ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Neuropsychological Aspects for Evaluating Learning Disabilities

Authors:

Abstract

This review surveys the empirical literature for assessments of learning problems in children from a neuropsychological perspective. An evaluation of children with learning problems must consider measures of working memory, attention, executive function, and comprehension (listening and written), particularly for children who do not respond to intervention. These constructs must be tied to intervention techniques, and their connections must be empirically verified. The response-to-intervention (RTI) perspective provides excellent support for the process in young children but is still developing the process for students above the second grade. This review provides information about the existing research on neurobiological correlates of learning disabilities, possible areas for further evaluation, and the link to the RTI movement.
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES
VOLUME 38, NUMBER 6, NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 2005, PAGES 563–568
Neuropsychological Aspects for
Evaluating Learning Disabilities
Margaret Semrud-Clikeman
Abstract
This review surveys the empirical literature for assessments of learning problems in children from a neuropsychological perspective. An
evaluation of children with learning problems must consider measures of working memory, attention, executive function, and compre-
hension (listening and written), particularly for children who do not respond to intervention. These constructs must be tied to interven-
tion techniques, and their connections must be empirically verified. The response-to-intervention (RTI) perspective provides excellent
support for the process in young children but is still developing the process for students above the second grade. This review provides
information about the existing research on neurobiological correlates of learning disabilities, possible areas for further evaluation, and
the link to the RTI movement.
L
earning disabilities have been de-
fined in various ways over time.
Terms such as minimal brain dys-
function, word blindness, and dyslexia
were once widely used. The term learn-
ing disabilities became popular particu-
larly with the passage of P.L. 94-142 in
1975 (Goldstein, 1997). A definition
proposed by the National Joint Com-
mittee for Learning Disabilities in 1981
suggested that “these disorders are in-
trinsic to the individual and presume
to be due to central nervous dysfunc-
tion” (Hammill, Leigh, McNutt, &
Larsen, 1981, p. 340). This definition in-
cluded difficulties with reading, math-
ematics, listening comprehension,
written language, and expressive and
receptive language. Although the term
learning disabilities has been under-
stood to be a heterogeneous term, most
laypeople and many teachers interpret
it to mean difficulties in reading. The
empirical field also shows this empha-
sis on reading with the abundance of
articles about reading disabilities com-
pared with those written on mathe-
matics, written language, or social
learning difficulties.
Learning disabilities are comor-
bid with other diagnoses including
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
der (ADHD), anxiety, and depression
(Martinez & Semrud-Clikeman, 2004).
ADHD has been found to co-occur in
approximately 20% to 50% of children
with reading difficulties, depending on
the method of calculating a learning
disability (Semrud-Clikeman et al.,
1992). ADHD has also been found to
co-occur with difficulties in mathemat-
ics (Semrud-Clikeman, 2003), written
language (Hargrave, Corlett, & Semrud-
Clikeman, 2002), and social–emotional
learning disabilities (Semrud-Clikeman,
2003).
Comorbidity of learning problems
with other diagnoses makes it impera-
tive to evaluate these possibilities
when developing a remediation pro-
gram for a child (Pennington, 1990).
Moreover, we are just beginning to un-
derstand the contribution of these re-
lated but separate diagnoses to learn-
ing. Far more research is available that
evaluates brain functioning in children
with reading disabilities than in those
with other learning problems or those
who show a combination of difficul-
ties. Although, because of restrictions
on length, the focus of this article will
be on reading disabilities, the other
areas of learning disabilities are equally
important and, I hope, can be high-
lighted at another time.
Learning disabilities have gener-
ally been identified through the use of
a discrepancy between measured abil-
ity (IQ) and achievement (Joshi, 1999).
As noted by other articles in this issue,
this type of assessment is very narrow,
does not directly lead to recommenda-
tions for remediation, and ignores the
various neuropsychological functions
underlying the ability to read, speak,
comprehend, write, and do mathemat-
ics well. This model also has difficul-
ties because it relies on a simple differ-
ence between two scores to determine
the presence of a learning disability
and provides little information as to
the predicted learning curve of the
child or adolescent being evaluated.
Moreover, this practice has also been
found to overidentify those children
with high IQs and average achieve-
ment and underidentify those with
lower IQs and below-average achieve-
ment (Birch & Semrud-Clikeman, 2002;
Semrud-Clikeman et al., 1992).
The acknowledgment of deficient
brain functioning in children with learn-
ing disabilities has become more widely
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES
564
accepted, particularly with the advent
of research evaluating the functioning
of the brain. A discussion of the most
recent findings about the neural struc-
tures involved in reading may high-
light the various aspects of the learning
process that need to be evaluated.
Brain Imaging and
Learning Disabilities
Emerging findings suggest that chil-
dren with learning disabilities process
information differently from those with-
out learning problems. Differences in
development have shown that fluent
adult readers utilize the frontal regions
more than do children who are begin-
ning to read (Schlaggar, 2003). The left
frontal region becomes more active
over the course of development, and
more fluent child readers activate this
area more than do children with diffi-
culties (Schlaggar et al., 2002). More-
over, children with learning problems
show a differential pattern compared
with normal readers; they activate the
parietal and occipital areas more than
the frontal regions, and also show
more activation in the right hemi-
sphere than in the left. This finding is
important because activation of the left
hemisphere, a region specialized for
language functions, plays an impor-
tant function in reading.
The change from posterior systems
in early reading (visual–perceptual pro-
cesses) to frontal systems by more flu-
ent readers suggests that the progres-
sion from simple letter and word
calling to comprehension requires a
maturation of neural pathways from
the back of the brain to the front (S.
