ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) quantifies autistic traits in adults. This paper adapted the AQ for children (age 9.8-15.4 years). Three groups of participants were assessed: Group 1: n=52 adolescents with Asperger Syndrome (AS) or high-functioning autism (HFA); Group 2: n=79 adolescents with classic autism; and Group 3, n=50 controls. The adolescents with AS/HFA did not differ significantly from the adolescents with autism but both clinical groups scored higher than controls. Approximately 90% of the adolescents with AS/HFA and autism scored 30+, vs. none of the controls. Among the controls, boys scored higher than girls. The AQ can rapidly quantify where an adolescent is situated on the continuum from autism to normality.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ)—Adolescent Version
Simon Baron-Cohen,
1,2
Rosa A. Hoekstra,
1
Rebecca Knickmeyer,
1
and Sally Wheelwright
1
The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) quantifies autistic traits in adults. This paper adapted
the AQ for children (age 9.8–15.4 years). Three groups of participants were assessed: Group 1:
n=52 adolescents with Asperger Syndrome (AS) or high-functioning autism (HFA); Group 2:
n=79 adolescents with classic autism; and Group 3, n=50 controls. The adolescents with AS/
HFA did not differ significantly from the adolescents with autism but both clinical groups
scored higher than controls. Approximately 90% of the adolescents with AS/HFA and autism
scored 30+, vs. none of the controls. Among the controls, boys scored higher than girls. The
AQ can rapidly quantify where an adolescent is situated on the continuum from autism to
normality.
KEY WORDS: AQ; adolescents; screening; autistic spectrum; Asperger Syndrome.
INTRODUCTION
In an earlier issue in this journal, we reported
on the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in adults
with high functioning autism (HFA) or Asperger
Syndrome (AS) (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skin-
ner, Martin, & Clubley, 2001). The adult AQ was
developed because of a lack of a quick and quan-
titative self-report instrument for assessing how
many autistic traits any adult has. The minimum
score on the AQ is 0 and the maximum 50. If an
adult has equal to or more than 32 out of 50 such
traits, this is highly predictive of AS. The AQ has
been found to correlate inversely with the Empathy
Quotient (EQ) (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2004),
the Friendship and Relationship Quotient (FQ)
(Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2003) and to corre-
late positively with the Systemizing Quotient (SQ)
(Baron-Cohen, Richler, Bisarya, Gurunathan, &
Wheelwright, 2003).
The AQ has also been found to be strongly
predictive of who receives a diagnosis of AS in a clinic
setting (Woodbury-Smith, Robinson, & Baron-Cohen,
2005). The AQ also reveals sex differences (males>
females) and cognitive differences (scientists>non-
scientists) (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001), a pattern of
results that has been closely replicated in a Japanese
sample (Wakabayashi, Baron-Cohen, & Wheelwright,
2004). This latter pattern suggests that these effects are
not culture-specific and may instead reflect sexual
dimorphism in the brain and differences in neural
organization between scientists (a clear example of
‘systemizersÕ) and non-scientists.
The AQ depends on self-report, which may be a
concern in individuals whose social deficits may
impair their accuracy in self-awareness. However, a
parent-version confirms that adults of normal IQ
with autism spectrum conditions are able to provide
such information reliably (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001).
Although the AQ is useful both clinically and in
research studies as a screen for diagnosis, it has not
been studied as a general population screen, and
indeed if it was tested for this purpose it is likely that
1
Department of Psychiatry, Autism Research Centre, University
of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
2
Correspondence should be addressed to: Simon Baron-Cohen,
Department of Psychiatry, Autism Research Centre, University
of Cambridge, Douglas House, 18b Trumpington Road, CB2
2AH, Cambridge, UK.; e-mail: sb205@cam.ac.uk
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 36, No. 3, April 2006 (Ó 2006)
DOI 10.1007/s10803-006-0073-6
Published Online: March 22, 2006
343
0162-3257/06/0400-0343/0 Ó 2006 Springer ScienceþBusiness Media, Inc.
a significantl y higher cut-off would need to be
employed to keep false-positives to a minimum.
Given the usefulness of the AQ, it is of interest to
test a revised version of this instrument with adoles-
cents. In this paper, we test (1) whether similar results
are found on an adolescent version of the AQ, and (2)
whether similar results are found in classic autism as in
AS. Whereas the ad ult AQ (for ages 16+) entails self-
report, the adolescent AQ requires a parent/carer to
complete it, but otherwise retains the same items and
structure as the adult version. The adolescent AQ was
designed to be short, easy to use, and easy to score.
The adolescent version is shown in Appendix 1.
It comprises 50 questions, made up of 10 questions
assessing 5 different areas: social skill (items
1,11,13,15,22,36,44,45,47,48); attention switching
(items 2,4,10,16,25,32,34,37,43,46); attention to detail
(items 5,6,9,12,19,23,28,29,30,49); communication
(items 7,17,18,26,27,31,33,35,38,39); and imagination
(items 3,8,14,20 ,21,24,40,41,42,50). Each of the items
listed above scores 1 point if the respondent records
the abnormal or autistic-like behaviour either
mildly or strongly (see below for scoring each item;
Abnormality—poor social skill, poor communication
skill, poor imagination, exceptional attention to
detail, poor attention-switching/strong focus of atten-
tion). Approximately half the items were worded to
produce a ‘disagreeÕ response, and half an ‘agreeÕ
response, in a high scoring person with AS/HFA.
This was to avoid a response bias either way.
Following this , items were randomized with respect
to both the expected response from a high-s corer, and
with respect to their domain.
