ArticlePDF Available

Effect of social networks on 10 year survival in very old Australians: The Australian longitudinal study of aging

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

To examine if social networks with children, relatives, friends, and confidants predict survival in older Australians over 10 years after controlling for a range of demographic, health, and lifestyle variables. Prospective longitudinal cohort study (the Australian longitudinal study of aging) Adelaide, South Australia. 1477 persons aged 70 years or more living in the community and residential care facilities. After controlling for a range of demographic, health, and lifestyle variables, greater networks with friends were protective against mortality in the 10 year follow up period. The hazard ratio for participants in the highest tertile of friends networks compared with participants in the lowest group was 0.78 (95%CI 0.65 to 0.92). A smaller effect of greater networks with confidants (hazard ratio = 0.84; 95%CI = 0.71 to 0.98) was seen. The effects of social networks with children and relatives were not significant with respect to survival over the following decade. Survival time may be enhanced by strong social networks. Among older Australians, these may be important in lengthening survival.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH REPORT
Effect of social networks on 10 year survival in very old
Australians: the Australian longitudinal study of aging
Lynne C Giles, Gary F V Glonek, Mary A Luszcz, Gary R Andrews
...............................................................................................................................
See end of article for
authors’ affiliations
.......................
Correspondence to:
Ms L C Giles, Department
of Rehabilitation and Aged
Care, Flinders University,
GPO Box 2100, Adelaide,
South Australia 5001,
Australia; Lynne.Giles@
rgh.sa.gov.au
Accepted for publication
23 November 2004
.......................
J Epidemiol Community Health 2005;59:574–579. doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.025429
Study objectives: To examine if social networks with children, relatives, friends, and confidants predict
survival in older Australians over 10 years after controlling for a range of demographic, health, and
lifestyle variables.
Design: Prospective longitudinal cohort study (the Australian longitudinal study of aging)
Setting: Adelaide, South Australia.
Participants: 1477 persons aged 70 years or more living in the community and residential care facilities.
Main results: After controlling for a range of demographic, health, and lifestyle variables, greater
networks with friends were protective against mortality in the 10 year follow up period. The hazard ratio
for participants in the highest tertile of friends networks compared with participants in the lowest group
was 0.78 (95%CI 0.65 to 0.92). A smaller effect of greater networks with confidants (hazard ratio = 0.84;
95%CI = 0.71 to 0.98) was seen. The effects of social networks with children and relatives were not
significant with respect to survival over the following decade.
Conclusions: Survival time may be enhanced by strong social networks. Among older Australians, these
may be important in lengthening survival.
O
ver the past quarter century, epidemiological studies
conducted in the USA,
1–10
Europe,
11–16
and Asia
17 18
have generally, but not always,
19–23
shown that social
relationships have beneficial effects on survival in adults. The
convergence in findings is impressive given that the follow up
time in these studies has ranged from two
17
to 17 years,
6
sample sizes have ranged from several hundred
2
to more than
17 000 participants,
11 12
and the participants’ ages have varied
from 18 to 94 years. Despite the general consensus of positive
effects of social relationships upon survival, several questions
remain unanswered concerning the effects of social relation-
ships on mortality in older people.
Firstly, it is unclear if all social relationships are equally
beneficial to survival among older people or if specific types
of relationship are more advantageous. Research by Seeman
et al
6
suggested ties with close friends or relatives, or both,
may protect against mortality in older people. However, this
work did not distinguish between ties with friends compared
with relatives. Little other research has distinguished
between the effect of kin and non-kin, or discretionary,
social relationships on mortality.
Secondly, most research concerning social relationships
and mortality has been conducted in North America. Four
studies cited above that found no effects of social relation-
ships on survival used Australian data.
19–22
Some of these
studies were small
19
or used a narrow range of measures of
social relationships,
20
so it is possible that their findings were
attributable to methodological features rather than the true
absence of an effect of social relationships upon mortality.
For example, a comprehensive study by Korten et al
21
included variables concerning social integration and avail-
ability of attachments, but did not differentiate between
types of social relationships. Similarly, McCallum and
colleagues
22
considered emotional social support and social
participation as potential predictors of survival over seven
years of follow up, but again did not consider different types
of social relationships.
Thirdly, some authors have reported a threshold of social
relationships above which little survival advantage is
gained.
1–4
We have found no research that has formally
tested the hypothesis of threshold effects of specific types of
social relationships upon mortality, and only one study that
considered threshold effects included older participants.
2
Finally, social relationships have been defined in many
ways.
24
Many studies have used single variables that purport
to measure social relationships, but do not capture the wider
social integration of an individual. Four composite measures
of social networks were recently developed by Glass et al
25
using data from a large US longitudinal study of aging. These
specific social network measures incorporated the structure
and specificity of network ties with children, other relatives,
friends, and confidants. The measures were designed to aid in
identifying the most ‘‘health-beneficial’’ social relationships
for older people,
25
and to overcome many of the problems
inherent in epidemiological studies that have examined the
effects of social relationships upon health.
We validated the measurement model developed by Glass
et al using data from the Australian longitudinal study of
aging (ALSA).
26
The resulting measures of specific social
networks are used in this study. The aims of this study were
to (1) assess the effects of specific social networks on 10 year
mortality in older Australians and (2) test for the existence of
a threshold effect of different types of social networks on 10
year mortality.
METHODS
We drew data from the ALSA that began in 1992 in Adelaide,
South Australia. ALSA’s major objectives were to assess the
effects of social, biomedical, behavioural, economic, and
environmental factors upon age related changes in the health
and wellbeing of older persons.
27
The study has been
described in detail elsewhere.
28 29
The primary sample was
randomly selected from the South Australian electoral roll,
and stratified by local government area, sex, and age group
(70–74, 75–79, 80–84, and >85 years). Older men were over-
sampled to ensure sufficient numbers of men for longitudinal
follow up. Persons were eligible for the study if they were
574
www.jech.com
resident in the Adelaide Statistical Division and aged 70 years
or more on 31 December 1992.
Seven waves of data have been collected to date. Interviews
with participants were held annually for the first four years
and then roughly every three years. The relevant ethics
committee approved the study, and each participant gave
written informed consent.
Of the original sample of 3263 persons, 2703 were eligible
for inclusion in the study and 1477 (55%) of these persons
agreed to participate. Those who refused were slightly older
and more likely to be female than the participants.
