and d).
Reciprocal effects of parent and child warmth interaction
behaviors. Analyses yielded no evidence for bidirectional
effects relating to warmth exhibited during the interaction
for either motherchild or fatherchild dyads (Paths g
and h).
Self-fullling effects of mothers and childrens percep-
tions of warmth. As detailed above, testing the self-
fullling prophecy hypothesis requires calculating and test-
ing a specic indirect effect. However, because none of the
constituent direct effects (i.e., Paths c, d, g, or h) associated
with either child- or parent-based self-fullling prophecies
for warmth approached signicance, the delta method is not
appropriate for calculating standard errors for the corre-
sponding specic indirect effects (Bollen, 1987). Hence, the
statistic for determining the signicance of the specic
indirect effects cannot be computed in rigorous fashion.
However, given that the constituent direct effects are non-
signicant and that the magnitudes of the specic indirect
effects pertaining to the child- and parent-based self-
fullling prophecy were negligible in all cases (all
s
.02), it is reasonable to conclude that if it had been possible
5
A specic effect is that part of the indirect or total effects
transmitted by a path or combination of individual paths(Bollen,
1987, p. 55). Thus, a specic indirect effect is that part of an
indirect effect that is transmitted specically by a particular path or
combination of paths.
464 MADON, GUYLL, AND SPOTH
to perform the calculation, the self-fullling prophecy ef-
fects would not have been statistically signicant.
Modeling MotherChild Effects as Unidirectional:
The Potential for Invalid Conclusions
The results reported above were based on analyses that
modeled the effects between parents and children as bidi-
rectional (i.e., simultaneous inclusion of both Path g and
Path h in Figure 1). Simultaneous inclusion of these paths
was essential to capture the bidirectional nature of parent
child effects, as advised by the bilateral model of parent
child relations (Kuczynski, 2003). To evaluate the impor-
tance of conforming to the bilateral framework in the
context of the current investigation, we next report ndings
from an analysis that only accounts for unidirectional ef-
fects between parents and children and, thereby, ignores the
bidirectional nature of interpersonal effects within the dy-
adic interactions.
Because the bidirectional model yielded the most inter-
esting ndings with respect to interpersonal hostility for the
motherchild dyads, and because we consider the unidirec-
tional model to be inferior, we limit our reporting of results
from the unidirectional model to those generated by a re-
analysis of the hostility-relevant data from motherchild
dyads. Specically, we repeated the analysis depicted in
Figure 1 two times. The rst re-analysis modeled interaction
effects as being unidirectional from children to mothers and
provided a signicantly worse t to the data compared with
the bidirectional model,
2
(1, N 658) 6.04, p .01.
Otherwise, this unidirectional model tended to yield a pat-
tern of ndings consistent with the results yielded by the
bidirectional model. The one exception was that the unidi-
rectional model indicated that childrens perceptions of their
mothers typical hostility directly predicted their mothers
actual hostility during the interaction (Path e), whereas the
bidirectional model indicated that they did not.
The second re-analysis modeled the interaction effects as
being unidirectional from mothers to children and also
provided a comparatively poor t to the data,
2
(1, N
658) 17.49, p .01. It also yielded a number of results
that diverged in critical respects from those produced by the
bidirectional model. First, the unidirectional model indi-
cated that childrens perceptions of their mothers typical
hostility accurately predicted mothersactual hostility (Path
e), whereas the bidirectional model indicated that they did
not. Second, the unidirectional model indicated that moth-
ers perceptions of their childrens typical hostility did not
accurately predict childrens actual hostility (Path b),
whereas the bidirectional model indicated that they did.
Third, the unidirectional model indicated that childrens
perceptions of their mothers typical hostility did not predict
childrens actual hostility (Path c), whereas the bidirectional
model indicated that they did. Fourth, the unidirectional
model indicated that mothers perceptions of their chil-
drens typical hostility predicted mothers actual hostility
(Path d), whereas the bidirectional model indicated that they
did not. Fifth, the unidirectional model indicated that moth-
ers hostility during the interaction predicted childrens
hostility during the interaction (Path h), whereas the bidi-
rectional model indicated that they did not. And, most
interestingly, the unidirectional model supported the exis-
tence of a self-fullling prophecy originating in mothers
that is, mothers perceptions of their childrens typical hos-
tility was mediated by its proximal effect on mothers actual
hostility and ultimately predicted their childrens actual
hostility during the interaction (via compound Path dh), a
pattern not supported by the bidirectional model. The dif-
ferent results that emerged when the reciprocal nature of the
interaction effects was eliminated from the model empha-
size the importance of accounting for the bidirectional na-
ture of interpersonal effects in the context of parentchild
interactions.
