ArticlePDF Available

The Effects of Exercise Advertising on Self-efficacy and Decisional Balance

Authors:

Abstract

To investigate the effects of exercise advertising on self-efficacy and decisional balance for changing exercise behavior. One hundred seventy-four university students (females = 108; males = 66) watched a video that contained health, appearance, or control advertising and completed stage of change, exercise self-efficacy, and decisional balance questionnaires. There was a significant condition by gender interaction indicating that men in the appearance condition had lower self-efficacy than did females. Health promotion advertising did not have any effect. This study provides evidence that the message in appearance-based advertising that men should be muscular may be detrimental.
Am J Health Behav.
2005;29(2):117-126
117
The Effects of Exercise Advertising on
Self-efficacy and Decisional Balance
Tanya R. Berry, PhD; Bruce L. Howe, PhD
Tanya R. Berry, Assistant Professor, Depart-
ment of Kinesiology and Physical Education,
Wilfrid Laurier University Waterloo, Ontario,
Canada. Bruce L. Howe, Professor, School of
Physical Education, University of Victoria, Victoria,
BC, Canada.
Address correspondence to Dr Berry, Depart-
ment of Kinesiology and Physical Education,
Wilfrid Laurier University Waterloo, Ontario, N2L
3C5, Canada. E-mail: tberry@wlu.ca
Objectives: To investigate the
effects of exercise advertising on
self-efficacy and decisional bal-
ance for changing exercise be-
havior. Methods: One hundred sev-
enty-four university students (fe-
males = 108; males = 66) watched
a video that contained health,
appearance, or control advertis-
ing and completed stage of change,
exercise self-efficacy, and deci-
sional balance questionnaires.
Results: There was a significant
condition by gender interaction
indicating that men in the appear-
ance condition had lower self-
efficacy than did females. Health
promotion advertising did not
have any effect. Conclusions: This
study provides evidence that the
message in appearance-based ad-
vertising that men should be mus-
cular may be detrimental.
Key words: advertising, self-effi-
cacy, decisional balance, exercise
Am J Health Behav. 2005;29(2):117-126
T
here is considerable evidence that
exercise is associated with the pre-
vention of chronic diseases such as
cardiovascular disease, obesity, type II
diabetes, colon cancer, osteoporosis, and
several psychological disorders.
1,2
Despite
this, a large proportion of North Ameri-
cans do not participate in sufficient physi-
cal activity to gain the health benefits.
2,3
One way to approach the issue of seden-
tary behavior is to promote exercise
through the media. Exercise advertising
already exists in various forms, from pub-
licly funded health promotion advertising
to advertising that uses appearance en-
hancement to sell a product. However,
little is known about the effects of such
advertising on consumers. Those who
promote exercise for health do not seem
to have much of an impact as the majority
of people still do not engage in adequate
amounts of physical activity,
4
and although
people are buying fitness products (an $8
billion US-a-year industry according to
Dishman
4
), this is not translating to in-
creased exercise participation.
Researchers have identified both
health and appearance as frequently cited
reasons for exercise.
5,6
Because of the
potential importance of appearance as a
motivator, Dishman
4
emphasized that the
focus of social marketers needs to expand
beyond the health promotion aspect to
include other possible motivators, such
as appearance and weight loss, that are
prevalent in commercial exercise adver-
tising. However, before this step is taken,
it is necessary to understand how such
messages can influence viewers. It is
possible that advertising that emphasizes
appearance may negatively influence an
individual’s self-concept,
7
making con-
sumers feel inadequate in terms of
weight, fitness level, or age
8
and subse-
quently influencing exercise participa-
tion.
As yet, little research has examined
the effects of exercise advertising on con-
Effects of Exercise
118
sumers’ attitudes and beliefs toward ex-
ercise. Of existing research, almost all
has evaluated the effects of public health
promotion campaigns with the general
finding that such advertising results in
small shifts in knowledge but no increases
in actual exercise behavior.
9,10
Very few
studies have looked at appearance-based
exercise messages. One study found that
health promotion advertising had a sig-
nificant positive effect on self-presenta-
tional beliefs for those who were already
active, whereas appearance-based adver-
tising had negative effects on
nonexercisers.
11
Other researchers found
a positive effect on exercise attitudes for
people who were low self-monitors who
read health-based persuasive exercise
messages.
12
One model that may be applied to such
research is the transtheoretical model
(TTM) of behavior change. This model
emphasizes the importance of increas-
ing knowledge of the benefits of exercise
and making people aware of the risks of
being inactive,
13
messages that are often
contained in exercise advertising. Slater
14
recommended the TTM as a theoretical
framework for examining the relation-
ship between communication interven-
tions and behavior change. The TTM iden-
tifies specific stages that individuals go
through when trying to change their pat-
terns of behavior and the processes of
change people use in the various stages.
15
Researchers have identified other fac-
tors within the model that serve as me-
diators between interventions and be-
havior including decisional balance and
self-efficacy. In the exercise domain, the
TTM has proven to be a useful model
when examining exercise behavior in
such diverse populations as high school
students, university students, employed
adults,
16
and seniors.
17
The stages of change identified by the
TTM include precontemplation, contem-
plation, preparation, action, and mainte-
nance. With exercise behavior, individu-
als who are in the precontemplation stage
are unaware that lack of exercise is prob-
lematic and have no intention of chang-
ing their behavior. In the contemplation
stage, individuals are aware that there is
a problem and are thinking of addressing
it, but have yet to commit to action. Dur-
ing the preparation phase, people start to
make small changes such as going for an
occasional walk but have not yet started
regular exercise. In the action stage,
individuals make a significant commit-
ment of time and resources and start
regular exercise. If individuals change
their behavior for up to a 6-month period
of time, they are considered to be in the
action stage. If they adhere to the behav-
ior changes for longer than 6 months,
they are considered to be in the mainte-
nance stage.
Decisional balance refers to the poten-
tial benefits (pros) and costs (cons) of
behavior change and can include approval
and instrumental gain by self and others
or disapproval and losses by self and oth-
ers.
18
In earlier stages, when no action to
change behavior has occurred, possible
costs of changing behavior outweigh pos-
sible benefits. Prochaska
13
reported strong
support for the use of pros and cons of
behavior change across 12 health behav-
iors including exercise. Marshall and
Biddle,
19
in a meta-analysis of the TTM
and exercise, conclude that the biggest
increase in evaluation of the benefits of
exercise occurs during the contempla-
tion phase; as stage increases, there are
small increases in perceived benefits,
but large decreases in the costs of change.
Self-efficacy has been well researched
in the exercise domain and has been
found to be a strong contributor to exer-
cise behavior. Exercise self-efficacy is
the belief that an individual holds in his
or her ability to achieve a given exercise
goal.