29
We used
data from the 1477 participants who completed a wave 1
interview. The retention of participants over the decade after
wave 1 interview in the study was excellent, with more than
75% of surviving participants interviewed at wave 6.
Covariates
Social networks
Social networks with children, other relatives, friends, and
confidants were hypothesised as predictors of survival. The
derivation of these variables has been described
25
and
validated previously.
26
The children network combined
information on the number of children, proximity of
children, and frequency of personal and phone contact with
children. The relatives network was calculated from the
number of relatives (apart from spouse and children) the
participant felt close to, and the frequency of personal and
phone contact with these relatives. The friends network
captured the number of close friends, personal contact, and
phone contact. The confidant network reflected the existence
of confidants and whether the confidant was a spouse. A
total social network score was calculated as the sum of the
children, relatives, friends, and confidant network scores. All
component variables were standardised before the derivation
of the social network variables.
We included the social networks variables as either
continuous or categorical in subsequent analyses, dependent
on the analyses being undertaken.
Demographic, health, and lifestyle variables
To control for confounding in the analyses, the effects of
demographic, health, and lifestyle variables upon mortality
were also considered. These covariates were derived from self
reported wave 1 data. Demographic variables included age
group, sex, and geographical area of residence. Place of
residence was classified as community or residential care.
Current marital status was classified as married/partnered or
not married. Annual household income was coded as less
than or equal to $A12 000, more than $A12 000, or missing.
This cut off point for income was similar to the single
persons’ aged pension rate in 1992. The age at which the
participant left full time education was categorised as less
than or equal to 14 years of age or more than 14 years of age.
This cut off point for education was chosen as about half of
the sample left school aged 14 years of less; there was no
legal minimum school leaving age for this cohort. Other
analyses of ALSA data have used these education and income
cut off points (for example, Andrews et al
29
).
Physical and mental health status were also incorporated
in the analyses. Self rated health was classified as excellent/
very good, good, and fair/poor. The number of chronic
conditions was derived from self reported information on
whether each participant had ever suffered from 10 common
conditions.
30
Disability was assessed via mobility.
31
Parti-
cipants were defined as having no mobility disability if they
reported they were able to walk up and down a flight of stairs
and walk half a mile without help. If either or both of these
activities could not be completed, they were classified as
having a mobility disability.
30
Self reported hearing and visual
difficulty were also included. Depressive symptomatology
Table 1 Summary statistics for 1477 participants in wave 1 of ALSA
Characteristic Number* Overall (n = 1477) Survivors (n = 570) Decedents (n = 907)
Social networks mean (95%CI)
Children 1477 0.00 (20.04 to 0.04) 0.08 (0.01 to 0.14) 20.04 (20.10 to 0.01)
Relatives 1477 0.00 (2 0.04 to 0.04) 0.11 (0.04 to 0.17) 20.06 (20.11 to 20.01)
Friends 1477 0.01 (20.03 to 0.05) 0.19 (0.13 to 0.25) 20.11 (20.16 to 20.06)
Confidants 1477 0.01 (2 0.03 to 0.05) 0.10 (0.04 to 0.16) 20.04 (20.09 to 0.00)
Total 1477 0.02 (20.06 to 0.11) 0.48 (0.34 to 0.61) 20.26 (20.37 to 0.15)
Social networks tertile cut off points
Children 0th, 33Mrd, 66 Ord, 100th 1477 21.67, 20.02, 0.48, 1.17 21.67, 20.02, 0.49, 1.17 21.67, 20.11, 0.47, 1.17
Relatives 0th, 33Mrd, 66Ord, 100th 1477 21.05,20.53, 0.37, 2.02 21.05,20.33, 0.47, 2.02 21.05,20.53, 0.23, 2.02
Friends 0th, 33Mrd, 66Ord, 100th 1477 21.54, 20.33, 0.46, 1.19 21.54, 20.06, 0.61, 1.19 21.54, 20.42, 0.32, 1.19
Confidants 0th, 33Mrd, 66Ord, 100th 1477 21.68, 0.07, 0.09, 0.77 21.68, 0.09, 0.77, 0.77 21.68, 0.07, 0.09, 0.77
Total 0th, 33Mrd, 66Ord, 100th 25.69, 20.63, 0.84, 4.97 25.41, 20.08, 1.22, 4.63 25.69, 20.99, 0.51, 4.97
Age mean (95%CI) 1477 79.8 (79.4 to 80.1) 76.2 (75.8 to 76.7) 82.0 (81.6 to 82.4)
Sex male n (%) 1477 928 (62.8) 326 (57.2) 602 (66.4)
Place of residence community n (%) 1477 1340 (91.3) 554 (97.2) 786 (86.7)
Marital status married n (%) 1477 771 (52.2) 330 (57.9) 441 (48.6)
Age left school (14 years n (%) 1463 830 (56.7) 306 (53.8) 524 (58.6)
Household income ($12000 n (%)* 1369 590 (39.9) 212 (37.2) 378 (41.7)
Number of morbid conditions median
(95%CI)
1477 1 (1 to 2) 1 (1 to 1) 1 (1–2)
Mobility disability 1455 506 (34.8) 113 (19.9) 393 (44.3)
Cognitive function poor n (%) 1440 219 (15.2) 36 (6.4) 183 (20.9)
Self rated health fair/poor n (%) 1472 469 (31.8) 116 (20.4) 353 (38.9)
Depressive symptoms n (%)* 1400 219 (14.8) 59 (10.4) 160 (18.6)
Hearing difficulty n (%) 1472 746 (50.7) 253 (44.4) 493 (54.7)
Vision difficulty n (%) 1410 375 (26.6) 92 (16.7) 283 (32.9)
Alcohol problem n (%) 1466 65 (4.4) 28 (4.9) 37 (4.1)
Current smoker n (%) 1461 123 (8.4) 31 (5.4) 92 (10.3)
Former smoker n (%) 1461 677 (46.3) 253 (44.5) 424 (47.5)
Pack years of smoking for current/former
smokers (median, 95%CI)
800 30.4 (27.7 to 32.4) 26.5 (21.5 to 30.4) 31.8 (29.2 to 35.4)
Sedentary n (%)* 1457 663 (45.5) 214 (37.6) 449 (50.6)
* Denominator in % is count of non-missing observations for each variable. If more than 74 (5%) observations were missing for a variable, then a category of
missing was added to ensure cases with missing observations were included in the analyses.