Shaywitz, 2003). Moreover, children
show a more diffuse activation when
they begin learning to read that gradu-
ally becomes more specialized as their
reading improves. Similarly, when nor-
mal readers were asked to read single
words, they showed left hemispheric
activation, whereas those with dyslexia
showed more right hemispheric activa-
tion (Breier et al., 2003; Papanicolaou,
2003). Changes from right hemispheric
processing to left hemispheric process-
ing have been found to occur with im-
provement in reading skills. These
changes are also found when improve-
ment in language functioning occurs.
Such changes are not found for chil-
dren with dyslexia, as their reading
process does not become automatic
and effortless.
Gabrieli (2003) found that the re-
gion most responsible for auditory
processing and language is more acti-
vated in good readers than in those
who had compensated for their dys-
lexia. These researchers found that
more activation correlated with higher
scores on reading measures. These
studies also found that improvements
were found in activation following re-
mediation of auditory processing abil-
ity. It is not clear at present whether
these changes continue over time. Fur-
ther study is needed to understand
possible brain response to remediation.
The Neuropsychology of
Learning Disabilities
An evaluation that centers solely on
the simple process of subtracting, or
regressing IQ from achievement, is a
narrow one that misses many of the
difficulties frequently seen in these chil-
dren. The processing of information is
a complex and distributed operation.
To evaluate the child’s learning skills,
one must understand the child’s ability
to process language, to understand
what he or she hears, and to organize
information; the speed with which the
child processes information; and the
child’s attention, ability to hold infor-
mation in mind while solving a prob-
lem, and ability to self-monitor the
reading process.
Language difficulties have fre-
quently accompanied problems in learn-
ing to read. These language problems
may be in receptive or expressive lan-
guage. The phonology of the language
can be tricky to master. Language is a
natural process of our brain, and brain
structures are devoted to its develop-
ment. Reading, however, is an acquired
skill that children must be directly
taught. When a child has a language
problem in addition to reading deficits,
the progress is much more difficult.
Approximately 70% to 80% of children
learn phonological coding skills with-
out difficulty. The remaining 20% to
30% show differing levels of success,
and based on previous studies, the de-
termining aspect may be the interven-
tion provided as well as the child’s
overall verbal skills.
The ability to decode words is a
fairly well-known area of difficulty for
children with learning disabilities; how-
ever, more recent research indicates
that the main difficulty is not just the
decoding of the word but also the rate
of decoding (Joshi, 1999; Woodcock,
1991). Speed of information processing
separates fluent from nonfluent read-
ers (Semrud-Clikeman, Guy, & Griffin,
2000). Children with reading disabili-
ties are slower at naming words and
nonwords as well as at naming letters
and numbers (Aaron et al., 1999).
An important aspect and goal for
reading is comprehension. The same
cognitive processes mediate listening
comprehension and reading compre-
hension, but they do so through a dif-
ferent modality (Joshi, 1999). Assessing
the ability to process information with-
out the confound of decoding allows
one to more fully evaluate the child’s
ability to understand and process lan-
guage and to determine whether the
difficulty lies with decoding or com-
prehension. An evaluation of these
skills is necessary to understand where
the breakdown in skills lies and, thus,
to develop the most appropriate inter-
vention.
An additional neuropsychological
process that is important to reading
skills development is working mem-
ory. Working memory is the ability to
hold information in mind while solv-
ing a problem, remembering a phone
number, or decoding a word. Working
VOLUME 38, NUMBER 6, NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 2005
565
memory is a crucial skill for early read-
ing recognition and later reading com-
prehension; one must assess it if one is
to develop the most appropriate method
of intervention (Teeter & Semrud-
Clikeman, 1997).
Adele Diamond studied working
memory in young children. She had a
child observe her hiding an object and
then asked the child a few seconds
later where the object was. Children
younger than 1 year could not find the
object and used the rule of “Out of
sight, out of mind.” Before age 1, the
frontal lobes are unable to process de-
layed information. However, as chil-
dren grow, they become more able to
retain information for a short amount
of time while processing information.
To decode words, the child’s working
memory must be functional and allow
the child to retain a “template” of the
letters until the word is sounded out. If
a breakdown occurs in the ability to
hold this information in mind, or if the
time required recalling the sound–
symbol relationship is prolonged, the
child will experience difficulty reading
(Semrud-Clikeman et al., 2000).
Working memory has also been
linked to the ability to organize a task’s
temporal aspect. Not only is input en-
coded; the task is also tagged to a time
when it was learned (Gazzaniga, Ivry,
& Magnum, 2002). The prefrontal cor-
tex is linked to memory systems that
allow the child access to previously
learned materials. If difficulty is pres-
ent at the outset, or at the working
memory stage, the child will have
difficulty recalling previously learned
skills (i.e., the letter c in c-a-t has a cer-
tain sound), and thus decoding will be
slower and effortful. Similar difficulty
arises in spelling and in learning math-
ematics. For example, in mathematics
the child needs to remember certain
mathematics facts as well as when to
use a particular procedure.
Executive functions are also skills
that are important for the learning
process. These skills apply to how
something is accomplished rather than
just to what is accomplished. They
help a child evaluate his or her perfor-
mance, and they also inhibit response
to irrelevant stimuli. The selection of
what is important to encode is essential
in learning to read, write, and do math-
ematics. In addition, a child needs to
learn to listen to what he or she is read-
ing (either orally or silently) and eval-
uate its correctness. This skill becomes
more important in older grades, as the
child must self-correct mistakes. The
awareness of “how I’m doing” is cru-
cial to the learning process and allows
the child to change behaviors or to take
corrective action as necessary. These
skills do not come into full fruition
until early adulthood, and some would
suggest that not until we are 32 years
of age do we have a fully mature brain
(Denckla, 2003). Thus, an important
issue to assessment would be to evalu-
ate the child’s ability to understand his
or her thinking processes.