Participants
3 groups of participants were tested:
Group 1 comprised n=52 adolescents with AS/
HFA (38 males, 14 females). This sex ratio of 2.7:1
(m:f) is similar to that found in other samples (Klin,
Volkmar, Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Rourke, 1995). All
participants in this group had been diagnosed by
psychiatrists using established criteria for autism or
AS (APA, 1994). They were recruited via several
sources, including the National Autistic Society (UK),
specialist clinics carrying out diagnostic assessments,
and adverts in newsletters/web-pages for children with
AS/HFA. Their mean age was 13.6 years (SD=2.0,
range 10.3–15.4). They all attended mains tream
schooling and by parental report, had an IQ in the
normal range. See below for a check of this. Because we
did not collect data on age of onset of language these
individuals are grouped together, rather than attempt-
ing to separate them into AS vs. HFA. The final sample
of 52 comprises those who responded from a larger
sample of 63. All participants were regarded as
independent, in being gen etically unrelated.
Group 2 comprised n=79 adolescents with clas-
sic autism (63 males, 16 females). Again, this sex ratio
of 3.9:1 is similar to that found in other samples. All
participants in this group had been diagnosed by
psychiatrists using established criteria for autism
(APA, 1994). They too were recruited via the
National Autistic Society (UK), specialist clinics
carrying out diagnostic assessments, and adverts in
newsletters/web-pages for children with autism. Their
mean age was 12.5 years (SD=1.7, range 9.8–16.0).
They all attended special schools for autism or
learning difficulties, and by parental report, had an
IQ below the normal range. See below for a check of
this. The final sample of 79 comprised those who
responded from a larger sample of 85.
Group 3 comprised 50 adolescents selected at
random (n=25 males and 25 females). They were
drawn from 200 adolescents. They were all attending
mainstream schools (2 primary and 2 secondary) in
the East Anglia area. Questionnaires were distributed
by the schoolteachers via the childrenÕs school class.
Their mean age was 13.6 years (SD=1.8, range 10.1--
16.5). The 3 groups did not differ significantly at the
p=.05 level for age.
In Groups 1 and 2, 15 individuals were randomly
selected from the individuals who had returned an
AQ and invited into the lab to check pro-rated IQ,
using 4 subtests of the WAIS-R (see below). These
parents were also asked to complete a second AQ as a
measure of test–retest reliabil ity.
Method
Parents were sent the AQ by post, and were
instructed to complete it as quickly as possible (to
avoid thinking about responses too long). To confirm
the diagnosis of Group 1 being high-functioning and
the diagnosis of Group 2 being lower-functioning, 15
of each were randomly selected and invited into the
lab for intellectual assessment using 4 subtests of
the WISC-R (Wechsler, 1958). The 4 subtests of the
WISC-R were Vocabulary, Similarities, Block
Design, and Picture Completion. On this basis, all
15 participants selected from Group 1 had a prorated
IQ of at least 85, that is, in the normal range
(mean=106.5, SD=8.0). These participantsÕ parents
were also given the AQ when they came into the lab
344 Baron-Cohen, Hoekstra, Knickmeyer, and Wheelwright
in person, in order to investigate test–retest reliability
for the instrument on a small subset of children. All
15 of the participants selected from Group 2 had a
prorated IQ of below 84, that is, below the normal
range (mean=72.6, SD=8.0). Note that we have
conservatively used an IQ of >85 as an inclus ion
criterion for AS/HFA.
Scoring the AQ
‘Definitely agreeÕ or ‘slightly agreeÕ responses
scored 1 point, on the following items: 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 9,
12, 13, 16, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 26, 33, 35, 39, 41, 42,
43, 45, 46. ‘Definitely disagreeÕ or ‘slightly disagreeÕ
responses scored 1 point, on the following items: 1, 3,
8, 10, 11, 14, 15 , 17, 24, 25, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 32, 34,
36, 37, 38, 40, 44, 47, 48, 49, 50. If a respond ent left
more than 5 items blank, this AQ was deemed to be
incomplete and was omitted from the sample.
Results
Mean AQ scores (total) for each group, broken
down by sex and by subdomain, are shown in
Table I. Comparing groups using an ANOVA of
Total AQ score by GROUP and SEX, we found as
predicted a significant effect of GROUP (F (2,
175)=201.49, p<.001). Post Hoc Scheffe tests
revealed that the two clinical groups scored signif-
icantly higher than the control group (p<.0001), but
that the two clinical groups did not differ from each
other. The main effect of SEX was not signi ficant (F
(1, 175)=2.84, p>.09), but there was a significant
two-way interaction of GROUPSEX (F (2,
175)=6.48, p=.002).
T-tests confirmed that there was a significant sex
difference (t=)3.27, p=.002) in the control group
(males scoring higher than females), confirming the
same effect reported with the adult AQ. There were
no significant sex differences in the clinical groups
(group 1: t=1.90, p =.063; group 2: t=)2.0, p=.049
(non-significant when Bonferroni correction for
multiple tests is used)). The clinical groups differed
from the control group on all subdomain scores (t-
tests: see Table I). A stepwise regression analysis in
the control group revealed a significant effect of sex
(F (1, 191)=23.24, p<.001, males scoring higher),
but no effect of age (t=)1.45, p=.149). Figure 1
shows the Group and Sex differences graphically.
Table I. Mean AQ and Subscale Scores (and SDs) by Group
Communication Social Imagination Local details Attention switching Total AQ
Group 1
AS/HFA (n=52) x 8.2 7.8 6.7 6.1 8.5 37.3
SD 1.6 1.8 2.2 2.4 1.7 5.8
AS/HFA boys (n=38) x 7.9 7.4 6.8 6.1 8.2 36.4
SD 1.6 1.7 2.3 2.5 1.9 6.0
AS/HFA girls (n=14) x 9.0 8.8 6.8 6.1 9.0 39.8
SD 1.4 1.4 2.0 2.4 1.0 4.3
Group 2
Autism (n=79) x 8.0 8.0 7.6 6.5 8.3 38.3
SD 1.5 1.9 2.0 2.1 1.6 6.0
Autism boys (n=63) x 8.1 8.2 7.7 6.6 8.3 39.0
SD 1.5 1.8 2.0 2.2 1.6 5.9
Autism girls (n=16) x 7.6 7.3 6.8 5.9 8.1 35.7
SD 1.6 2.0 2.1 1.9 1.8 6.1
Group 3
Controls (n=50) x 2.7 2.0 3.2 5.3 4.5 17.7
SD 1.7 1.9 2.3 2.4 2.0 5.7
Control boys (n=25) x 2.9 2.2 4.4 5.8 5.0 20.2
SD 1.8 1.9 2.2 2.6 1.7 4.8
Control girls (n=25) x 2.6 1.8 2.0 4.8 4.1 15.3
SD 1.6 1.9 1.8 2.2 2.1 5.7
Controls vs. AS/HFA t
a
)16.9 )15.7 )7.9 )1.7
#
)10.7 )17.2
Controls vs. autism t
a
)18.4 )17.7 )11.2 )2.8 )11.2 )19.2
AS/HFA vs. autism T
b
0.7 )0.8 )2.0 )0.8 )0.7 )0.9
a
All t values in this row are significant at the p<.001 level except where shown (
#
).
