ArticlePDF Available

Early Intervention in Low Birth Weight Premature Infants: Results at 18 Years of Age for the Infant Health and Development Program

American Academy of Pediatrics
Pediatrics
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

To assess whether improvements in cognitive and behavioral development seen in preschool educational programs persist, we compared those in a multisite randomized trial of such a program over the first 3 years of life (INT) to those with follow-up only (FUO) at 18 months of age. This was a prospective follow-up of the Infant Health and Development Program at 8 sites heterogeneous for sociodemographic characteristics. Originally 985 children were randomized to the INT (n = 377) or FUO (n = 608) groups within 2 birth weight strata: heavier low birth weight (HLBW; 2001-2499 g) and lighter low birth weight (LLBW; < or = 2000 g). Primary outcome measures were the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III), reading and mathematics subscales of the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement, youth self-report on the Total Behavior Problem Index, and high-risk behaviors on the Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System (YRBSS). Secondary outcomes included Weschler full-scale IQ, caregiver report on the Total Behavior Problem Index, and caregiver and youth self-reported physical health using the Medical Outcome Study measure. Assessors were masked as to study status. We assessed 636 youths at 18 years (64.6% of the 985, 72% of whom had not died or refused at prior assessments). After adjusting for cohort attrition, differences favoring the INT group were seen on the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement in math (5.1 points), YRBSS (-0.7 points), and the PPVT-III (3.8 points) in the HLBW youth. In the LLBW youth, the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement in reading was higher in the FUO than INT group (4.2). The findings in the HLBW INT group provide support for preschool education to make long-term changes in a diverse group of children who are at developmental risk. The lack of observable benefit in the LLBW group raises questions about the biological and educational factors that foster or inhibit sustained effects of early educational intervention.
Content may be subject to copyright.
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2005-1316
2006;117;771Pediatrics
and Patrick H. Casey
C. Bernbaum, Charles R. Bauer, Camilia Martin, Elizabeth R. Woods, Anne Martin
JudyJennifer Yu, Mikhail Salganik, David T. Scott, Forrest C. Bennett, Libby L. Kay,
Marie C. McCormick, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Stephen L. Buka, Julie Goldman,
of Age for the Infant Health and Development Program
Early Intervention in Low Birth Weight Premature Infants: Results at 18 Years
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/117/3/771.full.html
located on the World Wide Web at:
The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.
Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2006 by the American Academy
published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned,
PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
ARTICLE
Early Intervention in Low Birth Weight Premature
Infants: Results at 18 Years of Age for the Infant
Health and Development Program
Marie C. McCormick, MD, ScD
a
, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, PhD
b
, Stephen L. Buka, ScD, MS
a
, Julie Goldman, MSSA, MS
a
, Jennifer Yu, ScD, EdM
a
,
Mikhail Salganik, PhD
c
, David T. Scott, PhD
d
, Forrest C. Bennett, MD
e
, Libby L. Kay, MSSW, LCSW
f
, Judy C. Bernbaum, MD
g
,
Charles R. Bauer, MD
h
, Camilia Martin, MD, MS
i
, Elizabeth R. Woods, MD, MPH
j
, Anne Martin, DrPH, MPH
b
, Patrick H. Casey, MD
k
Departments of
a
Society, Human Development and Health, and
c
Biostatistics, Harvard School of Public Health, Boston, Massachusetts;
b
National Center for Children and
Families, Teachers College, and College of Physicians and Surgeons, Columbia University, New York, New York; Departments of
d
Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and
e
Pediatrics, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington;
f
Department of Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, Texas;
g
Department of
Pediatrics, University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania;
h
Department of Pediatrics, University of Miami
School of Medicine, Miami, Florida;
i
Department of Neonatology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Boston, Massachusetts;
j
Children’s Hospital Boston, Boston,
Massachusetts;
k
Department of Pediatrics, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock, Arkansas
The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
ABSTRACT
OBJECTIVE. To assess whether improvements in cognitive and behavioral develop-
ment seen in preschool educational programs persist, we compared those in a
multisite randomized trial of such a program over the first 3 years of life (INT) to
those with follow-up only (FUO) at 18 months of age.
METHODS. This was a prospective follow-up of the Infant Health and Development
Program at 8 sites heterogeneous for sociodemographic characteristics. Originally
985 children were randomized to the INT (n 377) or FUO (n 608) groups
within 2 birth weight strata: heavier low birth weight (HLBW; 2001–2499 g) and
lighter low birth weight (LLBW; 2000 g). Primary outcome measures were the
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III), reading and mathematics subscales of
the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement, youth self-report on the Total
Behavior Problem Index, and high-risk behaviors on the Youth Risk Behavior
Surveillance System (YRBSS). Secondary outcomes included Weschler full-scale
IQ, caregiver report on the Total Behavior Problem Index, and caregiver and youth
self-reported physical health using the Medical Outcome Study measure. Assessors
were masked as to study status.
RESULTS. We assessed 636 youths at 18 years (64.6% of the 985, 72% of whom had
not died or refused at prior assessments). After adjusting for cohort attrition,
differences favoring the INT group were seen on the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of
Achievement in math (5.1 points), YRBSS (0.7 points), and the PPVT-III (3.8
points) in the HLBW youth. In the LLBW youth, the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of
Achievement in reading was higher in the FUO than INT group (4.2).
CONCLUSIONS. The findings in the HLBW INT group provide support for preschool
education to make long-term changes in a diverse group of children who are at
developmental risk. The lack of observable benefit in the LLBW group raises
questions about the biological and educational factors that foster or inhibit sus-
tained effects of early educational intervention.
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/
peds.2005-1316
doi:10.1542/peds.2005-1316
Key Words
early intervention, long-term results, low
birth weight, randomized controlled trial
Abbreviations
IHDP—Infant Health and Development
Program
LLBW—lighter low birth weight
HLBW— heavier low birth weight
INT—intervention
FUO—follow-up only
YRBSS—Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance
System
BPI—Behavior Problem Index
WASI—Weschler Abbreviated Scale of
Intelligence
PPVT-III—Peabody Picture Vocabulary
Test-Version III
HSG— high school graduate
CI— confidence interval
LBW—low birth weight
Accepted for publication Aug 12, 2005
Address correspondence to Marie C.
McCormick, MD, ScD, Department of Society,
Human Development and Health, Harvard
School of Public Health, 677 Huntington Ave,
Boston, MA 02115. E-mail: mmccormi@hsph.
harvard.edu
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005;
Online, 1098-4275). Copyright © 2006 by the
American Academy of Pediatrics
PEDIATRICS Volume 117, Number 3, March 2006 771
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
I
NTENSIVE EARLY EDUCATIONAL interventions have
been documented to improve the cognitive outcomes
1
and, in some cases, reduce antisocial behavior early in
the school experience.
2
In several single-site, random-
ized studies of center-based interventions, longer-term
follow-up into adolescence and early adulthood has re-
vealed persistence of early-intervention effects seen as
greater school achievement,
3,4
less grade retention and
special education,
3,5
and more high school completion
6
and participation in college.