Learning Disability
Identification Process
Remediation suggests that an under-
standing of the underlying processes
in learning have been evaluated, either
formally or informally. The multitier
process suggested by response to in-
tervention (RTI) ties assessment to in-
tervention for those children requiring
more specialized and intensive treat-
ment than is available in the first tier or
in the general education classroom. A
feature of RTI is academic and behav-
ioral screening with a valid assessment
measure and continued monitoring if
substantial progress has not been dem-
onstrated. However, the screening tool
to be used is not defined or even ex-
plained. This difficulty is reminiscent
of the original definition of a learning
disability that required a “significant
discrepancy” but did not define what
significant entailed. Such ambiguity
has plagued this field and appears to
be continuing. Recommendations for
better defined specific tools or mea-
sures are important to help standard-
ize these procedures nationwide.
The research base for learning dis-
abilities has been complicated by diffi-
culties with definitions. States vary in
how learning disabilities are defined;
definitions range from few criteria to
very stringent. In Texas, for example, a
child can be identified as learning dis-
abled by a 16-point standard score
point discrepancy, whereas in Minne-
sota, the discrepancy must be more
than 2 standard deviations. Moreover,
a child who does not meet criteria for a
learning disability in Texas but who
shows at least an 8-point discrepancy
from IQ is classified as dyslexic. Such
unevenness of definitions makes iden-
tification of these children as well as
determination of appropriate interven-
tions for them more difficult.
The RTI model suggests that for
some children, identification would
not occur until they had failed, and use
of the model may lead to denial of ser-
vices to some children clearly at risk
for learning disabilities. A full assess-
ment would also not occur until after
the child had repeatedly failed at some
of the interventions. Although the goal
to tie how the child responds to inter-
vention has interesting possibilities,
the difficulty lies in how this response
to intervention is evaluated. If this
process is considered evaluative, then
psychometric properties for the assess-
ment need to be developed and at this
point are not provided.
Emerging evidence also indicates
that particular times in development
may be most advantageous for reme-
diation. The developing brain learns
new information through a set of neu-
ropsychological processes, and these
processes lay down new neural con-
nections that, once formed, may be dif-
ficult to reteach. Also important from a
neuropsychological point of view is
the finding that the brain is most ready
to learn these connections within cer-
tain points of time, namely between
the ages of 5 and 8, and for higher level
thinking skills from the ages of 12 to 15
(Teeter & Semrud-Clikeman, 1997).
The longitudinal study of dys-
lexia by S. Shaywitz (2003) found that
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES
566
poor readers who had compensated
for their difficulties through remedia-
tion utilized brain areas that were dif-
ferent from those used by readers who
continued to have difficulty. More im-
portant, the children who showed
compensation not only had higher ver-
bal ability scores than those who did
not; they also attended less disadvan-
taged schools. A control group received
the “usual” interventions and showed
very little improvement. By delaying
intervention until failure, the compen-
sated systems may not develop, or
they may develop less well than with
younger children. The study of the
time window in which remediation is
most effective has not been fully com-
pleted, but response to intervention
may differ depending on the age of the
child.
A partial solution may lie in de-
veloping appropriate screening instru-
ments that can assist in isolating those
children most at risk for later difficul-
ties and tracking their progress care-
fully through the early school years.
The multitier system can easily utilize
this procedure, but agreement is needed
as to what the most important aspects
are that are evaluated and monitored
early on. In addition, besides behav-
ioral measures, measures that tap neu-
ropsychological constructs such as at-
tention or working memory could be
incorporated into this screening.
Coupled with these concerns is
the suggestion that children with
learning problems be provided in-
struction in the regular classroom until
significant failure occurs. This model
assumes that the regular education
teacher has been taught the skills
needed not only to identify children
with learning problems but also to de-
vise an intervention to offset these dif-
ficulties. Most of the RTI research has
centered on children in kindergarten
and first-grade classrooms. Very little
empirical evidence suggests that this
program is appropriate for children at
older ages. Prior to implementation of
this program for all children, studies
with children in middle school and
high school must be conducted to de-
termine the appropriateness of the
model for children of this age. In addi-
tion, sorting out variables such as at-
tention and emotionality that may also
be part and parcel of a reading problem
is important.
Such assessments require special-
ized skills among staff, and experience
in administering these measures is re-
quired in a comprehensive individual
assessment. To achieve the laudable
goal of introducing regular education
professionals to working with these
children, it is necessary to provide ad-
ditional education for these teachers,
as well as providing master teachers
for support. Understanding the nature
of learning is also important. The link
from neuropsychological processes to
intervention has not yet been forged,
but the previous section on brain imag-
ing suggests that there is much to un-
derstand about how we learn which
will, one hopes, lead to appropriate in-
terventions.
The simple RTI model incorpo-
rates several features that are very use-
ful in our understanding of learning
disabilities and, more important, our
understanding as to appropriate inter-
ventions. The implementation of a uni-
versal screening procedure for speci-
fied skills is very useful. Moreover,
tying this screening procedure to proven
effective interventions is invaluable.
These steps are necessary for the iden-
tification of children with learning dis-
abilities but are insufficient for the fol-
lowing reasons.
First, the evaluation is skills fo-
cused and does not provide informa-
tion as to the ability of the child to gen-
eralize learning or to complete more
inferential or abstract tasks. This em-
phasis on skills may be appropriate for
early grades but becomes less appro-
priate for higher grades, beginning at
around Grade 4. For example, a child
may be able to read all of the words in
a passage but may not be able to com-
prehend the meaning behind the words.
A child in first or second grade may not
show a significant problem in this area,
but if this difficulty continues into
third grade and beyond, the problem
becomes more serious; it transcends
the reading class and has implications
for content courses such as science and
social studies as well as mathematics
word problems.
