ArticlePDF Available

The Influence of Writing Practice on Letter Recognition in Preschool Children: A Comparison between Handwriting and Typing

Authors:
  • French National Centre for Scientific Research; Marseille

Abstract and Figures

A large body of data supports the view that movement plays a crucial role in letter representation and suggests that handwriting contributes to the visual recognition of letters. If so, changing the motor conditions while children are learning to write by using a method based on typing instead of handwriting should affect their subsequent letter recognition performances. In order to test this hypothesis, we trained two groups of 38 children (aged 3-5 years) to copy letters of the alphabet either by hand or by typing them. After three weeks of learning, we ran two recognition tests, one week apart, to compare the letter recognition performances of the two groups. The results showed that in the older children, the handwriting training gave rise to a better letter recognition than the typing training.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The influence of writing practice on
letter recognition in preschool children:
A comparison between handwriting and typing
Marieke Longcamp
a
, Marie-The
´re
`se Zerbato-Poudou
b
,
Jean-Luc Velay
a,*
a
Institut de Neurosciences Cognitives de la Me
´diterrane
´e, UMR CNRS 6193,
31 chemin Joseph Aiguier, 13402 Marseille cedex 20, France
b
Institut Universitaire de Formation des Maı
ˆtres (IUFM), Universite
´de Provence, Marseille, France
Received 4 August 2004; received in revised form 27 October 2004; accepted 29 October 2004
Available online 4 January 2005
Abstract
A large body of data supports the view that movement plays a crucial role in letter repre-
sentation and suggests that handwriting contributes to the visual recognition of letters. If so,
changing the motor conditions while children are learning to write by using a method based on
typing instead of handwriting should affect their subsequent letter recognition performances.
In order to test this hypothesis, we trained two groups of 38 children (aged 3–5 years) to copy
letters of the alphabet either by hand or by typing them. After three weeks of learning, we ran
two recognition tests, one week apart, to compare the letter recognition performances of the
two groups. The results showed that in the older children, the handwriting training gave rise to
a better letter recognition than the typing training.
Ó2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
PsycINFO classification: 2330; 2343
Keywords: Children; Handwriting; Typing; Learning; Letter recognition; Motor–perceptual interactions
0001-6918/$ - see front matter Ó2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.10.019
*
Corresponding author. Tel.: +33 4 91 16 42 39; fax: +33 4 91 16 42 96.
E-mail address: velay@incm.cnrs-mrs.fr (J.-L. Velay).
Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79
www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy
1. Introduction
Nowadays, most adults probably write using a computer. Intensive use of word
processing programs and the handy tools they contain (copy/paste, automatic spell-
ing checks, etc.) is likely to modify high-level cognitive functions such as text com-
position (see Cochran-Smith, 1991). The present study deals with a more basic
aspect of computer writing: the effects of dramatic motor changes resulting from
the use of a keyboard instead of a pen. Computers are now being increasingly used
at school, even at the preschool level, by very young children. If children happen to
learn to write with a keyboard before they master handwriting, will this affect the
way they perceive written language?
The idea that our movements organize our perceptions and contribute to setting
up our spatial representations is not new and has by now become widely recog-
nized (see Viviani, 2002). Some of the properties of objects (such as their shape,
color and size) are perceived via the visual channel and others (such as their texture
and temperature) by touch; all the diverse sensory inputs involved are combined
together in time and space via active manipulatory movements, which also add
their own information (weight, size, etc.). During childhood, we learn to associate
actions with their correlated perceptions in order to build up unified, coherent rep-
resentations of objects. Once the neural network underlying a given representation
has been structured, any one of the inputs which was initially present suffices to
reactivate the whole network (Martin, Ungerleider, & Haxby, 2000; Pulvermu
¨ller,
1999). The existence of these motor-perceptual links has been observed with neu-
roimaging techniques in humans; in particular, the visual presentation of pictures
of objects, to which can be attributed a specific action, activated a premotor cor-
tical area, even when no actual motor response was required (Chao & Martin,
2000). Furthermore, a growing body of lesion study data (e.g. Sirigu, Duhamel,
& Poncet, 1991) suggests that sensorimotor knowledge about the functional prop-
erties of manipulatable objects is part of their representation, and can be used to
recognize or name them (Martin et al., 2000). These motor-perceptual interactions
involve associations of objects with potential actions: this is clearly what occurs in
the case of tools.
Although alphabetic characters are not graspable objects, motor-perceptual links
presumably contribute to their representation, since they are associated with highly
specific writing movements. The fact that inability to write letters can be associated
with reading deficits, due to an impaired ability to identify letters visually, is consis-
tent with the existence of a tight coupling between the visual and sensorimotor per-
ception of letter shapes (Anderson, Damasio, & Damasio, 1990). In addition, it has
been established that writing movements can help subjects whose reading abilities are
impaired: for instance, patients with pure alexia, who were no longer able to recog-
nize letters visually, sometimes succeeded in doing so when they were asked to trace
the outline of the letters with their fingers (Bartolomeo, Bachoud-Le
´vi, Chokron, &
Degos, 2002; Seki, Yajima, & Sugishita, 1995). Handwriting movements may there-
fore somehow activate the visual representation of letters. The writing order of the
numerous strokes composing ideograms is used as a cue to retrieve them from mem-
68 M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79
ory (Flores dÕArcais, 1994), which suggests that the motor scheme specific to each
ideogram may be an essential component of its representation. This idea has been
supported by neuroimaging studies on Japanese subjects who showed motor activa-
tion while looking at ideograms (Matsuo et al., 2001). Similarly, Longcamp, Anton,
Roth, and Velay (2003) reported that, the simple visual presentation of Roman char-
acters activated a premotor zone in the left hemisphere in right-handed subjects, even
though no motor response was required. The activation of the corresponding area in
the opposite hemisphere of left-handed subjects confirmed that this visually induced
activation depends on the writing hand (Longcamp, Anton, Roth, & Velay, submit-
ted for publication).