Social networks and 10 year survival in older Australians 575
www.jech.com
was assessed using the 20 item CES-D scale,
32 33
with scores
of >17 out of a possible 60 suggesting symptoms of
depression. Cognitive function was measured using a subset
of items from the mini-mental state examination.
34 35
Health behaviours were also considered. Participants were
classed as current, former, or never smokers based on their
responses to questions concerning smoking. Participants
were classified as having a hazardous drinking problem if
their score on the 10 item AUDIT scale was eight or more.
36
Participants were classified as exercisers or sedentary based
on questions about the exercise undertaken in the previous
fortnight.
28
Statistical analyses and data linkage
Survival status was ascertained by searches of official death
certificates conducted by the Epidemiology Branch of the
Department of Health in South Australia, and deaths were
confirmed by the South Australian Births, Deaths and
Marriages bureau. Full name, date of birth, and last known
address of ALSA participants were used in the data linkage
with the deaths database. If no direct match was made, the
electoral roll was checked for errors in birth dates, changes or
errors in recorded name, and changes or errors in recorded
address. The few participants who died interstate or overseas
could not be identified through this method, as the deaths
database only includes deaths that occur in South Australia.
Informants nominated by ALSA participants at wave 1 were
contacted if participants could not be located at subsequent
interviews. The date of death supplied by informants was
used if a participant died outside of South Australia. These
methods of death ascertainment for ALSA participants have
been validated previously.
37
The response variable was the number of days to death
from wave 1 interview for decedents and 3653 days for
participants who survived 10 years after their initial inter-
view.
The cumulative hazard of death over time was compared
graphically for the centile based classification of each social
network type using the Nelson-Aalen cumulative hazard
estimator.
38 39
Broadly, a higher cumulative hazard curve
indicates a greater risk over time.
For each type of social network, a separate Cox propor-
tional hazards model was fitted to the data,
40
controlling for
the demographic, health, and lifestyle covariates. The Efron
method was used to correct for ties in the time to death.
41
The existence of threshold effects was investigated within
the framework of the proportional hazards model. For each of
the social network variables, we considered separately
thresholds corresponding to the tertiles. For example, to test
for a threshold at the 33Mrd centile, a dummy variable
showing whether the social network observation lay above
this centile point was included in the proportional hazards
model along with the original continuous variable. A
significant dummy variable indicated a threshold effect.
Backward elimination was used to remove non-significant
covariates from the regression equations. The fit of models
was assessed using graphical methods based on martingale
residuals.
42 43
The assumption of proportional hazards was
assessed by regressing the scaled Schoenfeld residuals
against the log of time and testing for zero slope. A non-
zero slope provided evidence against proportional hazards.
44
Stata version 8.0 was used in all analyses (Stata Corporation,
College Station, TX).
RESULTS
Table 1 shows the characteristics of the 1477 participants at
wave 1 of ALSA. At the 10th anniversary of the wave 1
interview, 570 participants (326 male; 57%) were alive and
the remaining 907 participants (602 male; 66%) had died.
The mean specific and total network scores were higher for
the participants who survived 10 years after the wave 1
interview than for the participants who died in the
intervening decade. Tables detailing the relation between
each type of social network and the covariates are available
from the first author upon request.
Age group, sex, local government area, place of residence,
number of morbid conditions, cognitive function, self rated
health, and smoking status were significant predictors of
survival when the effects of the other covariates were
considered, and all subsequent analyses adjusted for these
variables. Table 2 presents the hazard ratios associated with
these variables. Non-proportional hazards were evident for
Table 2 Adjusted hazard ratios for effect of covariates on 10 year survival*
Variable HR 95% CI p Value
Sex, female 0.62 0.52 to 0.73 ,0.001
Age group 75–79 1.67 1.32 to 2.11 ,0.001
Age group 80–84 2.65 2.12 to 3.33 ,0.001
Age group 85+ 4.23 3.37 to 5.30 ,0.001
Dwelling residential aged care 1.36 1.09 to 1.71 0.008
Number of morbid conditions 1.09 1.03 to 1.16 0.006
Cognitive impairment yes 1.65 1.38 to 1.98 ,0.001
Self rated health good 1.26 1.06 to 1.50 0.009
Self rated health fair/poor 1.49 1.24 to 1.78 ,0.001
Smoking status former 1.14 0.97 to 1.34 0.117
Smoking status current 2.00 1.56 to 2.56 ,0.001
*Local government area (with 24 levels) not shown. Referent categories for adjusted hazard ratios are sex male,
age group 70–74, dwelling community, cognitive impairment no, self rated health excellent/very good, smoking
status never. Hazard ratios adjusted for other covariates.
1.1
1.0
0.9
0.8
HR (95% CI)
Children
(p = 0.640)
Relatives
(p = 0.990)
Friends
(p = 0.015)
Confidants
(p = 0.077)
Total
(p = 0.033)
Figure 1 Summary of adjusted hazard ratios (HR) and 95% confidence
intervals (95%CI) from Cox proportional hazards models for specific and
total social networks.
576 Giles, Glonek, Luszcz, et al
www.jech.com
mobility disability, and therefore all analyses were stratified
by disability status.
The continuous social network variables were fitted and
the results are summarised in figure 1. Plots of martingale
residuals against the respective social network variables
confirmed that a linear functional form was appropriate for
these variables. There was a significant protective effect of
larger friends and total social networks against mortality. The
effect of networks with confidants was marginally signifi-
cant, and again showed a protective effect. The effects of
social networks with children and relatives were not
significant.
The existence of a threshold effect of social networks was
investigated using the dummy variables corresponding to the
tertiles, and were not significant in any analyses.
The hazard ratios corresponding to the tertile groupings are
shown in table 3 for friends, confidant, and total social
networks. The table shows a gradient in terms of the social
network variables. The effect of the friends network on
survival was greatest for those with the greatest networks of
friends (that is, in the upper tertile of the friends network
distribution). The effect of the confidant network was
beneficial to survival for those in both the middle and upper
tertiles of confidant networks.
Figure 2 shows the observed Nelson-Aalen cumulative
hazard estimates in days from the wave 1 interview for
friends and confidant networks and total social networks. In
each case, the groups defined by stronger networks have a
lower cumulative hazard and hence a lower risk of mortality
over time.
