ArticlePDF Available

The Natural History of Alzheimer Disease

Authors:
  • Independent Statistics Teaching Expert

Abstract

Knowledge of the evolution of cognitive deficits in Alzheimer disease is important for our understanding of disease progression. Previous reports, however, have either lacked detail or have not covered the presymptomatic stages. To delineate the onset and progression of clinical and neuropsychological abnormalities in familial Alzheimer disease. Nineteen subjects with familial Alzheimer disease underwent serial clinical and neuropsychological assessments. Eight of these had undergone presymptomatic assessments. The follow-up period was 1 to 10 years (mean, 5 years). The relative timing of the occurrence of 3 markers of disease onset and progression (onset of symptoms, Mini-Mental State Examination score < or = 24, and impaired scores on a range of neuropsychological tests) were compared using the binomial exact test. Neurological abnormalities were not prominent, although myoclonus appeared early in some. Mini-Mental State Examination score was not sensitive to early disease. Memory and general intelligence deficits appeared at an earlier stage, in some patients when presymptomatic. Perceptual, naming, and especially spelling skills were preserved to a late stage. Familial Alzheimer disease may have a long prodromal phase of several years with subtle deficits initially of general intelligence and memory, while spelling, naming, and perception are relatively preserved until a late stage.
ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION
The Natural History of Alzheimer Disease
A Longitudinal Presymptomatic and Symptomatic Study of a Familial Cohort
Alison K. Godbolt, MRCP; Lisa Cipolotti, PhD; Hilary Watt, MSc; Nick C. Fox, MD, FRCP;
John C. Janssen, MRCP; Martin N. Rossor, MD, FRCP
Background: Knowledge of the evolution of cognitive
deficits in Alzheimer disease is important for our under-
standing of disease progression. Previous reports, how-
ever, have either lacked detail or have not covered the
presymptomatic stages.
Objective: To delineate the onset and progression of
clinical and neuropsychological abnormalities in famil-
ial Alzheimer disease.
Methods: Nineteen subjects with familial Alzheimer
disease underwent serial clinical and neuropsychologi-
cal assessments. Eight of these had undergone presymp-
tomatic assessments. The follow-up period was 1 to 10
years (mean, 5 years). The relative timing of the occur-
rence of 3 markers of disease onset and progression
(onset of symptoms, Mini-Mental State Examination
score 24, and impaired scores on a range of neuropsy-
chological tests) were compared using the binomial
exact test.
Results: Neurological abnormalities were not promi-
nent, although myoclonus appeared early in some. Mini-
Mental State Examination score was not sensitive to early
disease. Memory and general intelligence deficits ap-
peared at an earlier stage, in some patients when pre-
symptomatic. Perceptual, naming, and especially spell-
ing skills were preserved to a late stage.
Conclusion: Familial Alzheimer disease may have a long
prodromal phase of several years with subtle deficits ini-
tially of general intelligence and memory, while spell-
ing, naming, and perception are relatively preserved un-
til a late stage.
Arch Neurol. 2004;61:1743-1748
A
DETAILED UNDERSTAND-
ing of the clinical and neu-
ropsychological features of
Alzheimer disease (AD) is
essential for improving
the accuracy of diagnosis and prognosis
and the assessment of potential disease-
modifying treatments of the future. The
typical early, insidious decline in
episodic memory is well established,
1
but
surprisingly little has been reported of the
subsequent pattern of progression of neu-
ropsychological and clinical features. Most
studies have been cross-sectional, and
those longitudinal studies that have been
performed have focused on rates of pro-
gression rather than on how the pattern
of decline in 1 specific cognitive domain
relates to that in another.
2
One reason for the limited study of this
area is the difficulty in confident diagno-
sis of AD, especially at the early stages in
which deficits in specific cognitive do-
mains are of most interest. The occur-
rence of AD in an early-onset, autosomal
dominantly inherited familial form pro-
vides an opportunity to study patients in
whom early diagnosis can be made con-
fidently and in whom, unlike in elderly
populations, comorbidity is seldom a sig-
nificant consideration.
Previous studies of familial AD (FAD)
have either focused on reporting the ear-
liest features of the disease
3-6
or on pro-
gression in those already moderately af-
fected.
5
The course and pattern of cognitive
decline from the presymptomatic stages to
advanced disease have not been system-
atically reported in detail.
METHODS
Patients with autosomal dominant, early-
onset FAD were recruited into a longitudinal
study from referrals to the Dementia Re-
search Group at The National Hospital for Neu-
rology and Neurosurgery, London, England.
The study received ethical approval from the
institutional review board and informed con-
sent was obtained. All families of patients in-
cluded fulfilled 3 generational criteria for au-
Author Affiliations: Dementia
Research Group, Institute of
Neurology, (Drs Godbolt, Fox,
Janssen, and Rossor and Ms
Watt), and the Department of
Neuropsychology
(Dr Cipolotti), National
Hospital for Neurology and
Neurosurgery (Drs Godbolt,
Fox, Janssen, and Rossor),
London, England.
(REPRINTED) ARCH NEUROL / VOL 61, NOV 2004 WWW.ARCHNEUROL.COM
1743
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at Imperial College London, on February 26, 2007 www.archneurol.comDownloaded from
tosomal dominant inheritance, and at least1 affected member
of each family had undergone neuropathological examina-
tion. All patients, on completion of this study, fulfilled stan-
dard criteria for probable AD (except for the requirement of
the criteria of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders
and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders
Associations that symptoms develop at older than 40 years).
For patients who had participated in a longitudinal study of
subjects at risk of FAD prior to developing the disease, pre-
symptomatic data were included.