Reversal of Direction of Effects
As noted in the Method section, videotaping of the dyadic
interactions commenced as soon as the project staff had
completed the necessary preparations. As a result, some
family members participated in the interaction before com-
pleting the perception items. This raises the concern that
rather than perceptions inuencing subsequent interaction
behaviors, the interaction behaviors might have inuenced
subsequent questionnaire responses that were used to assess
family members perceptions. Because data regarding the
order of these tasks were not recorded, we addressed this
concern by re-analyzing the data using a model in which the
direction of the effects represented by Paths a through f was
reversed. Thus, this re-analysis assessed how well the data
are t by a model in which the interaction behaviors are
modeled as causes of the questionnaire responses. In com-
parison with the original model (i.e., with the direction of
effects as shown in Figure 1), which provided a good t for
all four analyses, all
2
s 0.04, all ps .16, the new model
provided a poor t to the data,
2
s ranged from 6.96 to 32.7,
ps .01 (for both models, df 1, Ns 658 motherchild
dyads, 576 fatherchild dyads), suggesting that childrens
and parents perceptions predicted and inuenced interac-
tion behaviors rather than the reverse.
Additional Analyses
We also evaluated whether the results might have been
affected by child gender and whether the dyadic interaction
was the rst or third videotaped task in which the parent and
child participated. We conducted a series of two-group
stacked LISREL analyses to test for differences in path
coefcients related to child gender. Across the four analy-
ses, the only gender difference to emerge was for the
fatherchild hostility data, in which the association between
the childrens perception of their own typical hostility (Path
a) was more predictive of childrens actual hostility for girls
than for boys. A second series of four stacked two-group
LISREL analyses tested whether the order of the interaction
affected the results. Analyses revealed only a single differ-
ence. Mothers perceptions of their own typical hostility
were more strongly linked to their actual hostility (Path f)
when the interaction was the third task in which they en-
465SELF-FULFILLING PROPHECIES
gaged. However, the child-based self-fullling prophecy
effects were not affected by task order: self-fullling proph-
ecy effects were nearly identical in magnitude when the
motherchild hostility data were analyzed separately by
task order.
Discussion
This research examined whether parents and childrens
perceptions of the others hostility and warmth led to self-
fullling prophecy effects during dyadic interactions. Re-
sults supported the existence of self-fullling prophecies
among the motherchild dyads with respect to the hostility-
relevant data. Childrens perceptions of their mothers typ-
ical hostility predicted childrens hostility during the inter-
action, which, in turn, predicted mothers hostility during
the interaction. Results did not, however, support reciprocal
self-fullling prophecy effects originating in mothers.
Mothers perceptions of their childrens hostility did not
predict childrens hostility during the interaction by virtue
of their relation to mothers interaction hostility. Results
also did not support the existence of self-fullling prophe-
cies among the motherchild dyads with respect to warmth-
relevant data, nor among the fatherchild dyads for either
the hostility- or warmth-relevant data. These ndings sug-
gest that the self-fullling prophecy effects observed in this
study were child-based rather than parent-based, were re-
stricted to mother-child interactions, and occurred for per-
ceptions of negative affective behaviors but not positive
affective behaviors.
MotherChild Differences in Self-Fullling Prophecy
Effects
Analyses of the hostility-relevant data pertaining to the
motherchild dyads indicated that self-fullling prophecy
effects operated through children but not through mothers.
There are at least two explanations for these differences, one
relating to predictive accuracy and a second relating to the
potential for different effects of agency in parents and
children. Focusing rst on predictive accuracy, the more
accurate ones belief for another is, the less opportunity
there is for the belief to create a self-fullling prophecy
(Jussim, 1991). In this study, the relation between mothers
perceptions of their childrens typical hostility and chil-
drens actual hostility (Path b) was larger than the corre-
sponding relation between childrens perceptions of their
motherstypical hostility and mothersactual hostility (Path
e). Consistent with this observation, a supplemental analysis
in which the effects pertaining to mothers and childrens
accuracy for the others hostility were constrained to be
equal (i.e., Path b Path e) produced a signicant reduction
in model t,
2
(1, N 658) 6.73, p .01, thereby
indicating signicantly greater accuracy on the part of
mothers. Accordingly, one reason that childrens percep-
tions might have produced stronger self-fullling prophecy
effects than mothers perceptions is because childrens per-
ceptions were less accurate.
Self-fullling prophecy effects may also have been me-
diated through childrens behavior but not through mothers
behavior because of differences between mothers and chil-
drens agency in pursuing their own goals. Children who
perceived their mothers to be hostile may have preemptively
expressed hostile behaviors within the interaction as a
means of demonstrating autonomy. By contrast, mothers
may have intentionally behaved in ways that they believed
would be most effective for attaining more long-term goals,
such as maintaining positive relationships with their chil-
dren (Kuczynski & Lollis, 2001).
It is also important to note that these explanations are not
mutually exclusive. Although mothers perceptions of chil-
drens typical hostility were more accurate than were chil-
drens perceptions of mothers typical hostility, mothers
perceptions did not perfectly predict their childrens actual
hostility, indicating that they were partially inaccurate. Yet,
the inaccurate portion of their perceptions did not produce a
self-fullling prophecy effect on their childrens behavior.