DISCUSSION
This study builds on previous work concerning social
relationships and mortality. Most other studies have used
ad hoc measures of social networks. Furthermore, there is a
paucity of research that has examined the effects of specific
social networks upon mortality. Through the use of objective
measures of specific social networks, developed originally for
a US sample and validated in ALSA, we have shown that
greater social networks with friends and confidants had
significant protective effects against mortality over a 10 year
follow up period. Networks with children and relatives were
not significant predictors of mortality over the same follow
up period. This highlights the importance of disaggregating
Table 3 Summary of adjusted hazard ratios for categorised social network variables
Network Tertile* HR 95% CI p Value
Friends 0–33Mrd 1.00
33M–66Ord 0.87 0.73 to 1.02 0.093
66O–100th 0.78 0.65 to 0.92 0.004
Confidants 0–33Mrd 1.00
33M–66Ord 0.85 0.72 to 1.00 0.049
66O–100th 0.83 0.70 to 1.00 0.044
Total 0–33Mrd 1.00
33M–66Ord 0.91 0.77 to 1.07 0.250
66O–100th 0.86 0.72 to 1.03 0.098
* Friends 0th, 33Mrd, 66Ord, 100th centile cut off points: 21.54, 20.33, 0.46, 1.19. Confidants 0th, 33Mrd,
66Ord, 100th centile cut off points: 21.68, 0.07, 0.09, 0.77. Total 0th, 33Mrd, 66Ord, 100th centile cut off
points: 25.69, 20.63, 0.84, 4.97. Hazard ratio adjusted for significant covariates.
1.5
1.0
0.5
0.0
4000
Time (days)
Friends
0 300020001000
1.5
1.0
0.5
0.0
4000
Time (days)
Confidants
Nelson-Aalen cumulative hazard
Nelson-Aalen cumulative hazard
Nelson-Aalen cumulative hazard
0 300020001000
1.5
1.0
0.5
0.0
4000
Time (days)
Total
Lower tertile
0 300020001000
Mid tertile
Upper tertile
Figure 2 Nelson-Aalen cumulative
hazard estimates by type of social
network.
Social networks and 10 year survival in older Australians 577
www.jech.com
kin and non-kin networks, rather than relying on measures
of total social networks.
The finding that total social networks are protective against
mortality suggests overall social integration is important, and
reinforces findings from other studies of older people.
26151618
Previous Australian studies
19–22
have not shown an effect of
social networks on mortality. However, these studies were
generally smaller or did not consider the specific types of
social networks that were investigated in this study.
Differences in the definitions of social relationships and
different analyses may have contributed to the disparities in
previous reports.
Earlier research has shown social relationships with close
friends and/or relatives were protective against mortality in
older adults,
6
and subsequent research
45
also pointed to the
importance of a confidant in the perceived adequacy of social
support. By differentiating between friends, children, and
other relatives, we were able to show that it is friends, rather
than children or relatives, which confer most benefit to
survival in later life. Our finding of a marginally significant
effect of confidants upon survival suggests that discretionary
relationships, with friends and confidants, as compared with
relationships where there is less choice concerning interac-
tion, with children and relatives, have important positive
effects on survival. This is consistent with the socioemotional
selectivity theory proposed by Carstensen and colleagues,
46
in
showing that with age, one’s social choices may become more
selective as a means of regulating emotions.
The results from this study raise important questions about
how social networks with friends in particular impact upon
mortality. The causal relationship between social networks
and health is not well understood.
47
A recent review
48
proposed culture, socioeconomic factors, politics, and social
change condition the extent, shape, and nature of social
networks. In turn, social networks provide opportunities for
‘‘psychosocial mechanisms’’ that include social support,
social influence, social engagement, interpersonal contact,
and access to financial and health care resources.
Psychosocial mechanisms may have an impact upon health
through behavioural, psychological, and physiological path-
ways.
48
If we consider social networks within this framework,
networks with friends may exert an influence upon health
behaviours such as smoking, alcohol consumption, and
exercise, variables that were controlled for in our analyses.
Friends possibly also encourage health seeking behaviour,
which in turn can affect survival. Friends can have effects on
depression, self efficacy, self esteem,
49
coping and morale,
50
or
a sense of personal control,
51
possibly through social
engagement by reinforcing social roles
48
or because interac-
tions with friends stem from choice
52
or selectivity.
46
The effects of specific social networks upon mortality in
our study were of a similar magnitude to those we have seen
for self rated health and number of morbid conditions.
Furthermore, social network variables exerted an effect on
mortality 10 years after they were measured. For the effects
to be sustained over this long period suggests social networks
are powerful factors in protecting against premature death.
These baseline effects persisted even though many other
changes may have occurred for participants in the decade
after the wave 1 interview, including widowhood, deaths of
friends, siblings, children, or geographical relocation of some
members of their overall social network. Future work is
planned to assess changes in social networks among ALSA
participants, and the impact of any changes upon mortality.
The findings from this study must be interpreted with
several caveats. A wide range of covariates were included in
the analyses, but complete data were unavailable for some
potentially important factors, such as diet. However, given
that diet contributes to overall health, our covariates
indirectly capture this potential effect. ALSA was not
explicitly designed to examine the effects of social networks
on mortality, and the analyses are based on self reported data
and adjust for covariates measured at baseline. However,
these same limitations are true of most studies that have
considered social relationships and mortality in older adults.
The non-respondents to ALSA may have been more socially
isolated than participants, although non-response bias has
generally been shown as minimal in other analyses of ALSA
data.
29 53 54
We believe these restrictions are balanced by
ALSA’s strengths, which include the richness of the baseline
data, the Australian setting, and the inclusion of residents in
aged care facilities. ALSA included a more heterogeneous
population sample than many other longitudinal studies of
aging.
In summary, we have shown that better social networks
with friends and confidants predict survival over the
following decade in a large cohort of older Australian men
and women. Strong social networks of discretionary relation-
ships may be important in ensuring longer survival.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank the participants in the Australian longitudinal study of
aging, who have given their time over many years, and without
whom this study would not have been possible. Sabine Schreiber of
the Centre for Ageing Studies, Flinders University, and the
Epidemiology Branch of the Department of Health in South
Australia are also thanked for their assistance with tracing
participants and identifying deaths.
Authors’ affiliations
.....................