20
It has been concluded that exercise
adherence is most affected by self-regula-
tory efficacy; that is, adherence is af-
fected by the strength of the belief that
one can make oneself to exercise regu-
larly in the face of numerous obstacles.
20
Self-efficacy is developed by various means
including vicarious learning through ob-
servation of others. For example, televi-
sion advertising has been identified as a
source of observational learning that can
model exercise behavior.
14,20
However, al-
though the media have been identified as
an important source of observational
learning in smoking,
21
the influence of
exercise advertising on self-efficacy needs
to be researched. Such research is par-
ticularly necessary as Slater
14
has identi-
fied that although the modeling of behav-
iors in health promotion campaigns can
be positive, a challenge to health promot-
ers is the potentially negative norms that
exist in entertainment media.
Some research has been conducted to
Berry & Howe
Am J Health Behav.
2005;29(2):117-126
119
examine the influence of health promo-
tion advertising on self-efficacy and deci-
sional balance. In a review of existing
literature, it was concluded that self-effi-
cacy has support as a mediator between
exercise interventions and behavior, but
that support for decisional balance as a
mediator has been mixed.
22
For example,
it has been found that using stage-
matched interventions that included tele-
vision advertising and comic strips in-
creased self-efficacy in targeted individu-
als. However, perceived costs and ben-
efits of physical activity did not change for
those participants in precontemplation
and contemplation stages.
23
This research
looked exclusively at health promotion
advertising and needs to be extended to
include appearance-based advertising.
The possibility that both health-based
and appearance-based advertising can
serve as a source of observational learn-
ing and influence self-efficacy beliefs and
decisional balance needs to be examined.
The purpose of this study was to exam-
ine the effects of health-based and ap-
pearance-based exercise advertising on
self-efficacy and decisional balance in an
undergraduate sample. Hypotheses re-
garding the effects of health-based adver-
tising were based on the findings of previ-
ous research that indicated that self-
efficacy can be enhanced by health pro-
motion advertising.
22,23
It was hypothesized
that self-efficacy would increase for par-
ticipants in the health condition. Be-
cause the purpose of health promotion
advertising is to increase awareness of
the benefits of physical activity, it was
hypothesized that the benefits (pros) of
changing exercise behavior would in-
crease for participants in the health con-
dition. Further, based on Slater’s
14
con-
tention that the norms present in the
entertainment media are a potentially
negative influence on health behavior; it
was hypothesized that the costs (cons) of
changing behavior would increase for
participants in the appearance condition.
METHOD
Participants and Procedures
Participants were 178 university stu-
dents (females = 108; males = 70) from
introductory psychology classes (total en-
rollment in the classes was 1865) who
participated for class credit. One week
prior to the study, the participation oppor-
tunity was posted on an online experi-
ment-scheduling system. Students were
able to choose from a number of experi-
ments posted by several researchers and
register in a time slot that was conve-
nient for them; when the available times
were full, the experiment was closed for
registration. The present experiment was
called “TV Watching,” and the description
on the website stated that participants
would be asked to “watch a 20-minute
video and answer some questionnaires.”
They were not told that the experiment
had specifically to do with exercise. Ethi-
cal approval was obtained from the uni-
versity, and all participants completed an
informed consent form and were given
the option to leave if they so chose.
A modified Solomon design was used so
that the possibility of pretest sensitiza-
tion could be assessed. The modified
Solomon design involved random assign-
ment to 1 of 6 groups. Although all groups
watched a video, 2 of the videos were for
the treatment conditions and as such
contained either health or appearance
advertising, and 1 of the videos was for a
control condition and contained no exer-
cise advertising. The experimental de-
sign is outlined in Table 1, including
sample sizes used in analyses after dele-
tion of outliers and missing data.
The data from 4 participants were not
used because either there were missing
data (N=1), or participants in the pretest
groups did not show up for the video and
posttest session (N=3), leaving 174 in the
final data set. One hundred ten partici-
pants (females = 69; males = 41) com-
pleted pretest questionnaires 1 week be-
fore watching 1 of the 3 videos and then
completed posttest questionnaires; 64
participants (females = 35; males = 29)
completed posttest questionnaires only.
After completion of the posttests a full
debriefing was provided. Participants were
randomly assigned to one of health, ap-
pearance, or control treatment conditions
and participated in groups of up to 5 par-
ticipants.
Materials: Questionnaires
Measures were obtained from the Can-
cer Research Prevention Centre (CRPC)
24
website and included a stages-of-change
algorithm, a self-efficacy questionnaire,
and a decisional balance questionnaire.
Stages-of-change algorithm. This
short-form stage questionnaire, devel-
oped from work by Marcus and Simkin,
25
Effects of Exercise
120
asks participants to mark 1 of 5 state-
ments that reflect 1 of the 5 stages of
behavior change. Researchers have con-
firmed its use in assessing stage of be-
havior change,
26
but conclude that it is
more useful for assessing moderate to
strenuous exercise, rather than mild ex-
ercise.
17
Self-efficacy. Self-efficacy was deter-
mined by responses to an 18-item ques-
tionnaire that consisted of a 5-point Likert
scale ranging from not at all confident (1)
to very confident (5). This 18-item scale
has 6 factors that have been validated by
the CRPC. The 6 factors with correspond-
ing alpha, as reported by the CRPC, are
negative affect (.852), excuse making
(.829), must exercise alone (.869), incon-
venient to exercise (.773), resistance from
others (.853), and bad weather (.837).
Decisional balance. Decisional bal-
ance was determined by responses to a
10-item questionnaire regarding benefits
and costs of behavior change. Each area
consisted of 5 items rated on a Likert
scale ranging from extremely important
(1) to not important (5). A low score on the
pros subscale indicates that the benefits
of behavior change are important whereas
a low score on the cons subscale indicates
that the costs of behavior change are
important. The questionnaire has been
shown to be valid with North American
adults.
24,27
Videos
The videos used in this study were
developed for previous research.
11
There
was 1 video for each of the 2 treatment
conditions of exercise for health promo-
tion and exercise for appearance, and 1
video for a control condition. Each video
contained an 18-minute television pro-
gram that portrayed aspects of Japanese
culture but made no reference to appear-
ance or exercise. In addition to the pro-
gram, each video contained 6 identical
neutral advertisements that made no
reference to exercise or appearance and
3 advertisements that were selected spe-
cifically for the 2 treatment and control
conditions. Each advertisement was 20 to
30 seconds long. The neutral and appear-
ance-based advertisements were selected
from commercial television. The appear-
ance advertisements promoted a private
fitness club, an exercise video targeting
the thighs, and a nutritional supplement
advertisement that featured an attrac-
tive female model in a fitness club who
talked about “looking like me” through
exercise with the help of the supplement.