Each patient had serial clinical and neuropsychological as-
sessments at the National Hospital for Neurology and Neuro-
surgery at approximately annual intervals, until untestable. As-
sessors were blinded to the outcome of assessments other than
their own.
Clinical assessment included history taking, neurological ex-
amination, and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE).
7
Onset
of symptoms was established from information given by the pa-
tient and their caregiver when the patient was first symptomatic.
Neuropsychological tests assessed intellectual function-
ing, current (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised)
8
and
premorbid (National Adult Reading Test)
9
; verbal and visual
recognition memory (Recognition Memory Test for Words and
Faces)
10
; naming (Graded Naming Test)
11
; spelling (Oral Graded
Difficulty Spelling Test)
12
; calculation (Graded Difficulty Arith-
metic Test)
13
; and visuospatial and perceptual abilities (cube
analysis and silhouettes from the Visual Object and Space Per-
ception Battery).
14
For all cognitive domains except intelligence, raw scores were
converted into percentiles, referring to published normative data.
Scores at or below the fifth percentile were taken to indicate
an impairment. For intelligence, a difference of 10 or greater
between the National Adult Reading Test and the perfor-
mance or verbal IQ was considered an impairment.
The relative timings of the onset of symptoms, impaired
scores on the MMSE (24), and impairments in the different
cognitive domains were analyzed in a pairwise fashion, using
the binomial exact test. In some patients, performance was im-
paired but then reverted to normal before declining again. We
determined the time of onset to be when a deficit was first ob-
served but also repeated the analysis taking the time of onset
to be when a deficit first became permanent.
Modified Kaplan-Meier plots were used to illustrate the rela-
tive survival of cognitive domains and unimpaired MMSE scores
across time following the onset of symptoms. The proportion
plotted at time zero represents those patients who were as-
sessed before the onset of symptoms and were unimpaired. Drops
in the proportion represent those patients who first showed a
deficit at the given assessment time. The number of patients
who had had an initial assessment but did not show a deficit
prior to this time was used as the denominator.
RESULTS
Nineteen patients participated (9 male). Seven patients
belonged to amyloid precursor protein (APP) gene mu-
tation pedigrees, with V717G (3 patients), V717I (3 pa-
tients), and V717L (1 patient) mutations. Eleven pa-
tients belonged to presenilin-1 (PSEN1) gene mutation
pedigrees, 8 with point mutations (G378V, M139V,
I143F,orL153V) and 3 with deletions ( 4 or 9). One
patient belonged to a pedigree in which we have not
been able to demonstrate a mutation in APP, PSEN1,or
presenilin-2.
Mean age at onset of symptoms was 44 years (range,
35-57 years) and at first assessment when symptomatic
was 46 years (range, 35-59 years). At the time of last con-
tact with the research team, 6 patients had died (mean,
8.2 years after onset of symptoms [range, 5.6-12.7 years]),
10 patients were living at home (mean, 7.3 years after
onset [range, 2.7-12.4 years]), and 3 patients were in resi-
dential care (mean, 8.9 years after onset [range, 7.0-
11.2 years]).
NEUROLOGICAL FEATURES
Clinical data were available for 18 subjects. Fifteen de-
veloped abnormalities on neurological examination dur-
ing the study, as follows: myoclonus (11 subjects), a mean
of 4 years (range, 1-11 years) after symptom onset; limb
dyspraxia (8); brisk reflexes (4); late visual disorienta-
tion (3); and nonspecifically slow gait (3). Two subjects
developed seizures during study follow-up, 5 and
6 years after symptom onset, respectively, and seizures
occurred in a further subject after follow-up ceased,
6 months prior to death.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL FEATURES
All 19 patients underwent at least 2 neuropsychological
assessments a minimum of 1 year apart (mean, 5.4 as-
sessments [range, 2-14 assessments]). Mean interval be-
tween assessments was 1 year, with a mean follow-up of
5 years (range, 1-10 years). Sixteen patients were untest-
able at the end of the study. Mean time from onset of
symptoms to final neuropsychological assessment in these
16 patients was 5 years (range, 1-10 years). Three pa-
tients are receiving ongoing follow-up.
Eight patients had presymptomatic neuropsychologi-
cal assessments at least 6 months before reported onset
of symptoms (starting a mean of 2.7 years [range, 0.8-
5.0 years] before onset of symptoms; mean age of 42 years
[range, 31-50 years]). One further subject was first as-
sessed only 2 months before developing symptoms. All
subjects had subsequent symptomatic assessments.
Table 1 presents the proportion of patients who
showed a deficit in each domain at initial and final pre-
symptomatic assessment and initial and final symptom-
atic assessment. At initial presymptomatic assessment,
impairments of cognition were restricted to general intel-
ligence (4/8) and memory (2/8). By the final presymp-
tomatic assessment, 5 of 9 of those assessed showed defi-
cits in general intelligence, 3 of 9 in visual memory, and
2 of 9 in verbal memory, whereas none showed deficits in
MMSE score. At the first symptomatic assessment, only
4 of 11 patients showed deficits in MMSE score. However,
14 of 19 showed deficits in intelligence, 14 of 18 in verbal
memory, and 11 of 18 in visual memory; 5 of 17 were dys-
calculic. Disorders of perception, naming, and spelling were
rare. By the time of the final symptomatic assessment, cog-
nitive deficits were pervasive with relative preservation of
naming and perception (consistent with previous re-
ports
15
). Spelling skills were especially resilient.