b
None of these t values are significant at the p<.05 level.
Adolescent AQ 345
An item analysis (percentage of each group
scoring on each item) is shown in Table II. On only 2
items out of 50 (items 29, and 30) did controls score
more than the clinical groups, strongly confirming the
value of these items for discriminating autism spec-
trum vs. controls. These two items were conserva-
tively retain ed in the analysis since, if anything, they
served to reduce the size of group differences. The
internal consistency of items in each of the 5 domains
was also calculated, and CronbachÕs a Coefficients
were all high (Communication=0.82; Social=0.88;
Imagination=0.81; Attention to Detail=0.66; Atten-
tion Switching=0.76). CronbachÕs a Coefficient for
the AQ as a whole was also high (=0.79). CronbachÕs
a Coefficients for each group were all in the range
0.6–0.9.
The pe rcentage of each group scoring at or
above each AQ score is shown in Table III. A useful
cut-off would discriminate the groups with as many
true positives and as few false positives as possible. In
the adults AQ, an AQ score of 32+ was chosen as a
useful cut-off, since 79.3% of the AS/HFA group
scored at this level, whilst only 2% of controls did so.
32+ also seemed to be a useful cut-off for distin-
guishing adult females with AS/HFA (92.3% scoring
at this point or above) vs. control adult females (1%
of whom score at this point or above). Regar ding the
adolescent AQ, if the same cut-off score is used (of
32+), none of the control participants scored at this
level, whilst all girls with AS/HFA scored above this
level, as do 73.7% of the boys with AS/HFA. In the
autism group, 68.8% of the girls, and 87.3% of the
boys scored at this level. If we decrease the cut-off
score to 30+, none of the controls score above this
level. All AS/HFA girls, and 86.8% of the AS/HFA
boys score abo ve this level, compared to 81.3% (girls)
and 90.5% (boys) in the autism group. A cut-off at
this point might be consider ed in future screening
studies.
Table III also shows that control females never
score as high as 29+, whereas 4% of control males
do. Note also that at AQ score 22+, there are almost
four times as many males (44%) as females (12%)in
the control group scoring at this intermediate point
on the scale. This suggests that there is not only a sex
difference on the child AQ overall (as reflected in the
male mean AQ being higher than the female mean),
and a sex difference at high levels on the AQ (reflected
in the sex ratio in Group 1 being 3.5:1), but that
significantly more males than females in the general
population show moderate levels of ‘‘autistic traits’’.
1
Again, this pattern of results replicates that found
with the adult AQ. No differences were found
between the autism and HFA/AS groups on the
AQ. Finally, test–retest reliability was high (r=.92,
p<.001).
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
0 to 5
6 to 10
11 to 15
16 to 20
21 to 25
26 to 30
31 to 35
36 to 40
41 to 45
46 to 50
AQ score
Percent of subjects
Control girls
Control boys
AS/HFA/Aut boys and girls
Fig. 1. Percent of each group scoring at each group score.
346 Baron-Cohen, Hoekstra, Knickmeyer, and Wheelwright
DISCUSSION
In this paper, we report data from the adolescent
version of the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ), for
measuring the degree to which an individual adoles-
cent shows autistic traits. As pr edicted, adolescents
with Asper ger Syndrome (AS)/high functioning aut-
ism (HFA) or with classic autism scored significantly
higher on the AQ than matched controls. Eighty
percent to 90% (mean=89.3%) scored above a critical
minimum of 30+, whereas none of the controls did
so. This demonstrates that the ado lescent AQ has
reasonable face validity, since the questionnaire pur-
ports to measure autistic spectrum traits, and people
with a diagnosis involving these traits score highly on
it. The adolescent AQ can also be said to have
reasonable construct validity, in that items purporting
to measure each of the 5 domains of interest (social,
communication, imagination, attention to detail, and
attention switching) show high a coefficients. Future
work needs to test the false negative rate. The
adolescent AQ has excellent test–retest reliability.
It is of interest that there were no significant
effects of age on adolescent AQ score, in the normal
control group. This suggests that what is being
measured by the AQ does not change with age, and
that the items are not biased towards one particular
age group. Regarding the comparison of classic
autism vs. HFA/AS, no significant differences were
found, which is also interesting. This may be because
the items are not biased towards language skills.
However, because of the communication subscale,
which includes items about conversational compe-
tence, we recommend that the AQ is primarily of
value for use with individuals with some speech, and
with an intelligence in the borderline average range
(70) or above.
Within the control group, males score slightly
but significantly higher than females, both overall,
and at intermediate and high levels of autistic traits.
This is consistent with the extreme male brain theory
of autism (Asperger, 1944; Baron-Cohen, 2002;
Baron-Cohen & Hammer, 1997) an d may have
implications for the marked sex ratio in autism and
AS (Wing, 1981).