Both men and women were featured in 2
of these advertisements, and all models
were young, attractive white people. The
exercise promotion advertisements were
obtained on request from Participation
Canada,
28
the Hillary Commission of New
Zealand,
29
and the Dairy Nutrition Coun-
cil of Alberta,
30
They featured people of all
ages, different ethnic groups, and both
genders and focused on disease reduction
and improved health through exercise.
The advertisements were shown in groups
of 3 during each of 3 breaks of the pro-
gram. Two of the advertisements during
each break were neutral, and 1 was a
treatment advertisement. They were
Table 1
An Overview of the Modified Solomon Design and Sample Size
Group Pretest Treatment Posttest
Group 1 O1 (n = 41) X1 Health Video O2 (n = 41)
Group 2 O3 (n = 36) X2 Appearance Video O4 (n = 36)
Group 3 O5 (n = 33) - Control Video O6 (n = 33)
Group 4 - X1 Health Video O7 (n = 21)
Group 5 - X2 Appearance Video O8 (n = 20)
Group 6 - - Control Video O9 (n = 19)
Berry & Howe
Am J Health Behav.
2005;29(2):117-126
121
yoked across the 3 conditions so that the
presence and order of neutral and target
advertisements appearing in the health,
appearance, and control conditions were
matched.
Analyses
Because a modified Solomon design
was used in this experiment, analyses
followed the recommendations of Braver
and Braver
31
for the statistical treatment
of a Solomon design. As such, analyses
followed the following steps:
Step 1: A factorial ANOVA to determine
if there is an interaction between treat-
ment condition and pretest (yes or no).
Step 2: If the interaction is not signifi-
cant, then a main effects ANOVA test is
performed on the experimental condi-
tions to determine if there is a treatment
effect.
Step 3: If this test is not significant,
then an ANCOVA is performed on the data
from the participants who completed both
pretests and posttests, using pretest
scores as a covariate. As Braver and
Braver
31
state, if the test done in step 2 is
“significant, there is unqualified evidence
of the treatment effect. This test clearly
is not the most powerful available, how-
ever, and if not significant it should not be
considered conclusive evidence against
a treatment effect” (p 151). Braver and
Braver therefore suggest an ANCOVA as a
further powerful test of treatment effect,
an approach also suggested by Huck and
Sandler.
32
Step 4: If the interaction between treat-
ment condition and pretest (yes or no) is
significant, a series of tests is performed
to determine if the pretest resulted in
sensitization to the treatment.
These procedures were followed so that
in the first step, a 3 (health, appearance,
or control condition) by 2 (gender) by 2
(pretest or no pretest) ANOVA was per-
formed for each of the dependent vari-
ables: self-efficacy, pros of changing be-
havior, and cons of changing behavior.
Similarly, a 3 (condition) by 4 (stage of
change) by 2 (pretest) ANOVA was per-
formed for each of the dependent vari-
ables. Following analyses depended on
the results of step 1.
RESULTS
Data were screened for outliers, and it
was found that there was 1 participant
with a self-efficacy score greater than 3
standard deviations from the mean, 3
participants with cons scores greater
than 3 standard deviations from the mean,
and 1 participant with a pro score greater
than 3 standard deviations from the mean.
These participants were deleted from the
data set.
At posttest, 5 participants were in the
precontemplation stage, 9 in the contem-
plation stage, 40 in the preparation stage,
39 in the action stage, and 77 in the
maintenance stage. Because of the small
numbers of participants in earlier stages,
participants in precontemplation and con-
templation stages were aggregated into 1
stage. Plotnikoff et al
27
used a similar
rationale for aggregating action and main-
tenance as stages of “action” when their
sample size was low for the action stage.
Stage of behavior change was very stable
for participants who completed both pre-
test and posttest questionnaires. Only 2
participants indicated a shift in stage of
change (both moved from maintenance to
action).
For participants who completed the pre-
test, there were no significant differences
at pretest in self-efficacy for treatment
condition, F(2, 92) = 2.60, P>.05, condition
by gender, F(1, 92) = .67, P>.05, or condi-
tion by stage, F(4, 92) = .34, P>.05. Simi-
larly, there were no significant differ-
ences at pretest in cons of behavior change
for condition, (2, 92) = .43, P>.05, condi-
tion by gender, F(2, 92) = 2.08, P>.05, or
condition by stage F(4, 92) = .78, P>.05.
Nor were there any differences at pretest
for pros of behavior change for treatment
condition, F(2, 92) = .90, P>.05, condition
by gender, F(1, 92) = .56, P>.05, or condi-
tion by stage, F(4, 92) = .25, P>.05.
Self-efficacy Analyses
Condition by gender by pretest. Step
1: There was no significant interaction of
condition by pretest (yes or no), F(2, 161) =
0.63, P>.05, or condition by gender by
pretest, F(2, 161) = 0.489, P>.05; there-
fore, main effects tests of condition and
condition by gender were performed, and
step 4 was not required.
Step 2: No significant effects were found
for treatment condition, F(2, 167) = 1.84,
P>.05, or treatment condition by gender,
F(2, 167) = 1.89, P>.05.
Step 3: The ANCOVA with pretest score
as a covariate showed a significant inter-
action of treatment condition by gender,
F(2, 105) = 4.49, P<.05, η
2
=.08. Follow-up
Effects of Exercise
122
univariate tests with a Bonferonni ad-
justment for multiple comparisons showed
that males in the appearance condition
had significantly lower self-efficacy than
did either males in the control condition
or females in the appearance and health
condition. Figure 1 shows the estimated
marginal means (adjusted for the
covariate).
Self-efficacy subscale. A similar se-
ries of analyses were also performed by
self-efficacy subscale. Steps 1 and 2 were
not significant for all subscales. However,
for the step 3 ANCOVA, a significant con-
dition by gender interaction was found for
exercising alone, F(2, 105) = 6.43, P<.005,
η
2
=.11. Follow-up univariate tests with a
Bonferonni adjustment for multiple com-
parisons showed that males in the ap-
pearance condition had significantly lower
self-efficacy than did females in the ap-
pearance and health conditions. A simi-
lar series of analyses showed a signifi-
cant condition by gender interaction when
there was resistance from others, F(2,
105) = 4.03, P<.05, η
2
=.07. Follow-up
univariate tests with a Bonferonni ad-
justment for multiple comparisons showed
that males in the appearance condition
had significantly lower self-efficacy than
did females in the appearance condition.
Condition by stage by pretest.
Step 1: There was no significant inter-
action of condition by pretest (yes or no),
F(2, 146) = 0.85, P>.05, or condition by
stage by pretest, F(6, 146) = 0.29, P>.05.