Second, RTI is unable to differen-
tiate learners with varying learning
needs. A child with an attentional prob-
lem may have a reading problem, but
the appropriate intervention is not the
same as one for a child who has de-
coding difficulties. Both children may
respond to small-group instruction,
but for different reasons. When these
children are reintroduced to the larger
classroom, the likelihood is high that
the child with attention problems will
not succeed—not because he or she
cannot do the reading, but because he
or she cannot follow through on the
work (Semrud-Clikeman et al., 1999).
The RTI framework does not directly
acknowledge the contributions that
can be made by neuropsychology and
thus lacks an integral part of our un-
derstanding of how children learn and
process the world around them.
A possible solution to these seem-
ingly discrepant models would be the
melding of the neuropsychological
framework and RTI into a more com-
plex model. Such an integration would
contribute to our understanding of
children who are not responding in the
manner that we would expect in the
initial tiers of RTI. Screening children
on predictor variables such as working
memory, attention, and executive func-
tions would be helpful not only to
monitor progress but also to “catch”
children who are a higher risk of not
responding to the intervention at an
earlier stage. The use of cutoff scores
could readily be incorporated into the
screening already routinely completed
by RTI. These measures are not time-
intensive and can provide additional
information for the teacher and the par-
ent and for older children themselves.
Performance that is more than 1 stan-
dard deviation below expectations for
the age of the child should be closely
monitored, and children scoring 2 stan-
dard deviations below average should
be referred for an evaluation to rule out
VOLUME 38, NUMBER 6, NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 2005
567
any conditions that may interfere with
their progress. Moreover, those stu-
dents that do not respond to the early
stages of RTI are appropriate for a
more comprehensive evaluation to de-
termine why they are not responding
to appropriate interventions.
Summary and
Conclusions
Educational practice is at an exciting
time in development. Not only have
we evidence that children with dys-
lexia (and possibly other learning dis-
abilities) have brain differences com-
pared with typically reading children;
emerging data indicate that they re-
spond relatively quickly to brain-
based and comprehensive teaching ap-
proaches that have empirical support
(Berninger, 2003). Additional findings
indicate that the most effective in-
terventions are those that involve sys-
tematic instruction that is explicit and
continues throughout their school
experience (B. Shaywitz, 2003). More-
over, predictions of response to inter-
vention are best completed by neuro-
psychological measures of language
and attention rather than through the
use of a discrepancy model (Stage, Ab-
bott, Jenkins, & Berninger, 2003). These
findings support the use of a multi-
method evaluation of skills required
for successful reading. Strassner,
Semrud-Clikeman, and Gerrard-Morris
(2003) found that teachers have lower
expectations of academic performance
from children who have ADHD or
learning problems. These expectations
may, in turn, lead to less attention in
the classroom and fewer appropriate
interventions.
One of the most important con-
clusions from research is that for chil-
dren with learning problems, learning
is hard work. A corollary to this find-
ing is that for their teachers, instruction
is very hard work and requires an
enormous amount of training and sup-
port. Children who have difficulty
learning to read or completing mathe-
matics problems will likely not benefit
from “more of the same” but require an
alternative method of teaching to assist
their learning.
Until now, we have emphasized a
specific type of educational placement
(resource or inclusion). However,
based on the data from neuroimaging
studies, we need to develop methods
from scientifically supported instruc-
tional strategies, and we need to un-
derstand whether different types of
interventions are interchangeable or
work as efficiently for most children.
Work on this aspect of RTI has not been
completed.
The definitional struggle that has
characterized the field of learning dis-
abilities is continuing. An important
piece of this puzzle, which has been
missing in the debate, is how children
respond to various interventions and
how we can match the intervention to
the difficulty. We have several years of
experience showing that the “usual”
method of teaching reading works for
most children and with an adjustment
(going from phonics to look-say, etc.)
works for many children who cannot
profit from a single method. What we
have not fully discovered, and what is
now developing, is the ability to work
with those children who have been de-
fined as “treatment resistant,” that is,
those children who do not seem to
profit from either general approach.
We also need to learn how these chil-
dren may differ early on, so that our in-
tervention can occur before significant
failure sets in.
The multitier approach to inter-
vention has much promise, but again,
we need to prepare our teachers so that
they are best able to identify the chil-
dren who need a fuller evaluation of
their abilities. Given the findings from
the neuroimaging and neuropsycho-
logical fields of deficient performance
on measures of working memory, pro-
cessing speed, auditory processing
ability, and executive functions, evalu-
ation of these skills is necessary to de-
termine the most appropriate program
to fit the individual child’s need. The
danger with not paying attention to in-
dividual differences is that we will
repeat the current practice of simple
assessments in curricular materials to
evaluate a complex learning process
and to plan for interventions with chil-
dren and adolescents with markedly
different needs and learning profiles.
ABOUT THE AUTHOR
Margaret Semrud-Clikeman, PhD, is a pro-
fessor in the Department of Educational Psy-
chology at the University of Texas at Austin and
is the program director for the APA-accredited
School Psychology Program. Her research in-
terests include neuroimaging in developmental
disorders, interventions for children with ADHD
or nonverbal disabilities, and neuropsychologi-
cal assessment. Address: Margaret Semrud-
Clikeman, PhD, Department of Educational
Psychology, 1 University Station, D 5800, Uni-
versity of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712;
e-mail: peg.semrud@mail.utexas.edu
REFERENCES
Aaron, P. G., Joshi, R. M., Ayotollah, M.,
Ellsberry, A., Henderson, J., & Lindsey, K.
(1999). Decoding and single-word nam-
ing: Are they two independent compo-
nents of word-recognition skills? Reading
and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,
14, 89–127.
Berninger, V. W. (2003, November). Effects of
phonological, morphological, and ortho-
graphic treatment on brain activation. Paper
presented at the International Dyslexia
Association conference, San Diego, CA.