Figure 1 illustrates the typical ordering of deficits
occurring in the different domains. Domains placed to-
ward the left of the diagram generally show deficits at
earlier points than domains placed further toward the
right. When there is no dashed circle/oval enclosing a pair
(REPRINTED) ARCH NEUROL / VOL 61, NOV 2004 WWW.ARCHNEUROL.COM
1744
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at Imperial College London, on February 26, 2007 www.archneurol.comDownloaded from
of domains, the relative order of deficits occurring achieves
statistical significance. The statistical details of this are
shown in
Table 2. Mini-Mental State Examination score,
calculation, visuoperceptual and visuospatial skills, nam-
ing, and spelling all showed deficits significantly later than
symptom onset and in the order shown in Figure 1. When
the data were reanalyzed using the time when deficits first
became permanent (rather than sometimes showing tran-
sient improvement), verbal IQ and visual memory also
showed deficits significantly later than symptom onset,
but there were no other differences.
Figure 2 shows the
modified Kaplan-Meier plots for survival of each do-
main, for visual comparison.
CORRELATION OF
NEUROLOGICAL ABNORMALITIES
AND MEMORY DEFICITS
Five patients were examined when presymptomatic and
when memory scores were normal. Four had subtle abnor-
malities at this early stage: myoclonus (3) and tremor (1).
At the time of first memory impairment, 14 patients
had a neurological examination. Seven had subtle ab-
normalities: myoclonus (5), dyspraxia (1), pout reflex
(1), and broken pursuit eye movements (1).
COMMENT
This study provides detailed longitudinal data across a
prolonged follow-up period of up to 10 years of the neu-
ropsychological and clinical features of FAD. Our sub-
jects carried a range of pathogenic APP and PSEN1 mu-
tations and belonged to well-characterized FAD pedigrees.
The inclusion of presymptomatic assessments and the long
follow-up allowed an evaluation of the deficits across
much of the disease course of FAD.
It is seldom possible to date symptom onset more ac-
curately than half a year, and data should be interpreted
in light of this. Initial symptoms begin very insidiously,
sometimes across a period of several years before the sub-
ject seeks medical attention and meets accepted criteria
for the diagnosis of AD.
1
It is also recognized that histo-
Table 1. Proportion of Patients Assessed Showing Deficits in Each Domain at Initial
and Final Presymptomatic and Initial and Final Symptomatic Assessments*
Initial Presymptomatic
Assessment†
Final Presymptomatic
Assessment‡
Initial Symptomatic
Assessment
Final Symptomatic
Assessment
Total No. of subjects assessed 8 9 19 19
Psychometric domain
MMSE 0/2 0/4 4/11 16/16
Verbal IQ 3/8 4/9 14/19 18/19
Performance IQ 4/8 5/9 13/19 16/19
Verbal memory 1/7 2/9 14/18 19/19
Visual memory 2/8 3/9 11/18 19/19
Naming 0/8 0/9 1/18 3/18
Spelling 0/7 0/7 2/15 6/18
Calculation 0/8 0/8 5/17 12/18
Visuospatial 0/8 0/8 3/17 10/18
Visuoperceptual 0/7 0/7 3/16 6/18
Abbreviation: MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination.
*Values are expressed as number of subjects of all subjects assessed with that psychometric domain.
†Subjects assessed at least 6 months before the onset of symptoms.
‡Subjects with any presymptomatic assessments.
Verbal Memory
Verbal IQ
Performance IQ
Visual Memory
Onset Calculation PerceptualMMSE Spelling
Spatial
Naming
Figure 1. Typical ordering of deficits occurring in different domains during the progression of Alzheimer disease. Definitions of failure were as follows: onset,
clinical onset of disease as estimated by year of initial symptoms; Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), MMSE score of 24 or lower; cognitive domains, fifth
percentile or below for verbal and visual memory, calculation, perception, spatial skills, naming, and spelling and a difference of 10 or greater between
performance IQ and the National Adult Reading Test and verbal IQ and the National Adult Reading Test. Domains placed toward the left of the diagram generally
show deficits at earlier points than domains placed further toward the right. When there is no dashed circle/oval enclosing a pair of domains, the relative order of
deficits occurring achieves statistical significance (P.05).
(REPRINTED) ARCH NEUROL / VOL 61, NOV 2004 WWW.ARCHNEUROL.COM
1745
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at Imperial College London, on February 26, 2007 www.archneurol.comDownloaded from
pathological change may have begun even earlier, some
years before symptom onset.
16
Abnormalities of the neurological examination were
not prominent, though myoclonus was noted at or be-
fore the time of the first objective impairment of memory
in some. This was usually fine finger myoclonus and may
not have been apparent to a casual observer. Of note, pa-
tients and their spouses did not comment on myoclo-
nus at this early stage.
Our study, in line with previously reported data,
3
in-
dicated early impairment of episodic memory and gen-
eral intelligence in FAD. Formal neuropsychological as-
sessment was essential for the detection of these early
cognitive changes. As expected, the MMSE was less sen-
sitive than these measures, though performed reason-
ably well in detecting impairment before more wide-
spread cognitive deficits were present. In terms of early
diagnosis, however, it was limited, with 50% of patients
still scoring higher than the cutoff score of 24 of 30 nearly
4 years after onset of symptoms.
Previous cross-sectional studies have reached contra-
dictory conclusions about the timing of occurrence of
naming deficits. Ardila et al
6
report naming difficulties
in their poorly educated subjects without dementia who
carried a mutation. However, Warrington et al
15
re-
ported relatively preserved naming in FAD compared with
sporadic AD. Our data show that naming is frequently
preserved many years after initial symptoms and also
document that perception is a relatively resistant cogni-
tive skill in FAD. The very late preservation of spelling
is remarkable, presenting an island of preserved cogni-
tive function when subjects were moderately or se-
verely affected.