Step 2: No significant results were
found for the main effects tests of condi-
tion, or condition by stage, F(2, 158) =
0.87, P>.05. There was a significant dif-
ference on self-efficacy scores by stage of
behavior change, F(3, 158) = 6.26, P<.001,
η
2
=.10. Posthoc tests with a Bonferroni
adjustment for multiple comparisons
showed that participants in the mainte-
nance stage had significantly higher self-
efficacy than did participants in the prepa-
ration or contemplation stages. Partici-
pants in the action stage also had signifi-
cantly higher self-efficacy than did par-
ticipants in the contemplation stage.
Step 3: No significant results were
found for the ANCOVA with pretest scores
as covariate, F(6, 97) = 0.48, P>.05.
No significant results were found for
any of the self-efficacy subscales.
Cons Analyses
Steps 1: No significant interactions
were found for condition by gender by
pretest, F(2, 158) = 0.12, P>.05 or condi-
tion by stage by pretest, F(6, 146) = 1.70,
P>.05.
Steps 2: Subsequent analyses showed
no significant differences for condition, F
(2, 158) = 1.27, P>.05, condition by gender,
F(2, 158) = 2.26, P>.05, or condition by
stage, F(6, 158) = 0.73, P>.05.
Steps 3: No significant results were
found for the condition by gender ANCOVA
with pretest scores as covariate, F(2, 103)
= .411, P>.05. Similarly, there were no
significant results for condition by stage
of behavior change, F(6, 97) = .20, P>.05.
Pros Analyses
Steps 1: No significant interactions
were found for condition by gender by
pretest, F(2, 158) = .19, P>.05, or condition
by stage by pretest, F(6, 146) = .49, P>.05.
Steps 2: Subsequent analyses showed
no significant differences for condition,
F(2, 164) = .05, P>.05, condition by gender,
F(2, 164) = .05, P>.05, or condition by
stage, F(6, 158) = .61, P>.05. There was a
significant difference on pros scores by
stage of behavior change, F(3, 160) = 2.72,
P<.05, η
2
=.05. Posthoc tests with a
Bonferroni adjustment for multiple com-
parisons showed that participants in the
60
58
56
54
52
50
48
46
44
Figure 1
Estimated Marginal Means
for Self-efficacy
Health Appearance Control
55.32
53.26
55.02
46.57
52.93
57.15
T r e a t m e n t C o n di t i o n
Female
Male
Berry & Howe
Am J Health Behav.
2005;29(2):117-126
123
maintenance and action stages rated the
pros of changing exercise behavior as
significantly more important than did
participants in the contemplation stage.
Steps 3: No significant results were
found for the condition by gender ANCOVA
with pretest scores as covariate, F(2, 103)
= .01, P>.05. Similarly, there were no
significant results for condition by stage
of behavior change, F(6, 97) = .54, P>.05.
Estimated marginal means for the con-
dition by gender ANCOVAs for each de-
pendent variable and the self-efficacy
subscales are shown in Table 2.
DISCUSSION
The results of this study add to the
literature that examines the effects of
exercise advertising on consumers. Al-
though the hypotheses were not supported
in that there were no overall main effects
of condition for any of the dependent vari-
ables, there were significant interactions.
Results showed that males in the appear-
ance condition were negatively affected
by the advertising. Men who viewed ap-
pearance-based exercise advertising had
a significant negative change in their
overall self-efficacy and, more specifi-
cally, their self-efficacy for exercising
alone or for exercising when there is
resistance from others. This study pro-
vides evidence that appearance-based
advertising that focuses on images of
very attractive exercisers can have nega-
tive effects on self-efficacy for men in a
similar way that images of attractive,
thin women can have deleterious effects
on women’s body image.
These results support other research-
ers who have found that men who were
briefly exposed to advertising that fea-
tured muscular men showed a greater
discrepancy between their ideal body size
and their actual body size.
33
The present
study extended existing advertising re-
search to include exercise advertising,
and it is possible that it is the exercise
Table 2
ANCOVA Results and Estimated Marginal Means for
Condition by Gender for All Primary Dependent
Variables and Self-efficacy Subscales
Health Appearance Control
Female Male Female Male Female Male
Variable (n=26) (n=15) (n=24) (n=12) (n=21) (n=12) F
Self-efficacy 55.32 53.26 55.02 46.57 52.93 57.15 4.49*
S-E
a
alone 10.69 10.46 11.16 8.06 9.67 11.30 6.43**
S-E resistance 10.14 9.76 10.53 7.79 9.99 10.84 4.03*
S-E excuses 7.80 6.95 7.32 7.89 7.67 7.30 1.25
S-E inconvenient 8.66 8.02 8.66 7.94 8.76 7.98 0.01
S-E weather 9.25 9.39 8.94 8.38 8.73 10.70 2.44
S-E affect 8.82 7.86 9.10 7.98 8.36 8.99 1.66
Pros 10.02 10.86 9.27 9.90 10.06 11.40 1.10
Cons 22.12 20.59 21.95 20.70 21.99 21.55 0.41
* P<.05
** P<.005
a self-efficacy
Effects of Exercise
124
component of the advertisements used,
rather than just images of attractive
people, that made the male participants
particularly susceptible to appearance-
based exercise messages. It has been
found that men who read fitness maga-
zines had greater body dissatisfaction and
self-objectification than did men who did
not read such magazines and that men
read significantly more fitness magazines
than women did, whereas women read
more beauty magazines.
34
Further, Berry
and Lauzon
35
found that men’s fitness
magazines invariably focused on a very
muscular look, and other researchers
have found that 85% of sampled college-
aged men wanted to be more muscular.
36
The results of the present study showed
that besides body dissatisfaction, expo-
sure to appearance-based advertising may
result in men’s having less confidence in
their ability to exercise.
That there were no significant inter-
actions for any of the dependent variables
by stage of behavior change does not sup-
port the findings of Berry and Howe,
11
who
found that health-based exercise adver-
tising had a positive effect for those al-
ready active whereas appearance-based
advertising had a negative effect on
nonexercisers. These results are also
contrary to the findings of Bauman et al,
37
who reported that a mass media cam-
paign aimed at increasing exercise re-
sulted in positive changes in exercise
self-efficacy. However, these authors used
only 3 questions to measure self-efficacy
and did not specify what aspects of exer-
cise self-efficacy were examined. Fur-
ther research is needed to clarify whether
exercise advertising has different effects
for people at different stages of exercise
behavior.
Prochaska
13
recommended that public
health policies be dedicated to increasing
the benefits of healthy behavior change,
while decreasing the costs. However,
these recommendations may be prema-
ture as only mixed support for decisional
balance as a mediator between physical
activity interventions and behavior has
been found.
22
Further, the present study
found that neither health-based nor ap-
pearance-based exercise advertising af-
fected decisional balance for changing
exercise behavior. However, Rosen
38
found
that weakly argued messages (eg, exer-
cise will make you less bored) resulted in
more thoughts opposing exercise than did
messages that were strongly argued (eg,
exercise will result in more energy and
will make you more comfortable with your
body).