Birch, K.G., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (in
preparation). Phonological processing, auto-
maticity, auditory processing and memory in
slow learners and children with reading dis-
abilities.
Breier, J. I., Simos, P. G., & Fletcher, J. M.
(2002). Abnormal activation of tempo-
parietal language areas during phonetic
analysis in children with dyslexia. Neu-
ropsychology, 17, 610–621.
Denckla, M. B. (2003, October). Executive
functions. Paper presented at the Capital
Area IDA meeting, Washington, DC.
Gabrieli, J. (2003, November). Neuroimaging
evidence about the brain basis of dyslexia.
Paper presented at the International Dys-
lexia Association conference, San Diego,
CA.
Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun,
G. R. (2002). Cognitive Neuroscience. New
York: W. W. Norton.Goldstein, S. (1997).
Managing attention and learning disorders
in late adolescence and young adulthood.
New York: John Wiley.
Hammill, D. D., Leigh, J. E., McNutt, G., &
Larsen, S. C. (1981). A new definition of
learning disabilities. Learning Disability
Quarterly, 4, 336–342.
Hargrave, J., Corlett, M., & Semrud-
Clikeman, M. (2002, August). Written ex-
pression deficiencies in children with ADHD.
Paper presented at the annual conference
of the American Psychological Associa-
tion, Chicago, IL.
Joshi, R. M. (1999). A diagnostic procedure
based on reading component model. In
I. Lundberg, F. E. Tonnessen, & I. Austad
(Eds.), Dyslexia: Advances in theory and
practice (pp. 207–219). Dordrecht, The
Netherlands: Kluwer.
Martinez, R., & Semrud-Clikeman, M.
(2004). Emotional adjustment of young
adolescents with different learning dis-
ability subtypes. Journal of Learning Dis-
abilities, 37, 411–420.
Papanicolaou, A. C. (2003, November).
Brain imaging in normal and impaired read-
ing: A developmental-educational perspec-
tive. Paper presented at the International
Dyslexia Association conference, San Di-
ego, CA.
Schlaggar, B. L. (2003, November). FMRI
and the development of single word reading.
Paper presented at the International Dys-
lexia Association conference, San Diego,
CA.
Schlaggar, B. L., Brown, T. T., Lugar, H. M.,
Visscher, K. M., Miezin, F. M., & Petersen,
S. E. (2002). Functional neuroanatomical
differences between adults and school-
age children in processing of single words.
Science, 296, 1476.
Schnoebelen, S., Semrud-Clikeman, M.,
Guli, L., & Corlett, M. (2002, August).
Planning and organization in children with
learning disabilities. Paper presented at the
annual conference of the American Psy-
chological Association, Chicago, IL.
Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2003). Executive
functions and social communication dis-
orders. Perspectives, 29, 20–22.
Semrud-Clikeman, M., Biederman, J.,
Sprich-Buckminster, S., Krifcher Lehman,
B., Faraone, S. V., & Norman, D. (1992).
The incidence of ADHD and concurrent
learning disabilities. Journal of the Ameri-
can Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy-
chiatry, 31, 439–448.
Semrud-Clikeman, M., Guy, K. A., & Grif-
fin, J. D. (2000). Rapid automatized nam-
ing in children with reading disabilities
and attention deficit hyperactivity disor-
der. Brain and Language, 74, 70–83.
Semrud-Clikeman, M., Harrington, K.,
Parle, N., Clinton, A., & Connor, R. (1999).
Innovative interventions with children
with attentional difficulties in the school
setting. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32,
581–590.
Shaywitz, B. (2003, November). Disruption
of neural systems in dyslexia and their im-
provement with intervention. Paper pre-
sented at the International Dyslexia As-
sociation conference, San Diego, CA.
Shaywitz, S. (2003). Overcoming dyslexia: A
new and complete science-based program for
reading problems at any level. New York:
Alfred A. Knopf.
Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fulbright,
R. K., Skudlarski, P., Mencl, W. E., Con-
stable, R. T., et al. (2003). Neural Systems
for compensation and persistence: Young
adult outcomes of childhood reading dis-
ability. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 25–33.
Stage, S. A., Abbott, R. D., Jenkins, J. R., &
Berninger, V. W. (2003). Predicting re-
sponse to early reading intervention from
verbal IQ, reading-related language abil-
ities, attention ratings, and verbal IQ–
word reading discrepancy: Failure to
validate discrepancy method. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 36, 24–33.
Strassner, E., Semrud-Clikeman, M., &
Gerrard-Morris, A. (2003,). Social difficul-
ties in children with developmental disorders.
Paper presented at the International Dys-
lexia Association conference, San Diego,
CA.
Teeter, P. A., & Semrud-Clikeman, M.
(1997). Child Neuropsychological Assess-
ment and Intervention. Boston: Allyn &
Bacon.
Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Woodcock Lan-
guage Proficiency Battery–Revised. Chicago:
Riverside.
568
JOURNAL OF LEARNING DISABILITIES
... A combination of learning disorders is accompanied by a heterogeneous frame of cognitive impairments in these children, with different processes recognized as affected, such as verbal comprehension, processing speed, and working memory (WM) [6][7][8]. Working memory is the most consistently affected process in these children [9,10] and can predict future academic difficulties [11]. A defective WM means a diminished capacity to access, maintain, or retrieve information. ...
... Table 1 shows the main descriptive characteristics of both groups. In children with LDs, there are instances of deficits in verbal comprehension, working memory, and processing speed [6][7][8]; however, in this study, the four indices (including perceptual reasoning) were taken as independent regressors of the ERP data described below, although according to the literature [33,43,44,47], we only expected to find WM and verbal comprehension as significant predictors of the ERP components. ...