These various data converge to indicate that the cerebral representation of letters
might not be strictly visual, but might be based on a complex neural network includ-
ing a sensorimotor component acquired while learning concomitantly to read and
write. Close functional relationships between the reading and writing processes
might occur at a basic level, in addition to the interactions that have been described
at a more cognitive level (Fitzgerald & Shanahan, 2000).
However, the existence of a sensorimotor component of this kind does not neces-
sarily mean that it is involved in identifying letters. Nevertheless, there is some evi-
dence which strongly suggests that writing movements are involved in letter
memorization. For instance, repeated writing is an aid that is commonly used to help
Japanese children memorize ideograms. In the same vein, Japanese adults often re-
port that they write with their finger in the air to identify complex characters. In fact,
it has been reported that learning by writing facilitated subjectsÕmemorization of
graphic forms but not that of ideograms, words or syllables (Naka & Naoi, 1995).
This effect was stronger when the forms were freely recalled in writing, but visual rec-
ognition of graphic designs was also enhanced by writing. The results of a subse-
quent study by Naka (1998) confirmed the positive effect of writing training on
free recall of graphic designs, but no such effect was observed on visual recognition.
Visual recognition was also studied by Hulme (1979), who compared childrenÕs
learning of a series of abstract graphic forms, depending on whether they simply
looked at the forms or looked at them as well as tracing the forms with their index
finger. The tracing movements seemed to improve the childrenÕs memorization of the
graphic items; Thus, it was suggested that the visual and motor information might
undergo a common representation process.
There do exist some discrepancies between the results of the studies devoted to the
effects of motor activity on letter memorization. Some authors have examined
whether the graphic movements involved in tracing or writing may enhance the
high-level cognitive processes involved in the acquisition of reading skills (see Gra-
ham & Weintraub, 1996 for a review). This was the case in the few studies in which
the respective advantages of learning by handwriting versus typewriting were com-
pared. In one study (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1990), children spelled words which
were learned by writing them by hand better than those learned by typing them on a
computer. However, subsequent studies did not confirm the advantage of the hand-
writing method (Vaughn, Schumm, & Gordon, 1992, 1993). The results obtained in
these studies showed that childrenÕs word writing and recognition performances were
M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79 69
not affected when they had used a typing method, and since the act of typing is
simpler than handwriting, typewriting was thought to constitute an efficient method
of teaching moderately mentally retarded students how to read and write (Calhoun,
1985).
We assume that the main process, if any, influenced by motor activity is likely to
be a spatial process, in the initial step in the recognition of written characters. In
other terms, writing movements may contribute to memorizing the shape and/or ori-
entation of characters. If this is so, changing the motor conditions present during
learning by using a typing method instead of a handwriting one will probably affect
the subjectsÕrepresentation of letters and hence their subsequent letter recognition
performances. We therefore studied the early letter learning process in very young
children (3–5 years) who had not yet begun to learn to read and write at school.
The two modes of learning, i.e. handwriting and typing, were compared between
two groups of children by testing their letter recognition performances. Since the
children who participated were very young and their age range was large (2 years),
and since they had obviously not all reached the same stage of cognitive and motor
development, we assumed that all the children would not benefit identically from the
learning. We therefore compared the learning modes as a function of the childrenÕs
age. Finally, it should be pointed out that most studies dealing with the effects of
writing movements on reading ability have focused on quite short retention intervals
(Cunningham & Stanovich, 1990; Vaughn et al., 1992 but see Vaughn, Schumm, &
Gordon, 1993). Yet as mentioned by Hulme (1979), in teaching children to read and
write, one is dealing with long term memory, since the information learned is re-
tained for periods of time. Two recognition tests were therefore carried out in the
present study: the first immediately following the learning phase (T1) and the second,
one week later (T2).
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
Seventy-six children, 41 boys and 35 girls, with a mean age of 3:10 years (46
months) and an age range of 2:9 (33 months) to 4:9 years old (57 months) partici-
pated in the experiment. They were tested in three classrooms at three different
preschools.
2.2. Procedure
2.2.1. Learning groups
All the children were first subjected to a battery of pre-tests. An adapted ver-
sion of the Bender–Gestalt test for children younger than 6 years old was used
to assess their perceptual-motor development. Manual dexterity was assessed using
a 9-hole pegboard in which the children had to insert nine cylindrical pieces as fast
as they could. The experimenter noted the time spent and the hand used. Manual
70 M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79
laterality was assessed using a simplified version of the Edinburgh Handedness
Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). In order to evaluate their initial level of letter knowl-
edge before learning, the children were submitted to a letter recognition pre-test.
This test was exactly the same as the test used after learning. The twelve uppercase
letters, which were to be learned after the pre-test, were used (B, C, D, E, F, G, J,
L, N, P, R, Z). The children were seated in front of a computer screen on which
four character-like patterns, including three distractors, were presented (Fig. 1).
Three distractors were used with each letter: the mirror image of the letter, a
transformed letter (with a stroke added or missing from the letter), and the mirror
image of the transformed letter. The instructions were: ‘‘Look carefully at the four
letters on the screen: only one of them is the ÔproperÕletter, which is the letter you
have learned with us. The other three are not correct. Show me this ÔproperÕletter
with your index finger!’’. No speed instructions were given. The childrenÕs re-
sponses were recorded by the experimenter on the keyboard of the computer used
for the test. Each of the four possible responses (upper left, upper right, lower left
and lower right) was associated to a given key of the keyboard. Each letter and its
distractors were presented twice in a random order (24 trials per session). The po-
sition of the four different letters and distractors on the screen varied randomly
across trials.
After the letter recognition pre-test, we computed a score for each child for the
number of correct responses (CR). In order to minimize the risk of getting a correct
response by chance, we considered that a letter was known only when two CR were
given in the two trials for this letter. This procedure reduced the number of CR but
allowed us to distinguish the children who really knew the letters from those who
gave random responses. The maximum score that could be scored in the test was
thus 12, corresponding to 24 CR. The results of the pre-test are given in Fig. 2.