It is important to mention that this study was
not in a position to compare the adolescent AQ to
other instruments that have been developed to
measure AS, such as the ASSQ (Ehlers, Gillberg, &
Wing, 1999), the CAST (Scott, Baron-Cohen, Bol-
ton, & Brayne, 2002), or the Australian Scale for
Asperger Syndrome (Attwood, 1997). It will be of
Table II. Item Analysis: Percentage of Each Group Scoring on
Each Item
ItemAS/HFA (n=52)Autism (n=79)Controls (n=50)Subdomain
1 67.3 83.5 22.0 S
2 84.6 89.9 54.0 A
3 55.8 70.3 18.0 I
4 90.4 94.9 68.0 A
5 80.8 87.3 50.0 D
6 65.4 72.2 68.0 D
7 78.8 54.4 28.0 C
8 56.9 75.0 20.0 I
9 40.4 39.7 26.0 D
10 86.3 92.4 26.0 A
11 86.5 88.6 26.0 S
12 92.3 85.9 62.0 D
13 55.8 55.7 8.0 S
14 59.6 84.8 34.0 I
15 90.4 88.6 26.5 S
16 88.5 91.1 62.0 A
17 88.5 92.4 8.0 C
18 73.1 39.7 48.0 C
19 45.1 45.6 34.0 D
20 75.0 78.7 34.0 I
21 51.9 60.3 42.0 I
22 92.3 89.9 20.0 S
23 63.5 72.2 40.0 D
24 63.5 64.5 48.0 I
25 82.7 81.0 32.0 A
26 92.3 89.9 24.0 C
27 86.5 96.2 42.0 C
28 78.8 84.4 36.0 D
29 51.9 48.1 76.0 D
30 51.9 66.7 76.0 D
31 82.7 92.4 28.0 C
32 86.5 93.7 42.0 A
33 75.0 80.8 10.0 C
34 78.8 74.7 16.0 A
35 71.2 88.6 26.0 C
36 90.4 70.9 14.3 S
37 80.8 57.0 26.0 A
38 84.6 97.5 10.0 C
39 88.5 73.1 48.0 C
40 88.2 96.2 24.0 I
41 69.2 58.2 28.0 I
42 82.7 94.8 38.0 I
43 75.0 65.8 62.0 A
44 50.0 65.8 12.0 S
45 94.2 93.7 44.0 S
46 94.2 86.1 66.0 A
47 63.5 74.7 14.0 S
48 84.6 91.1 12.0 S
49 44.2 49.4 62.0 D
50 78.8 89.9 34.0 I
Key: S, Social skills; A, Attention switching; D, Attention to detail;
C, Communication; I, Imagination.
Adolescent AQ 347
interest in future studies to test how many cases of
AS each of these instruments correctly identifies (true
positive rate) as well as the rate of false negatives.
Future work could also examine any relationship
between adolesc ent AQ score and severity of
symptoms.
We wish to underline that the AQ is not
diagnostic, but may serve as a useful instrument in
Table III. Percent of Participants in Groups 1 and 2 Scoring at or Above each AQ Score—Overleaf
AQ
Score
AS/HFA
(n=52)
AS boys
(n=38)
AS/HFA
girls (n=14)
Autism
(n=79)
Autism boys
(n=63)
Autism girls
(n=16)
Controls
(n=50)
Control
boys (n=25)
Control
girls(n=25)
0 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100
1 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100
2 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100
3 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100
4 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100
5 100 100 100 100 100 100 98.0 100 96.0
6 100 100 100 100 100 100 98.0 100 96.0
7 100 100 100 100 100 100 98.0 100 96.0
8 100 100 100 100 100 100 98.0 100 96.0
9 100 100 100 100 100 100 92.0 100 84.0
10 100 100 100 100 100 100 90.0 100 80.0
11 100 100 100 100 100 100 88.0 100 76.0
12 100 100 100 100 100 100 84.0 100 68.0
13 100 100 100 100 100 100 80.0 96.0 64.0
14 100 100 100 100 100 100 74.0 88.0 60.0
15 100 100 100 100 100 100 72.0 88.0 56.0
16 100 100 100 100 100 100 70.0 84.0 56.0
17 100 100 100 100 100 100 60.0 72.0 48.0
18 100 100 100 100 100 100 52.0 64.0 40.0
19 100 100 100 100 100 100 48.0 60.0 36.0
20 100 100 100 100 100 100 44.0 56.0 32.0
21 100 100 100 100 100 100 34.0 48.0 20.0
22 100 100 100 100 100 100 28.0 44.0 12.0
23 100 100 100 100 100 100 22.0 36.0 8.0
24 98.1 97.4 100 100 100 100 14.0 24.0 4.0
25 98.1 97.4 100 98.7 100 93.8 14.0 24.0 4.0
26 98.1 97.4 100 98.7 100 93.8 6.0 12.0 0
27 98.1 97.4 100 96.2 96.8 93.8 4.0 8.0 0
28 94.2 92.1 100 93.7 95.2 87.5 4.0 8.0 0
29 92.3 89.5 100 92.4 93.7 87.5 2.0 4.0 0
30 90.4 86.8 100 88.6 90.5 81.3 0 0 0
31 88.5 84.2 100 87.3 90.5 75.0 0 0 0
32 80.8 73.7 100 83.5 87.3 68.8 0 0 0
33 76.9 68.4 100 83.5 87.3 68.8 0 0 0
34 71.2 65.8 85.7 75.9 77.8 68.8 0 0 0
35 65.4 60.5 78.6 73.4 74.6 68.8 0 0 0
36 63.5 57.9 78.6 68.4 71.4 56.3 0 0 0
37 59.6 52.6 78.6 64.6 69.8 43.8 0 0 0
38 57.7 52.6 71.4 60.8 66.7 37.5 0 0 0
39 46.2 39.5 64.3 53.2 57.1 37.5 0 0 0
40 38.5 31.6 57.1 49.4 54.0 31.3 0 0 0
41 32.7 26.3 50.0 43.0 46.0 31.3 0 0 0
42 30.8 26.3 42.9 38.0 41.3 25.0 0 0 0
43 21.2 18.4 28.6 30.4 34.9 12.5 0 0 0
44 17.3 15.8 21.4 19.0 22.2 6.3 0 0 0
45 9.6 7.9 14.3 13.9 17.5 0 0 0 0
46 3.8 2.6 7.1 10.1 12.7 0 0 0 0
47 1.9 2.6 0 5.1 6.3 0 0 0 0
48 0 0 0 3.8 4.8 0 0 0 0
49 0 0 0 2.5 3.2 0 0 0 0
50 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
348 Baron-Cohen, Hoekstra, Knickmeyer, and Wheelwright
identifying the extent of autistic traits shown by a
person of normal intelligence. Currently this woul d
be for research purposes primarily, as the AQ has
not been tested as a screening instrument in the
general population. Fut ure research could compare
the sensitivity, specificity, and positive predictive
value (PPV) of this instrument in community
screening. Finally, the hope is that with quantitative
instruments to measure the autistic spectrum across
different ages, this will improve comparability across
research studies, assist in defining the phenotype in
genetic studies, and improve screening of undiag-
nosed cases who need to be referred for a full
diagnostic assessment.