Thus, the differing patterns of mothers and childrens self-
fullling prophecy effects most likely reect a combination
of predictive accuracy operating in conjunction with differ-
ences in mother and child agency.
Negative Versus Positive Self-Fullling Prophecy
Effects
The self-fullling prophecy effects found in this research
were limited to hostile behaviors. This suggests that when it
comes to expressions of positive and negative affect, neg-
ative self-fullling prophecies may be more powerful than
positive ones. The tendency for perceptions of negative
affective behaviors to create more powerful self-fullling
prophecies than perceptions of positive affective behaviors
has important implications for family interventions seeking
to improve parentchild relationships. In particular, it sug-
gests that interventions may benet from emphasizing the
value of developing positive perceptions of family members
and providing skills training to help family members better
communicate their positive perceptions to each other. Inter-
ventions may also benet by raising childrens awareness of
how preexisting beliefs might affect their behavior and
encourage them to respond to parents actual interaction
behaviors. Such efforts should reduce negative interactions,
increase positive interactions, and promote parentchild
bonding, which facilitates positive adolescent outcomes
(Catalano & Hawkins, 1996).
MotherFather Differences in Self-Fullling
Prophecy Effects
A child-based self-fullling prophecy emerged between
children and mothers but not between children and fathers.
Because the self-fullling prophecy could only occur if an
emotional expression was elicited from the target, the dif-
ference between the parentchild dyads might have oc-
curred if the motherchild dyads were characterized by
greater emotional expressiveness. Consistent with this idea,
paired-samples t tests did show that mothers exhibited more
hostility and warmth than fathers, ts(553) 3.89, ps .01,
466 MADON, GUYLL, AND SPOTH
and that children exhibited more hostility with their moth-
ers, t(553) 3.23, p .01. Previous research has docu-
mented similar differences in dyadic emotional expressive-
ness and suggests that they arise because mothers and
children spend more time with each other and share a
broader range of experiences (e.g., Buhrmester, Camparo,
Chistensen, & Gonzalez, 1992).
Limitations
Interpretation of naturalistic data. When investigating
self-fullling prophecies with naturalistic data, analyses
must control for that portion of perceivers perceptions that
accurately predict, without inuencing, targets behavioral
outcomes. When a valid predictor is omitted from an ana-
lytic model, accuracy and self-fullling prophecy effects are
under- and overestimated, respectively. Although all natu-
ralistic studies are susceptible to the omitted variable prob-
lem, the current data were only interpreted as reecting a
self-fullling prophecy to the extent that perceivers per-
ceptions had an indirect effect on targets behavioral out-
comes that was specically mediated by the perceivers own
behaviors during the interaction. Thus, in the current anal-
ysis, if valid predictors of targets behavioral outcomes had
been omitted from the model, to the degree that they cor-
related with perceiver perceptions, that accuracy-related
portion of the relation between the perceivers perceptions
and target outcomes would have been captured entirely by
the direct effects between these variables (i.e., Paths b and
e; Jussim et al., 1996).
However, it is possible that the analytic model omitted a
variable that both correlated with perceivers perceptions
and had a causal effect on perceivers behavior during the
interaction. In this case, the effect that perceivers percep-
tions of the target had on their own behavior would have
been overestimated, raising the possibility that their self-
fullling inuences could have been smaller than suggested.
Although we cannot rule out this possibility, our self-
fullling prophecy interpretation is consistent with a long
history of experimental ndings demonstrating that perceiv-
ers perceptions inuence targets behaviors by means of
self-fullling prophecies (see Snyder & Stukas, 1999, for a
review). The convergence of our ndings with those of
previous investigations increases condence that our results
reect self-fullling prophecies between mothers and
children.