The health messages in the present
study were that exercise can “add years to
your life,”
29
that “it’s all about prevention
from diseases like diabetes, cancer, and
heart disease,”
28
and that you “won’t feel
as tired from a hectic day of living.”
30
These arguments could be described as
“strong-argument” messages and so may
not have resulted in many thoughts (ei-
ther positive or negative) regarding exer-
cise. It should be noted that participants
in Rosen’s
38
study read the messages and
so may have had more opportunity for
processing the information than did par-
ticipants in the present study, who saw
televised advertisements. Rosen con-
cluded that “one approach when targeting
people with low readiness for change is
using messages that require little pro-
cessing, such as imagery or paired asso-
ciations. Images of attractive people with
gym membership are examples of such
an approach” (p 178). In the present study
“images of attractive people with gym
memberships” had no effect on changing
attitudes.
Limitations in the present study in-
clude a convenience sample of students
enrolled in introductory psychology
classes. Results of a meta-analysis showed
that studies that had passive recruit-
ment, as in the present study, had fewer
precontemplators than did studies that
were randomly sampled.
19
Another limi-
tation is that the advertisements used in
this study included images of both attrac-
tive women and men, and so conclusions
are necessarily limited as to whether it
was the images of men, women, or both
that resulted in a decrease in self-effi-
cacy for men. Further, it is possible that
participants had previously been exposed
to the neutral or appearance advertise-
ments through regular television watch-
ing. It is less likely that participants
would have previously seen the health
advertisements because the promotion
campaigns were either from different
geographical areas than the one in which
the study took place, or the promotion
campaign had finished more than 2 years
prior to the study’s starting. A final limi-
tation is that there was no measure of
advertisement recall. It may be that dif-
ferent types of advertising are recalled
differently or processed differently. Fu-
Berry & Howe
Am J Health Behav.
2005;29(2):117-126
125
ture research should address these limi-
tations.
CONCLUSION
It has been claimed that “boys learn to
view their bodies as a tool to master the
environment whereas girls learn that
their bodies should be used to attract
others.”
39
This may be true, but the re-
sults of this study highlight that men are
susceptible to mass media messages and
that perceived pressure to be strong and
muscular may be detrimental. Bandura
20
has identified that cultural modeling of
gender-role stereotypes is an influence
on self-efficacy and that people who watch
more television hold stronger gender-role
stereotypes than do people who watch less
television. Further, television images
have been identified as a major influence
on mental well-being and self-esteem.
40
The results of the present study showed
that appearance-based images of exer-
cisers can indeed have a negative influ-
ence on exercise self-efficacy in men.
These results may have strong implica-
tions because self-efficacy has been con-
sistently shown to increase across stages
of behavior change;
25
therefore, advertis-
ing that decreases self-efficacy may re-
sult in less actual exercise behavior.
These results have implications for
health promoters who are trying to in-
crease exercise participation. Although
Dishman
4
recommended that health and
body-image approaches be melded, the
results of this study would indicate that
this suggestion is premature. Health-
based exercise advertising did not have
any effect in the present study, whereas
appearance-based advertising had a nega-
tive influence. Slater
14
identified that a
challenge to health promoters is the nega-
tive modeling of health behaviors that is
evident in much entertainment and ad-
vertising media. This research provides
evidence for one way in which health may
be negatively influenced by entertain-
ment media images. With many North
Americans not engaging in adequate
amounts of physical activity, the re-
sults of this study highlight an impor-
tant health issue that needs to be fur-
ther examined.
REFERENCES
1.Blair SN, Brodney S. Effects of physical inac-
tivity and obesity on morbidity and mortality:
current evidence and research issues. Med Sci
Sports Exerc. 1999;31(11 Suppl):S646-S662.
2.U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Preven-
tion, 1996. Physical Activity and Health: A
Report of the Surgeon General (on-line). Avail-
able at: http://www.cdc.gov/nccdphp/sgr/
adults.htm. Accessed November 28, 2002.
3.Canadian Fitness and Lifestyle Research In-
stitute, 2001. 2001 Physical Activity Monitor
(on-line). Available at: http:.//www.cflri.ca/
cflri/pa/surveys/2001survey/
2001survey.html. Accessed April 3, 2003.
4.Dishman RK. The problem of exercise adher-
ence: fighting sloth in nations with market
economies. Quest. 2001;53(3):279-294.
5.Davis C, Fox J, Brewer H, et al. Motivations to
exercise as a function of personality charac-
teristics, age, and gender. Personality and
Individual Differences. 1995;19(2):165-174.
6.Gill K, Overdorf V. Incentives for exercise in
younger and older women. Journal of Sport
Behavior. 1994;17(2):87-97.
7.Lavine H, Sweeney D, Wagner SH. Depicting
women as sex objects in television advertis-
ing: effects on body dissatisfaction. Personal-
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin.
1999;25(8):1049-1058.
8.Brooks CM. Sport/exercise identity theory
and participation marketing: theory formation
and theoretical justification. Sport Marketing
Quarterly. 1998;7(1):38-47.
9.Marcus BH, Owen N, Forsyth LH, et al. Physi-
cal activity interventions using mass media,
print media, and information technology. Am
J Prev Med. 1998;15(4):362-378.
10.Cavill N. National campaigns to promote physi-
cal activity: can they make a difference?
International Journal of Obesity.1998;22(Suppl
2):S48-S51.
11.Berry TR, Howe BL. Effects of health-based
and appearance-based exercise advertising
on exercise attitudes, social physique anxi-
ety, and self-presentation in an exercise set-
ting. Social Behavior and Personality.
2004;32(1):1-12.
12.Rhodes RE, Courneya KS. Effects of a health-
based versus appearance based persuasive
message on attitudes towards exercise: test-
ing the moderating role of self-monitoring.
Journal of Social Behavior and Personality.
2001;15:321-330.
13.Prochaska JO. Strong and weak principles
for progressing from precontemplation to ac-
tion on the basis of twelve problem behaviors.
Health Psychol. 1994;13(1):47-51.
14.Slater MD. Integrating application of media
effects, persuasion and behavior change theo-
ries to communication campaigns: a stages-of-
change framework. Health Comm.
1999;11(4):335-354.
15.Prochaska JO, Norcross JC. Systems of Psy-
chotherapy. A Transtheoretical Analysis (3
rd
Ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole,
1994:453-492.
16.Rodgers WM, Courneya KS, Bayduza AL. Ex-
amination of the transtheoretical model and
Effects of Exercise
126
exercise in 3 populations. Am J Health Behav.
2001;25(1):33-41.
17.Schumann A, Nigg CR, Rossi JS, et al. Con-
struct validity of the stages of change of
exercise adoption for different intensities of
physical activity in 4 samples of differing age
groups. Am J Health Promot. 2002;16(5):280-
287.