Familial AD is often said to follow a more aggressive
disease course than sporadic AD. This is not supported
by our data; 6 cases (5 alive at last contact) are so far
known to have lived for more than 10 years. Survival com-
pares favorably with that in later-onset sporadic AD.
17
CONCLUSIONS
The length of the disease course was very similar to that
reported in sporadic AD. Myoclonus in FAD may begin
early, at or before the time of first objective memory im-
pairment, though it was not prominent. Familial AD may
have a long prodrome during which cognitive deficits are
subtle and may be initially limited to general intelli-
gence and memory. Spelling was the most resilient cog-
nitive domain, and naming and perception were also pre-
served to a late stage.
Accepted for Publication: March 19, 2004.
Correspondence: M. N. Rossor, MD, FRCP, Dementia
Research Group, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square,
Table 2. Order of Development of Deficits in Different Psychometric Domains and Time of Disease Onset: Pairwise Comparisons*
Onset RMT-W
10
RMT-F
10
Verbal IQ Performance IQ
8
MMSE Calculation
13
Visuo-
spatial
14
Visuo-
perception
14
Naming
11
Spelling
12
Onset
RMT-W
10
P = .29;
6, 2
RMT-F
10
P = .18;
7, 2
P = .69;
4, 2
Verbal IQ
8
P = .51;
6, 3
P.99;
3, 4
P = .34;
3, 7
Performance IQ
8
P = .55;
7, 4
P.99;
5, 4
P = .77;
5, 7
P.99;
4, 3
MMSE Onset;
P = .008;
8, 0
P = .22;
5, 1
P = .45;
5, 2
P = .45;
5, 2
P.99;
4, 3
Calculation
13
Onset;
P.001;
12, 0
RMT-W;
P = .003;
12, 1
RMT-F;
P = .02;
9, 1
Verbal IQ;
P = .006;
11, 1
Performance IQ;
P = .006;
11, 1
P = .07;
9, 2
Visuospatia
8
Onset;
P.001;
14, 0
RMT-W;
P.001;
14, 0
RMT-F;
P.001;
12, 0
Verbal IQ;
P = .001;
14, 1
Performance IQ;
P = .003;
12, 1
MMSE;
P = .001;
11, 0
P = .29;
6, 2
Visuoperception
14
Onset;
P.001;
15, 0
RMT-W;
P.001;
15, 0
RMT-F;
P = .001;
14, 0
Verbal IQ;
P = .001;
14, 0
Performance IQ;
P = .001;
12, 0
MMSE;
P = .001;
12, 0
P = .07;
7, 1
P = .34;
7, 3
Naming
11
Onset;
P.001;
16, 0
RMT-W;
P.001;
16, 0
RMT-F;
P.001;
15, 0
Verbal IQ;
P.001;
15, 0
Performance IQ;
P.001;
13, 0
MMSE;
P.001;
13, 0
P = .11;
8, 2
P = .07;
7, 1
P.99;
2, 2
Spelling
12
Onset;
P.001;
14, 0
RMT-W;
P = .002;
13, 1
RMT-F;
P.001;
12, 0
Verbal IQ;
P = .002;
13, 1
Performance IQ;
P = .003;
12, 1
MMSE;
P = .001;
11, 0
Calculation;
P = .02; 9, 1
P = .29;
6, 2
P.99;
3, 3
P.99;
1, 2
Abbreviations: MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; RMT -F, Recognition Memory T est–Faces; RMT -W, Recognition Memory Test–W ords.
*Values are expressed as the P value (from the binomial exact test) followed by the number of patients where the domain named in the row heading shows a deficit
first followed by the number of patients where the domain named in the column heading shows a deficit first. Where the P value reached statistical significance (P.05),
the name of the domain with the earlier deficit also appears in the relevant cell. Patients where both domains show a deficit together or not at all or where missing values
meant the order could not be determine were excluded. Onset is defined as the clinical onset of disease as estimated by year of initial symptoms.
(REPRINTED) ARCH NEUROL / VOL 61, NOV 2004 WWW.ARCHNEUROL.COM
1746
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at Imperial College London, on February 26, 2007 www.archneurol.comDownloaded from
London WC1N 3BG, England (m.rossor@dementia.ion
.ucl.ac.uk).
Author Contributions: Study concept and design:
Cipolotti, Fox, Janssen, and Rossor. Acquisition of
data: Godbolt, Cipolotti, Fox, and Janssen. Analysis and
interpretation of data: Godbolt, Cipolotti, Watt, Fox, and
Rossor. Drafting of the manuscript: Godbolt, Cipolotti,
Watt, Fox, and Janssen. Critical revision of the manu-
script for important intellectual content: Godbolt,
Cipolotti, Watt, Fox, Janssen, and Rossor. Statistical
analysis: Watt. Obtained funding: Cipolotti and Rossor.
Administrative, technical, and material support: Janssen.
Study supervision: Cipolotti, Fox, and Rossor.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by pro-
gram grant G9626876 from the Medical Research Coun-
cil, London, England.
Additional Information: Dr Fox holds a Medical Re-
search Council senior clinical fellowship.
Acknowledgment: We thank the patients and their fami-
lies, assistants of the Neuropsychology Department of
the National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery,
London, England, and Angus Kennedy, MD, who per-
formed some of the early assessments.
REFERENCES
1. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical
diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group un-
der the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on
Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939-944.