L C Giles, Department of Rehabilitation and Aged Care, Flinders
University, Adelaide, Australia
G F V Glonek, Department of Applied Mathematics, University of
Adelaide, Adelaide, Australia
M A Luszcz, School of Psychology and Centre for Ageing Studies,
Flinders University
G R Andrews, Centre for Ageing Studies, Flinders University
Funding: this study was supported in part by grants from the South
Australian Health Commission, the Australian Rotary Health Research
Fund, and the US National Institute on Aging (grant no AG 08523-02).
Competing interests: none declared.
Ethics approval: ethics approval for the study was granted by the
Committee on Clinical Investigation, Flinders Medical Centre, South
Australia.
Key points
N
Better social networks with friends and confidants
predict survival over the following decade in older
Australians.
N
No effect of social networks with children or relatives
upon survival was found.
N
We did not find a threshold effect of specific social
relationships upon survival.
Policy implications
Strong social networks of discretionary relationships are
important in ensuring longer survival. Strategies to promote
the establishment and maintenance of such relationships in
later life deserve further attention.
578 Giles, Glonek, Luszcz, et al
www.jech.com
REFERENCES
1 Berkman LF, Syme SL. Social networks, host resistance and mortality: a nine-
year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. Am J Epidemiol
1979;109:186–204.
2 Blazer DG. Social support and mortality in an elderly community population.
Am J Epidemiol 1982;115:684–94.
3 Hanson BS, Isaacson SD, Janzen L, et al. Social network and social support
influence mortality in older men. Am J Epidemiol 1989;128:370–80.
4 House JS, Robbins C, Metzne r HL. The association of social relationships and
activities with mortality: prospect ive evidence from the Tecumseh community
health study. Am J Epidemiol 1982;116:123–40.
5 Schoenbach VJ, Kaplan BJ, Fredman L, et al. Social ties and mortality in Evans
County, Georgia. Am J Epidemiol 1986;123:577–91.
6 Seeman TE, Kaplan GA, Knudsen L, et al. Social network ties and mortality
among the elderly in the Alameda county study. Am J Epidemiol
1987;126:714–23.
7 Seeman TE, Berkman LF, Kohout F, et al. Inter-community variations in the
association between social ties and mortality in the elderly: a comparative
analysis of three communities. Ann Epidemiol 1993;3:325–35.
8 Kawachi I, Colditz G, Ascherio A, et al. A prospective study of social networks
in relation to total mortality and cardiovascular disease in men in the USA.
J Epidemiol Community Health 1996;50:245–51.
9 Steinbach U. Social networks, institutionalization, and mortality among elderly
people in the United States. J Gerontol Soc Sci 1992;47:S183–90.
10 Vogt TM, Mullooly JP, Ernst D, et al. Social networks as predictors of ischemic
heart disease, cancer, stroke and hypertension: incidence, survival and
mortality. J Clin Epidemiol 1992;45:659–66.
11 Berkman LF, Melchior M, Chastang JF, et al. Social integration and mortality:
a prospective study of French employees of Electricity of France-Gas of
France: the GAZEL cohort. Am J Epidemiol 2004;159:167–74.
12 Orth-Gormer K, Johnson JV. Social network interaction and mortality: a six
year follow-up study of a random sample of the Swedish population. J Chron
Dis 1987;40:949–57.
13 Jylha¨M, Aro S. Social ties and survival among the elderly in Tampere,
Finland. Int J Epidemiol 1989;18:158–64.
14 Kaplan JR, Salonen JT, Cohen RD, et al. Social connections and mortality from
all causes and cardiovascular disease: prospective evidence from eastern
Finland. Am J Epidemiol 1988;128:370–80.
15 Welin L, Tibblin G, Svardsudd K, et al. Prospective study of social influences
on mortality: the study of men born in 1913 and 1923. Lancet
1985;i:915–18.
16 Penninx B, van Tilburg T, Kriegsman D. Effects of social support and personal
coping resources on mortality in older age: the longitudinal aging study
Amsterdam. Am J Epidemiol 1997;146:510–19.
17 Ho SC. Health and social predictors of mortality in a Chinese elderly cohort.
Am J Epidemiol 1991;133:901–21.
18 Sugisawa H, Liang J, Liu X. Social networks, social support and mortality
among older people in Japan. J Gerontol 1994;49:3–13.
19 Jorm AF, Henderson AS, Kav DW. Mortality in relation to dementia,
depression and social integration in an elderly community sample.
Int J Geriatric Psychiatr 1991;6:5–11.
20 Simons LA, McCallum J, Friedlander Y, et al. Predictors of mortality in the
prospective Dubbo study of Australian elderly. Aust N Z J Med
1996;26:40–8.
21 Korten AE, Jorm AF, Jiao Z, et al. Health, cognitive, and psychosocial factors
as predictors of mortality in an elderly community sample. J Epidemiol
Community Health 1999;53:83–8.
22 McCallum J, Shadbolt B, Wang D. Self-rated health and survival: a 7-year
follow up study of Australian elderly. Am J Public Health 1994;84:1100–5.
23 Shahtahmasebi S, Davies R, Wenger GC. A longitudinal analysis of factors
related to survival in old age. Gerontologist 1992;32:104–13.
24 O’Reilly P. Methodological issues in social support and social network
research. Soc Sci Med 1988;26:863–73.
25 Glass TA, Mendes de Leon CF, Seeman TE, et al. Beyond single indicators of
social networks: a LISREL analysis of social ties among the elderly. Soc Sci
Med 1997;44:1503–17.
26 Giles LC, Metcalf PA, Anderson CS, et al. Social networks among older
Australians: a validation of Glass’ model. J Cancer Epidemiol Prev
2002;7:195–204.
27 Centre for Ageing Studies. The Australian longitudinal study of ageing
and a collaborative study of aging in the US and Australia: handbook.
Adelaide: Centre for Ageing Studies, 1993.
28 Finucane P, Giles LC, Withers RT, et al. Exercise profile and mortality in an
elderly population. Aust N Z J Public Health 1997;21:155–8.
29 Andrews G, Clark M, Luszcz M. Successful ageing in the Australian
longitudinal study of ageing: applying the MacArthur model cross-nationally.
J Soc Issues 2002;58:749–65.
30 Giles LC, Metcalf PA, Glonek GFV, et al. The effects of social networks upon
disability in older Australians. J Aging Health 2004;16:517–38.