4
In addition to better edu-
cational outcomes, early-intervention programs have
also resulted in better social outcomes such as less ado-
lescent parenting
4
and less delinquency and fewer ar-
rests.
6–8
Parallel effects have been seen in follow-up of at
least 1 home-visiting program
9
and a nonrandomized
program conducted in several centers in 1 city, the Chi-
cago Child-Parent Center Program.
10
However, all of
these studies involve healthy, low-income, poor chil-
dren generally from a single site. Thus, inferences about
the generalizability of these results to groups more het-
erogeneous for socioeconomic status and health are lim-
ited.
The Infant Health and Development Program (IHDP)
was a multisite, randomized, controlled trial of an edu-
cational intervention until 3 years of age for low birth
weight (LBW) preterm infants. Infants were sampled
from 2 birth weight strata (2000 g and 2001–2500 g) to
assure an oversample of those at higher developmental
risk (the lighter LBW [LLBW] group) and continuity
with studies of poor, normal birth weight children (the
heavier LBW [HLBW] group) at 8 sites, and as a result,
the sample for this study is quite heterogeneous for
socioeconomic status and health status, particularly neu-
rologic factors that might influence school outcomes.
The intervention consisted of home visits every week for
the first year and every other week in the second and
third year, daily center-based education beginning at 12
months corrected for duration of gestation continuing
until 36 months of age, with the addition of parent
support groups. All of the children received intensive
pediatric follow-up care. The program has been de-
scribed in detail elsewhere.
11–13
The results of the trial through 8 years of age have
been published previously
14–17
and will be summarized
briefly here. The assessment of outcomes was conducted
in 3 prior phases. At 36 months of age corrected for
duration of gestation, at the end of phase I (the trial
phase), the IQs of those who received the intervention
(INT) were substantially higher than those who received
follow-up services only (FUO): a 14-point difference for
the HLBW group and 7 points for the LLBW group.
Likewise, lower behavior problem scores were seen in
the INT group. By age 5 (phase II, the preschool period)
and age 8 (phase III, the early school period), no differ-
ences in behavior scores were observed. At both ages 5
and 8, the INT in the HLBW group still experienced a
significant advantage in IQ scores (4 points higher), as
well as mathematics achievement testing (4 points
higher). No differences in IQ or achievement between
INT and FUO groups were seen for the children in the
LLBW group.
The IHDP is based on the design and curriculum of
the Abecedarian Project,
18
and the results in IQ scores
and achievement testing parallel results seen in the
Abecedarian Project
18
at 8 years of age. More recent data
from that project reveal persisting differences in IQ and
achievement up to 21 years of age,
19
prompting us to
extend the follow-up of the IHDP to 18 years with a
fourth wave of data collection (phase IV). The purpose of
this effort is to ascertain whether such persistent differ-
ences would be observed in the IHDP, especially in the
HLBW group. In line with other studies of normal birth
weight children, we hypothesized that we would see
persistent IQ and achievement differences and less risky
behavior favoring the INT group.
METHODS
Study Population
Detailed information on the recruitment of the study
population and follow-up through 8 years of age has
been published previously
11–17,19
and will be summarized
briefly. To be eligible for the trial, an infant must have
been born in 1 of the participating hospitals at each of
the 8 sites at a birth weight 2500 g, a gestational age of
37 weeks, and reside in a catchment area defined by
distance from the early educational center. Infants were
enrolled between October 1984 and August 1985 in 2
birth weight strata: 2000 g, and 2001–2500 g. Within
birth weight strata, infants were randomly assigned to
the INT group (n 377) or FUO group (n 608).
Although the sites were heterogeneous for a variety of
characteristics, the randomization procedure resulted in
comparability between the INT and FUO groups at study
entry.
14
Infants in both arms of the study received periodic
medical, developmental, and social service assessments
with referral for community services when indicated.
Those in the INT group received an educational program
delivered through home visits (weekly during the first
year and every other week during the second and third
years of life), a daily center-based program beginning at
12 months corrected for duration of gestation, and par-
ent support groups coinciding with the start of the cen-
ter-based program.
11
These parent support groups met
every other month. At the end of the INT period at 3
years of age, the children received whatever community
education programs were available at the site. Although
the availability of such services varied greatly among the
sites, there were no differences between the INT and
FUO groups in receipt of such services.
15
As noted above,
major assessment points for the intervention occurred at
3, 5, and 8 years of age. Completion rates at these ages
772 MCCORMICK, et al
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
were 92%, 82%, and 89%, respectively. The lower rate
at age 5 reflects the lack of funding for staff to assess
children who were not close to any study site.
Data Collection at 18 Years of Age (Phase IV)
Measures
The phase IV assessment at 18 years of age was designed
to obtain information in the areas of academic achieve-
ment, behavior, cognitive abilities, and physical health.
To the extent possible, instruments were selected to
provide continuity with previous assessment periods and
to have known references or to have been used in large
population studies.
Academic Achievement
Academic achievement was assessed using the Wood-
cock-Johnson Tests of Achievement-Revised.
20
These
tests yielded letter-word identification, passage compre-
hension, calculation, and applied problems subtests of
the standard scores. Two scores were calculated, the
Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement in reading
and in mathematics, referenced to a mean of 100 and an
SD of 15, with higher scores indicative of better achieve-
ment.
In addition, the youth’s mother or other caregiver
was asked about his/her progress in school. Specifically,
these questions deal with completion of school, grade
repetition, classification for special education, and school
dropout.
Behavior
Two aspects of behavior were assessed: risk behaviors
and more general behavioral problems. Risk behaviors
were assessed using youth self-report on questions taken
from the Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System
(YRBSS).
21
Items from this survey were selected to char-
acterize the youth along dimensions affected by early
intervention in early studies: conduct problems, suicidal
ideation/attempts, smoking, alcohol and marijuana use,
and risky sexual activity (see the Appendix for the scor-
ing algorithm). More general behavioral problems were
ascertained using the Behavior Problem Index (BPI),
22
completed by both the youth and the primary caregiver
(the latter for comparison with earlier assessments). For
both the YRBSS and the BPI, higher scores indicate more
problems.
Finally, we asked the caregiver and the youth specific
questions about youth involvement with the justice sys-
tem. These questions included items on trouble with the
police, arrests, and time in jail. A positive event was
coded if either reported it.
Cognitive Development
Two measures of cognitive development were admin-
istered: the Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence
(WASI)
23
and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Ver-
sion III (PPVT-III).
24
The latter, a measure of receptive
vocabulary, has been used in this sample at each assess-
ment point since 3 years of age and was designated the
primary outcome because of this continuity over time.
Both tests are referenced to a mean of 100 and an SD of
15. The WASI provides 4 subscales (vocabulary, block
design, similarities, and matrix reasoning) and can gen-
erate a verbal IQ and performance IQ, as well as a full IQ
score, with only the last being considered here.
Physical Health
Physical health was characterized by youth self-report
and caregiver report on the 36-Physical Component
Summary measure.