On the basis of all the pre-tests, we divided the child sample into two learning
groups of 38 children which were matched in terms of age, sex, handedness, manual
dexterity, educational level and letter recognition level. In addition, the children con-
stituting both groups were equally distributed in each classroom in order to prevent
any Ôteacher effectÕfrom occurring. In each learning group, the age range was 24
months. The 38 children of each group were sorted by age and divided into three
equivalent sub-groups of 8-month ranges (see Table 1). Six groups were thus ob-
tained, depending on the method used during learning, and age: Handwriting-older,
Fig. 1. Two example of visual configurations displayed on the computer screen during the letter
recognition test.
M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79 71
Handwriting-middle, Handwriting-younger, Typing-older, Typing-middle and Typ-
ing-younger. In order to ensure that the two learning groups were not different with
regard to initial letter knowledge before the beginning of learning, the CR were ana-
lyzed by means of a two (Learning mode: Handwriting vs. Typing) by three (Age:
Older, Middle and. Younger) ANOVA. The results of the ANOVA showed that nei-
ther the main factors (Learning mode and Age) nor their interaction reached the sig-
nificance level. In particular, the slight difference between handwriting and typing in
the older children was far from significant (F(1, 70) < 1).
handwriting
typing
pre-test
younger
middle
older
0
1
2
3
4
5
Correct Responses
T1
younger
middle
older
T2
y
ounger
middle
older
Fig. 2. Correct responses produced by younger, middle-age and older children, in the three recognition
tests in the case of both learning methods. Solid line: handwriting group. Dashed line: typing group. T1:
recognition test run immediately at the end of the learning session. T2: recognition test run one week later.
Error bars denote 95% confidence intervals.
Table 1
Mean ages and mean scores in the pre-tests for each of the six groups
Group Children
number
Age
(months)
Laterality
quotient (%)
Bender
test score
Time spent for
peg-board (s)
CR
number
Handwriting Younger 13 38.5 76.2 4.41 30.6 0.8
Middle 12 45.4 66.5 5.92 27.7 1.3
Older 13 53.2 58.4 10.77 24.7 1.6
Typing Younger 13 38.1 66.7 4.33 29.6 0.8
Middle 13 45.0 77.1 6.84 25.2 1.2
Older 12 53.5 61.6 10.75 24.2 1.3
72 M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79
2.2.2. Learning
The aim of the experiment was for the children to implicitly learn the form of 12
uppercase letters by writing them. We used uppercase letters for several reasons:
first, they are the simplest letters to write and they were the first letters that were
taught at these schools, and second, the uppercase letters are what one sees on com-
puter keyboards. Furthermore, we chose these 12 letters because they are not sym-
metrical, so we could use their mirror image as a distractor in the recognition tests.
We did not want children to be trained with too many letters because we had just a
few weeks to carry out the training and the tests. In order to make the learning
more attractive, instead of teaching the 12 letters separately, they were included
in four words. To form words, we added three vowels (A, O, I), which were not
included in the recognition tests. The words were included in turn in a story that
was told to the children by the teachers for a few weeks prior to the learning period.
The four words were: LAPIN (rabbit), JOB (the rabbitÕs name in the story), CERF
(stag) and ZADIG (the stagÕs name in the story). Each letter to be learned was pres-
ent only once in the four words (L-P-N, J-B, CERF, Z-D-G). The learning period
lasted for 3 weeks, and consisted of one half-hour session per week. The children
were trained in groups of four, in presence of two experimenters, in a quiet class-
room. The parents were informed about the aim of the experiment and they were
asked not to have the children practice at home during the duration of the
experiment.
2.2.2.1. Typing training. Each word was displayed separately on the upper left side of
the computer screen, with 3 cm tall characters. The children were asked to look at
the letters constituting the word, find the appropriate letters on the keyboard, and
type each of them. No constraints were imposed regarding the order of the letters.
When the child typed a wrong letter, the experimenter informed him/her, cancelled
the wrong letter, and asked the child to find and type the correct letter. When all
the letters composing the target word had been typed, the experimenter displayed the
next word. The keyboard had been adapted for the purpose of the study: all the keys
other than the 15 keys required to type the words (plus the backspace key and
the carriage return bar for the experimenter) were removed from the keyboard.
The 15 remaining keys were arranged in the central part of the keyboard. The four
words were copied twice at each session, so the children typed each letter twice per
session.
2.2.2.2. Handwriting training. Each word was presented on the upper left side of a
piece of paper with 3 cm tall characters. The children were asked to copy the letters
of the word underneath the model with a felt-tip pen. The letters could be copied
anywhere on the paper and in any order. No constraints were imposed regarding
the size of the letters. Since we wanted both types of training to be as similar as
possible, we gave the same kind of feedback in handwriting and typing. In the typ-
ing training, the only errors that could occur were lack of a letter and confusion
between two letters. In handwriting, confusion was not possible but the chil-
dren could miscopy the letter. We did not correct the children when the form of
M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79 73
the written letter was not perfect. However, if a letter was not written, the experi-
menter informed the child that a letter was lacking and asked him/her to find the
lacking letter on the model and write it. When all the letters composing the target
word had been written, the experimenter replaced the worksheet with the following
one on which the next model word was presented. Each letter was copied twice at
each session.
2.2.3. Letter recognition tests
The letter recognition tests were exactly the same as the aforementioned test per-
formed before learning. A first test was run immediately at the end of the last learn-
ing session (T1) and repeated one week later (T2).
3. Results
The number of CR was computed for T1 and T2 as previously described for the
pre-test. The overall CR results are presented in Fig. 2. In order to analyze the im-
pact of learning on the letter recognition, the CR in the pre-test and in T1 were sub-
mitted to a two (Learning mode: handwriting vs. typing) by three (Age: older,
middle and younger) by two (Time of testing: pre vs. T1) ANOVA, with repeated
measures on the third factor. This first analysis showed that the number of CR
was significantly greater after learning (T1) than before (pre-test) (F(1, 70) = 11.1,
p< 0.002). The ÔTime of testingÕby ÔAgeÕinteraction was not significant
(F(2, 70) = 2.29, ns), but the increase of CR was only significant in the older
(F(1, 23) = 14.7, p< 0.001) and not in the middle (F(1, 23) = 1.95, ns) and younger
children (F(1, 24) < 1). Finally, in the older children, the increase in CR was signif-
icant in the handwriting group (F(1, 12) = 17.5, p< 0.005) but not in the typing
group (F(1, 11) = 1.76, ns).