Timing of videotaping. Parentchild interactions were
videotaped before and after family members completed the
questionnaire items. Consequently, some participants re-
ported their perceptions after they had already engaged in
the dyadic interaction tasks. This procedural aspect of the
study raises the concern that the interaction behaviors might
have inuenced family members perceptions rather than
the reverse. Unfortunately, information regarding the pre-
cise sequence of the completion of the perception items
relative to the interaction task was not recorded. Although
this fact precluded us from specically testing whether the
timing of the videotaping inuenced participantsresponses
to the perception items, there are several reasons why we
believe that the direction of effects was from perceptions to
interaction behaviors. First, nearly all of the items assessing
family members perceptions of their own and the others
typical hostility and warmth inquired about behaviors that
had occurred during the past month. Thus, the wording of
these items instructed parents and children to report their
perceptions on the basis of behaviors that had preceded the
interaction. Second, had the direction of effect been re-
versed (with interaction behaviors inuencing perceptions),
one would have expected to see very large direct effects
from perceptions to behaviors. That is, the self-reported
perceptions would seem to have been extremely accurate
because they would have been based on the actual interac-
tions that had just occurred. In the current data, a self-
fullling prophecy was indicated for hostility in mother
child interactions and originated in the childs perception of
the mother. However, the direct effect of childrens percep-
tions on mothers behavior was nearly zero for this analysis,
thereby providing no support for the idea that mother inter-
action hostility inuenced child perceptions. In addition,
even if interaction behaviors had inuenced self-reported
perceptions, these effects would have been attributed to
accuracy (i.e., direct effects) and not to a self-fullling
prophecy (i.e., specic indirect effects). Third, as reported
in the Results section, we re-analyzed the data modeling the
direction of causal inuence as being from interaction be-
haviors to perceptions. In comparison with the original
model, the new model was found to provide a poor tto
both the hostility- and warmth-relevant data for both
motherchild and fatherchild dyads. Thus, the wording of
the perception items, the lack of accuracy in child reports of
mothers hostility, and the comparison of empirical results
generated by the two alternative models all argue against the
idea that interaction behaviors inuenced family members
perceptions. On the basis of this evidence, it seems most
plausible that the direction of effect was from self-reported
perceptions to behaviors.
Perception items. The perception items used in this
research reected family membersperceptions of their own
and the others typical affective behaviors. Family mem-
bers actual affective behaviors, in contrast, were assessed
during a specic and circumscribed situation. Thus, the
wording of the perception items was not perfectly matched
to the affective behaviors that were assessed. It is possible,
therefore, that family membersperceptions may have more
accurately predicted parentchild affective behaviors in the
context of mundane parentchild interactions than they
predicted parentchild affective behaviors in the contrived
context of the dyadic interaction tasks. Although this pos-
sibility does not undermine our conclusion that childrens
perceptions of their motherstypical hostility predicted their
mothers actual hostility during the interaction task via the
process of a self-fullling prophecy, it does highlight the
importance of exercising caution when generalizing our
ndings to other less contrived situations in which family
members perceptions of one anothers behaviors may be
more accurate than they were in this research.
A second issue related to the perception items and the
generalizability of our ndings is whether similar patterns
467SELF-FULFILLING PROPHECIES
would have emerged had the perception items directly as-
sessed family members perceptions about affective behav-
iors pertaining specically to the context of the interaction
task. Although peoples perceptions tend to be more accu-
rate for circumscribed behaviors than for global behaviors
(Swann, 1984), the dyadic interaction tasks in which parents
and children engaged were unfamiliar to most family mem-
bers. Therefore, any perceptions that parents and children
did have regarding the affective behaviors each would ex-
hibit in this context would probably have been less accurate
than are most perceptions regarding circumscribed behav-
iors. Thus, had we assessed perceptions that were more
closely tied to the behaviors occurring during the interaction
task, we may have found stronger self-fullling prophecy
effects than reported. Of course, our discussion of this issue
is highly speculative and requires empirical validation be-
fore rm conclusions can be drawn.
Conclusion
This research tested for reciprocal self-fullling prophecy
effects among parents and their adolescent children. Al-
though such effects were not found among fatherchild
dyads, data were consistent with a self-fullling prophecy
effect among the motherchild dyads with respect to
hostility-relevant data. Childrens perceptions of their moth-
ers typical hostility predicted how hostile they behaved
toward their mothers during an interaction, which, in turn,
predicted how hostile their mothers behaved toward them.
Although mothers perceptions did predict childrens hos-
tility, this relationship was due to mothersaccuracy and not
to a self-fullling prophecy. Thus, the self-fullling proph-
ecy effects observed in this study were unidirectional from
child to mother and not vice versa. However, additional
analyses based on an inferior model that considered only
unidirectional effects suggested that mothers had a self-
fullling prophecy effect on their childrens behavior.
These differing conclusions highlight the importance of
using a bilateral framework to investigate interpersonal
relations.
The tendency for children to exert stronger self-fullling
prophecy effects than mothers contrasts with past work
showing that the person with greater power in a relationship
tends to have greater self-fullling inuences (Snyder &
Stukas, 1999). The current nding likely reects the unique
character of the parentchild relationship and suggests that
the present pattern of results may not be typical of relation-
ships in general. Indeed, patterns of self-fullling inuence
between parents and their children have previously been
shown to differ from patterns typically observed in the
context of nonfamilial relationships (Madon, Guyll, Spoth,
Cross, & Hilbert, 2003). Further investigation of how these
patterns of interpersonal inuence vary across time, in re-
sponse to interventions designed to improve parentchild
relationships, and how they vary with the characteristics of
the individual family members are necessary to enhance
understanding of relationship dynamics within the family.
References
Bell, R. Q. (1971). Stimulus control of parent or caretaker behavior
by offspring. Developmental Psychology, 4, 6372.