31 Rosow I, Breslau N. A Guttman health scale for the aged. J Gerontol
1966;21:556–9.
32 Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the
general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1977;1:385–401.
33 Radloff LS, Teri L. Use of the Center for Epidemiological Studies-depression
scale with older adults. Clin Gerontologist 1986;5:119–36.
34 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: a practical method for
grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res
1975;12:189–98.
35 Luszcz MA, Bryan J, Kent P. Predicting episodic memory performance of very
old men and women: contributions from age, depression, activity, cognitive
ability, and speed. Psychol Aging 1997;12:340–51.
36 Barbor TF, de la Fuente JR, Saunders J, et al. AUDIT The alcohol use disorders
identification test: guidelines for use in primary health care. Geneva: World
Health Organisation, 1992.
37 Anstey KA, Luszcz MA, Giles LC, et al. Demographic, health, cognitive, and
sensory variables as predictors of mortality in very old adults. Psychol Aging
2001;16:3–11.
38 Fleming TR, Harrington DP. Counting processes and survival analysis. New
York: Wiley, 1991.
39 Nelson W. Hazard plotting for incomplete failure data. J Qual Technol
1969;1:27–52.
40 Cox DR. Regression models and life-tables (with discussion). J Stat Soc Series B
1972;34:187–220.
41 Efron B. The efficiency of Cox’s likelihood function for censored data. JAm
Stat Ass 1977;72:557–65.
42 Grambsch PM, Therneau TM. Proportional hazards tests and diagnostics
based on weighted residuals. Biometrika 1994;81:515–26.
43 Grambsch PM, Therneau TM, Fleming TR. Diagnostic plots to reveal functional
form for covariates in multiplicative intensity models. Biometrics
1995;51:1469–82.
44 Therneau TM, Grambsch PM. Modeling survival data: extending the Cox
model. New York: Springer-Verlag, 2000.
45 Seeman TE, Berkman LF. Structural characteristics of social networks and their
relationship with social support in the elderly: who provides support. Soc Sci
Med 1988; 26:737–49.
46 Carstensen LL, Fung HH, Charles ST. Socioemotional selectivity theory and the
regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation and Emotion
2003;27:103–23.
47 Berkman LF, Glass T, Brissette I, et al. From social integration to health:
Durkheim in the new millennium. Soc Sci Med 2000;51:843–57.
48 Berkman LF, Glass TA. Social integration, social networks, social support, and
health. In: Berkman LF, Kawachi I, eds. Social epidemiology. New York:
Oxford University Press, 2000.
49 Lee GR, Shehan CI. Social relations and the self-esteem of older persons. Res
Aging 1989;11:427–42.
50 Litwin H. Social network type and morale in old age. Gerontologist
2001;41:516–24.
51 Anstey KA, Luszcz M, Andrews G. Psychosocial factors, gender and late-life
mortality. Ageing Int 2002;27:73–89.
52 Lee GR, Ishii-Kuntz M. Social interaction, loneliness and emotional well-being
among the elderly. Res Aging 1987;9:459–82.
53 Luszcz MA, Bryan J, Kent P. Predicting episodic memory performance of very
old men and women: contributions from age, depression, activity, cognitive
ability, and speed. Psychol Aging 1997;12:340–51.
54 Anstey KJ, Luszcz MA. Mortality risk varies according to gender and
change in depressive status in very old adults. Psychosom Med
2002;64:880–8.
Social networks and 10 year survival in older Australians 579
www.jech.com
... In support of our predictions, prior work has shown that better cognitive ability is related to having large, heterogeneous social networks (Giles et al., 2005;Seeman et al., 2001), but declining cognition is associated with having smaller networks, poor social support, and/or low frequency of contact (Kuiper et al., 2016;Penninkilampi et al., 2018). One reason for this dissociation might be that more familiar social interactions, much like well-practiced cognitive tasks, require less cognitive effort (Hess et al., 2021). ...
... Our findings that general cognitive function is uniquely related to social bridging networks are also consistent with prior work. Specifically, longitudinal and cross-sectional studies have shown that general cognitive ability predicts the size and structure of older adults' personal social networks (Giles et al., 2005;Seeman et al., 2001). Moreover, better general cognitive function is also associated with having expansive, heterogeneous social networks that include a mix of close relationships and acquaintances (Cornwell, 2009a(Cornwell, , 2009bIwase et al., 2012). ...
... Our finding that executive function was related to factors associated with social bridging (e.g., larger networks; Giles et al., 2005) is consistent with prior work (Casey et al., 2021;Gross et al., 2016;Johnson et al., 2007), including work showing that executive function is engaged during social interactions (Moriguchi, 2014). For example, a longitudinal study found that having higher executive function was related to having larger than expected social networks over time for older adults (Casey et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
The mechanisms by which older adults maintain large, complex social networks are not well understood. Prior work has primarily focused on general cognitive ability (e.g., executive function, episodic memory), largely overlooking social cognition—the ability to process, store, and remember social information. Because social cognition plays a key role in navigating social interactions and is distinct from general cognition, we examined whether general and social cognition uniquely predicted the nature of older adults’ personal social networks. Our study leveraged comprehensive measures of general cognition (executive function, episodic memory), social cognition (face memory and dynamic measures of cognitive and affective theory of mind), and a rigorous measure of personal social networks from 143 community-dwelling older adults. We found that, when modeled together and controlling for sociodemographic variables, only executive function and dynamic cognitive theory of mind positively predicted having social networks with relatively unfamiliar, loosely connected others, accounting for 17% of the unique variance in older adults’ social connectedness. Interestingly, having a social network comprised primarily of close, tightly knit relationships was negatively associated with affective theory of mind performance. Findings are discussed in the context of the social–cognitive resource framework—which suggests that social cognition may be more engaged in relatively unfamiliar, versus close, interactions. Specifically, our results show that social–cognitive processes may be relatively automatic for individuals whose primary social relationships are very close but may be more strongly engaged for individuals whose interactions include at least some relatively less close relationships.
... The focus on SEB is based on the well-documented association between social integration and reduced morbidity in the literature [16]. Moreover, the relevance of SEB to health outcomes and cortisol has been further accentuated by (1) the observation of a longitudinal association between social integration and health outcomes [7,17,18], and (2) the observation of an association between reduced social network activity [19] or emotional support [20] and higher cortisol levels. The hypothesis of the present study can be subsumed within the conceptual model linking social networks with health outcomes proposed by Berkman et al. [21], which can be appropriately applied to understand the health impact of the social contextual changes confronting the aging population. ...