18.Biddle SJH, Nigg CR. Theories of exercise
behavior. International Journal of Sport Psychol-
ogy. 2000;31(2):290-304.
19.Marshall SJ, Biddle SJH. The transtheoretical
model of behavior change: a meta-analysis of
applications to physical activity and exercise.
Ann Behav Med. 2001;23(4):229-246.
20.Bandura A. Self-efficacy: The Exercise of
Control. New York: W. H. Freeman, 1997:1-
604.
21.Wakefield M, Flay B, Nichter M, et al. Role of
the media in influencing trajectories of youth
smoking. Addiction. 2003;98(Suppl 1):79-103.
22.Lewis BA, Marcus BH, Pate RR, et al. Psycho-
social mediators of physical activity behavior
among adults and children. Am J Prev Med.
2002;23(Suppl 2):26-35.
23.Renger R, Steinfelt V, Lazarus S. Assessing
the effectiveness of a community-based media
campaign targeting physical inactivity. Fam
Community Health. 2002;25(3):18-30.
24.Cancer Prevention Research Center (on-line).
Available at: http://www.uri.edu/research/
cprc/measures.htm#Exercise. Accessed June
12, 2002.
25.Marcus BH, Simkin LR. The stages of exercise
behavior. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. 1993;33:83-
88.
26.Reed GR, Velicer WF, Prochaska JO, et al.
What makes a good staging algorithm: ex-
amples from regular exercise. Am J Health
Promot. 1997;12(1):57-66.
27.Plotnikoff RC, Blanchard C, Hotz SB, et al.
Validation of the decisional balance scales in
the exercise domain from the Transtheoreti-
cal model: a longitudinal test. Measurement in
Physical Education and Exercise Science.
2001;5(4):191-206.
28.Participation (Producer). Physical Activity: the
best investment you’ll ever make [Advertise-
ment]. 2001 Available: Participaction, 6-2400
St. W. Suite 726, Mississauga, Ontario, L5K
2R8.
29.Hillary Commission (Producer). Push Play
Campaign. [Advertisement]. 2001 Available:
Sport & Recreation New Zealand, Level 4, 78
Victoria Street, PO Box 2251,Wellington
New Zealand.
30.Dairy Nutrition Council of Alberta (Producer).
The nutrition file [Advertisement]. 2001 Avail-
able: Dairy Nutrition Council of Alberta.
31.Braver MCW, Braver SL. Statistical treatment
of the Solomon four-group design: a meta-
analytic approach. Psychol Bull.
1988;104(1):150-154.
32.Huck SW, Sandler HM. A note on the Solomon
4-group design: appropriate statistical analy-
ses. Journal of Experimental Education.
1973;42(2):54-55.
33.Leit RA, Pope JJ, Gray HG. The media’s
representation of the ideal male body: a case
for muscle dysmorphia? Int J Eat Disord. 2002;
31(3); 334-338.
34.Morry MM, Staska SL. Magazine exposure:
internalization, self-objectification, eating at-
titudes, and body satisfaction in male and
female university students. Canadian Journal
of Behavioral Science. 2001;33(4):269-279.
35.Berry T, Lauzon L. A content analysis of
fitness magazines. Avante. 2003;9(1):25-33.
36.Vartanian LR, Giant CL, Passino RM. Ally
McBeal vs. Arnold Schwarzenegger: Compar-
ing mass media, interpersonal feedback and
gender as predictors of satisfaction with body
thinness and muscularity. Social Behavior and
Personality. 2001;29(7):711-724.
37.Bauman AE, Bellew B, Owen N. Impact of an
Australian mass media campaign targeting
physical activity in 1998. Am J Prev Med.
2001;21(1):41-47.
38.Rosen CR. Integrating stage and continuum
models to explain processing of exercise mes-
sages and exercise initiation among seden-
tary college students. Health Psychol.
2000;19(2):172-180.
39.Groesz LM, Levine MP, Murnen SK. The effect
of experimental presentation of thin media
images on body satisfaction: a meta-analytic
review. Int J Eat Disord. 2002;31(1):1-16.
40.Signorielli N. Mass Media Images and Impact
on Health: A Sourcebook. Westport, CT: Green-
wood Press, 1993:1-28.
... Research indicates that fitness advertisements tend to project two distinct slogan tactics for their gyms: one emphasizing health and wellness and the other focusing on appearance and aesthetics (Berry & Howe, 2005;Cunningham & Woods, 2011;Cunningham et al., 2008;Hadfield et al., 2022). In this study, and similar to Cunningham and Woods (2011), the physiquebased slogans focus on persuading audience members that, if they were to join the gym, they could acquire a more toned, lean, and defined body-type. ...
... In the realm of fitness advertisements, the application of signaling theory is especially pertinent as individuals contemplating gym membership often form judgments and make inferences about a fitness organization based on the information presented in its advertisement (Cunningham & Woods, 2011). These inferences significantly influence their decision-making process regarding whether they perceive a specific gym as suitable for their needs and thus decide to join (Berry & Howe, 2005; Cunningham & Woods, 2011). As described above, model body-type and slogan-type featured in Instagram gym advertisements can signal to audiences whether the gym is a good fit (Cunningham & Woods, 2011), which has been shown to lead to message effectiveness and persuasive outcomes (Stice et al., 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous health communication research has demonstrated the negative psychological and health effects of depicting thin-sized models in mass media advertisements including on social media sites such as Instagram. However, gym advertisements are one common source for the presentation of lean and thin-sized models on Instagram. Therefore, the current study guided by social comparison theory and signaling theory aimed to experimentally examine the effect of thin-sized models relative to plus-sized models as well as slogan-type (health and wellness versus physique-based) on women’s appearance comparison, body satisfaction, perceived gym fit, and intentions to join the gym. A sample of 217 undergraduate students who identified as women were randomly assigned to one of four Instagram gym advertisement conditions varying in model body-size and slogan-type. Appearance comparisons, perceived gym fit, and intentions to join the gym were measured post advertisement exposure and body satisfaction was measured pre-and-post advertisement exposure. As expected, exposure to Instagram gym advertisements featuring thin-sized models resulted in greater appearance comparisons and lower body satisfaction than exposure to Instagram gym advertisements featuring plus-sized models. Moreover, the combination of plus-sized models with health and wellness slogans in Instagram gym advertisements resulted in greater gym fit perceptions although there was no effect of model body-size and slogan-type on intentions to join the gym. This study supports social comparison theory, signaling theory, and practically the findings indicate that Instagram gym advertisements that depict plus-sized models (versus thin) with health-and-wellness slogans (versus physique) generate fewer body image concerns and lead to greater gym fit perceptions.