Bollen, K. A. (1987). Total, direct, and indirect effects in structural
equation models. In Clifford C. Clogg (Ed.), Sociological meth-
odology (Vol. 17, pp. 3769). Washington, DC: American So-
ciological Association.
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Statistical equations with latent variables.
New York: Wiley.
Buhrmester, D., Camparo, L., Christensen, A., & Gonzalez, L. S.
(1992). Mothers and fathers interacting in dyads and triads with
normal and hyperactive sons. Developmental Psychology, 28,
500509.
Catalano, R. F., & Hawkins, J. D. (1996). The social development
model: A theory of antisocial behavior. In J. D. Hawkins (Ed.),
Delinquency and crime: Current theories (pp. 149197). New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Hinde, R. A. (1979). Toward understanding relationships. Lon-
don: Academic Press.
Hinde, R. A., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1987). Interpersonal rela-
tionships and child development. Developmental Review, 7,
121.
Jo¨reskog, K. G., & So¨rbom, D. (1999). LISREL 8.30: Users
reference guide. Chicago: Scientic Software.
Jussim, L. (1991). Social perception and social reality: A
reectionconstruction model. Psychological Review, 98, 54
73.
Jussim, L., Eccles, J., & Madon, S. (1996). Social perception,
social stereotypes, and teacher expectations: Accuracy and the
quest for the powerful self-fullling prophecy. In M. P. Zanna
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 28, pp.
281388). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Kuczynski, L. (2003). Beyond bidirectionality: Bilateral concep-
tual frameworks for understanding dynamics in parentchild
relations. In L. Kuczynski (Ed.), Handbook of dynamics in
parentchild relations (pp. 324). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kuczynski, L., & Lollis, S. (2001). Four foundations for a dynamic
model of parenting. In J. R. M. Gerris (Ed.), Dynamics of
parenting (pp. 445462). Appledoorn, the Netherlands: Garant.
Lollis, S. (2003). Conceptualizing the inuence of the past and the
future in present parentchild relationships. In L. Kuczynski
(Ed.), Handbook of dynamics in parentchild relations, (pp.
6787). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Madon, S., Guyll, M., Spoth, R. L., Cross, S. E., & Hilbert, S.
(2003). The self-fullling inuence of mother expectations on
childrens underage drinking. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84, 11881205.
Melby, J. N., & Conger, R. D. (2001). The Iowa Family Interaction
Rating Scales: Instrument summary. In P. Kerig & K. Lindahl
(Eds.), Family observational coding systems: Resources for sys-
tematic research (pp. 3358). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Melby, J. N., Conger, K. J., & Puspitawati, H. (1999). Insider,
participant observer, and outsider perspectives on adolescent
sibling relationships. In F. M. Berardo (Series Ed.) & C. L.
Shehan (Vol. Ed.), Contemporary perspectives on family re-
search: Vol. 1. Through the eyes of the child: Re-visioning
children as active agents in family life (pp. 329351). Stamford,
CT: JAI Press.
Merton, R. K. (1948). The self-fullling prophecy. Antioch Re-
view, 8, 193210.
Molgaard, V., Kumpfer, K., & Fleming, B. (1997). The Strength-
ening Families Program: For parents and youth 1014. Ames,
IA: Iowa State University Extension.
468 MADON, GUYLL, AND SPOTH
Snyder, M., & Stukas, A. A., Jr. (1999). Interpersonal processes:
The interplay of cognitive, motivational, and behavioral activi-
ties in social interaction. Annual Review of Psychology, 50,
273303.
Sobel, M. (1986). Some new results on indirect effects and their
standard errors in covariance structure models. In N. B. Tuma
(Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 159186). Washington,
DC: American Sociological Association.
Spoth, R. L., Redmond, C., Trudeau, L., & Shin, C. (2002).
Longitudinal substance initiation outcomes for a universal pre-
ventive intervention combining family and school programs.
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16, 129134.
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1984). Quest for accuracy in person perception:
A matter of pragmatics. Psychological Review, 91, 457477.
Received June 6, 2003
Revision received November 17, 2003
Accepted January 6, 2004
New Editors Appointed, 2006–2011
The Publications and Communications Board of the American Psychological Association an-
nounces the appointment of seven new editors for 6-year terms beginning in 2006. As of January
1, 2005, manuscripts should be directed as follows:
Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology (www.apa.org/journals/pha.html), Nancy K.
Mello, PhD, McLean Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 115
Mill Street, Belmont, MA 02478-9106.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology (www.apa.org/journals/abn.html), David Watson, PhD, De-
partment of Psychology, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242-1407.
Journal of Comparative Psychology (www.apa.org/journals/com.html), Gordon M. Burghardt,
PhD, Department of Psychology or Department of Ecology & Evolutionary Biology, University
of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37996.