25
This scale reflects limitations in
activities of daily living because of health, as well as
positive health. It is referenced to a mean score of 50 and
an SD of 10, with higher scores indicative of better health.
Of these measures, math and reading achievement
tests, the risk behavior scale, youth self-report on the
BPI, and the PPVT-III were considered the primary out-
comes for the analysis. However, the results for the
WASI, caregiver-reported behavior problem score, and
self-report and caregiver report on physical health are
provided for continuity with earlier reports.
Baseline Measures
As in previous analyses of the IHDP, a standard set of
prerandomization variables was used to compare partic-
ipants and nonparticipants in this assessment and to
adjust the outcomes for differences across sites and birth
weight strata. These variables included birth weight in
grams, maternal age in years, race/ethnicity (black, His-
panic and white/other), gender of child (male/female),
maternal educational attainment at the time of the birth
of the child (less than high school graduate [HSG], HSG,
and more than HSG), and Neonatal Health Index,
26
a
measure of neonatal length of stay in the hospital rela-
tive to birth weight, which serves as an indication of the
severity of the neonatal course. This index is referenced
to a mean of 100 and SD of 16. Baseline variables also
included study site: Arkansas (Little Rock, AK), Einstein
(South Bronx, NY), Harvard (Boston, MA), Miami (Mi-
ami, FL), Pennsylvania (Philadelphia, PA), Texas (Dallas,
TX), Washington (Seattle, WA), and Yale (New Haven,
CT). Gestational age at birth was also available.
Assessments at Age 8
In addition, we compared participants and nonpartic-
ipants for the cognitive, achievement, and behavioral
assessments done at 8 years of age. These measures
included the Wechsler full-scale IQ score,
27
achievement
scores in math and reading,
20
the PPVT-Revised,
28
and
behavior problem scores.
29
Analysis
Participants and nonparticipants at the 18-year assess-
ment were compared on the prerandomization variables
and 8-year outcomes noted above. A subject was con-
sidered a participant if any of the outcomes of interest
PEDIATRICS Volume 117, Number 3, March 2006 773
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
were observed at the 18-year visit and as a nonpartici-
pant otherwise. The comparison of the differences be-
tween participants and nonparticipants in the INT and
FUO groups were evaluated by using the t statistic for
continuous measures and
2
statistics for categorical
measures.
To compare the INT and FUO groups on the outcomes
of interest at age 18, separate linear (analysis of covari-
ance) models were developed for each outcome variable
within each of 2 birth weight strata using the S-plus
software described by Venables and Ripley.
30
The same
set of 7 baseline variables measured at enrollment as
noted above was used in each model of the comparison
of the INT versus FUO. Multivariate models were used to
estimate the differences between the INT and FUO
groups to adjust for differences in sample sizes among
the sites.
14
The linear models were used for estimation of
population marginal mean values and differences (INT
versus FUO) for each outcome. The estimated marginal
means were obtained by substituting into the linear
model the average values of the covariates for a partic-
ular birth weight stratum. Nominal P values derived
from the linear models were used for primary outcome
comparisons.
We evaluated the robustness of these conventional
estimates of the difference between the INT and FUO
groups by comparing them with the results of the anal-
ysis of covariance where each of the respondents was
weighted by the inverse of the outcome-dependent es-
timated propensity score
31
to assess the effect of attrition.
We assumed that the propensity score (which, in our
analysis, is the outcome-dependent probability of ob-
serving an outcome of interest at the 18-year visit) de-
pended on the values of the full-scale IQ and child
behavior checklist at the 8-year visit and baseline char-
acteristics of a subject. Thus, for example, subjects from
Harvard with low IQs at age 8 were less likely to partic-
ipate at age 18, and the average participant with these
characteristics would be assigned higher weights (more
influence) in the propensity-adjusted analyses of the
difference between the INT and FUO groups using this
approach. Under the modeling assumptions, this method
removes nonresponse bias.
Human Subjects
The study was approved by the institutional review
boards of all participating institutions. In addition, certi-
fication of confidentiality was obtained for this phase.
Written informed consent was obtained separately for all
of the participants: study subject and caregiver.
RESULTS
Comparison of Participants and Nonparticipants at 18 Years of
Age
At age 18, 636 (64.6%) of the 985 of the participants in
the original IHDP trial were assessed: 67.4% of the INT
group and 62.8% of the FUO group (odds ratio: 1.22; P
.15; 95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.93 to 1.60). This
represents 72% of those eligible to be followed (ie, those
who had not died or refused additional participation by
8 years of age). Refusal rates were low (2.7% of the 338
in the INT group and 4.6% of the 540 in the FUO group
seen at age 8). There was 1 death (in the INT group),
which occurred after the assessment was scheduled but
before it could be completed. Because the family wished
to participate in this round and provided both caregiver
and sibling responses, this individual was considered in
the participant category. The major reason for lack of
response was the inability of the site teams to locate the
subjects, or subjects repeatedly missed appointments. As
seen in Table 1, those seen at 18 years of age did not
differ from the original sample in the baseline character-
istics except for maternal educational attainment at the
time of enrollment, race/ethnicity, and study site. In the
INT group, mothers of lower educational attainment and
Hispanic ethnicity were less likely to participate in phase
IV. These factors also influenced participation in the
HLBW INT group with the additional lower participation
among males (41.5% among participants vs 62.5%
among nonparticipants; P .028). Within the LLBW
INT group, only race/ethnicity was a significant predic-
tor. Race/ethnicity and male gender were also associated
with participation status in the HLBW FUO group. The
effect of study site was seen in all of the groups overall
and within birth weight strata. The Einstein, Harvard,
and Washington sites had relatively lower follow-up
rates, and Arkansas, Pennsylvania, and Yale sites were
relatively higher. When participants and nonparticipants
were compared for the assessments at age 8 (Table 2),
participants in the INT group had a somewhat higher IQ,
PPVT-III, and reading and math achievement and lower
behavior problem (ie, fewer problems) scores than non-
participants. In the FUO group, participants had fewer
behavior problems than nonparticipants.
Comparison of INT and FUO Groups on Primary and Secondary
Outcomes
Because the initial trial was designed and powered to
permit comparisons within birth weight strata, and be-
cause subjects were sampled differently in each birth
weight stratum, we have displayed the results separately
for the HLBW and LLBW groups (Table 3). As in the
previous assessments at 5 and 8 years of age, no statis-
tically significant differences were seen between INT and
FUO groups in the group weighing 2000 g in compar-
isons adjusted for the baseline variables only (Table 3
line “a” for each outcome), although a difference in
reading achievement scores favoring the FUO group
achieved P .08. When the results were also adjusted
for attrition (Table 3, line “b”), the difference in reading
favoring the FUO group was strengthened.
Although the use of the covariates was considered
774 MCCORMICK, et al
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
appropriate because of the differences in the original
sample sizes among sites, the effect of this adjustment
was minor. For example, the unadjusted mean PPVT-III
scores for the LLBW INT and FUO groups were 94.1 and
96.1, respectively, compared with 93.7 and 96.1 in Table
3.