... These findings are also relevant to the current discussion of fitness clubs, as potential fitness consumers are likely to draw inferences concerning the club based on the information provided to them. Berry and Howe (2002) suggested that clubs are likely to convey one of two images: a focus on health and wellness or a focus on appearance. Thus, they are likely to espouse these values in their external communications, including advertisements and other promotional materials. ...
... Results from our study suggest that although fitness clubs commonly emphasize how exercise potentially impacts one's appearance (Berry & Howe, 2002), this practice has limited effectiveness. On the other hand, the emphasis on health and wellness was strongly associated with consumer beliefs that their values matched those of the fitness club. ...
... This is because ATT plays an essential role in mediating it (Wardana et al., 2020) towards the people's mindset. The other path is advertising value (ADV) to self-efficacy (SEF) because of its decisional balance for changing exercise behavior (Berry & Howe, 2005). Hence, we have the following hypotheses: H1: "ATT contributes positively on SEF" H2: "ADV contributes positively on SEF" ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose. The paper aims to explain a proposal framework composed of social media advertising value (SMAV) and self-efficacy (SEF) constructs with complementary factors such as risk and protection perception, sentiment, and motivations in users under chronic and pandemic disease environments. Methodology. SMAV-SEF literature review with additional factors in a theoretical questionnaire survey to 500 regular consumers (Sep-Nov, 2021). Results. Unexpected null relationship of SEF to people sentiment and positive contribution to risk perception. Originality' is based on the SMAV-SEF as the main constructs to be complemented with factors of a final framework explaining how to be used against chronic and pandemic diseases.
... 46 Appearance and well-being frames have been assessed in relation to eating disorders and sports psychology among young adult women. [47][48][49][50] An appearance-framed message focuses on looking better physically, while a well-being frame would be a message that focuses on feeling better overall. ...
Article
Objective: This study explores college women’s beliefs and experiences about exercise informed by different framing strategies, and how they may influence exercise engagement. Participants: Four focus groups were conducted with 19 undergraduate women at a large public Midwestern university. Methods: Four differently framed group exercise advertisements were used to prompt focus group discussions on beliefs and intentions to exercise. Transcribed data were analyzed with a thematic content approach. Results: Undergraduate female participants described more positive beliefs that influence intention with a well-being-framed advertisement. More negative beliefs were described when the advertisement focused on enhancing appearance. Participants of color believed that seeing more representation of diversity in advertisements would make engaging in exercise easier. Conclusion: Positive beliefs linked to a “well-being” frame and diverse imagery of women in exercise advertisements may improve intention to exercise, increasing physical activity among women.
... This temporal nature of change readiness has been empirically documented across dozens of health behaviors among adolescents and adults. Some of those studies include unidimensional change such as increasing exercise (Berry & Howe, 2005;De Bourdeaudhuij et al., 2005), smoking cessation (Bledsoe, 2006), and condom use (Grossman et al., 2008;Prat et al., 2012). Others include multidimensional change such as the component behaviors of depression management (Levesque et al., 2011), living with heart disease (Eshah, 2019), and advance care planning (Fried et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The aim of this study was to document the behaviors that adults who stutter (AWS) may engage in to make positive changes to living with stuttering. Method We interviewed 23 key stakeholders, including 11 AWS and 12 speech-language pathologists who specialize in stuttering therapy. The semi-structured interviews began with the primary question, “If an adult who stutters was making positive changes to living with stuttering, what would they be doing?” Follow-up probing questions focused the interviews on identifying actionable behaviors that would suggest positive changes. The interviews were transcribed and qualitatively analyzed using applied and reflexive thematic analyses to develop multilevel themes. Results Meaningful units extracted from the interviews contributed to three high-order global themes: (a) noticing and adjusting physical behaviors involved in speaking, to the extent that it is personally important to do so; (b) developing neutral or positive thoughts and feelings about stuttering; and (c) participating more fully in social and professional activities, even if the person stutters or thinks they might stutter. We developed 35 low-order basic themes, which we grouped into 11 mid-order organizing themes, to richly illustrate the three global themes. Conclusions These findings extend the ongoing discussion regarding best practices for therapy targets in stuttering intervention. We identified measurable, multidimensional actions that clinicians can integrate in their therapy plans with AWS. While these actions represent a holistic approach to making positive changes, it grants clients and clinicians space to develop individualized intentions and outcomes.
... Appearance and well-being are socially constructed concepts present in studies ranging in objectives (Aubrey, 2010;Berry & Howe, 2005;Sabiston & Chandler, 2010;Willis & Knobloch-Westerwick, 2014). An appearance frame focuses on looking better physically, while a well-being frame refers to a message that focuses on feeling better overall. ...
Article
Full-text available
The overall health benefits of physical activity are well known, yet females are less physically active than men overall. To examine how to increase female physical activity levels, the effect of four advertisement frames (communal, agentic, appearance, and well-being) on young adult females' intention to attend group exercise classes were investigated. 177 undergraduate females reported their level of intention to attend group exercise for each of the differently framed advertisements. A linear mixed model with random effect for subject analysis found a well-being frame generated significantly more positive intention scores while an appearance frame resulted in significantly more negative intention scores to engage in group exercise. The results indicate the importance of using well-being frames and avoiding appearance-based frames in communication designed to increase young adult female intention to engage in physical activity.
... For instance, it decreases women's achievement motives and ambition (Geis et al., 1984), attitudes to involvement in politics and self-esteem (Schwarz et al., 1987), performance on math tests (Davies et al., 2002), interest in quantitative domains compared to verbal ones (Davies et al., 2002;Van Loo and Rydell, 2014), preference for leadership roles (Simon and Hoyt, 2013) compared to problem solver roles (Davies, Spencer and Steele, 2005), as well as non-traditional job choices (Gaucher, Friesen and Kay, 2011). Men too were shown to respond with lowered self-efficacy having seen appearance-based real exercise commercials (Berry and Howe, 2005) and higher sexism having seen gender stereotypy in advertising (Rudman and Borgida, 1995), especially if their attitudes were moderate to start with (Garst and Bodenhausen, 1997). ...
... For example, previous studies demonstrated that self-efficacy can be enhanced by health promotion advertising (Lewis, Marcus, Pate, & Dunn, 2002;Renger, Steinfelt, & Lazarus, 2002). Further, Berry and Howe (2005) concluded that self-efficacy has support as a mediator between exercise intervention promotions, such as public service announcements, and behaviour. Bandura explains that perceived self-efficacy can be readily changed by relevant modelling when people have had little prior experience on which to base evaluations of their personal competence. ...
Article
Previous academic research into how consumers evaluate advocacy advertising identified many possible paths involving potentially reflexive effects on how people perceive an advocacy advertising sponsor, the advocated issue and themselves. This paper has examined one possible scenario within this complicated phenomenon: that of advertising advocating a specific environmental consumer action, recycling. In the specific context of this study, structural equation modelling demonstrated clear causal relationships among consumer perceptions of the recycling advertisements’ sponsoring organization, consumer self-efficacy and perceived consumer effectiveness of complying with the advocated issue (recycling behaviour). These factors were shown to impact specific advocacy advertising goals (termed message effectiveness in this study) such as behavioural intention toward the advocated recycling issue and perceived changes in how consumers evaluate the sponsoring organization.