Journal of Counseling Psychology (www.apa.org/journals/cou.html), Brent S. Mallinckrodt,
PhD, Department of Educational, School, and Counseling Psychology, 16 Hill Hall, University
of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (www.apa.org/
journals/xhp.html), Glyn W. Humphreys, PhD, Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre, School of
Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, United Kingdom.
Joural of Personality and Social Psychology: Attitudes and Social Cognition section
(www.apa.org/journals/psp.html), Charles M. Judd, PhD, Department of Psychology, Univer-
sity of Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309-0345.
Rehabilitation Psychology (www.apa.org/journals/rep.html), Timothy R. Elliott, PhD, Depart-
ment of Psychology, 415 Campbell Hall, 1300 University Boulevard, University of Alabama,
Birmingham, AL 35294-1170.
Electronic submission: As of January 1, 2005, authors are expected to submit manuscripts
electronically through the journals Manuscript Submission Portal (see the Web site listed above
with each journal title).
Manuscript submission patterns make the precise date of completion of the 2005 volumes uncertain.
Current editors, Warren K. Bickel, PhD, Timothy B. Baker, PhD, Meredith J. West, PhD, Jo-Ida C.
Hansen, PhD, David A. Rosenbaum, PhD, Patricia G. Devine, PhD, and Bruce Caplan, PhD,
respectively, will receive and consider manuscripts through December 31, 2004. Should 2005
volumes be completed before that date, manuscripts will be redirected to the new editors for
consideration in 2006 volumes.
469SELF-FULFILLING PROPHECIES
... Research confirms that children can have a self-fulfilling prophecy effect on their parents. For example, the diagnosis of a hearing loss in a child may cause some parents to lower their expectations for what can be accomplished by their child (Madon, Guyll, & Spoth, 2004). But the self-fulfilling prophecy effect is not necessarily reciprocal, perhaps because parents have more power in the relationship. ...
... This means that parents' hearing loss is less likely to affect children's expectations of their parents. Moreover, negative self-fulfilling prophecies may be more powerful than positive ones (Madon et al., 2004). Self-fulfilling prophecies involve complex interpersonal and intrapersonal issues that warrant further study among people with hearing loss along a continuum of educational, communicative, and familial variables. ...
Article
Full-text available
... Research confirms that children can have a self-fulfilling prophecy effect on their parents. For example, the diagnosis of a hearing loss in a child may cause some parents to lower their expectations for what can be accomplished by their child (Madon, Guyll, & Spoth, 2004). But the self-fulfilling prophecy effect is not necessarily reciprocal, perhaps because parents have more power in the relationship. ...
... This means that parents' hearing loss is less likely to affect children's expectations of their parents. Moreover, negative self-fulfilling prophecies may be more powerful than positive ones (Madon et al., 2004). Self-fulfilling prophecies involve complex interpersonal and intrapersonal issues that warrant further study among people with hearing loss along a continuum of educational, communicative, and familial variables. ...
Article
This article presents a systematic and comprehensive framework of proactive psychosocial attributes and tactics that individuals who are deaf use to maximize their professional and social potential. Empirical studies regarding deafness appear to lack such a framework which may be due to how the research is conducted. A framework of proactive psychosocial attributes and tactics was found in Reff Ginsberg, and Gerber's (1995) study of learning disabilities. Reff et al. 's framework was thereafter used to frame a review of empirical studies related to deafness. It was found that individuals who are deaf likely maximize their potential using 2 sets of proactive psychosocial attributes and tactics: (1) skills that individuals with typical hearing use and (2) specialized skills for circumventing deafness-related dfflculties. It isfurt her argued that adult participants who are deaf will provide greater insight into how psychosocial potential is maximized. This literature review will be complemented by empiricalfindings in a later article.
... Research confirms that children can have a self-fulfilling prophecy effect on their parents. For example, the diagnosis of a hearing loss in a child may cause some parents to lower their expectations for what can be accomplished by their child (Madon, Guyll, & Spoth, 2004). But the self-fulfilling prophecy effect is not necessarily reciprocal, perhaps because parents have more power in the relationship. ...
... This means that parents' hearing loss is less likely to affect children's expectations of their parents. Moreover, negative self-fulfilling prophecies may be more powerful than positive ones (Madon et al., 2004). Self-fulfilling prophecies involve complex interpersonal and intrapersonal issues that warrant further study among people with hearing loss along a continuum of educational, communicative, and familial variables. ...
Article
Full-text available
While the concurrent use of the hearing-aid (HA) microphone with frequency modulation (FM) technology can decrease speech-recognition performance, the FM+HA condition still is an important setting for users of both HA and FM technology. The primary goal of this investigation was to evaluate the effect of attenuating HA gain in the FM+HA listening condition on the signal-to-noise ratio for 50% sentence recognition in two situations: (1) when speech was presented to the FM transmitter microphone and (2) when speech was presented to the HA microphone. A second goal was to determine whether or not measures based on the Articulation Index (AI) could be used to predict speech-recognition performance. The best speech-recognition performance for the first situation occurred when the HA gain was attenuated by 20 dB, while the best performance for the second situation occurred when there was 0 dB of attenuation. The AI measures were directly related to speech-recognition performance.