NOTE
1. i.e. those traits which people with AS or HFA
tend to endorse on the AQ.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors were supported by a grant from the
MRC and the Lurie-Marks Foundation, during the
period of this work. Rosa Hoekstra was supported by
a grant from the HsN Hersenstichting Nederland,
the Bekker-La Bastide Fund and the Schuurman
Schimmel van Outeren Foundation.
APPENDIX 1: The Adolescent AQ
Definitely
agree
Slightly
agree
Slightly
disagree
Definitely
disagree
1. S/he prefers to do things with others rather than on her/his own.
2. S/he prefers to do things the same way over and over again.
3. If s/he tries to imagine something, s/he finds it very easy to create a picture in her/his mind.
4. S/he frequently gets so strongly absorbed in one thing that s/he loses sight of other things.
5. S/he often notices small sounds when others do not.
6. S/he usually notices car number plates or similar strings of information.
7. Other people frequently tell her/him that what s/he has said is impolite, even though s/he thinks
it is polite.
8. When s/he is reading a story, s/he can easily imagine what the characters might look like.
9. S/he is fascinated by dates.
10. In a social group, s/he can easily keep track of several different people’s conversations.
11. S/he finds social situations easy.
12. S/he tends to notice details that others do not.
13. S/he would rather go to a library than a party.
14. S/he finds making up stories easy.
15. S/he finds her/himself drawn more strongly to people than to things.
16. S/he tends to have very strong interests, which s/he gets upset about if s/he can’t pursue.
17. S/he enjoys social chit-chat.
18. When s/he talks, it isn’t always easy for others to get a word in edgeways.
19. S/he is fascinated by numbers.
20. When s/he is reading a story, s/he finds it difficult to work out the characters’ intentions.
21. S/he doesn’t particularly enjoy reading fiction.
22. S/he finds it hard to make new friends.
23. S/he notices patterns in things all the time.
24. S/he would rather go to the theatre than a museum.
25. It does not upset him/her if his/her daily routine is disturbed.
26. S/he frequently finds that s/he doesn’t know how to keep a conversation going.
27. S/he finds it easy to ‘‘read between the lines’’ when someone is talking to her/him.
28. S/he usually concentrates more on the whole picture, rather than the small details.
29. S/he is not very good at remembering phone numbers.
30. S/he doesn’t usually notice small changes in a situation, or a person’s appearance.
31. S/he knows how to tell if someone listening to him/her is getting bored.
32. S/he finds it easy to do more than one thing at once.
33. When s/he talks on the phone, s/he is not sure when it’s her/his turn to speak.
34. S/he enjoys doing things spontaneously.
35. S/he is often the last to understand the point of a joke.
36. S/he finds it easy to work out what someone is thinking or feeling just by looking at their face.
Adolescent AQ 349
REFERENCES
APA. (1994). DSM-IV diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders, 4th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric
Association.
Asperger, H. (1944). Die ‘‘Autistischen Psychopathen’’ im
Kindesalter. Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten,
117, 76–136.
Attwood, T. (1997). AspergerÕs Syndrome. UK: Jessica Kingsley.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.
Baron-Cohen, S., & Hammer, J. (1997). Is autism an extreme form
of the male brain?. Advances in Infancy Research, 11, 193–217.
Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., &
Wheelwright, S. (2003). The Systemising Quotient (SQ): An
investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome or high
functioning autism and normal sex differences. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, Special issue on
‘‘Autism: Mind and Brain’’, 358, 361–374.
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The Friendship
Questionnaire (FQ): An investigation of adults with Asperger
Syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differ-
ences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 509–
517.
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The Empathy Quo-
tient (EQ). An investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome
or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Jour-
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., &
Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ):
Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/high functioning autism,
males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.
Ehlers, S., Gillberg, C., & Wing, L. (1999). A screening question-
naire for Asperger Syndrome and other high-functioning
autism spectrum disorders in school-age children. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 129–141.
Klin, A., Volkmar, F., Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., & Rourke, B.
(1995). Validity and neuropsychological characterization of
Asperger Syndrome: Convergence with nonverbal learning
disabilities syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
chiatry, 36, 1127–1140.
Scott, F., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., & Brayne, C. (2002). The
CAST (Childhood Asperger Syndrome Test): Preliminary
development of UK screen for mainstream primary-school
children. Autism, 6(1), 9–31.
Wakabayashi, A., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The
Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) Japanese version: Evidence
from high-functioning clinical group and normal adults. The
Japanese Journal of Psychology, 75, 78–84.
Wechsler, D. (1958). Sex differences in intelligence : The measure-
ment and appraisal of adult intelligence. Baltimore: Williams &
Wilking.
Wing, L. (1981). Asperger Syndrome: A clinical account. Psycho-
logical Medicine, 11, 115–130.
Woodbury-Smith, M., Robinson, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2005).
Screening adults for Asperger Syndrome using the AQ: diag-
nostic validity in clinical practice. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 35, 331–335.