Article
Full-text available
Based on a framework derived from theories of social integration and health, this study examined the relationships between motivation to cultivate social ties or network cultivation, and diurnal salivary cortisol in Chinese older adults. The main objective was to determine the behavioral mechanism that translated a higher motivation to cultivate social ties into healthier diurnal cortisol rhythms in 82 community-dwelling elders in Shanghai. Participants were provided four saliva samples immediately, 30 minutes, and 3 hours after waking and at bedtime on two consecutive days to determine their cortisol range. Participants completed a questionnaire measuring (1) network cultivation, (2) social contacts and behaviors, (3) loneliness, and (4) subjective socioeconomic status. Cortisol concentrations were analyzed using an enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay. Mediation analysis using the PROCESS macro v4.2 showed that a composite measure of social integration or embeddedness, created by combining social behaviors and loneliness, partially mediated the relationship between network cultivation and cortisol dynamic range. Given the relevance of diurnal cortisol to well-being in older adults, the empirical findings on the mediating effect of social integration could inform the formulation of effective psychosocial interventions to promote well-being and health in older adults, which will have important implications for public health in aging societies.
... For older adults, friendship may be important because of the types and frequency of activity with friends in providing an escape from everyday mundanities of home [76]. Furthermore, it has been suggested that friendship is of particular importance for older LGBT individuals, perhaps because they are less likely to report being in an intimate relationship than their cisgender heterosexual counterparts and may lack support from family members [77,78]. It may also be the case that family of choice networks have not yet been fully developed among many SGM young adults, particularly when the social spaces for meeting other LGBTQ+ youth are restricted [79] as these were in the UK and elsewhere during the pandemic [80]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Studies have emphasized the importance of social support in mitigating the relationship between psychological distress and mental health effects, with family of origin and family of formation providing key sources of social support over the life course. However, LGBTQ+ people may experience family of origin relationships as a source of distress, while partners and friends may buffer the relationship between minority stress and psychological wellbeing. Through our online survey (March–June 2022), which was conducted when the social restrictions of the COVID-19 pandemic were lifted in the UK, we considered the association between psychological wellbeing and sources of social support by sampling n = 1330 LGBTQ+ and cisgender heterosexual adults. LGBTQ+ adults generally experienced poorer psychological wellbeing outcomes than did cisgender heterosexual people. For LGBTQ+ adults, social support from family of origin, a special person, or friends was not associated with depression, anxiety, or stress levels, but social support from family or a special person was positively associated with higher levels of life satisfaction. Our findings indicate the importance of considering negative as well as positive wellbeing.
... Since these early findings, well-replicated work across countries and cultures has established a life expectancy advantage for more socially connected people while controlling for other longevityboosting explanations (e.g., physical activity) (Giles et al., 2005;Glass et al., 1999). A meta-analysis surveying 148 studies and 308,849 participants found a 50% increased likelihood of survival across time for participants with stronger social connections (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Many lifestyle and psychosocial factors are associated with a longer lifespan; central among these is social connectedness, or the feeling of belongingness, identification, and bond as part of meaningful human relationships. Decades of research have established that social connectedness is related not only to better mental health (e.g., less loneliness and depression) but also to improved physical health (e.g., decreased inflammatory markers, reduced cortisol activity). Recent methodological advances allow for the investigation of a novel marker of biological health by deriving a predicted “age of the brain” from a structural neuroimaging scan. Discrepancies between a person’s algorithm-predicted brain-age and chronological age (i.e., the brain-age gap) have been found to predict mortality and psychopathology risk with accuracy rivaling other known measures of aging. This preregistered investigation uses the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study to examine connections between the quality of social connections, the brain-age gap, and markers of mortality risk to understand the longevity-promoting associations of social connectedness from a novel biological vantage point. While social connectedness was associated with markers of mortality risk (number of chronic conditions and ability to perform activities of daily living), our models did not find significant links between social connectedness and the brain-age gap, or the brain-age gap and mortality risk. Supplemental and sensitivity analyses suggest alternate approaches to investigating these associations and overcoming limitations. While plentiful evidence underscores that being socially connected is good for the mind, future research should continue to consider whether it impacts neural markers of aging and longevity.
... Como explica Barros (2004), los modos de vida individuales de estos se insertan en prácticas colectivas, y en conjunto van construyendo actividades cotidianas de socialización que se tornan relevantes para el grupo en ese contexto. Esto coincide con diversas investigaciones (Giles et al., 2005;Loucks et al., 2006) que muestran cómo este círculo tiene impacto en la calidad de vida de las personas, lo que justifica el deseo de mantenerlos cerca en el futuro (Sousa y Cerqueria-Santos, 2011), así como con lo expresado por Santos (2017), quien destaca que la adolescencia y la juventud están caracterizados por un importante deseo por el bienestar de la familia. ...
Article
Full-text available
El presente estudio tiene como objetivo la identificación de los imaginarios colectivos e imaginarios utópicos de la población universitaria peruana en el contexto de la pospandemia. Al abordar los imaginarios sociales, esta investigación busca comprender las formas en las cuales las mentalidades colectivas de las personas jóvenes universitarias fueron impactadas por el contexto de la pandemia y, en ese sentido, cómo el contexto del tiempo presente, así como del futuro muestran sus realidades, sus esperanzas y sus temores. Se trata de un estudio básico-descriptivo con un enfoque cualitativo que analiza 20 cartas redactadas por un grupo de jóvenes de una universidad peruana como parte de una actividad académica en un curso de nivel de pregrado. La intención es reconocer los pensamientos, emociones y vivencias de estos individuos. La dinámica de la escritura creativa facilitó una aproximación más íntima a las historias personales del estudiantado, evidenciando la relevancia del círculo familiar y amistoso en sus vidas. Además, se identificaron las principales afectaciones que resaltaron durante la pandemia, como los problemas de salud mental y la fragilidad de sus relaciones sociales. Las conclusiones principales invitan a cuestionar la falta de una perspectiva política tradicional entre estas personas jóvenes, quienes experimentan y reflexionan sobre su entorno social únicamente desde la órbita de lo más cercano (ellas mismas, su familia y sus amistades).