... Furthermore, parents' and teachers' perceptions and expectations affect their interactions with other children, which can in turn affect children's behavior and academic success (Chi & Hinshaw, 2002). Several studies reveal teachers' and parents' expectations may create a self-fulfilling prophecy and even affect the level of post-secondary education a child would attain (Madon, Guyll, & Spoth, 2004). ...
Article
Full-text available
The diagnosis of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is mainly based on structured scales, such as Conners' ADHD Rating Scale (EDAH in the Spanish version) and DSM interviews. The use of technologies in neuropsychological assessment, such as the AULA virtual reality based test leads to more accurate diagnosis. The current study presents findings from analyzing the external validity of AULA and its contribution to the diagnosis of ADHD. Four hundred and seven children (272 girls and 135 boys) from 6 to 16 years old (213 with ADHD diagnosis, 105 inattentive children, 108 combined-type, and 194 controls) were evaluated. First, a factor analysis of AULA variables was conducted in order to reduce data to factor and 5 factors or components that account for 82.37% of the total variance were obtained from 407 subjects, namely, sustained attention, impulsivity control, processing speed, response variability, and control of motor activity. Second, a discriminant analysis was then performed on data obtained by participants from whom the five factors were obtained, showing that AULA presents moderate levels of both specificity and sensitivity. Finally, in order to study whether AULA adds relevant information in the diagnosis of ADHD, a cluster analysis was carried out, showing 4 clusters in the analysis of conglomerates with the control group and 6 groups of clusters in the ADHD group. In summary, AULA test shows adequate external validity, allows correct classification of children with and without attentional problems, and confirms and provides additional ADHD diagnostic information that it is essential for the design of interventions.
... Rubie-Davies 2006;Tiedemann 2000). Other diverse instances include children's self-fulfilling effect on their mothers' hostility (Madon et al. 2004), postoperative pain experience (Logan and Rose 2005), or associations of parental expectations with children's subsequent alcohol (Madon et al. 2006) and marijuana use (Lamb and Crano 2014). Thus, in the present study, children more capable of self-regulation might exhibit superior olfactory performance simply because they were expected to do so by their parents, as reflected by the temperamental assessments. ...
Article
Full-text available
A body of research predominantly in young adults has suggested a link between olfactory perception, especially sensitivity, and personality characteristics, particularly Neuroticism. Nevertheless, it is not clear whether these associations are present outside this particular age range and whether they involve other olfactory abilities, such as odor identification and discrimination. Also, implicit in such investigations is the assumption of the generalizability of such links, in which case they should already be found in young children and involve general, constitutionally based differences in reactivity and self-regulation, that is, temperament. On the one hand, extrapolating from studies with adults to children, those scoring high on Negative Affectivity, which broadly maps onto Neuroticism, should outperform the low-scoring ones. On the other hand, well-developed self-regulatory processes referred to as Effortful Control, which modulate the expression of such tendencies and manifest themselves in the ability to voluntarily sustain focus on a task, shift attention from one task to another, initiate action and inhibit it, might also contribute to better olfactory performance. Aim and Hypotheses The aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of the temperamental factors on olfactory performance. Namely, we hypothesized that superior olfactory performance would be delivered by children relatively higher on Negative Affectivity as well as those exhibiting greater self-regulation. Methods Odor identification and discrimination in 143 children (72 boys) aged six to eight years were assessed with the Sniffin' Sticks, controlling for their verbal fluency. Parents provided reports of their children's temperament by means of the short form of the Children's Behavior Questionnaire. The potential influence of parental responsiveness and demands on temperamental attributions were controlled for with hypothetical vignettes representing parenting styles. Results There was an effect of Effortful Control (but not Negative Affectivity) on the total identification (but not discrimination) scores. Namely, children who were perceived as more capable of self-regulation exhibited higher odor identification scores. Girls did not outperform boys on either of the olfactory tests but were perceived by their parents as more capable of self-regulation. Conclusions Our findings indirectly point to the effect of self-regulatory processes on odor identification in young children. However, they did not corroborate the idea that individuals varying in neuroticism differ in terms of olfactory performance. Given the narrow age range of children recruited in the present study, further studies with preadolescent and adolescent participants are needed to gain more insight into the nature of these relationships. The final publication is available at: Springer via http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12078-016-9216-0 http://www.readcube.com/articles/10.1007/s12078-016-9216-0?author_access_token=0oNI2wolH52qj40R_ABKD_e4RwlQNchNByi7wbcMAY6tQ_funxEXoFRkNshB0iE6eX44yxRKoDqazapzrfslSH5cd_g1BONHqGIwV3I1zNHB4xIYCOiGcB4X7k2_T_cLtr8yG6JnGHKTLqa95UPJfw%3D%3D
... Research confirms adolescents can have a self-fulfilling prophecy effect on their parents, but not necessarily the other way around. In short, the self-fulfilling prophecy is not necessarily reciprocal, perhaps because parents have more power in the parent-child relationship (Madon, Guyll, & Spoth, 2004). Positive family support is essential for healthy socioemotional growth of children with hearing loss (Kent & Smith, 2006). ...