In order to compare the performances of children immediately after learning (T1)
and one week later (T2), the CR were submitted to a two (Learning mode: handwrit-
ing vs. typing) by three (Age: older, middle and. younger) by two (Time of testing: T1
vs. T2) ANOVA, with repeated measures on the third factor. The analysis showed
that the number of CR was not the same at all ages (F(2, 70) = 9.71, p< 0.001):
the older children gave more CR than the middle (F(1, 46) = 6.51, p< 0.005) and
younger children (F(1, 47) = 18.3, p< 0.001). The performance of middle and youn-
ger children did not differ significantly (F(1, 47) = 2.92, ns).
As a whole, the children who wrote the letters gave more CR than those who
typed them, but the difference only marginally reached the significance level
(F(1, 70) = 3.86, p< 0.06). However, the ÔLearning modeÕby ÔAgeÕinteraction was
significant (F(1, 70) = 4.50, p< 0.02). As can be seen in Fig. 2, the older children
who used the handwriting method produced a larger number of CR than the same
age children who used the typing method (F(1, 23) = 7.35, p< 0.02). Conversely,
the performances showed no change with the learning method in the middle and
younger children. Finally, neither the time of testing nor the interactions implying
this factor reached the significance level.
74 M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79
4. Discussion
The aim of this experiment was to determine whether two different types of writ-
ing training could induce different letter memorization. As a whole, the level of learn-
ing was low, since only the older children showed an increase in CR between pre-test
and T1 and more precisely, only the older children who learned by handwriting.
Such a low effect of learning can be explained by several causes. First, we did not
explicitly teach the form of the letter because we wanted to compare two quite dif-
ferent writing methods and it was impossible to give comparable feedback in both
types of learning. In typing, the letter is displayed with its perfect shape and no feed-
back has to be given about the typing movement. Thus we did not give feedback in
the handwriting training to be as close as possible to the typing situation. In addi-
tion, we never directed the childrenÕs attention towards the exact form of the letter
they wrote. In consequence, the learning was implicit. Furthermore, the effective time
for learning was short (1.5 h within 3 weeks) with respect to the time usually required
for learning to write and read. A longer time would have been more efficient but it
was not possible to extend the teaching sessions. Despite this, the older children who
were trained by handwriting performed more successfully in the letter recognition
tests than those who were trained by typing.
In this debate about the importance of motor conditions when learning to read
and write, the results of the present study are in agreement with those showing that
writing letters facilitates their memorization and their subsequent recognition
(Hulme, 1979; Naka & Naoi, 1995). However, a negative result was obtained in a
study in which learning methods differing in their motor involvement were compared
in children (aged 3–6 years) (Courrieu & De Falco, 1989). In the motor situation,
instead of writing, the children had to trace over a picture of the letters where the
dynamics of the tracing movement were indicated graphically. After the training
phase, no effects of the motor involvement which had occurred during the learning
period were observed. Another negative result was obtained by Naka (1998), who
observed that an advantage of handwriting showed up only when the memorization
was tested by free writing recall, and not in simple visual recognition tests.
There are points that should be mentioned which may help to explain the advan-
tage of handwriting we observed. First, the subjects were very young: they were pre-
school children who had not had any previous experience of learning to read and
write before the experiment. We were hence able to examine the very first step in
the learning process, when the letters are still perceived as graphic forms without
any particular phonological meaning. During the learning phase, the children were
not required to learn the words they saw, and not even to name the letters, but only
to write or type them. Likewise, in the recognition test, we did not ask the children to
identify or spell words, but only to detect the letters among the distractors and to
point at them without any verbalization. The whole procedure was therefore focused
on a low level spatial processing of letters where sensorimotor signals might play a
crucial role.
Secondly, another difference in comparison with previous studies was that learn-
ing extended over a period of three weeks, whereas in other studies on this topic, the
M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79 75
training period generally consisted of a single session lasting anything from a few
minutes to an hour. However, motor performances are known to evolve slowly,
requiring many repetitions during several training sessions (Karni, 1996). The acqui-
sition of motor skills seems to involve two main stages: an early, fast learning stage
in which performances improve considerably as the result of a single training session,
and a later, slow learning stage in which further gains can occur during several ses-
sions (and even weeks) of practice (Ungerleider, Doyon, & Karni, 2002). A 3-week
learning period certainly seems to provide favorable conditions for the children to set
up a motor program for writing each letter and create the perceptual-motor links
with its visual form. Assuming that the motor program associated with a letter is
automatically reactivated when looking at it (Longcamp et al., 2003), this mecha-
nism might help to recognize letters among close graphic forms.
Handwriting training allowed the older children to improve their performance in
character recognition whereas the same training was not efficient in the children
younger than fifty months. Of course, several reasons might explain this age-related
difference: in particular, the lesser cognitive development of the younger children.
Another reason might be that the fine motor control involved in handwriting was
not mature enough in the younger children for them to produce the writing move-
ments exactly. The fact that the graphic performances of 4- to 6-year old children
have been found to correlate better with a motor score than with chronological
age (van Galen, 1980) confirms that motor development is a key factor in handwrit-
ing. It has been suggested that handwriting development may be characterized by in-
creased efficiency in inhibiting noise in the neuromotor and muscular system (van
Galen, Portier, Smits-Engelsman, & Schomaker, 1993). Therefore, failure to inhibit
the neuromotor noise might be the most likely cause of poor handwriting (Smits-
Engelsman & van Galen, 1997). For the same maturational reasons, the peripheral
kinaesthetic signals accompanying movements might be particularly noisy in youn-
ger children. The authors of studies on changes in kinaesthetic sensitivity with age
have claimed that more than 30% of all 5-year old children may be Ôkinaesthetically
ineptÕ(Laszlo & Broderick, 1991). Finally, the interhemispheric relationships in-
volved in visuo-motor coordination develop from 4 to 14 years of age, without
acquiring all the characteristics present in adults (Hay & Velay, 2003). Thus, at
the ages of the children who participated, a few months difference in age is undoubt-
edly crucial in terms of motor system maturation. In the younger children, the sen-
sorimotor signals associated with movements might be too noisy to generate a
correct sensorimotor representation of letters.