... Children with LDs have been previously found to have impairments in cognitive domains such as verbal comprehension, working memory, and processing speed [6][7][8]. The WISC-IV scale gives an index for each of these domains and an index for perceptual reasoning. ...
Article
Full-text available
Children with learning disorders (LD children) often have heterogeneous cognitive impairments that affect their ability to learn and use basic academic skills. A proposed cause for this variability has been working memory (WM) capacity. Altered patterns of event-related potentials (ERPs) in these children have also been found in the N400 component associated with semantic priming. However, regarding the semantic priming effect in LD children, no distinction has been made for children with varying WM abilities. This study aims to explore the relationship of WM with the brain’s electrophysiological response that underlies semantic priming in LD children that performed a lexical decision task. A total of 40 children (8-10 years old) participated: 28 children with LD and 12 age-matched controls. The ERPs were recorded for each group and analyzed with permutation-based t-tests. The N400 effect was observed only in the control group, and both groups showed a late positive complex (LPC). Permutation-based regression analyses were performed for the results from the LD group using the WISC-IV indices (e.g., Verbal Comprehension and WM) as independent predictors of the ERPs. The Verbal Comprehension Index, but not the WM index, was a significant predictor of the N400 and LPC effects in LD children.
... According to the literature, D-KEFS has been used in both clinical and non-clinical populations to assess executive functions for various purposes [9]. In particular, it can predict executive dysfunctions in various clinical populations such as, in cases of traumatic brain injury, frontal lobe dysfunction, psychiatric disorders, neurodegenerative, learning and neurodevelopmental diseases, in both adults and children [9][10][11][12][13][14][15][16][17]. In addition, it has been also used for the prediction of neurodevelopmental disorders, because according to the study of Ridley et al. (2011) [13] who investigated the characteristics of the autistic phenotype in a typical population, D-KEFS can predict these characteristics in a clinical and even in non-clinical population. ...
... Despite that DFT has not been previously adapted in Greek adult population and therefore this is the normative data study, however the verbal fluency task is available for people between 18-79 years old in the study of Kosmidis et al. (2004) [36]. However, in their study, they calculated norms for people with 1-9 years of education, in comparison to our study which did not extracted normative data for this educational range due to lack of participants 20 years 10 after the first study conducted in Greek population. Additionally, Kosmidis et al. (2004) [36] used a version of a verbal fluency test which does not include switching, and therefore, this is the main gap which the current study aims to fill. ...
... They have balanced behaviors in the home environment, but when learning, they have defects in receiving, processing and storing information due to disruption in the neural pathways and brain communication ability (Pickard, Malloy, Porteous, Blackwood, & Muir, 2005). It is important to carefully examine the cause of learning disorders because these behaviors may have appeared as a sign of metabolic diseases or genetic defects that definitely need follow-up and treatment (Semrud-Clikeman, 2005). ...
... Studies have reported defects in executive function domains such as verbal working memory, sequencing [19,20], set-shifting [21], planning, timing [22], and response inhibition [23] in children with SLD. Executive function skills shape various routine and non-routine activities of daily life and also participation in the various areas of occupation [24][25][26]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background In addition to academic problems, children with a specific learning disability (SLD) encounter problems with participation in activities outside school. Purpose To investigate the efficacy of Occupational Performance Coaching (OPC) with and without Four Quadrant Model of Facilitated Learning (4QM) in the mothers of children with SLD. Method A single-blinded, parallel-group randomized clinical trial will be carried out. Mothers of children with SLD will constitute the participants and be allocated to experimental (OPC+4QM) and control (OPC alone) groups. Key issues Children's occupational performance and satisfaction, participation in activities outside school, goals attainment, executive function, and academic achievement along with self-efficacy of mothers will be measured in baseline, post-intervention, and one-month follow-up stages. Implications An OPC intervention protocol may improve children's participation in activities outside school and may help clarify whether 4QM promotes maternal empowerment and better results for children with SLD.
... In addition, results from these assessments can tell educators which children might benefit from extra support. However, expecting early education teachers to test young children on and interpret the results of a neuropsychological assessment, like a working memory task, has challenges (see Semrud-Clikeman, 2005). Fortunately, there are a number of relatively new, digital screeners that assess preschool children's EF skills and require little training for teachers to be able to use them with children (e.g., EF Touch for preschoolers; Kuhn et al., 2017). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Many of the skills children need to be successful in school are related to executive functioning (EF) skills. There is evidence that preschoolers’ EF skills, including their working memory, attention shifting, inhibition, and emotional regulation, help them to develop the academic and social-emotional skills required for success in school. Given how critically important these skills are, it is somewhat surprising that EF skills are not typically assessed in early education settings. Despite the absence of EF assessments in early childhood classrooms, there are a number of developmentally appropriate assessments that measure young children’s EF skills. This chapter focuses on the various methods used to assess EF skills in early childhood education settings. First, this chapter reviews the literature on EF skills in preschool-age children. Next, assessments that are appropriate for measuring EF skills in young children are described. Then, several evidence-based interventions that incorporate assessment of EF skills into the preschool program are discussed. The chapter concludes with recommendations for implementing EF assessments in early childhood education settings and future directions for research related to early assessment of EF skills.KeywordsExecutive functionEarly educationEarly interventionPreschoolAssessment
... Executive functions refer to a set of cognitive processes, including initiation, planning, decision-making, organization, reasoning, and self-regulation that previous studies in children with SLD have reported defects in executive function skills [9][10][11][12]. Children with executive dysfunction may have deficits in managing occupations with dynamic task demands and multiple steps, such as social participation and instrumental activities of daily living, which can lead to restrictions in their life roles [13,14]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Children with a specific learning disability (SLD) have deficits in everyday occupations along with executive function in addition to academic issues. Objective: The present study is aimed at investigating the effectiveness of Occupational Performance Coaching (OPC) and the Four-Quadrant Model of Facilitated Learning (4QM) interventions on the participation in occupational performance and executive function skills in children with SLD. Method: This study was a single-case experimental design (multiple baselines) in which six children with SLD were randomly assigned to three groups. In the baseline phase, three groups of children underwent repeated executive function assessments using the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) in the multiple baselines. In the intervention phase, all six mothers of children with SLD individually received OPC and 4QM interventions once a week for 14 sessions of 60 minutes and during this period, children were evaluated six more times for executive function skills according to SCWT and WCST. In addition, The Canadian Occupational Performance Measure (COPM) and Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) at the beginning and the end of the baseline phase and the end of the intervention phase were completed by mothers of children with SLD. Results: More than 50% PND of the SCWT and WCST in the visual analysis graph's information along with significant changes in COPM scores and large effect size of BRIEF subscales (Cohen's d ≥ 0.8) in pre- and postintervention showed the effectiveness of OPC and 4QM on the participation in occupational performance and executive function skills in children with SLD. Conclusion: The results of the study support the effectiveness of OPC and 4QM interventions on children with SLD. However, research with more participants and experimental methods can provide further evidence.