In contrast, among those 2000 g, there were statis-
tically significant elevations in PPVT-III and math
achievement scores favoring the INT group in the results
adjusted for the baseline covariates. The INT group ex-
perienced 5- and 6-point higher reading and math
achievement scores, respectively, with the latter at P
.01. No statistically significant difference was seen in
self-reported behavior problems or the risk behavior
scale from the YRBSS in these analyses. When the com-
parisons were adjusted for attrition, the difference in
reading achievement diminished but that for the math
achievement remained statistically significant. A differ-
ence was seen favoring the INT group on the YRBSS
score as having fewer risky behaviors (P .05). When
the INT and FUO groups were compared on the mea-
sures of secondary outcomes, no statistically significant
differences were seen, although the difference favoring
the INT group on the full-scale IQ score among the
HLBW group (Table 4) had a P value of .07.
Fig 1 provides a summary of the published outcomes,
where comparable, from all 4 of the assessment periods.
In the HLBW group, the 14-point difference seen at age
3 diminished to 4 points, but this difference has re-
mained stable and consistent over time. Likewise, the
differences in PPVT scores favoring the INT have per-
sisted after age 3. Changes in the achievement scores in
TABLE 1 Comparison of Participants and Nonparticipants in Phase IV (18 Years) According to Baseline
Characteristics and Study Arm
Characteristic INT FUO
Participant Nonparticipant Participant Nonparticipant
Total 254 123 382 226
% of total 67.4 32.6 62.8 37.2
Birth weight, g
Mean (SD) 1817 (441) 1824 (436) 1795 (465) 1758 (473)
P .89 .35
2000 g, % 63.0 61.0 64.1 62.8
2000 g, % 37.0 39.0 35.9 37.2
P .79 .81
Gestational age, wk
Mean (SD) 33.1 (2.4) 33.0 (2.8) 33.1 (2.7) 32.9 (2.9)
P .72 .37
NHI
Mean (SD) 100.6 (15.1) 100.8 (17.7) 99.4 (15.1) 100.0 (17.1)
P .93 .66
Maternal age, y
Mean (SD) 24.6 (5.8) 24.7 (6.2) 25.1 (6.2) 24.6 (6.0)
P .88 .34
Maternal education
High school, % 36.6 56.1 35.9 42.0
High school, % 30.3 22.0 28.3 25.7
High school, % 33.1 22.0 35.9 32.3
P .01 .32
Race/ethnicity
Black, % 50.8 58.5 53.7 49.1
Hispanic, % 7.5 14.6 9.4 14.2
White/other, % 41.7 26.8 36.9 36.7
P .01 .18
Male, % 47.2 56.9 47.1 49.1
P .10 .70
Study site (% of total)
Arkansas 14.6 8.9 17.5 5.8
Einstein 11.0 14.6 13.1 18.6
Harvard 8.7 18.7 11.5 21.7
Miami 11.8 11.4 9.9 8.0
Pennsylvania 15.4 7.3 12.0 3.1
Texas 10.2 18.7 12.3 18.1
Washington 13.0 14.6 10.2 18.1
Yale 15.4 5.7 13.4 6.6
P .01 .001
NHI indicates Neonatal Health Index.
27
PEDIATRICS Volume 117, Number 3, March 2006 775
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
this group were generally parallel while exhibiting the
expected decline in achievement scores with aging into
the late teen years. The figure also reveals that results
from the current phase suggesting that small differences
favoring the FUO group in PPVT, IQ, and math achieve-
ment scores also emerged early and persisted.
To examine whether the results for those 2000 g at
birth might reflect a heavier weighting of children with
significant neurologic complications, we reran the anal-
yses for those children who had an IQ 85 at age 1 year.
Restriction of the analysis to this group led to estimate
differences in reading scores (3.1 [95% CI: 7.1 to
0.09], P .13, adjusted for baseline covariates; 2.8
[95% CI: 6.9 to 1.2], P .17, for the propensity-
weighted estimates) that were not statistically signifi-
cant.
We also explored differences in specific behavioral
and school outcomes. Among the INT LLBW group, 16%
had been arrested compared with 20% of the FUO group
(with P .11, adjusted for baseline covariates and attri-
tion). The comparable figures for the HLBW group was
21% and 26%, respectively (P .56). Approximately
12% to 13% of both birth weight groups had already
been in jail with no differences between the INT and
FUO groups. Among the LLBW group, 10% of the INT
group and 8% of the FUO group were classified as hav-
ing dropped out of school (P .40); in the HLBW group,
it was 11% and 12%, respectively (P .58). With regard
to ever being classified for special education, 30% of INT
group and 26% of the FUO group in the LLBW stratum
had been so classified (P .39) and 17% and 24% of the
HLBW stratum (P .12).
DISCUSSION
The results of this phase of the IHDP suggest a persistent
benefit of the intervention for the subset of HLBW par-
ticipants and absence or even reversal of any interven-
tion effect for the youth born weighing 2000 g. These
results parallel those seen at 8 years of age.
16
As ex-
pected, IQ and achievement scores were lower and be-
havior problem scores higher than expected population
references, reflective of patterns seen among those who
were LBW and/or premature at birth.
32
Our rates of juvenile arrest at age 18 are comparable
with those the seen in the Chicago study,
10
but the
difference between the INT and FUO groups is smaller,
4% to 5% as compared with 8% in the Chicago study,
where the difference did achieve statistical significance.
Likewise, our incarceration rates are similar to those
reported in the Abecedarian intervention group
4
but
somewhat lower than the rates in the control group. The
differences in incarceration rates also fail to achieve sta-
tistical significance. Comparisons with the Perry Pre-
school Project cannot be made, because they report the
percentage with 5 arrests at rates substantially higher
than arrest records in other studies.
Comparison of our educational outcomes with other
studies is complicated by the fact that our sample has not
yet established a post– high school educational pattern in
terms of enrollment in higher education as reported in
Abecedarian,
4
and even final high school graduation
rates are unknown. Our dropout rate is substantially
lower than the 50% seen in the Chicago study,
10
as is
the difference between intervention and control groups.
Our rates of special education and differences between
INT and FUO in the HLBW group are similar to those in
the Chicago study, where the difference was highly sig-
nificant.
In this analysis, we have not examined outcomes by
prerandomization variables other than birth weight. This
decision reflects the results at age 8 where no difference
in effect size is seen at different levels of maternal edu-
cation in contrast to a hypothesized greater effect of the
intervention for children from more disadvantaged
backgrounds.
16
The persistence of an intervention effect in the HLBW
group provides substantial reinforcement to the emerg-
ing literature on longer-term effects of early educational
intervention.
4–9
First, the IHDP was implemented in 8
sites and is modeled on the Abecedarian Project,
4
argu-
ing for replicability of the results. Second, the IHDP has
been characterized as methodologically rigorous with
strict randomization and masked assessments at major
outcome points at all of the follow-up phases and has
avoided some of the concerns associated with other
projects.