From the sensorimotor point of view, handwriting and typing are clearly two dis-
tinct ways of writing, and these writing methods may well involve distinct central
processes. The children who participated in this experiment did not type with both
hands as expert typists do, nor did they even use several digits. Actually, since they
had just one key to press at a time, they used their index finger, as most beginners do.
During the training period, in the case of both handwriting and typing, a hand move-
ment was therefore associated with the visual image of a given letter, but the two
movements performed were quite different. On the one hand, the handwriting learn-
ing method requires the writer to perform a movement that completely defines the
76 M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79
shape of the letter in order to build an internal model of the character. Once the
learning is completed, there exists a unique correspondence between a given printed
letter and the movement that is used to write this letter. On the other hand, typing is
also a complex form of spatial learning in which the beginner typist has to build a
cognitive map of the keyboard (see for instance Gentner, 1983; Logan, 1999). Learn-
ing typewriting consists in precisely locating a key in the keyboard and pressing it,
but since the trajectory depends on the location of the finger before it goes into ac-
tion, no specific relationship between the visual form of a letter and a given move-
ment is built. Moreover, there is nothing in this pointing movement which might
inform about the shape of the letters. In short, handwriting provides on-line signals
from several sources, including vision, motor commands, and kinaesthetic feedback,
which are closely linked and simultaneously distributed in time. No such spatio-tem-
poral pattern occurs in typewriting. In addition to the motor differences, there exist
other differences between the two writing methods. In particular, attentional differ-
ences are apparent, since learning to write a letter may require a deeper level of pro-
cessing than finding a letter on a keyboard. Typing, in contrast to handwriting,
inherently requires visual discrimination among letters in the process of key selec-
tion. These different aspects probably play a role in the implicit learning during both
writing methods.
Finally, we sought to determine whether the knowledge implicitly learned by writ-
ing practice was maintained after long delays; To this aim, we observed that, in the
handwriting group, the recognition performance at the end of training and after one
week were identical. This is in agreement with data showing that, once it has been
thoroughly learned and stabilized, motor memory can last for very long periods of
time without any further practice (Shadmehr & Brashers-Krug, 1997). It is clear that
much longer delays should have been tested because the learning process in question
here involves memory extending over much longer periods of time.
In conclusion, the present results indicate that, provided they are not too young,
handwriting learning helps children to memorize the form of a letter. Clearly, we
cannot tell whether this process plays a role in reading, where whole words are per-
ceived instead of isolated letters. Yet, as it seems to be widely accepted that letter rec-
ognition is the first stage in reading (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler,
2001), the way children perceive letters might indeed affect the way they read. Func-
tional links have been found to exist between global motor skills performance and
reading disabilities, in both children (Fawcett, Nicolson, & Dean, 1996) and adults
(Nicolson et al., 1999; Velay, Daffaure, Giraud, & Habib, 2002), and further research
is now required to be able to answer the question as to whether learning how to write
really helps children to learn how to read.
Acknowledgments
The research was funded by the French Research Ministry (A.C.I. ÔCognitiqueÕ
COG73). Marieke Longcamp was supported by a fellowship from the French Re-
search Ministry (A.C.I. ÔCognitiqueÕ). We thank M. Besson and J.C. Gilhodes for
M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79 77
critical comments on the manuscript and Reyna Leigh Gordon for revising the Eng-
lish. We are particularly grateful to the teachers of the three schools in Marseille
(Cha
ˆteau-Sec, Desautel and Valmante) who allowed us to work with their pupils
during school time.
References
Anderson, S. W., Damasio, A. R., & Damasio, H. (1990). Troubled letters but not numbers: Domain
specific cognitive impairments following focal damage in frontal cortex. Brain, 113, 749–766.
Bartolomeo, P., Bachoud-Le
´vi, A.-C., Chokron, S., & Degos, J.-D. (2002). Visually- and motor-based
knowledge of letters: Evidence from a pure alexic patient. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1363–1371.
Calhoun, M. L. (1985). Typing contrasted with handwriting in language arts instruction for moderately
mentally retarded students. Education and Training of the Mentally Retarded, 20, 48–52.
Chao, L. L., & Martin, A. (2000). Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream.
NeuroImage, 12, 478–484.
Cochran-Smith, M. (1991). Word processing and writing in elementary classrooms: A critical review of
related literature. Review of Educational Research, 61, 107–155.
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model
of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.
Courrieu, P., & De Falco, S. (1989). Segmental vs. dynamic analysis of letter shape by preschool children.
Current Psychology of Cognition, 9(2), 189–198.
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Early spelling acquisition: Writing beats the computer.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 159–162.
Fawcett, A. J., Nicolson, R. I., & Dean, P. (1996). Impaired performance of children with dyslexia on a
range of cerebellar tasks. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 259–283.
Fitzgerald, J., & Shanahan, T. (2000). Reading and writing relationships and their development.
Educational Psychologist, 35, 39–50.
Flores dÕArcais, G. B. (1994). Order of strokes writing as a cue for retrieval in reading Chinese characters.
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 6, 337–355.
Gentner, D. R. (1983). The acquisition of typewriting skill. Acta Psychologica, 54, 233–248.
Graham, S., & Weintraub, N. (1996). A review of handwriting research: Progress and prospects from 1980
to 1994. Educational Psychology Review, 8(1), 7–87.
Hay, L., & Velay, J. L. (2003). Interhemispheric relationships in 4- to 14-year-old children pointing to
lateral targets. Neuroreport, 14, 1041–1044.
Hulme, C. (1979). The interaction of visual and motor memory for graphic forms following tracing.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 249–261.
Karni, A. (1996). The acquisition of perceptual and motor skills: A memory system in the adult human
cortex. Cognitive Brain Research, 5, 39–48.