Article
Mastering writing skills is preceded by a significant developmental pathway of graphomotor abilities, which starts early in childhood. Considering that graphomotor ability and visual perception are prerequisites for mastering the writing, this study analyzed the graphomotor abilities in preschool-aged children, with the aim of identifying children exhibiting elements suspected of dysgraphia. The sample consisted of 100 preschool-aged children (age 5-6), of both genders (49 girls and 51 boys). Pre-writing skills were assessed using the Predictive Test for Dysgraphia. The research was conducted in a Preschool Institution in Danilovgrad, in March and April 2024. The results of analysis showed that preschool-aged children have specific difficulties in meeting the following criteria: accurately following the given sequence of figures and their careful and precise drawing (size and shape). It was also found that children of this age group have a particular problem in maintaining the specified distance of the figures from the edge of the rectangle (drawing around the edge). The only test item that the majority of respondents were able to respond to was perseverance in completing tasks, in terms of finishing the drawing (a series of started figures). Based on inadequate visual discrimination of the size and shape of given and drawn figures, and poor spatial orientation on paper, it is possible to identify children with graphomotor difficulties. Early detection of children with visual-perceptual and graphomotor difficulties will help overcome these deficiencies through systematic exercises through speech therapy treatments, so that children can start school being better prepared.
Article
Full-text available
The brain is the most complex organ in the human body and, as such, its study entails great challenges (methodological, theoretical, etc.). Nonetheless, there is a remarkable amount of studies about the consequences of pathological conditions on its development and functioning. This bibliographic review aims to cover mostly findings related to changes in the physical distribution of neurons and their connections—the connectome—both structural and functional, as well as their modelling approaches. It does not intend to offer an extensive description of all conditions affecting the brain; rather, it presents the most common ones. Thus, here, we highlight the need for accurate brain modelling that can subsequently be used to understand brain function and be applied to diagnose, track, and simulate treatments for the most prevalent pathologies affecting the brain.
Chapter
In this chapter, the authors describe further the three major assessment developments and demonstrate how they can improve the way practitioners understand and interpret assessment findings. These include the integration of psychometric Fluid‐Crystallized theories and the Cross‐Battery Assessment approach; the emergence of Response‐to‐Intervention and the patterns of strengths and weaknesses alternative research‐based procedure for SLD identification; and the integration of theories from the psychometric and neuropsychological traditions. Attention is paid to describing how the assessment developments are related, in particular neuropsychology and CHC theories, and how, when integrated, they have the potential to inform diagnosis and intervention more thoroughly. To that end, they begin with a brief description of each of the major assessment developments in the field of school psychology and its contributions to the various levels of assessment and evaluation in the schools. The late 1990s marked the first of three developments that influenced cognitive test selection and interpretation and school neuropsychological evaluations.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined whether and how two groups of young adults who were poor readers as children (a relatively compensated group and a group with persistent reading difficulties) differed from nonimpaired readers and if there were any factors distinguishing the compensated from persistently poor readers that might account for their different outcomes. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we studied three groups of young adults, ages 18.5-22.5 years, as they read pseudowords and real words: 1) persistently poor readers (PPR; n = 24); 2) accuracy improved (compensated) readers (AIR; n = 19); and 3) nonimpaired readers (NI, n = 27). Compensated readers, who are accurate but not fluent, demonstrate a relative underactivation in posterior neural systems for reading located in left parietotemporal and occipitotemporal regions. Persistently poor readers, who are both not fluent and less accurate, activate posterior reading systems but engage them differently from nonimpaired readers, appearing to rely more on memory-based rather than analytic word identification strategies. These findings of divergent neural outcomes as young adults are both new and unexpected and suggest a neural basis for reading outcomes of compensation and persistence in adults with childhood dyslexia.
Article
Full-text available
Event-related magnetic fields were recorded using magnetoencephalography in children with (n=12) and without (n=11) dyslexia while they discriminated between pairs of syllables from a voice onset time series (/ga/-/ka/). Nonimpaired readers exhibited left-hemisphere predominance of activity after the resolution of the N1m, whereas children with dyslexia experienced a sharp peak of relative activation in right temporoparietal areas between 300 and 700 ms post-stimulus onset. Increased relative activation in right temporoparietal areas was correlated with reduced performance on phonological processing measures. Results are consistent with the notion that deficits in appreciating the sound structure of both written and spoken language are associated with abnormal neurophysiological activity in temporoparietal language areas in children with dyslexia.