Article
The outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic caused significant disruptions to conventional educational methodologies, resulting in a swift transition towards online and remote learning. This shift presented notable obstacles, especially for students with specific requirements like autism spectrum disorder (ASD). This current theoretical analysis explores the potential of electronic learning as a tool to facilitate the academic advancement of ASD students from the perspective of social neuroscience. Studies in social neuroscience emphasize the profound consequences of social seclusion on mental well-being and emotional health. Prolonged periods of social distancing and the absence of in-person interactions may have adverse effects on cognitive growth and academic achievement. Nonetheless, the digital realm could provide distinct benefits for ASD students, who frequently encounter difficulties in social interaction and emotional management within traditional class settings. The assessment delves into the social-cognitive deficiencies linked with ASD and how online learning could supplement the educational requirements of these students. Despite conflicting findings on the effectiveness of online education for ASD, this review proposes that an adaptable and personalized approach tailored to individual needs could improve learning prospects. Ultimately, the infusion of social neuroscience principles into educational methodologies could facilitate the establishment of a more comprehensive and supportive learning atmosphere for all students, including those with specific needs.
Article
Full-text available
Urban parks play a crucial role in enhancing the social interactions of older adults. However, despite the broad recognition of urban parks’ benefits, there is a notable gap in research focusing on their role in promoting social interactions, particularly in Asia. This study explores the effects of personal, social, and physical factors and park use patterns on older adults’ social interactions. Survey data from 589 older adults aged 50 years or older were collected through face-to-face and online questionnaires and were analyzed using a hierarchical multiple regression model. The results showed that personal factors, social factors, physical factors, and park use patterns explained 10.8%, 8.2%, 9.4%, and 2.3% of the total variance in park social interactions, respectively. Key factors like gender, health status, social cohesion, features, conditions, accessibility, and park use patterns were found to significantly influence these interactions. This study provides empirical evidence to support the important role of urban parks in facilitating social interactions among older adults and contributes to a deeper understanding of the complex factors affecting these interactions. To meet the needs of older adults and maximize the social health benefits, these prominent factors should be emphasized in policy development and interventions, integrating older adults’ perceptions and personal experiences.
Article
Full-text available
A total of 1,054 Hong Kong Chinese subjects aged 70 years or over were recruited into a cohort study to investigate the relation between social support and health outcomes. More than 30 social, health, and behavioral charactenstics were recorded as baseline information when the study began in 1985. Mortality data were obtained dunng a 2-year follow-up. Logistic regression analyses were used to determine the roles of these variables in predicting mortality. The mortality patterns of Hong Kong and of the studied cohort closely resemble that of Western developed countnes with cancer, heart disease, and cerebrovascular diseases as the leading causes of death. Besides sex and place of residence (whether living in the community or in homes for the elderly), the independent predictors of mortality included five baseline variables: being single or widowed, limited ability in activities of daily living, smoking habit, low body mass index, and poor self-evaluated health status. Subjects with at least three of these predictors had a relative risk of 3.9 (95% confidence interval 2 4–6.2) compared with those with zero to two of these characteristics Am J Epidemiol 1991; 133: 907–21
Article
Incomplete failure data consisting of times to failure on failed units and differing running times on unfailed units are called multiply censored. Data on units operating in the field, for example, are usually multiply censored. Presented in this paper is a method of plotting multiply censored data on hazard paper to obtain engineering information on the distribution of time to failure. Step-by-step instructions on how to plot and interpret data on hazard paper are given with the aid of examples based on real and simulated data. Hazard paper is presented here for the exponential, Weibull, normal, log normal, and extreme value distributions. The theory underlying hazard paper and plotting is presented in an appendix.
Article
The authors investigated associations between social integration and all-cause and cause-specific mortality among French employees of Electricity of France-Gas of France. A total of 12,347 men aged 40-50 years in 1989 and 4,352 women aged 35-50 years in 1989 comprised the sample. In age-adjusted survival analyses for all causes of death, men who were least socially integrated were 4.42 times as likely to die during follow-up (1993-1999) as those with the highest level of integration (p Language: en
Article
This study examined the relationship between social network interaction and total and cardiovascular mortality in 17,433 Swedish men and women between the ages of 29 and 74 during a 6 year follow-up period. The study group was interviewed concerning their social network interactions and a total score was formed which summarized the availability of social contact. A number of sociodemographic and health related background variables known to be associated with mortality risk were also considered. Mortality was examined by linking the interview material with the Swedish National Mortality Registry. In the 6-year follow-up period 841 deaths occurred. The crude relative risk of dying during this period was 3.7 (95% CL 3.2; 4.3) when the lower social network tertile was compared to the upper two tertiles. When controlling for potential confounding effects, only age had a major influence on the association between social network interaction and mortality (RR age-adjusted = 1.46, 95% CL 1.25; 1.72). Controlling for age and sex, age and educational level, age and employment status, age and immigrant status, age and smoking, age and exercise habits and age and chronic disease at interview left the relative risk virtually unchanged. Controlling simultaneously for age, smoking, exercise and chronic illness yielded a risk estimate of 1.36 (95% CL 1.06; 1.69). Similar results were obtained when separately analyzing for cardiovascular disease mortality in an identical manner.
Article
Half Title Title Copyright Dedication Preface Contents
Book
Introduction.- Estimating the Survival and Hazard Functions.- The Cox Model.- Residuals.- Functional Form.- Testing Proportional Hazards.- Influence.- Multiple Events per Subject.- Frailty Models.- Expected Survival.
Article
D.R. Cox has suggested a simple method for the regression analysis of censored data. We carry out an information calculation which shows that Cox's method has full asymptotic efficiency under conditions which are likely to be satisfied in many realistic situations. The connection of Cox's method with the Kaplan-Meier estimator of a survival curve is made explicit.
Article
The CES-D scale is a short self-report scale designed to measure depressive symptomatology in the general population. The items of the scale are symptoms associated with depression which have been used in previously validated longer scales. The new scale was tested in household interview surveys and in psychiatric settings. It was found to have very high internal consistency and adequate test- retest repeatability. Validity was established by pat terns of correlations with other self-report measures, by correlations with clinical ratings of depression, and by relationships with other variables which support its construct validity. Reliability, validity, and factor structure were similar across a wide variety of demographic characteristics in the general population samples tested. The scale should be a useful tool for epidemiologic studies of de pression.