8
In contrast to previous studies, which involved
largely poor, healthy children, the IHDP expands the
results of early educational intervention in 2 ways. First,
the sample is heterogeneous with regard to health, es-
pecially neurodevelopmental status, by virtue of it being
a sample of preterm, LBW children. Although not all
LBW children sustain neurologic injury,
32
being LBW
TABLE 2 Comparison of Participants and Nonparticipants in Phase
IV (18 years) According to Cognitive, Behavioral, and
Achievement Scores at Age 8
Variable INT FUO
Participant Nonparticipant Participant Nonparticipant
Full-scale IQ
Mean (SD) 87.5 (29.2) 79.1 (23.3) 86.5 (27.8) 83.4 (26.6)
P .02 .22
PPVT-III
Mean (SD) 86.1 (23.9) 80.9 (19.6) 86.2 (23.1) 83.7 (22.3)
P .07 .26
W-J reading
Mean (SD) 99.6 (20.5) 93.1 (22.2) 98.1 (21.2) 95.9 (21.1)
P .01 .30
W-J math
Mean (SD) 97.7 (23.1) 90.3 (23.3) 96.4 (21.9) 93.5 (22.1)
P .01 .16
CBCL total
Mean (SD) 30.6 (20.7) 33.7 (19.7) 30.1 (18.5) 34.1 (20.8)
P .23 .02
W-J indicates Woodcock-Johnson Achievement Test
19
; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist.
30
776 MCCORMICK, et al
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
confers increased risk of neurodevelopmental disability
that may make academic achievement difficult. For ex-
ample, in unpublished analyses, children with cerebral
palsy in the IHDP experienced no advantage in cognitive
function at age 3 from the intervention. Second, the
IHDP was more heterogeneous with regard to socioeco-
nomic status than previous studies. We have docu-
mented that children of mothers with some college or
more education did not benefit from the intervention
immediately at the end of the trial.
33
Thus, although the
effects sizes are modest, they reinforce the existing litera-
ture because they reflect a more heterogeneous study pop-
ulation biased against finding a difference. It should be
noted that the pattern of persistence seen in Fig 1, includ-
ing diminishing performance on achievement testing with
age, closely parallels results for the Abecedarian Project.
18
As in the 8-year assessment, questions might be
raised about the significance of a 4-point difference in
achievement or the PPVT-III. However, it should be
noted that the difference in mean scores also indicates a
shift in the entire distribution in favor of the INT group.
In the past, we have argued that such a shift would
decrease the percentage of children classified as intellec-
tually deficient.
16
Others have also argued the impor-
tance of such differences and the potential to alter the
distribution of those who will do poorly in school.
34
The lack of persistence of an effect in the LLBW
children is also relevant to this emerging literature in
that it raises questions about greater attention to the
factors that foster or inhibit sustained effects. The LLBW
groups did initially benefit from the intervention as in-
dicated by a 6.6-point difference in IQ scores favoring
the INT group at 3 years of age.
14
Moreover, the lack of
sustained effect was not because of the experience of
very premature or very small infants, because the same
effect size is seen among those 1500 g and even 1000
g in birth weight.
35
However, differences between the
INT and FUO groups among the LLBW children disap-
pear by age 5 years.
15
One analysis suggests that the
intensity of participation may be a factor in that sus-
TABLE 3 Major Outcomes at Age 18 Years for Youth in the IHDP
Variable n Marginal
Mean Scores
Difference INT vs FUO
Estimated Value (95% CI)
P
INT FUO INT FUO
Birth weight 2000 g
PPVT-III
a 154 234 93.7 96.1 2.5 (5.5 to 0.5) .10
b 152 232 93.0 95.5 2.4 (5.4 to 0.5) .10
Woodcock Johnson
Reading
a 153 233 92.5 96.2 3.7 (7.9 to 0.4) .08
b 151 231 91.4 95.6 4.2 (8.4 to 0.0) .05
Math
a 153 234 87.3 89.4 2.0 (5.2 to 1.1) .21
b 151 231 86.0 88.7 2.7 (5.9 to 0.6) .11
TBPI-Y
a 145 226 9.8 9.8 0 (1.2 to 1.2) .98
b 144 224 9.9 10.0 0 (1.2 to 1.2) .99
YRBSS
a 148 231 2.4 2.2 0.2 (0.3 to 0.7) .41
b 147 229 2.4 2.1 0.3 (0.2 to 0.8) .29
Birth weight 2000 g
PPVT-III
a 91 132 99.6 94.5 5.1 (1.2 to 9.0) .01
b 89 132 98.8 95.0 3.8 (0.3 to 7.8) .07
Woodcock Johnson
Reading
a 90 130 100.4 95.0 5.3 (0.4 to 11.1) .07
b 88 130 98.9 95.7 3.3 (2.5 to 9.1) .27
Math
a 91 130 95.5 89.4 6.1 (1.6 to 10.6) .01
b 89 130 94.9 89.8 5.1 (0.6 to 9.7) .03
TBPI-Y
a 85 126 10.2 9.3 0.9 (0.6 to 2.3) .23
b 83 126 10.4 9.3 1.1 (0.4 to 2.6) .15
YRBSS
a 89 127 2.4 2.9 0.5 (1.3 to 0.3) .19
b 87 127 2.2 3.0 0.7 (1.5 to 0.0) .05
TBPI-Y indicates Total Behavior Problem Index-Youth Self-Report; a, adjusted for all the variables in Table 1 except gestational age; b, adjusted for
variables in Table 1 except for gestational age and attrition.
PEDIATRICS Volume 117, Number 3, March 2006 777
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
tained effects at age 8 were seen among those attending
400 days of center-based education (out of a maximum
of 500) during the intervention period.
36
In addition, we
speculate that earlier entry into special education may also
play a role and needs additional investigation.
Several limitations affect the interpretation of our
study. The first is the lower response rate than previous
rounds of assessment in the IHDP, particularly with loss
among more disadvantaged children and those study
participants with lower previous cognitive development.
Our adjustment of the results for these factors suggests
that the losses to the cohort do not significantly affect
the results, but differences may have occurred in un-
measured factors.
Second, the age of assessment, 18 years, may not be
optimal in identifying economically important out-
comes, such as higher educational attainment, definitive
occupational status, and eventual income. In the Perry
Preschool Project, economically significant differences
are seen at 27 and now 40 years of age.
6,8
Even at 21
years of age, the Abecedarian Project
4
did not detect
significant differences in employment but, in results par-
allel to ours, did find differences in risk behaviors. They
did, however, find an intervention advantage in the
proportion enrolled in a 4-year college or university.
Third, because our study sample is not all disadvan-
taged, the power to detect significant differences in some
areas is limited. As noted earlier, arrest rates in the Perry
Preschool Project
8
are substantially higher than other
longer-term follow-up studies of early education. With
all rates of adverse outcomes, such as arrest or special
education classification, differences favoring the INT
group, especially the HLBW youth, are either similar or
about half the effect size. Thus, we would have needed a
substantially larger sample, in the order of the 1500 in
the Chicago study,
10
to achieve statistical significance.