Appendix 1. (Continued)
Definitely
agree
Slightly
agree
Slightly
disagree
Definitely
disagree
37. If there is an interruption, s/he can switch back to what s/he was doing very quickly.
38. S/he is good at social chit-chat.
39. People often tell her/him that s/he keeps going on and on about the same thing.
40. When s/he was younger, s/he used to enjoy playing games involving pretending with other
children.
41. S/he likes to collect information about categories of things (e.g. types of car, types of bird, types
of train, types of plant, etc.).
42. S/he finds it difficult to imagine what it would be like to be someone else.
43. S/he likes to plan any activities s/he participates in carefully.
44. S/he enjoys social occasions.
45. S/he finds it difficult to work out people’s intentions.
46. New situations make him/her anxious.
47. S/he enjoys meeting new people.
48. S/he is a good diplomat.
49. S/he is not very good at remembering people’s date of birth.
50. S/he finds it very to easy to play games with children that involve pretending.
Ó MRC-SBC/SJW Feb 1998.
350 Baron-Cohen, Hoekstra, Knickmeyer, and Wheelwright
... The AQ assesses five different areas: social skill, attention switching, communication, imagination and attention to detail. In 2006, the AQ was adapted for adolescents [5]. The AQ can be used to detect whether a person is suspected of having ASD. ...
... The Baron-Cohen method of scoring was used, where the two highest and two lowest scores for an item are combined in a dichotomous response [4]. A cut-off point of 30 for the total AQ score is recommended in an article using the adolescent version of the AQ [5]. The authors proposed that a cut-off of 30 might be considered in future screening studies with adolescents because the percentage of those with autism (especially those with Asperger's syndrome) scoring above a cut-off point of 32 is much lower. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background When screening for autism spectrum disorders (ASD), the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) is generally considered to be useful. Whether the AQ is also a suitable screener for ASD in juveniles with severe behavioral problems (SBPs) is unknown. Due to the overlap of symptoms between ASD and SBPs, particularly in juveniles low on empathy, the screening capacity of the AQ might be constrained. The aim of the present study was to investigate whether (comorbid) SBPs affect the screening capacity of the AQ. The hypothesis is that male juveniles with SBPs - but without a diagnosis of ASD - will score higher than male juveniles without both SBPs and ASD. Method The AQ was completed by 216 male juveniles aged 15–18 years treated at an outpatient department of child and adolescent psychiatry. The 216 participants were categorized into four groups according to a clinical diagnosis of ASD and SBPs (defined as disruptive behavior disorder and/or delinquent behavior). Using multinomial logistic regression, we investigated whether the four identified groups, based on a diagnosis of ASD and SBPs, scored differently for the total score and subscales of the AQ. Results Participants in the group with ASD (ASD⁺) but without SBPs (SBP⁻) were more likely to report higher levels of autistic traits than the reference group without both ASD and SBPs (ASD⁻SBP⁻), except for the subscale on attention to detail (ASD⁺SBP⁻ OR = 1.04; 95%CI = 0.98–1.11). Participants in the group with both ASD and SBPs were more likely to report higher levels for the total AQ score (ASD⁺SBP⁺ OR = 1.03; 95%CI = 1.00–1.05) and the communication subscale of the AQ (ASD⁺SBP⁺ OR = 1.18; 95%CI = 1.07–1.31) than the reference group without both ASD and SBPs. Conclusion In outpatient male juveniles, SBPs do not affect the screening capacity of the AQ for autistic traits. In spite of the well-known overlap of symptoms between ASD and SBPs, male juveniles with SBPs but without a diagnosis of ASD do not score higher on the AQ than male juveniles without SBPs and without a diagnosis of ASD.
... Inspection of participants' records showed that they were all high functioning, held an official ASD diagnosis by clinicians using DSM-5 (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th Edition) criteria (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013), and qualified for a "broad ASD" on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Lord et al., 2000). We corroborated the clinical diagnoses of the participating children and adolescents with the Autism Spectrum Quotient (child (Auyeung et al., 2008) and adolescent (Baron-Cohen et al., 2006) Greek versions) to quantify ASD traits. Any participant who presented comorbid disorders (i.e., attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder, psychiatric illnesses) as well as Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) below 70 was excluded. ...
Article
Full-text available
Developmental executive function (EF) theories have proposed distinct domains of EF (cool and hot) but the EF development in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has been assessed mainly with cool EF measures. Little is known regarding the development of hot EF in ASD. EF is suggested to strongly associate to theory of mind (ToM) in typical development but the correlation between hot EF and ToM is understudied in ASD. The current study was aimed at investigating the cross-sectional developmental changes of cool and hot EF in middle childhood and adolescence and their association to ToM in ASD. Eighty-two children and adolescents (7-16 years) were assessed with measures of cool EF (inhibition, working memory, planning, and cognitive flexibility), hot EF (affective decision-making and delay discounting), and ToM (2nd-order false belief and ignorance; mental state/emotion recognition). Our results showed that the trajectories of all cool EF presented linear age-related improvements in ASD. In terms of hot EF, trajectories demonstrated non-linear functions of age for affec-tive decision-making and delay discounting. Both EF domains were found to associate with ToM over and above age, while hot EF associated with ToM over and above cool EF as well. The investigation of the developmental trajectories of cool and hot EF may help define potential cognitive phenotypes across age within ASD.