Laszlo, J. I., & Broderick, P. (1991). Drawing and handwriting difficulties: Reasons for and remediation of
dysfunction. In J. P. Wann, A. M. Wing, & N. Søvik (Eds.), The development of graphic skills
(pp. 259–280). London: Academic Press.
Logan, F. A. (1999). Errors in copy typewriting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 25, 1760–1773.
Longcamp, M., Anton, J. L., Roth, M., & Velay, J. L. (2003). Visual presentation of single letters activates
a premotor area involved in writing. NeuroImage, 19, 1492–1500.
Longcamp, M., Anton, J. L., Roth, M., & Velay, J. L. (submitted for publication). The premotor
activations in response to visually presented single letters depend on the hand used to write: A study on
left-handers.
Martin, A., Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Category specificity and the brain: The sensory/
motor model of semantic representations of objects. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive
neurosciences (pp. 1023–1036). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
78 M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79
Matsuo, K., Kato, C., Tanaka, S., Sugio, T., Matsuzawa, M., Inui, T., et al. (2001). Visual language and
handwriting movement: Functional magnetic resonance imaging at 3 T during generation of
ideographic characters. Brain Research Bulletin, 55, 549–554.
Naka, M. (1998). Repeated writing facilitates childrenÕs memory for pseudocharacters and foreign letters.
Memory and Cognition, 26, 804–809.
Naka, M., & Naoi, H. (1995). The effect of repeated writing on memory. Memory and Cognition, 23,
201–212.
Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., Berry, E. L., Jenkins, I. H., Dean, P., & Brooks, D. J. (1999). Association
of abnormal cerebellar activation with motor learning difficulties in dyslexic adults. Lancet, 353(9165),
1662–1667.
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.
Pulvermu
¨ller, F. (1999). Words in the brain language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 253–336.
Seki, K., Yajima, M., & Sugishita, M. (1995). The efficacy of kinesthetic reading treatment for pure alexia.
Neuropsychologia, 33, 595–609.
Shadmehr, R., & Brashers-Krug, T. (1997). Functional stages in the formation of human long-term motor
memory. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 409–419.
Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J. R., & Poncet, M. (1991). The role of sensorimotor experience in object recognition:
A case of multimodal agnosia. Brain, 114, 2555–2573.
Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., & van Galen, G. P. (1997). Dysgraphia in children: Lasting psychomotor
deficiency or transient developmental delay?. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 164–184.
Ungerleider, L. G., Doyon, J., & Karni, A. (2002). Imaging brain plasticity during motor skill learning.
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 78, 553–564.
van Galen, G. P. (1980). Handwriting and drawing: A two stage model of complex motor behavior. In G.
Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 567–578). Amsterdam: North Holland.
van Galen, G. P., Portier, S. J., Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M., & Schomaker, L. R. B. (1993). Neuromotor
noise and poor handwriting in children. Acta Psychologica, 82, 161–178.
Vaughn, S., Schumm, J. S., & Gordon, J. (1992). Early spelling acquisition: Does writing really beat the
computer? Learning Disability Quarterly, 15, 223–228.
Vaughn, S., Schumm, J. S., & Gordon, J. (1993). Which motoric condition is most effective for teaching
spelling to students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26(3),
191–198.
Velay, J. L., Daffaure, V., Giraud, K., & Habib, M. (2002). Interhemispheric sensorimotor integration in
pointing movements: A study on dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia, 40, 827–834.
Viviani, P. (2002). Motor competence in the perception of dynamic events: A tutorial. In W. Prinz & B.
Hommel (Eds.). Common mechanisms in perception and action, attention and performance (vol. XIX,
pp. 406–442). Oxford: University Press.
M. Longcamp et al. / Acta Psychologica 119 (2005) 67–79 79
... For instance, the author mentions a few studies that compared learning in typing and handwriting tasks. Results showed a better recognition of new characters in the handwriting condition than in the typing one, both in children (Longcamp et al., 2005) and adults (Longcamp et al., 2006). So, even nowadays in the era of digital tools that are impacting immensely the writing experience -and consequently, learning and reading -these findings suggest that handwriting still stands out, and has a strong influence on literacy development. ...
... The authors sustain the importance of being careful when choosing which task to investigate spelling since findings show that linguistic effects are robustly found in certain spelling tasks but not in others. Several studies are discussed, which show how handwriting contributes to the visual recognition of letters (Longcamp et al., 2010), that learning how to write letters is beneficial for children's recognition of them (Longcamp et al., 2005), and that adult students who take notes by hand understand better and remember more information than the ones who take notes on a laptop (Mueller & Oppenheimer, 2014). ...
... It says a lot about the cohesion and the integrative quality of the book that the study by Longcamp et al. (2005) is mentioned in both sections (theoretical and methodological) of the book -the study had not only an obvious theoretical implication, but also a methodological one for future investigators, who should be careful when choosing which tasks to perform in their studies, since findings showed robust linguistic effects in certain spelling tasks but not in others. Interestingly enough, in a very recent study, Wiley & Rapp (2021) also found that handwriting training during letter learning of an Arabic script strengthened reading and spelling skills in adults. ...
... The accomplishment of children to recognize similarities and differences in the production of letter/form indicates their readiness for a more challenging handwriting level (42). Most studies acknowledged and emphasized letter perception skills in the pre-writing intervention (28)(29)(30)(31)33,34,(36)(37)(38). ...
Article
This scoping mapped the evidence regarding pre-writing interventions for developmental disabilities and typically developing children. This review followed the PRISMA-Scoping Review guidelines. Evidence between January 2011 and July 2021 was retrieved from four databases: EBSCO, ScienceDirect, Scopus, and Web of Science. Selected articles were evaluated for quality using the Crowe Critical Appraisal Tool (CCAT). Ten articles were included in the study, with CCAT scores ranging between 68% and 85%. Findings were arranged in five themes: (1) pre-writing interventions, (2) pre-requisite handwriting skills, (3) protocol and interventions, (4) intervention pioneer, and (5) conveyance of interventions. Generally, pre-writing interventions were aligned with the pre-requisite skills of handwriting. Digitized-based interventions were commonly utilized and expanded in developing pre-writing skills. Acquiring effective pre-writing interventions for both populations will refine the handwriting performance. Further exploration of the integration of pre-writing skills with digitized-based intervention is required to evaluate the effectiveness of the intervention.