This finding also suggests caution in extrapolating cost-
effectiveness arguments of early educational interven-
tion based on reductions in costs among populations
with less risk of poor educational attainment and crim-
inal behavior.
Despite these limitations, this phase of the IHDP pro-
vides important support for the efficacy of early educa-
tional interventions in the longer-term outcomes of chil-
dren. It also presents significant challenges in identifying
those who would most benefit and those who may need
continuing support to achieve academically. However,
the results provide support for extending such educa-
tional opportunities to a broader spectrum of children
than included in previous studies.
TABLE 4 Secondary Outcomes at Age 18 Years of Youth in the IHDP
Variable n Marginal
Mean Scores
Difference INT vs FUO
Estimated Value (95% CI)
P
INT FUO INT FUO
Birth weight 2000 g
WASI full-scale IQ
a 154 236 89.4 91.2 1.8 (4.5 to 0.9) .19
b 152 233 88.5 90.7 2.2 (5.0 to 0.6) .12
TBPI-CAR
a 144 234 9.2 9.1 0.2 (1.2 to 1.6) .81
b 142 230 9.9 9.3 0.5 (0.9 to 2.0) .46
PCS-CAR
a 155 238 53.6 54.0 0.4 (1.6 to 0.8) .51
b 153 235 53.3 54.0 0.6 (1.9 to 0.6) .31
PCS-YA
a 149 234 53.4 54.2 0.8 (2.3 to 0.7) .31
b 148 232 53.4 54.1 0.8 (2.3 to 0.8) .33
Birth weight 2000 g
WASI full-scale IQ
a 92 132 94.6 91.3 3.3 (0.3 to 6.9) .07
b 90 132 93.7 92.0 1.7 (2.0 to 5.3) .38
TBPI-CAR
a 82 124 8.5 9.3 0.9 (2.7 to 0.9) .34
b 80 123 8.9 9.6 0.7 (2.6 to 1.1) .45
PCS-CAR
a 86 131 55.5 54.9 0.6 (1.0 to 2.1) .47
b 84 130 55.5 55.0 0.6 (1.0 to 2.1) .48
PCS-YA
a 90 129 55.1 54.3 0.9 (0.9 to 2.7) .34
b 88 129 55.3 54.6 0.7 (1.1 to 2.5) .46
TBPI-CAR indicates Total Behavior Problem Index by caretaker report; PCS-CAR, Physical Health Scale by caretaker report; PCS-YA, Physical Health
Scale by youth report; a, adjusted for all the variables in Table 1 except gestational age; b, adjusted for variables in Table 1 except for gestational
age and attrition.
778 MCCORMICK, et al
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
APPENDIX
The YRBSS questionnaire has no established scoring al-
gorithm. We identified questions in 6 risk areas (antiso-
cial behavior, suicidal ideation/attempt, smoking, alcohol
usage, and marijuana usage) considered most relevant to
the study. Because there were different numbers of
items in each of these categories, a simple sum across the
questions would have weighted some areas more than
others. Thus, to obtain an interval score for risk behav-
iors, we developed a 3-level score for each area, assign-
ing the numeric values of 0, 1, and 2 to the low, me-
dium, and high scores. Cut points were assigned so that
15% of the group fell into the highest-risk group, the
majority (60%) in the low-risk group, and the remain-
der in the moderate-risk group. We summed these area-
dependent scores to produce an overall score of risky
behavior. This approach parallels that of Flisher et al,
37
in
a report of behavior in a community sample of children
and adolescents.
1
Of the 636 subjects, data for this anal-
ysis was missing on 5%. The resulting score had a mean
of 2.4 in our study population, with an SD of 2.6 and a
range of 0 –11. Initial bivariate comparisons of INT and
FUO groups were done, therefore, using nonparametric
techniques. Adjusting for baseline covariates and attri-
tion, however, relied on parametric statistics as described
in “Methods.”
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study was supported through grant 039543 from
the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. The funder pro-
vided funding for the data collection and reviewed both
the proposal and the final results.
FIGURE 1
Cognitive and achievement scores at different assess-
ment points in the IHDP for the heavier birth weight
group. A, IHDP: IQ results over time—HLBW group
(adjusted for baseline covariates;
a
P .05). B, IHDP:
PPVT results over time—LLBW group (adjusted for
baseline covariates;
a
P .05). C, IHDP: Woodcock-
Johnson Achievement Test reading results over
time—HLBW group (at 18, P .10; adjusted for base-
line covariates, P .05). D, IHDP: Woodcock-Johnson
Achievement Test math results over time—HLBW
group (adjusted for baseline covariates;
a
P .05). E,
IDHP: PPVT results over time—LLBW group (adjusted
for baseline covariates;
a
P .05). F, IDHP: IQ results
over time—LLBW group (adjustedfor baseline covari-
ates;
a
P .05). G, IDHP: Woodcock-Johnson Achieve-
ment Test reading results over time—LLBW group (at
18, P .10; adjusted for baseline covariates). H, IDHP:
Woodcock-Johnson Achievement Test math results
over time—LLBW group (adjusted for baseline covari-
ates). Baseline covariates: birth weight, maternal age
at youth’s birth, race/ethnicity, child gender, maternal
educational attainment at youth’s birth, Neonatal
Health Index,
26
and study site. Values for 3- and 5-year
assessments are from ref 16; values for 8-year assess-
ments are from ref 16.
PEDIATRICS Volume 117, Number 3, March 2006 779
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
For previous participants in the IHDP, see refs 14 –16.
REFERENCES
1. Barnett WS. Long-term effects of early childhood programs on
cognitive and school outcomes. Future Child. 1995;5:25–50
2. Yoshikawa H. Long-term effects of early childhood programs
on social outcomes and delinquency. Future Child. 1995;5:
51–75
3. Campbell FA, Ramey CT. Cognitive and school outcomes for
high-risk African American students at middle adolescence;
positive effects of early intervention. Am Educ Res J. 1995;32:
743–772
4. Campbell FA, Ramey CT, Pungello E, et al. Early childhood
education: Young adult outcomes from the Abecedarian
Project. App Dev Sci. 2002;6:42–57
5. Gray SW, Ramsey B, Claus R. From 3 to 20: The Early Training
Project. Baltimore, MD: University Part Press; 1982
6. Schweinhart LJ, Barnes HV, Weikart DP, et al. Significant
Benefits: The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study Through Age 27.
Monographs of the High/Scope Educational Research Founda-
tion, No. 10. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Educational Research
Foundation; 1993
7. Lally JR, Mangione PL, Honig AS. The Syracuse University
Family Development Research Project: long-range impact of an
early intervention with low-income children and their families.
In: Powell DR, ed. Parent Education as Early Childhood
Interventions: Emerging Directions in Theory, Research and Practice.
Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1988
8. Schweinhart LJ, Montie J, Xiang Z, Barnett WS, Bellfield CR,
Nores M. Lifetime Effects: The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study
Through Age 40. Monographs of the High/Scope Educational
Research Foundation No. 14. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
2006
9. Olds D, Henderson CR, Cole R, et al. Long-term effects of nurse
home visitation on children’s criminal and antisocial behavior:
15-year follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. JAMA.