... The AQ-J is a brief questionnaire including five subscales (social skills, attention switching, attention to detail, imagination, and communication). Prior work with the AQ-J has been depicted across cultures (32) and ages (33,34). Notably, the AQ is sensitive to the broader autism phenotype. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Social skills training (SST) is used to help individuals with autism spectrum conditions (ASC) better understand the perspectives of others and social interactions, develop empathy skills, and learn how to engage with others socially. However, many individuals with ASC cannot easily sustain high motivation and concentration during such an intervention when it is administered by humans. We developed a social skills training program using multiple humanoid robots (STUH), including an android robot, that aimed to enable individuals with ASC to become familiar with the perspectives of others and improve their sociability and empathy skills. The objective of the present study was to investigate the effectiveness of STUH for these individuals. Methods In STUH, we prepared 50 social exercises that consisted of conversations and behavioral interactions between an android robot and a simple humanoid robot. We prepared another humanoid robot that featured a cartoon-like and mechanical design, which played the role of host. In the first half-session of STUH, participants worked on the exercise from the perspective of an outsider. In the second half-session of STUH, they simulated experience by using robots as their avatars. The intervention associated with STUH was conducted for five days in total. We conducted an analysis of variance (ANOVA) featuring the intervention time point as the independent variable to examine changes in each score on the sociability index items. Results In total, 14 individuals with ASC participated in the study. The results of multiple comparison tests using the Bonferroni method indicated that all sociability index items improved between preintervention and follow-up. Our program enabled the participants to become familiar with the perspectives of others and improve their sociability. Discussion Given the promising results of this study, future studies featuring long-term follow-up should be conducted to draw definitive conclusions about the efficacy of our training system.
... The AQ-J is a short questionnaire containing five subscales (social skills, attention switching, attention to detail, imagination, and communication). Previous studies of the AQ-J have been replicated across cultures (30) and ages (31,32). Notably, the AQ is sensitive to broader autism phenotypes. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Job interviews are a major barrier to employment for individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). During the coronavirus pandemic, establishing online job interview training at home was indispensable. However, many hurdles prevent individuals with ASD from concentrating on online job interview training. To facilitate the acquisition of interview skills from home for individuals with ASD, we developed a group interview training program with a virtual conferencing system (GIT-VICS Program) that uses computer graphics (CG) robots. Methods This study investigated the feasibility of the GIT-VICS Program in facilitating skill acquisition for face-to-face job interviews in pre-post measures. In the GIT-VICS Program, five participants were grouped and played the roles of interviewees (1), interviewers (2), and human resources (2). They alternately practiced each role in GIT-VICS Program sessions conducted over 8 or 9 days over three consecutive weeks. Before and after the GIT-VICS Program, the participants underwent a mock face-to-face job interview with two experienced human interviewers (MFH) to evaluate its effect. Results Fourteen participants completed the trial procedures without experiencing any technological challenges or distress that would have led to the termination of the session. The GIT-VICS Program improved their job interview skills (verbal competence, nonverbal competence, and interview performance). Discussion Given the promising results of this study and to draw clear conclusions about the efficacy of CG robots for mock online job interview training, future studies adding appropriate guidance for manner of job interview by experts are needed.
Article
Full-text available
Transcranial pulse stimulation has been proven effective to improve cognition, memory and depressive symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease, but supporting evidence on other neurological diseases or neuropsychiatric disorders remains limited. This study aimed to investigate the effects of transcranial pulse stimulation on the right temporoparietal junction, which is a key node for social cognition for autism spectrum disorder, and to examine the association between transcranial pulse stimulation and executive and social functions. This double-blinded, randomized, sham-controlled trial included 32 participants (27 males), aged 12–17 years with autism spectrum disorder. All eligible participants were randomized into either the verum or sham transcranial pulse stimulation group, on a 1:1 ratio, based on the Childhood Autism Rating Scale screening score. Sixteen participants received six verum transcranial pulse stimulation sessions (energy level: 0.2–0.25 mJ/mm2; pulse frequency: 2.5–4.0 Hz, 800 pulse/session) in 2 weeks on alternate days. The remaining 16 participants received sham transcranial pulse stimulation. The primary outcome measure included Childhood Autism Rating Scale score changes, evaluated by parents, from baseline to 3-month follow-ups. Secondary outcomes included a self-reported questionnaire responded to by parents and cognitive tests responded to by participants. A licensed mental health professional evaluated clinical global impression severity, improvement, efficacy and total score. Results revealed significant interactions in Childhood Autism Rating Scale and other secondary outcomes. Significant group and time effects were found in most secondary outcomes. Additionally, significant differences were found between the transcranial pulse stimulation and sham transcranial pulse stimulation groups in Childhood Autism Rating Scale and clinical global impression improvement and total score immediately after 2 weeks of transcranial pulse stimulation intervention (all P < 0.05), and effects were sustainable at 1- and 3-month follow-up, compared with baseline. The effect size of Childhood Autism Rating Scale (d = 0.83–0.95) and clinical global impression improvement (d = 4.12–4.37) were large to medium immediately after intervention and sustained at 1-month post-stimulation; however, the effects were reduced to small at 3-month post-stimulation (d = 2.31). These findings indicated that transcranial pulse stimulation over right temporoparietal junction was effective to reduce the core symptoms of autism spectrum disorder, as evidenced by a 24% reduction in the total Childhood Autism Rating Scale score in the verum transcranial pulse stimulation group. Additionally, the clinical global impression total score was reduced by 53.7% in the verum transcranial pulse stimulation group at a 3-month follow-up, compared with the baseline. Participants in the verum transcranial pulse stimulation group had shown substantial improvement at 1- and 3-month follow-ups, compared with baseline, although some of the neuropsychological test results were deemed statistically insignificant. Future replication of this study should include a larger sample derived from multi-nations to determine transcranial pulse stimulation as an alternative top-on treatment option in neuropsychiatry.
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies reported mixed findings on autistic individuals' pitch perception relative to neurotypical (NT) individuals. We investigated whether this may be partly due to individual differences in cognitive abilities by comparing their performance on various pitch perception tasks on a large sample (n = 164) of autistic and NT children and adults. Our findings revealed that: (i) autistic individuals either showed similar or worse performance than NT individuals on the pitch tasks; (ii) cognitive abilities were associated with some pitch task performance; and (iii) cognitive abilities modulated the relationship between autism diagnosis and pitch perception on some tasks. Our findings highlight the importance of taking an individual differences approach to understand the strengths and weaknesses of pitch processing in autism.