... It is possible that handwriting facilitates letter learning. Longcamp et al. (2005Longcamp et al. ( , 2008 found that preschool children who were trained to learn letters through handwriting performed better in a subsequent letter recognition task than children who were trained by typing letters. They argued that better performance in the handwriting group was due to the motor movements involved in handwriting strengthening memory for the letter shape, a claim for which there may be some neurological support (James & Engelhardt, 2012;Li & James, 2016). ...
... Verbunden mit visuellen orthographischen Merkmalen können diese spezifischen sensomotorischen Muster anteilig in die orthographische Wissensrepräsentation im Gedächtnis eingehen (Bosse, Chaves & Valdois, 2014). Ergebnisse empirischer Studien verweisen auf den Vorteil der handschriftlichen Buchstabenproduktion, die das Wiedererkennen sowie das langfristige Behalten eines Buchstabens und seiner Spezifik unterstützt (Velay & Longcamp, 2012;Longcamp, Zerbato-Poudou & Velay 2005). Auf Wortebene zeigten Cunningham & Stanovich (1990) (Effenberg, Schmitz, Baumann, Rosenhahn & Kroeger, 2015). ...
Article
Zusammenfassung. Hintergrund: Besondere Schwierigkeiten im Rechtschreiben weisen ohne geeignete Intervention oftmals eine hohe Stabilität und langfristige Persistenz auf. Digitale Trainingsprogramme bieten vielfältige Übungsmöglichkeiten, wobei die Texteingabe in der Regel über Tastatur erfolgt. Studien verweisen auf die positive Rolle der Handschrift beim Orthografieerwerb. Im Rahmen eines vom BMBF geförderten Forschungsprojektes wurde daher ein Tablet-basiertes Rechtschreibtrainingsprogramm mit handschriftlicher Eingabemöglichkeit und direktem Feedback über die Wortkorrektheit entwickelt und angewendet. Methoden: Nach einer erfolgreichen Machbarkeitsstudie 2015/2016 im häuslichen Umfeld (n = 9, zweite bis fünfte Klasse) wurde das weiterentwickelte Programm in der Hauptstudie 2019/2020 mit rechtschreibschwachen Kindern (Prozentrang PR >≤ 16 im DERET) der dritten und vierten Jahrgangsstufe im schulischen sowie häuslichem Kontext eingesetzt (Covid19-bedingter Ortswechsel). Ergebnisse: Bei Kindern mit einer sehr niedrigen Rechtschreibausgangsleistung (Prozentrang >≤ 16) zeigte sich eine signifikante Verbesserung der Rechtschreibfertigkeiten in der Trainings- (n = 14) im Vergleich zur Kontrollgruppe (n = 27) – bei hoher Programmakzeptanz. Diskussion: Die Kombination von digitalem Medium und handschriftlicher Eingabemöglichkeit mit direktem Fehlschreibungs-Feedback ermöglicht rechtschreibschwachen Kindern eine effektive und gut in den schulischen bzw. häuslichen Alltag integrierbare Unterstützung beim Orthografieerwerb.
... Consistent with embodiment theories, several training studies in preschool children and adults showed that handwriting training of new letters resulted in better letter recognition in a subsequent test than typing training (e.g. Longcamp et al., 2005). This demonstrates that handwriting, which links rich sensory-motor representations to perceptual letter shapes, improves subsequent letter reading performance compared with typewriting. ...
Chapter
Concepts held in long-term memory are the basic building blocks of cognition because they constitute the meaning of objects, events, and abstract ideas. Embodied cognition theories assume that concepts are essentially constituted by reinstatements of experiences related to perception and introspection as well as to bodily actions. In this article, we provide a comprehensive overview of the latest research on embodied cognition in the domains of memory for concrete, abstract and number concepts as well as for reading and writing. Reviewed evidence indicates that even the most complex and abstract thoughts are grounded in modal systems and are not abstract-symbolic.
Article
Full-text available
Trubadur je odprtokodna platforma za urjenje glasbenega posluha z avtomatiziranimi vajami ritmičnega in intervalnega nareka. Platformo smo ovrednotili z dijaki Konservatorija za glasbo in balet Ljubljana v šolskih letih 2018/19–2020/21. Rezultati evalvacije so pokazali, da lahko uporaba platforme poveča uspešnost pri testih in predstavlja dopolnitev učenja na daljavo.
Article
Full-text available
Drawing is an activity that requires visual, emotional, and movement skills. This study compares, for the first time, children's brain activity during drawing on paper versus drawing on a tablet. First, we examined drawing activity in 26 right-handed children using the EEG combined with Event-Related Desynchronization (ERD) and Event-Related Synchronization (ERS) methods. Then, we asked participants to copy a house model where we used a Neurosoft EEG system to record the data and analyse it using the Brainstorm application. Both experimental conditions activate the brain's anterior and posterior cortices, but the activity in the anterior cortices was slightly higher during the drawing on paper than on the tablet. Conversely, compared to the paper condition, brain activity in the posterior cortices was slightly higher while drawing on the tablet.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The main objective of this paper was to define the differences in the effects of traditional strength bodyweight training matched load weight training with kettlebells. Forty young karate athletes aged 10 to 14 were divided in two experimental groups: standard experimental group 1 (EKS 1) – strength training with bodyweight (n=20), and experimental group 2 (EKS 2) (n=20)– strength kettlebell training. All respondents were subjected to anthropometric measurement and twelve motoric tests (coordination, three repetitive strength, and segmental movement speed tests, three each), to determine the difference of effects on motor abilities. Both groups had frequency of two training sessions a week during six weeks period. Groups were matched by training load parameters, with the weight added training being the only difference in EKS 2. The results measurement showed that the EKS 1 significantly progressed in four motoric tests: (1 test trivial effect, 2 tests small effect, 1 test moderateeffect). EKS 2 achieved significantlyhigher effects, with statistically significant progress in applied motoric tests (1 trivial effect, 7 tests small effects, and 4 tests moderate effects). The results of this research show progress inmotor abilities for both strength training groups, with more significant changes observed in the group with additional weight load, confirming the justifiability of the application of kettlebell as additional load in young karate athletes. Keywords: weight training, bodyweight training, kettlebell
Article
Full-text available
Handwriting plays an important role in written communication, reading, and academic success. However, little is known about the neural correlates of handwriting in children. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and a copying task, we investigated regional brain activation and functional lateralization associated with Chinese handwriting in children (N = 36, 9–11 years old), as well as their relations to reading skills. We found significant activation of the bilateral frontal motor cortices, somatosensory cortex, intraparietal sulcus (IPS), fusiform gyrus (FuG), and cerebellum during handwriting, suggesting that an adult-like brain activation pattern emerges by middle childhood. Moreover, children showed left-lateralized and bilateral activation of motor regions and right-lateralized activation of the FuG and cerebellum during handwriting, suggesting that functional lateralization of handwriting is not fully established by this age. Finally, the activation of Exner’s area and the lateralization of the IPS and cerebellum during handwriting were correlated with reading skills, possibly representing a neural link between handwriting and reading in children. Collectively, this study reveals the brain correlates of handwriting and their relation to reading development in Chinese children, offering new insight into the development of handwriting and reading skills.