Article
Full-text available
Early adolescents (Grades 6-8) with multiple learning disabilities (LD; reading and math) in inclusive settings were compared to adolescents with single LD (reading or math) and typically achieving (TA) peers regarding their psychosocial functioning in two areas of adolescent well-being: emotional adjustment and school functioning. The Behavior Assessment System for Children (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1998) Self-Report of Personality for adolescents was used to determine well-being. One hundred twenty middle school students-15 boys and 15 girls in each group-were included in the current study. The results confirmed that adolescents with multiple LD (reading and math) reported poorer functioning (i.e., higher T scores) on school maladjustment, clinical maladjustment, emotional symptoms index, attitude to school, atypicality, and depression when compared to TA peers but not when compared to peers with a single LD (reading or math). All three groups differed from the TA group (but not from each other) on sense of inadequacy, with the multiple LD group reporting the highest T scores. Additional analyses indicated significant differences between girls and boys, regardless of disability status. Girls reported higher T scores on the emotional symptoms index, social stress, and depression, but boys reported greater school maladjustment and sensation seeking. Implications for practice and recommendations for future research are discussed.
Chapter
It is estimated that approximately 25 percent of the elementary age school children have some type of reading problem (Stedman & Kaestle, 1987). It is not surprising, therefore, that millions of dollars are spent on specialized programs such as Title I and other remedial reading programs to make children better readers. It is also recognized that early identification is essential if remediation is to be effective. If these children are not identified early for remedial purposes, a vicious cycle sets in resulting in the Matthew Effect in education (Stanovich, 1986; Walberg & Tsai, 1983).
Article
Word recognition skill is the foundation of the reading process. Word recognition could be accomplished by two major strategies: phonological decoding and sight-word reading, the latter being a marker for proficient reading. There is, however, a controversy regarding the relationship between decoding and sight-word reading, whether the two are independent or the latter is built on the foundations of the former. A related controversy about instructional strategy could be whether to use whole-word method to improve word recognition skills, or to first build decoding skills and then introduce sight words. Five goals were set up to address these issues: (a) developing a criterion that can be used easily by classroom teachers to assess sight-word reading ability, (b) examining this relationship between decoding and sight-word reading, (c) identifying the mechanism that can explain the relationship, (d) examining factors that facilitate sight-word reading, and (e) discussing potential instructional implications of these findings. In order to accomplish these goals, naming time and word-naming accuracy of three groups of subjects (elementary school children, children identified as having reading disability, and college students) were studied by using a variety of verbal materials. The over-all conclusions are that the difference in naming time of letters and words can be used as a metric for assessing sight-word reading skill. Sight-word reading appears to be intimately related to decoding. Sight-word reading is accomplished by parallel processing of constituent letters of words and is influenced also by the semantic nature of words. It is conjectured that sight-word reading instruction is likely to be successful if decoding skills are firmly established first.
Article
Exposición de propuestas concretas para auxiliar en la educación lectora de personas disléxicas, dirigida quizás con mayor énfasis a padres de niños, pero con un enfoque que abarca diferentes grupos generacionales y situaciones educativas.
Article
A widely variable overlap ranging from 10 to 92% has been reported in the literature between attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity (ADDH) and learning disability (LD), most likely a result of inconsistencies in the criteria used to define LD in different studies. The following study seeks to more accurately determine rates of LD in clinically referred children. Using a psychometrically reliable methodological approach, it was expected that the rate of LD in ADDH children would be far more modest than previously reported. Subjects were referred children with ADDH (N = 60), children with academic problems (N = 30), and normal controls (N = 36) of both sexes with available psychological and achievement testing. Using a liberal definition of LD, significant differences were found between the groups (ADDH = 38% versus academic problems = 43% versus normals = 8%; p = 0.002). In contrast, more modest rates were found using two more stringent methods of assessment (23 and 17%; 10 and 3%; 2 and 0%, respectively; p = 0.02). Arithmetic-based LD appears to be equally identified by both stringent methods, whereas the liberal definition overidentified children in all three groups. These findings show that a liberal definition of LD overidentifies LD not only in ADDH children but also in normal children.
Article
Learning disabilities is a generic term that refers to a heterogeneous group of disorders manifested by significant difficulties in the acquisition and use of listening, speaking, reading, writing, reasoning or mathematical abilities. These disorders are intrinsic to the individual and presumed to be due to central nervous system dysfunction. Even though a learning disability may occur concomitantly with other handicapping conditions (e.g., sensory impairment, mental retardation, social and emotional disturbance) or environmental influences (e.g., cultural differences, insufficient/inappropriate instruction, psychogenic factors), it is not the direct result of those conditions or influences.
Article
Seventy-one children in three groups (reading disabilities, ADHD without reading disabilities, and normal controls) were compared on their ability to rapidly name colors, letters, numbers, and objects (RAN Tasks) and alternating letters/numbers and letters/numbers/colors (RAS tasks). Children with reading disabilities were found to be slower on letter- and number-naming tasks and made more errors on all tasks than controls or children with ADHD. There was an age effect for the RAN/RAS tasks, with younger children with reading disabilities performing more poorly on all tasks, while the older children with reading disabilities showed poorer performance only on the letter- and number-naming tasks.
Article
A critical issue in developmental cognitive neuroscience is the extent to which the functional neuroanatomy underlying task performance differs in adults and children. Direct comparisons of brain activation in the left frontal and extrastriate cortex were made in adults and children (aged 7 to 10 years) performing single-word processing tasks with visual presentation; differences were found in circumscribed frontal and extrastriate regions. Conceivably, these differences could be attributable exclusively to performance discrepancies; alternatively, maturational differences in functional neuroanatomy could exist despite similar performance. Some of the brain regions examined showed differences attributable to age independent of performance, suggesting that maturation of the pattern of regional activations for these tasks is incomplete at age 10.