Article
Full-text available
We tested the effects of advertisements about a fictitious exercise class—derived using the theoretical constructs of agency and communion—on recipients' perceptions about, and interest in, the class. The final sample consisted of 150 adults (M age = 44.69, SD = 15.83). Results revealed that participants who received a communal-oriented message reported significantly greater exercise task self-efficacy and more positive affective attitudes relative to those who received an agentic-oriented message. Communal (relative to agentic) messages were also indirectly responsible for greater intentions to attend the class, via more positive self-efficacy beliefs and affective attitudes. These findings were obtained despite the use of another manipulation to orient participants to either agency or communion goals. The results indicate that the primacy of communion over agency for message recipients may extend to exercise settings and may occur irrespective of whether participants are situationally oriented toward agency or communion.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of a health-based versus appearance-based persuasive message on exercise attitudes and intentions and to determine if these effects were moderated by the personality trait of self-monitoring. Two hundred and seven undergraduate university students completed the self-monitoring questionnaire and read persuasive messages of either physical appearance advantages or health benefits of exercise before completing measures of exercise attitude and intention. The results of the 2 (self-monitoring) × 2 (message-type) MANOVA on the dependent variables of attitude and intention was significant (p<.05). Post-hoc analysis found that low self-monitoring individuals responded with significantly stronger attitude scores from exposure to the health-based message than the appearance-based message. Further, the health-based message produced significantly higher attitude scores among low self-monitors than high self-monitors. These results suggest interventions utilizing exercise motives may require consideration of a participant's propensity to self-monitor.
Article
Full-text available
A central problem in the planning of communication campaigns to change health behaviors is how to identify and apply appropriate communication, persuasion, and behavior change theories to overcome obstacles to behavior change. The stages-of-change model (Prochaska, DiClemente, & Norcross, 1992) provides a framework for integrating theories of media effects, such as agenda setting and multistep flow; theories of persuasion, such as the elaboration likelihood model and protection motivation theory; and theories of behavior change, such as the theory of reasoned action, social cognitive theory, and attitude accessibility, for communication campaign purposes. Implications for audience segmentation, selection of objectives, campaign strategy, and message design are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the validity and reliability of decisional balance scales for exercise (i.e., pros and cons) in a large longitudinal population-based randomized sample of Canadian adults ages 18 to 65 years (N = 703). Assessments were taken over 3 time points with 6-month intervals between testing. Content, factorial, concurrent, and construct validity along with internal consistency and test-retest reliability were established for the decisional balance scales. The developed measures have utility for researchers and practitioners who test and apply the exercise decisional balance constructs of the Transtheoretical Model.
Article
Full-text available
The public sector uses health as the primary motivator in physical activity campaigns whereas the private sector often uses appearance as a motivator, but whether these motivators can be successful in changing physical activity behavior remains to be determined. The purpose of this research was to test the effects of televised health promotion and appearance-based exercise advertising on exercise attitudes, social physique anxiety, and self-presentation. Participants completed pretest questionnaires one week prior to viewing a video that contained health or appearance exercise advertising. They then completed posttest questionnaires. Results showed that health-based advertising had significant positive effects on social physique anxiety and self-presentation for exercisers in the health condition, while appearance-based advertising had negative effects on nonexercisers' attitudes towards exercise.
Article
In analyzing the data associated with a Solomon Four-Group Design, the posttest scores are initially subjected to a 2x2 factorial ANOVA, with the two main effects being a.) pretest versus no pretest and b.) treatment versus no treatment. Campbell and Stanley (2) maintain that if this analysis yields non-significant F-ratios for both the main effect of pretesting and the pretesting-treatment interaction, it might be advantageous to reanalyze the data from the two pretested groups with an analysis of covariance (using pretest and posttest scores as the covariate and criterion variables, respectively). Assuming a high pretest-posttest correlation, the more powerful covariance might pick up a significant treatment effect which was not found in the initial analysis. Although Campbell and Stanley were correct in noting that this use of covariance must be preceeded by a non-significant pretesting-treatment interaction, the present authors argue that the covariance analysis is completely valid even if there is a true main effect for pretesting. A second point of this paper involves a recommendation for data analysis if the interaction from the initial two-way ANOVA does, in fact, turn out to be significant.
Article
In analyzing the data associated with a Solomon Four-Group Design, the posttest scores are initially subjected to a 2x2 factorial ANOVA, with the two main effects being a.) pretest versus no pretest and b.) treatment versus no treatment. Campbell and Stanley (2) maintain that if this analysis yields non-significant F-ratios for both the main effect of pretesting and the pretesting-treatment interaction, it might be advantageous to reanalyze the data from the two pretested groups with an analysis of covariance (using pretest and posttest scores as the covariate and criterion variables, respectively). Assuming a high pretest-posttest correlation, the more powerful covariance might pick up a significant treatment effect which was not found in the initial analysis. Although Campbell and Stanley were correct in noting that this use of covariance must be preceeded by a non-significant pretesting-treatment interaction, the present authors argue that the covariance analysis is completely valid even if there is a true main effect for pretesting. A second point of this paper involves a recommendation for data analysis if the interaction from the initial two-way ANOVA does, in fact, turn out to be significant.
Article
This study examined whether exposure to TV ads that portray women as sex objects causes increased body dissatisfaction among women and men. Participants were exposed to 15 sexist and 5 nonsexist ads, 20 nonsexist ads, or a no ad control condi- tion. Results revealed that women exposed to sexist ads judged their current body size as larger and revealed a larger discrepancy between their actual and ideal body sizes (preferring a thinner body) than women exposed to the nonsexist or no ad condition. Men exposed to the sexist ads judged their current body size as thinner, revealed a larger discrepancy between their actual and ideal body size (preferring a larger body), and revealed a larger discrepancy between their own ideal body size and their percep- tions of others' male body size preferences (believing that others preferred a larger ideal) than men exposed to the nonsexist or no ad condition. Discussion focuses on the cognitive, attitudinal, and behavioral consequences of exposure to gender stereotypic television advertising.
Article
Susceptibility to appearance-related mass media, interpersonal feedback, and instrumentality were compared as predictors of body satisfaction and shape concerns regarding thinness and muscularity in a sample of 287 undergraduate men and women. Results indicate that both genders experience body dissatisfaction, but along different shape dimensions; susceptibility to appearance-related mass media and negative appearance-related feedback appear to operate in similar ways to affect men's and women's overall body satisfaction. Directions for additional research are discussed.