2. Hui JS, Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Gilley DW, Evans DA. Rate of cogni-
tive decline and mortality in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 2003;61:1356-1361.
3. Fox NC, Warrington EK, Seiffer AS, Agnew SK, Rossor MN. Presymptomatic cog-
nitive deficits in individuals at risk of familial Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal
prospective study. Brain. 1998;121:1631-1639.
4. Diaz-Olavarrieta C, Ostrosky-Solis F, Garcia DLC, Rodriguez Y, Alonso E. Neu-
ropsychological changes in subjects at risk of inheriting Alzheimer’s disease.
Neuroreport. 1997;8:2449-2453.
5. Rosselli MC, Ardila AC, Moreno SC, et al. Cognitive decline in patients with fa-
milial Alzheimer’s disease associated with E280a presenilin-1 mutation: a lon-
gitudinal study. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22:483-495.
6. Ardila A, Lopera F, Rosselli M, et al. Neuropsychological profile of a large kin-
dred with familial Alzheimer’s disease caused by the e280a single presenilin-1
mutation. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2000;15:515-528.
7. Folstein M, Folstein S, McHughs P. The “Mini Mental State”: a practical method
.6
1
.8
.4
.2
0
No. at Risk
MMSE
7
Performance IQ
8
Verbal IQ
8
0
9
9
9
2
9
4
8
6
3
8
1
1
9
3
10
5
5
7
2
9
0
10
0
5521455200
4421334200
Duration of Symptoms of Alzheimer Disease, y
Proportion of Patients
Remaining Unimpaired
A
MMSE
Performance IQ
Verbal IQ
.6
1
.8
.4
.2
0
No. at Risk
MMSE
7
RMT-F
10
RMT-W
10
0
9
9
9
2
9
4
8
6
3
8
1
1
9
3
10
5
5
7
2
9
0
10
0
6511553100
4411533100
Duration of Symptoms of Alzheimer Disease, y
Proportion of Patients
Remaining Unimpaired
B
MMSE
RMT-F
RMT-W
.6
1
.8
.4
.2
0
No. at Risk
MMSE
7
Cube Analysis
14
Silhouettes
14
0
9
9
9
2
9
4
8
6
3
8
1
1
9
3
10
5
5
7
2
9
0
10
0
994411 10 7 4 2 0
11 10 5 411 10 8 4 2 1
Duration of Symptoms of Alzheimer Disease, y
Proportion of Patients
Remaining Unimpaired
C
MMSE
Cube Analysis
14
Silhouettes
14
.6
1
.8
.4
.2
0
No. at Risk
MMSE
7
Oral Graded Difficulty
Spelling Test
12
GNT
11
0
9
9
9
2
9
4
8
6
3
8
1
1
9
3
10
5
5
7
2
9
0
10
0
12 11 5 411 12 7 4 2 0
12 11 5 310 12 8 4 2 1
Graded Difficulty
Arithmetic Test
13
91074310 9 6 3 1 0
Duration of Symptoms of Alzheimer Disease, y
Proportion of Patients
Remaining Unimpaired
D
MMSE
Spelling
GNT
Arithmetic
Figure 2. Modified Kaplan-Meier plots for survival of each domain, for visual comparison. The plots show a decreasing proportion of patients who are intact in the
specified domain by length of time from onset of symptoms. A, Intellectual decline and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score. B, Memory and MMSE
score. C, Visuospatial, visuoperceptual skills, and MMSE score. D, Naming, calculation, spelling, and MMSE score. GNT indicates Graded Naming Test;
RMT-F, Recognition Memory Test–Faces; RMT-W, Recognition Memory Test–Words.
(REPRINTED) ARCH NEUROL / VOL 61, NOV 2004 WWW.ARCHNEUROL.COM
1747
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at Imperial College London, on February 26, 2007 www.archneurol.comDownloaded from
for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;
12:189-198.
8. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York,
NY: Psychological Corp; 1981.
9. Nelson HE, Willison J. The National Adult Reading Test (NART) Manual. 2nd ed.
Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson; 1991.
10. Warrington EK. Manual for the Recognition Memory Test for Words and Faces.
Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson; 1984.
11. McKenna P, Warrington EK. The Graded Naming Test. Windsor, England: NFER-
Nelson; 1983.
12. Baxter DM, Warrington EK. Measuring dysgraphia: a graded-difficulty spelling
test. Behav Neurol. 1994;7:107-116.
13. Jackson M, Warrington EK. Arithmetic skills in patients with unilateral cerebral
lesions. Cortex. 1986;22:611-620.
14. Warrington EK, James M. The Visual Object and Space Perception Battery. Bury
St Edmunds, England: Thames Valley Test Co; 1991.
15. Warrington EK, Agnew SK, Kennedy AM, Rossor MN. Neuropsychological pro-
files of familial Alzheimer’s disease associated with mutations in the presenilin 1
and amyloid precursor protein genes. J Neurol. 2001;248:45-50.
16. Morris JC, Storandt M, McKeel DW, et al. Cerebral amyloid deposition and dif-
fuse plaques in “normal” aging: evidence for presymptomatic and very mild Alz-
heimer’s disease. Neurology. 1996;46:707-719.
17. Brookmeyer R, Corrada MM, Curriero FC, Kawas C. Survival following a diag-
nosis of Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2002;59:1764-1767.