1998;280:1238 –1244
10. Reynolds AJ, Temple JA, Robertson DL, et al. Long-term effects
of an early child childhood intervention on educational
achievement and juvenile arrest. JAMA. 2001;285:2339 –2346
11. Gross RT, Spiker D, Haynes C, eds. Helping Low Birth Weight
Premature Babies. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press; 1997
12. Ramey CT, Bryant DM, Wasik BH, et al. Infant Health and
Development Program for low birth weight, premature infants:
program elements, family participation, and child intelligence.
Pediatrics. 1992;89:454 465
13. Liaw FR, Brooks-Gunn J. Patterns of low birth weight chil-
dren’s cognitive development and their determinants. Dev Psy-
chol. 1993;29:1024 –1035
14. The Infant Health and Development Program. Enhancing the
outcomes of low-birth-weight, premature infants: a multisite,
randomized trial. JAMA. 1990;263:3035–3042
15. Brooks-Gunn J, McCarton CM, Casey PH, et al. Early inter-
vention in low-birth-weight premature infants: results through
age 5 years from the Infant Health and Development Program.
JAMA. 1994;272:1257–1262
16. McCarton CM, Brooks-Gunn J, Wallace IF, et al. Results at age
8 years of early intervention for low-birth-weight premature
infants. The Infant Health and Development Program. JAMA.
1997;277:126 –132
17. McCormick MC, McCarton C, Brooks-Gunn J, et al. The Infant
Health and Development Program: interim summary. J Dev
Behav Pediatr. 1998;19:359–370
18. Campbell FA, Pungello EP, Miller-Johnson S, et al. The devel-
opment of cognitive and academic abilities: growth curves
from an early childhood educational experiment. Dev Psychol.
2001;37:231–242
19. Constantine, WL, Haynes CW, Spiker D, et al. Recruitment and
retention in a clinical trial for low birth weight, premature
infants. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1993;14:1–7
20. Woodcock RW, Johnson MB. Manual for the Woodcock-Johnson
Tests of Achievement-Revised. Allen, TX: RCL Enterprises; 1990
21. Kolbe LJ, Kann L, Collins JL. Overview of the Youth Risk
Behavior Surveillance System. Public Health Rep. 1993;108
(supp 1):2–10
22. Peterson JL, Zill N. Marital disruption, parent-child relation-
ships and behavior problems in children. Marriage Fam. 1986;
48:295–307
23. Psychological Corporation. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelli-
gence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1999
24. Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 3rd ed.
Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc; 1997
25. Ware JE, Kosinski M. SF-36 Physical & Mental Health Summary
Scales: A Manual for Users of Version 1. 2nd ed. Lincoln, RI:
QualityMetric Incorporated; 2001
26. Scott DT, Bauer CR, Kraemer HC, et al. The Neonatal Health
Index. Chapter 25. In Gross RT, Spiker D, Haynes CW, eds.
Helping Low Birth Weight, Premature Babies. The Infant Health and
Development Program. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press;
1977
27. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children.
3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1991
28. Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Manual for Peabody Picture Vocabulary
Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MI: American Guidance Service; 1981
29. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C. Manual for the Child Behavior
Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT:
Queen City Printers; 1983
30. Venables WN, Ripley BD. Modern Applied Statistics With S. 4th
ed. New York, NY: Springer; 2002
31. Little JA, Rubin D. Statistical Analysis With Missing Data. Balti-
more, MD: Wiley; 2002
32. Hack M, Klein NK, Taylor HG. Long-term developmental out-
comes of low birth weight infants. Future Child. 1995;5:
176 –196
33. Brooks-Gunn J, Gross RT, Kraemer HC, et al. Enhancing the
cognitive outcomes of low birth weight premature infants: For
whom is the intervention most effective? Pediatrics. 1992;84:
1209 –1215
34. Rock DA, Stenner AJ. Assessment issues in the testing of
children at school entry. Future Child. 2005;15;13–32
35. McCormick MC, McCarton C, Tonascia J, et al. Early educa-
tional intervention for very low birth weight infants: Results
from the Infant Health and Development Program. J Pediatr.
1993;123:527–533
36. Hill JL, Brooks-Gunn J, Waldfogel J. Sustained effects of high
participation in an early intervention for low-birth-weight pre-
mature infants. Dev Psychol. 2003;39:730–744
37. Flisher AJ, Kramer RA, Hoven CW, et al. Risk behavior in a
community sample of children and adolescents. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 2000;39:881–887
780 MCCORMICK, et al
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2005-1316
2006;117;771Pediatrics
and Patrick H. Casey
C. Bernbaum, Charles R. Bauer, Camilia Martin, Elizabeth R. Woods, Anne Martin
JudyJennifer Yu, Mikhail Salganik, David T. Scott, Forrest C. Bennett, Libby L. Kay,
Marie C. McCormick, Jeanne Brooks-Gunn, Stephen L. Buka, Julie Goldman,
of Age for the Infant Health and Development Program
Early Intervention in Low Birth Weight Premature Infants: Results at 18 Years
Services
Updated Information &
ml
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/117/3/771.full.ht
including high resolution figures, can be found at:
References
ml#ref-list-1
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/117/3/771.full.ht
at:
This article cites 21 articles, 2 of which can be accessed free
Citations
ml#related-urls
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/117/3/771.full.ht
This article has been cited by 37 HighWire-hosted articles:
Subspecialty Collections
orn_infant_sub
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/fetus:newb
Fetus/Newborn Infant
nt:behavioral_issues_sub
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/developme
Development/Behavioral Issues
the following collection(s):
This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in
Permissions & Licensing
ml
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xht
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
Reprints
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml
Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2006 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published,
PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly
by guest on February 10, 2014pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
... Overall, these reviews demonstrated a positive impact on CLS outcomes for mothers and youth, though they often cite the same few randomized control trials of NFP by Olds and colleagues (Kitzman et al., 2019;Olds et al., 1997Olds et al., , 1998. In one exception, Cohen and colleagues' (2010) review of early childhood interventions cited evidence from a trial of the Infant Health and Development Program demonstrating a positive impact on juvenile arrest (McCormick et al., 2006). ...
... Their review of FHV almost exclusively relies on evidence from trials of Nurse Family Partnership, including evidence that participating mothers had fewer arrests (Olds et al., 1997) and youth had fewer arrests, convictions, and violations of probation. The authors also note findings from a trial of the Infant Health and Development Program, which demonstrated that program participation reduced the likelihood of juvenile arrest (McCormick et al., 2006). In addition to their review of early childhood interventions, the authors include cost calculations for the value of lifetime costs imposed on society for each of the "social ills" (p. ...