... Research confirms adolescents can have a self-fulfilling prophecy effect on their parents, but not necessarily the other way around. In short, the self-fulfilling prophecy is not necessarily reciprocal, perhaps because parents have more power in the parent-child relationship (Madon, Guyll, & Spoth, 2004). Positive family support is essential for healthy socioemotional growth of children with hearing loss (Kent & Smith, 2006). ...
... Research confirms adolescents can have a self-fulfilling prophecy effect on their parents, but not necessarily the other way around. In short, the self-fulfilling prophecy is not necessarily reciprocal, perhaps because parents have more power in the parent-child relationship (Madon, Guyll, & Spoth, 2004). Positive family support is essential for healthy socioemotional growth of children with hearing loss (Kent & Smith, 2006). ...
Book
This text is structured to provide the reader with the basics of auditory-verbal practices from a historical perspective, including the knowledge to understand how it evolved to current evidence-based practices. Families who learn that one of its members has a hearing loss will experience varied reactions. To best serve these families, practitioners must provide family assessment, support, and information. The book begins by examining the theoretical and practical bases of family therapy models, and the development of a systemic viewpoint that is crucial to practitioners who must evolve to serve more than just the parent-child dyad. Essential family therapeutic strategies that are needed to effectively work with families are presented, and from an objective perspective, current auditory-verbal practices and various ethical issues are examined. Varied family-based intervention models are discussed, with the family-centered approach considered the ideal to which practitioners aspire. The book explains how the merging of auditory-verbal and systemic family therapy strategies can effectively culminate in the implementation of family-based approaches to intervention. Evidence-based strategies embraced by family therapists and family-centered intervention service providers that can be implemented by auditory-verbal practitioners are shared by a cross-cultural collaboration of contributors to this book. The strategies and discussions contained in this comprehensive resource will be of special interest to speech-language pathologists, educational audiologists, and teachers for children with hearing loss, as well as early intervention service providers and social workers -- Back cover.
Article
Purpose: The purpose of this study was to measure congruence in perceptions of family dynamics in eating-disordered adolescents and their parents. Design and methods: A pilot sample of 35 matched parent-child dyads completed either the Parent Version or the newly modified Client Version of the Family Experience with Eating Disorders Scale (FEEDS). Findings: Differences between parent and child perceptions on subscale totals were not demonstrated, but statistically significant differences on select items were shown. Practice implications: The FEEDS offers a systematic way to explore differences and commonalities in perception among family members and to engage the family in treatment.
Article
Full-text available
Teacher attitudes in the classroom can affect a child's level of accomplishment. (CK)
Article
Full-text available
This article presents a reflection–construction model of relations between social perception and social reality. The introduction suggests that one theme underlying much social psychological theorizing and research is the belief that social perception is a major force in the creation (construction) of social reality. Strong constructivist perspectives largely ignore or discount accuracy in social perception. To redress this limitation, a new theoretical model is presented, the reflection–construction model, which explicitly specifies several ways in which social perception may relate to social reality. This model incorporates phenomena such as the ability of social perception to accurately predict without influencing social reality; to create social reality through self-fulfilling prophecies, self-sustaining prophecies, and self-defeating prophecies; and to lead to biased judgments regarding social reality. When interpreted through the reflection–construction model, empirical research on relations between social perception and social reality often provides more evidence of accuracy than of self-fulfilling prophecy or biases. The evidence, therefore, supports a weaker version of the social constructivist view. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Chapter
This chapter provides an overview of research on accuracy, error, bias, and self-fulfilling prophecies. It also reviews a research showing that teacher expectations predict student achievement—mainly because they are accurate, although they do lead to small self-fulfilling prophecies and biases. The conditions under which self-fulfilling prophecies might be considerably more powerful are embarked. The results of new research showing that teacher expectancy effects are more powerful among girls, students from lower socioeconomic status (SES) backgrounds or African–Americans are also addressed. Some evidence of bias show differences in teacher's perceptions of students from the differing groups corresponded well to actual differences among those same groups of students. The chapter also analyzes ways to distinguish among self fulfilling prophecies, perceptual biases, and accuracy, and examines processes underlying expectancy-related phenomena—discoveries have some relevance and applicability to many other relationships beyond teachers and students. Conceptual model of relationships between teacher perceptions and student achievement and some evidence regarding the role of stereotypes in naturally occurring person perception is also explained in the chapter.