Call for Papers
A
RCHIVES Express
The A
RCHIVES launched a new ARCHIVES Express section
in the September 2000 issue. This section will enable the
editors to publish highly selected papers within approxi-
mately 2 months of acceptance. We will consider only
the most significant research, the top 1% of accepted pa-
pers, on new important insights into the pathogenesis
of disease, brain function, and therapy. We encourage
authors to send their most exceptional clinical or basic
research, designating in the cover letter a request for ex-
pedited A
RCHIVES Express review. We look foward to pub-
lishing your important new research in this accelerated
manner.
Roger N. Rosenberg, MD
Editor
(REPRINTED) ARCH NEUROL / VOL 61, NOV 2004 WWW.ARCHNEUROL.COM
1748
©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at Imperial College London, on February 26, 2007 www.archneurol.comDownloaded from
... Il est désormais acquis que durant la phase de pré-démence, les trajectoires de déclins cognitifs mesurées par des marqueurs psychométriques sont non linéaires avec une accélération du déclin cognitif qui se manifeste par un changement de pente (Wilson et al., 2012;Rajan et al., 2017;Li et al., 2017) et ces trajectoires sont très hétérogènes entre sujets (Amieva et al., 2014). De plus, le processus de dégradation semble se dérouler enétapes successives (Godbolt et al., 2004;Amieva et al., 2008).À partir de ces observations, les chercheurs ontémis l'htpothèse de l'existence d'une cascade pathologique atteignant successivement différentes dimensions de la cognition dont lesétapes ont eté décrites dans des modèles théoriques (Jack et al., 2010(Jack et al., , 2013Verlinden et al., 2016). Néanmoins, ces derniers restent hypothétiques et des méthodes statistiques sont nécessaires pour les valider. ...
... Moreover, it has been found that the predementia phase was quite long and that a temporal order existed in the degradation process (Godbolt et al., 2004;Amieva et al., 2008). By comparing the temporal decline of some abilities and anatomic functions, researchers were able to build hypothetical theoretical schemes of the degradation process which led them to consider the development of the pathology as a continuum (Jack et al., 2010(Jack et al., , 2013Verlinden et al., 2016). ...
Thesis
The aim of this work was to propose inferential methods to describe natural history of the pre-diagnosis phase of dementia. During this phase, which can last around fifteen years, the cognitive decline trajectories are nonlinear and heterogeneous between subjects. Because heterogeneity and nonlinearity, we chose a random changepoint mixed model to describe these trajectories. A first part of this work was to propose a testing procedure to assess the existence of a random changepoint. Indeed, in some subpopulations, the cognitive decline seems smooth and the question of the existence of a changepoint itself araises. This question is methodologically challenging because of identifiability issues on some parameters under the null hypothesis that makes standard tests useless. We proposed a supremum score test to answer this question. A second part of this work was the comparison of the temporal order of different markers changepoint. Dementia is a multidimensional disease where different dimensions of the cognition are affected. Hypothetic cascade models exist for describing this natural history but have not been evaluated on real data. Comparing change over time of different markers measuring different cognitive functions gives precious insight on this hypothesis. In this spirit, we propose a bivariate random changepoint model allowing proper comparison of the time of change of two cognitive markers, potentially non Gaussian. The proposed methodologies were evaluated on simulation studies and applied on real data from two French cohorts. Finally, we discussed the limitations of the two models we used that focused on the late acceleration of the cognitive decline before dementia diagnosis and we proposed an alternative model that estimates the time of differentiation between cases and non-cases.
... The timeline of deterioration of skills depends on the particular form of AD. In a 10 year longitudinal prospective study of 19 people with ADAD, it was observed that in the early stages, naming, spelling and visuo-perceptual skills were better preserved than in LOAD [104]. Visual deficits occurred in the late stages [20], particularly in PSEN1 mutations after codon 200 [68]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this review is to compare and highlight the clinical and pathological aspects of genetic versus acquired Alzheimer’s disease: Down syndrome-associated Alzheimer’s disease in (DSAD) and Autosomal Dominant Alzheimer’s disease (ADAD) are compared with the late-onset form of the disease (LOAD). DSAD and ADAD present in a younger population and are more likely to manifest with non-amnestic (such as dysexecutive function features) in the prodromal phase or neurological features (such as seizures and paralysis) especially in ADAD. The very large variety of mutations associated with ADAD explains the wider range of phenotypes. In the LOAD, age-associated comorbidities explain many of the phenotypic differences.
... al. 1989 Die neuen Forderungen nach "dynamic studies" (s.a. Fox et al.2000;2004;Scahill et al. 2003;Jack,2004 So weiß man heute definitiv, dass gesundes Altern sich nicht in EEG-Veränderungen, wie etwa einer Verlangsamung der Grundaktivität bekundet (Duffy et al.1993;Giaquinto & Nolfe, 1985;Green et al. 1988;Hubbard et al. 1976;Katz & Horowitz 1982;Koyama et al. 1997;Niedermeyer, 1981;Oken & Kaye, 1992;Torres et al. 1983 (Terry et al. 1987;Beyreuther et al.1993;West et al. 1994;Morrison &Hof, 1997;Gomez-Isla et al. 1997;Fox et al. 1999;Weinberger & McClure, 2002;Pakkenberg et al. 2003;Schott et al. 2003;Scahill et al. 2003;Godbolt et al. 2004;Aizenstein et al. 2005;Smith et al. 2007 1996;Jack, 2004). -Bloom et al.1998;Cummings et al. 1998;Rogers & Friedhoff, 1998) Pseudodemenz" anzunehmen ist (Lazarus et al.1987;Jordan et al.1989;Sachdev et al. 1990;Alexopoulos et al. 1993 Rubin et al., 1998;Knopman, 1998;Hyman, 2004). ...