Article
Full-text available
Each year, millions of families with children in the United States (US) come into contact with the criminal legal system (CLS), the deleterious consequences of which are well documented. Families exposed to the CLS often face many stressors and may benefit from supports and services designed to enhance parent–child relationships and connect them to health-promoting resources and services. Early childhood family home visiting (FHV) is a two-generation strategy to support pregnant women and families with infants and young children, many of whom encounter the CLS. Yet, little is known about the CLS experiences of families receiving FHV. This scoping review summarizes the published research on CLS experiences among FHV-enrolled families in the US. Seven online databases were used to identify research published between 1967 and 2022. Following PRISMA guidelines, articles were required to focus on FHV and CLS involvement. Twenty-eight articles met inclusion criteria; five were systematic reviews or meta-analyses, 22 were primary sources with quantitative measures of CLS, and one was a qualitative study. Among the primary quantitative sources, more than half (55%) included CLS measures to describe the sample and the others included CLS variables as outcomes. CLS involvement was a common experience among families receiving FHV services. This scoping review provides an important first step in describing the existing research on FHV participants’ CLS involvement and can inform future efforts to serve this group of families.
... Therefore, some programmes have been developed for full-term infants in low-socioeconomic situations to provide early educational support, which successfully improves intellectual development [47,48]. However, the benefits of these programmes are mixed when applied to preterm infants and the benefit is even less clear to those with brain injury [49]. Although it has been shown that greater maternal education mitigates neurodevelopmental impairment by brain injury, it is unclear how these infants compare to the rest of the population as they are typically unaccounted for in developmental studies [44,50]. ...
... Decreasing effects over time, especially in high-risk environments, may be attributable to environmental influences on children's developmental tracks after the intervention period (Fernald, et al., 2017). It is therefore important to investigate the biological and environmental factors that support or inhibit the sustained effects of early nutritional intervention (McCormick et al., 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
A community‐based, cluster non‐randomized controlled trial was conducted in Kurram district, Pakistan between January 2018 to December 2020. Age‐appropriate lipid‐based nutrient supplements and health education (sessions conducted in the households) were given to pregnant women and their born children (6–23 months) in the intervention arm (n = 40 clusters) versus health education only in the control arm (n = 40 clusters) to evaluate its effect on child development. The first and second developmental assessments were completed at ~24 months (n = 689) and ~32 months (n = 608), respectively, using the Caregiver‐Reported Early Development Instrument Long form. The overall and domain‐specific (motor, language, cognitive and socio‐emotional) scores were computed with higher scores indicating better child development. Higher development scores, including overall (β = 0.40, 95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.14, 0.65; p = 0.002), cognitive (β = 0.27, 95% CI: 0.10, 0.45; p = 0.002), motor (β = 0.39, 95% CI: 0.22, 0.56; p < 0.001) and language (β = 0.33, 95% CI: 0.14, 0.51; p = 0.001) were reported for children who received the intervention compared to the control arm at first developmental assessment. However, the effect was not sustained after the discontinuation of the intervention. The LNS received by the mothers (during pregnancy and first 6 months after delivery) and by children during 6–23 months of age was beneficial for the children. The trial is registered in the International Standard Randomised Controlled Trial Number Registry (ID: ISRCTN94319790) on December 11, 2017.
Article
Children with complex congenital heart defects often show delays and deficits in cognitive, language, motor, and social-emotional functioning. As such, the American Academy of Pediatrics and the American Heart Associated recommend ongoing monitoring and support of development. In conjunction with the formal therapeutic supports frequently recommended for young children with CHD, caregivers are instrumental in providing an enriching environment to enhance development in the hospital, at home, and in early childhood settings. In this article, we review the common developmental sequelae associated with CHD, underlying causes, and ways in which parents, family members, and professional caregivers can support development for children with CHD from birth to three years old.
Article
Full-text available
Researchers and policymakers aspire for educational interventions to change children’s long-run developmental trajectories. However, intervention impacts on cognitive and achievement measures commonly fade over time. Less is known, although much is theorized, about social–emotional skill persistence. The current meta-analysis investigated whether intervention impacts on social–emotional skills demonstrated greater persistence than impacts on cognitive skills. We drew studies from eight preexisting meta-analyses, generating a sample of 86 educational randomized controlled trials targeting children from infancy through adolescence, together involving 56,662 participants and 450 outcomes measured at posttest and at least one follow-up. Relying on a metaregression approach for modeling persistence rates, we tested the extent to which posttest impact magnitudes predicted follow-up impact magnitudes. We found that posttest impacts were equally predictive of follow-up impacts for cognitive and social–emotional skills at 6- to 12-month follow-up, indicating similar conditional persistence rates across skill types. At 1- to 2-year follow-up, rates were lower, and, if anything, cognitive skills showed greater conditional persistence than social–emotional skills. A small positive follow-up effect was observed, on average, beyond what was directly predicted by the posttest impact, indicating that interventions may have long-term effects that are not fully mediated by posttest effects. This pattern of results implied that smaller posttest impacts produced more persistent effects than larger posttest impacts, and social–emotional skill impacts were smaller, on average, than cognitive skill impacts. Considered as a whole, intervention impacts on both social–emotional and cognitive skills demonstrated fadeout, especially for interventions that produced larger initial effects. Implications for theory and future directions are discussed.
Article
The Infant Health and Development Program was an eight-site randomized controlled trial testing the efficacy of early intervention to enhance the cognitive, behavioral, and health status of low birth weight, premature infants. The 377 intervention families received for the first 3 years of life: (1) pediatric follow-up, (2) home visits, (3) parent support groups, and (4) a systematic educational program provided in specialized child development centers. The control group (n = 608) received the same pediatric follow-up and referral services only. This paper describes the delivery of the intervention and its outcomes. A Family Participation Index that was the sum of participation frequencies in each of the program modalities unique to the intervention revealed that program implementation was not different across the eight sites. Index scores did not vary systematically with mother's ethnicity, age, or education or with child's birth weight, gender, or neonatal health status; but they were positively related to children's IQ scores at age 3. Only 1.9% of children of families in the highest tercile of participation scored in the mentally retarded range (IQ ≤70), whereas 3.5% and 13% of children in the middle and lowest participation terciles, respectively, scored in the retarded range. Similar findings were obtained for borderline intellectual functioning. These findings are consistent with previous research linking intensity of intervention services with degree of positive cognitive outcomes for high-risk infants. The determinants of variations in individual family participation remain unknown.
Book
A guide to using S environments to perform statistical analyses providing both an introduction to the use of S and a course in modern statistical methods. The emphasis is on presenting practical problems and full analyses of real data sets.
Article
The Infant Health and Development Program is an eight-site clinical trial designed to evaluate the efficacy of a comprehensive early intervention in reducing the developmental and health problems of low-birth-weight (≤2500 g) premature (≤37 weeks) infants. Nine hundred eighty-five infants, stratified by site and weight (≤2000 g or 2001 to 2500 g), were randomly assigned to receive an educational curriculum focused on child development, as well as family support and pediatric follow-up, or only pediatric follow-up. At corrected age 36 months, the intervention group had significantly higher mean IQ scores than the follow-up group (mean difference in the heavier group was 13.2 and in the lighter group 6.6), significantly fewer maternally reported behavior problems, and a small, but statistically significant, increase in maternally reported minor illnesses for the lighter-birth-weight group only, with no difference in serious health conditions.