Article
Full-text available
Using a technique appropriate to preschool children, a confusion matrix of Roman letters written in printed script was generated for the purpose of determining what classes of letters are likely to be confused by preschoolers. An experiment in learning to visually perceive the "difficult" letters was then conducted using a different sampling of children from the same population. All participants took a letter-discrimination test before and after the learning period In some cases, the learning phase involved analysis of letters into simple segments, and in others it involved dynamic tracing of each letter in imitation of the writing gesture. The results show that analysis into simple segments has a significant beneficial effect on learning to perceive letters, while dynamic representation is ineffective. These findings support the hypothesis that perception processes are analytic, and do not rely on the parametric representation of the written trace.
Article
Full-text available
A brief retrospective is first provided on the study of reading and writing relations. Next, it is suggested that research has supported the theoretical contention that reading and writing rely on analogous mental processes and isomorphic knowledge. Four basic types of shared knowledge are delineated. Then, reasons are articulated about why it is also important to consider the separability of reading and writing. Further, over time, as reading and writing are learned, the nature of their relation changes. A description is then offered of a preliminary developmental outlook on the relation of reading and writing. The article concludes with theoretical and practical implications for use of a developmental model.
Article
This paper is on the execution stage of complex motor behavior. In our view handwriting and drawing, and more generally response execution of all internally monitored motor behavior, must be considered as containing two structurally different stages: A 'focal' stage of motor pattern selection, in which stage an abstract representation of the selected goal response is retrieved from a dynamic long term motor memory, and a second, 'ambient' stage of parameter estimation for actual muscle innervation. In two experiments, one with young children and the other with normal, right-handed subjects we tested some of the implications of the Two Stage Model. Specifically it could be shown that repetition of a grapheme within a serial choice reaction time drawing task results in a significant reduction of response latency but not in any change of the motor time. The results are interpreted as giving support to the proposition of a sustained, focal process of motor pattern retrieval, followed by a transient process of ambient parameter estimation.
Article
This research evaluated the efficacy of handwriting, letter tiles, and computer-based instruction on the early spelling acquisition of normal achieving (NLD) and learning disabled (LD) elementary students. The study replicated and extended the Cunningham and Stanovich (1990) study. Forty-eight primary-grade students (24 non-learning disabled; 24 learning disabled) received spelling training under three experimental conditions that involved different types of instructional activity: writing, sorting letter tiles, or typing on the computer. Results indicated no significant differences between the LD and NLD groups on words spelled correctly for any of the three conditions. Since the number of spelling words learned was low across conditions and groups, the number of correctly learned bigrams was examined. No within-group differences emerged for condition (writing, tile, computer); however, significant differences between the groups were found on number of bigrams learned for writing, tile, and computer, with the NLD group outperforming the LD group on all three conditions. Statistically significant time effects for bigrams were also noted for all three conditions. Unlike the Cunningham and Stanovich (1990) study, results did not replicate the superiority of the handwriting condition for the NLD group, nor was the handwriting condition found to be significantly more effective for the LD group.
Article
Word processing has been widely endorsed as one of the most promising uses of microcomputers in the elementary school curriculum. This article reviews the burgeoning literature that pertains to word processing and writing in elementary classrooms by constructing five major propositions that cut across individual studies and methods. Together, the propositions provide a schema for understanding and categorizing what is known and still needs to be known about word processing and young writers, a framework for probing the significant theoretical and substantive issues underlying the findings, and a point of departure for discussing the most provocative themes and questions that emerge from many fields of study. Throughout the review, I demonstrate that using word processing for writing in individual classrooms is a practice that is social as well as technical. I argue, therefore, that we cannot determine how word processing is most effectively used in classrooms apart from the ways particular teachers work in particular instructional contexts and that we cannot understand how word processing affects the quality, quantity, or processes of children’s writing apart from the ways these are embedded within, and mediated by, the social systems of classrooms. I conclude that, although we know a great deal about the capacities of word processing to influence students’ writing, we know much less about the ways this technology is actually introduced and used in school settings over time or the ways these introductions and settings interact with the social processes of classrooms.
Article
Chinese characters are composed of a number of strokes, varying from 1 to 23. The strokes of each character have to be written in a precise order, which is codified in a number of rules, and which is learned in the process of literacy acquisition. The present study tested the hypothesis that stroke writing order has been coded in memory as an essential component of the orthographic knowledge of a character, and that this specific motor schema is used as a cue in lexical retrieval. In the first experiment reported here, fragments of Chinese characters consisting of “early” or of “late” strokes (namely strokes which are written first or last during writing) were pre-exposed to target characters to be named as fast as possible. The results indicated that “early” strokes were better retrieval cues for character names than “late” strokes. In the second experiment, subjects were requested to make same-different judgements about two characters which had in common either “early” or “late” strokes. Different characters sharing “early” strokes were more frequently erroneously judged as being identical than characters sharing “late” strokes.