... Declarative memory is hippocampal-dependent both in early stages of memory consolidation and also in recall [24] and after consolidation occurs, memories become hippocampal-independent [25]. Specifically in AD, declarative episodic memory is affected in the early stages, while other types of memory only deteriorate as the disease progresses [2,[26][27][28]. ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, the idea that sleep is critical for cognitive processing has gained strength. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common form of dementia worldwide and presents a high prevalence of sleep disturbances. However, it is difficult to establish causal relations, since a vicious circle emerges between different aspects of the disease. Nowadays, we know that sleep is crucial to consolidate memory and to remove the excess of beta-amyloid and hyperphosphorilated tau accumulated in AD patients’ brains. In this review, we discuss how sleep disturbances often precede in years some pathological traits, as well as cognitive decline, in AD. We describe the relevance of sleep to memory consolidation, focusing on changes in sleep patterns in AD in contrast to normal aging. We also analyze whether sleep alterations could be useful biomarkers to predict the risk of developing AD and we compile some sleep-related proposed biomarkers. The relevance of the analysis of the sleep microstructure is highlighted to detect specific oscillatory patterns that could be useful as AD biomarkers.
... Various research have suggested that the period of the Alzheimer's disease is a chronic period and neurodegeneration has begun many years before the appearance of the clinical symptoms (2). Moreover, the fact that the process of the illness is prolonged provides the time needed to study the causes and symptoms of the disease. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Alzheimer's disease is a neurodegenerative disease, which usually helps some biomarkers, such as amyloid proteins, to diagnose the disease. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to compare the expression of a protein binding protein to the adjuvant responder to circular adenosine monophosphate (CREB) in peripheral blood of patients to Alzheimer's and healthy elderly people as a biomarker for diagnosing Alzheimer. Materials and Methods: In this case-control study, 32 patients with Alzheimer's disease and 32 normal blood samples were taken. Using real time PCR, CREB expression was evaluated. Results: The mean CREB level in the case group was 0.89 ± 0.30 and in the control group was 1.01 ± 0.03. The mean of BDNF level in the case group was significantly higher than the control group (P <0.001). There was no significant relationship between the level of CREB with age, sex, MMSE score and Cornell scale for depression in dementia (P> 0.05). Conclusion: Reducing CREB levels in people with Alzheimer's disease can be a factor in diagnosis in comparison to healthy people.
... Анализа и праћење учинка у вербалној флуентности, међутим, показује се посебно вредном када је потребно идентификовати старија лица која су под ризиком од развоја деменције Алцхајмеровог типа (у даљем тексту АД) или код којих је ова болест у почетној фази. АД представља најучесталији вид деменције, који се развија током дугачког временског периода који се мери деценијама ( Godbolt et al., 2004). Предвиђања су да ће се, како просечна старост светске популације расте, број оболелих од АД увећати четири пута до 2050. ...
Article
Full-text available
Old age is a multifactorial process with a progressive decline in homeostatic mechanisms, caused by reduce in adaptive capacity of the organism to the change. The third age is accompanied by numerous changes of organs and organ systems, resulting in progression of loss and damage of various functions, which ultimately results in frequent appearance to the respective chronic diseases, multimorbidity, which further influences the development of functional disability of the el-derly. In old age, disease manifestation is atypical. In geriatric patients the polipharmacy is frequent - elderly take 4.5 medications per day on average. Basics of healthy and active aging, to preserve functional capa-bilities and imporving health in old age, is put at a young age through its own particular decisions about the positive health behavior. Im-perative of geriatric-gerontology activities is to improve the quality of life of the elderly to mantain as long as active and independent in with its own enviroment. Respecting the existing credist, Geriatric Depart-ment KBC „Zvezdara“ received from its founder, the city of Belgrade, on the 50th anniversary, approval to call this Department after his first chief, Prof.dr Petar Korolija. Key words: elderly, morbidity.
Article
Full-text available
Familial Alzheimer's disease (FAD) is caused by autosomal dominant mutations in the PSEN1, PSEN2 or APP genes, giving rise to considerable clinical and pathological heterogeneity in FAD. Here we investigate variability in clinical data and the type and distribution of Aβ pathologies throughout the cortical layers of different FAD mutation cases. Brain tissue from 20 FAD cases [PSEN1 pre-codon 200 (n = 10), PSEN1 post-codon 200 (n = 6), APP (n = 4)] were investigated. Frontal cortex sections were stained immunohistochemically for Aβ, and Nissl to define the cortical layers. The frequency of different amyloid-beta plaque types was graded for each cortical layer and the severity of cerebral amyloid angiopathy (CAA) was determined in cortical and leptomeningeal blood vessels. Comparisons were made between FAD mutations and APOE4 status, with associations between pathology, clinical and genetic data investigated. In this cohort, possession of an APOE4 allele was associated with increased disease duration but not with age at onset, after adjusting for mutation sub-group and sex. We found Aβ pathology to be heterogeneous between cases although Aβ load was highest in cortical layer 3 for all mutation groups and a higher Aβ load was associated with APOE4. The PSEN1 post-codon 200 group had a higher Aβ load in lower cortical layers, with a small number of this group having increased cotton wool plaque pathology in lower layers. Cotton wool plaque frequency was positively associated with the severity of CAA in the whole cohort and in the PSEN1 post-codon 200 group. Carriers of the same PSEN1 mutation can have differing patterns of Aβ deposition, potentially because of differences in risk factors. Our results highlight possible influences of APOE4 genotype, and PSEN1 mutation type on Aβ deposition, which may have effects on the clinical heterogeneity of FAD.
Article
Full-text available
Improved medical care of individuals with Down syndrome (DS) has led to an increase in life expectancy to over the age of 60 years. In conjunction, there has been an increase in age‐related co‐occurring conditions including Alzheimer's disease (AD). Understanding the factors that underlie symptom and age of clinical presentation of dementia in people with DS may provide insights into the mechanisms of sporadic and DS‐associated AD (DS‐AD). In March 2019, the Alzheimer's Association, Global Down Syndrome Foundation and the LuMind IDSC Foundation partnered to convene a workshop to explore the state of the research on the intersection of AD and DS research; to identify research gaps and unmet needs; and to consider how best to advance the field. This article provides a summary of discussions, including noting areas of emerging science and discovery, considerations for future studies, and identifying open gaps in our understanding for future focus.
Article
Background: There are no validated prognostic instruments to evaluate severe Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients. Objective: To validate the prognostic value of the Baylor Profound Mental Status Examination (BPMSE). Methods: We selected 200 patients with severe AD. The following prognostic variables were collected: hospitalization, use of the emergency room, death, and prescription of drugs. ROC curve analysis was performed to see the overall behavior of the test when predicting the adverse event. We analyzed the AUC ROC and the best cut point was determined, and by using contingency tables, the risk was calculated. Results: For a BPMSE ≥16 points, there was a risk of 1.8 (95% CI 0.9-3.4) of prescription of psychotropic drugs in 12 months. For memantine in 12 months, for a BPMSE ≥16 points, there was a risk of 2.9 (95% CI 1.1-7.2). Emergency room visits, for a BMPSE ≤15 points, showed a risk of 1.7 (95% CI 1-3.2). The risk of hospitalization at 12 months, for a BPMSE ≤15, was 1.4 (95% CI 0.8-2.6). When comparing medians, patients with a higher BPMSE were prescribed more drugs at 12 months. Conclusions: BPMSE has a limited predictive value for the variables studied.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper we describe the construction of a graded-difficulty spelling test for adults consisting of two alternative forms each containing 30 words (GDST, Forms A and B). The spelling test, together with background tests of verbal and non-verbal skills, was administered to 100 control patients with orthopaedic injuries. The two forms of the spelling test were highly correlated (0.92). Spelling was highly correlated with reading (0.75, 0.77) and moderately correlated with vocabulary (0.57) and naming (0.39, 0.40). There was no correlation between spelling skills and non-verbal reasoning. The test was validated in a group of 26 patients with left hemisphere and 20 patients with right hemisphere lesions. Spelling was shown to be lateralized to the left hemisphere and there appeared to be a shift in scores of the left hemisphere group towards the lower quartile, with 65% of the left hemisphere group falling within this band. The most severe spelling impairments were invariably associated with other language disorders but a number of dissociations were documented at spelling levels falling between the 5th and 25th percentile band. Two patients with left hemisphere lesions (8%) were identified as having selective dysgraphias. The lack of overlap between the anatomical sites of the two patients with specific lexical dysgraphia argues against a single site for this type of dysgraphia and argues for further refinement of this classification of spelling disorder.
Article
It was hypothesized that subjective memory complaints represent the earliest sign of dementia in carriers of the presenilin-1 (PS1) mutation. A total of 122 subjects (44 males, 78 females) were included in this study. Forty of them were positive for the mutation in the PS1 gene (mutation positive, MP) whereas 82 showed negative results (mutation negative, MN). Subjects were active, functionally normal, even though some of them complained of memory difficulties. Two groups of neuropsychological instruments were administered: (a) The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) neuropsychological test battery (Morris et al., 1989), and (b) some additional neuropsychological tests (Raven Test, Wechsler Memory Scale, Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure, Boston Naming Test, Naming of Categories, Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, Memory of Three Phrases, Knopman Test, Digit Symbol, and Visual “A” Cancellation Test). Performance in both groups was quite similar. In a secondary analysis, the MP group was subdivided into two subgroups: without and with memory complaints. When comparing both subgroups, a better performance in the first subgroup was found throughout the different subtests. Statistically significant differences were observed in the following test scores: Mini-Mental State Examination, Naming Test (Low Frequency), Memory of Words Test, Recall of Drawings, Wechsler Memory Scale (Logical Memory, Associative Learning, and Total Score), Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (Immediate Recall Condition), Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (Complex Ideational Material Subtest), Memory of Three Phrases Test, Serial Verbal Learning (maximum score and Delayed Recall), Knopman Test (First Trial, Second Trial, and Recall after 5 Minutes), Digit Symbol, and Visual “A” Cancellation Test (Additions). Results supported the hypothesis that memory complaints represent the earliest symptom of familial Alzheimer's disease. In addition to the memory difficulties, other minor cognitive impairments were also found, particularly, mild anomia, concentration difficulties and defects in the understanding of complex verbal material.
Article
In this paper we describe the construction of a Graded Difficulty Arithmetic test (GDA) consisting of 12 additions and 12 subtractions which are orally presented. The test was administered to a control group of 100 volunteer subjects with extra-cerebral neurological disorders and to two experimental groups of patients with unilateral cerebral lesions of the left and right hemisphere. In the control group performance on the GDA was found to correlate highly with other measures of verbal intelligence, namely the National Adult Reading Test, the WAIS Arithmetic subtest and the WAIS Digit Span subtest. Between group analysis showed a significant groups effect on the GDA, the left hemisphere lesion group showing greater impairment compared to the right hemisphere lesion group and the controls. Using "cut-off" scores the left hemisphere lesion group's performance was shown to be significantly worse than that of the right hemisphere lesion group, who in turn were not significantly worse than the control group.