Article
Full-text available
Background The main traits of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) are difficulties in social communication and interactions. Autism is frequently related to motor impairments. In the last decades, to develop and promote the social and motor skills of autistic people, several sports training programmes have been designed. In the present systematic narrative review, we aimed at providing an overview of studies evaluating the effectiveness of these sports training programmes to synthesize their strengths and weaknesses. Method To formulate the eligibility criteria for the systematic review, the PICO protocol was used. The PRISMA protocol was applied to four databases for the search strategy. 30 papers were reviewed, and their main features were tabulated and critically discussed. Results Findings highlighted that 8 types of sports were applied to autistic individuals and that the majority of them were focused on two individual sports (i.e., swimming and horseback riding). In sum, findings revealed that sports training programmes provided positive benefits both on motor/social skills as well as on autistic traits (e.g., repetitive behaviours) and individual functioning. The narrative synthesis revealed also that future studies should enroll more autistic females and preschoolers. To encourage the social inclusion of autistic individuals, a group of typically developing peers should be involved in future sports training programmes. Discussion In conclusion, professionals trained on autistic traits and knowledgeable in the target sport are required; consultations with the autistic community as guidance for designing novel sports training programmes are recommended.
Aim: Very preterm (VPT) birth is a known early vulnerability factor, impacting both physical and mental health over the life-span. The additional burden of psychiatric illness in VPT adolescents is likely to adversely affect critical developmental tasks and personal, social and academic/vocational trajectories. Our aim was to examine the magnitude and extent of the risk of psychological burden by determining the prevalence of psychiatric disorders in our prospectively followed-up VPT and full-term (FT) control cohorts, in this period of developmental transition at age 17 years. Methods: Rates of psychiatric disorder in the VPT and FT control cohorts were ascertained at clinical interview of the adolescents and their care giver(s) by an adolescent psychiatrist. Results: VPT birth was associated with a greater risk of generalised anxiety disorder (VPT vs. FT risk ratio (RR) 2.33; 95% confidence interval (CI): 1.16, 4.67, P = 0.02), as well as attentional problems (VPT vs. FT RR 3.46; 95% CI: 1.01, 11.88, P = 0.03). Although care givers of VPT adolescents reported many social and communication difficulties, and observation at clinical interview supported this, our data did not reach clinical threshold for group differences in autistic spectrum disorder. For all other psychiatric disorders, there was no difference between VPT and FT control adolescents. Conclusion: Our longitudinal cohort follow-up study examining the late effects of VPT birth has demonstrated increased rates of clinically significant psychiatric disorder in this period of important developmental transition. Families and health professionals need to be aware of the increased risk so they can monitor for symptoms and seek effective mental health treatments and support.
Article
Background: Sensory processing differences are reported both in children with ADHD and in children with autism. Given the substantial overlap between autism and ADHD, the current study examined which sensory features were uniquely predictive of autistic traits after controlling for ADHD symptoms, age, IQ, and sex in a sample of children and adolescents with autism aged 6-17 years. Methods: The sample included 61 children and adolescents with autism. The Sensory Profile was used to examine Dunn's quadrant model (seeking, sensitivity, avoiding, registration), ADHD symptoms were measured using hyperactivity and attention problems BASC-2 T-scores, and autistic traits were measured using the AQ. Results: After controlling for age, IQ, sex, and ADHD symptoms, Dunn's sensitivity quadrant predicted autistic traits. Conclusions: Findings provide insight into the phenotype of autism and ADHD. Sensory sensitivity may be unique to autism over and above elevated ADHD symptoms that are commonly seen in this population.
Article
Full-text available
Currently there are no brief, self-administered instruments for measuring the degree to which an adult with normal intelligence has the traits associated with the autistic spectrum. In this paper, we report on a new instrument to assess this: the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Individuals score in the range 0–50. Four groups of subjects were assessed: Group 1: 58 adults with Asperger syndrome (AS) or high-functioning autism (HFA); Group 2: 174 randomly selected controls. Group 3: 840 students in Cambridge University; and Group 4: 16 winners of the UK Mathematics Olympiad. The adults with AS/HFA had a mean AQ score of 35.8 (SD = 6.5), significantly higher than Group 2 controls (M = 16.4, SD = 6.3). 80% of the adults with AS/HFA scored 32+, versus 2% of controls. Among the controls, men scored slightly but significantly higher than women. No women scored extremely highly (AQ score 34+) whereas 4% of men did so. Twice as many men (40%) as women (21%) scored at intermediate levels (AQ score 20+). Among the AS/HFA group, male and female scores did not differ significantly. The students in Cambridge University did not differ from the randomly selected control group, but scientists (including mathematicians) scored significantly higher than both humanities and social sciences students, confirming an earlier study that autistic conditions are associated with scientific skills. Within the sciences, mathematicians scored highest. This was replicated in Group 4, the Mathematics Olympiad winners scoring significantly higher than the male Cambridge humanities students. 6% of the student sample scored 327plus; on the AQ. On interview, 11 out of 11 of these met three or more DSM-IV criteria for AS/HFA, and all were studying sciences/mathematics, and 7 of the 11 met threshold on these criteria. Test—retest and interrater reliability of the AQ was good. The AQ is thus a valuable instrument for rapidly quantifying where any given individual is situated on the continuum from autism to normality. Its potential for screening for autism spectrum conditions in adults of normal intelligence remains to be fully explored.
Article
Empathy is an essential part of normal social functioning, yet there are precious few instruments for measuring individual differences in this domain. In this article we review psychological theories of empathy and its measurement. Previous instruments that purport to measure this have not always focused purely on empathy. We report a new self-report questionnaire, the Empathy Quotient (EQ), for use with adults of normal intelligence. It contains 40 empathy items and 20 filler/control items. On each empathy item a person can score 2, 1, or 0, so the EQ has a maximum score of 80 and a minimum of zero. In Study 1 we employed the EQ with n = 90 adults (65 males, 25 females) with Asperger Syndrome (AS) or high-functioning autism (HFA), who are reported clinically to have difficulties in empathy. The adults with AS/HFA scored significantly lower on the EQ than n = 90 (65 males, 25 females) age-matched controls. Of the adults with AS/HFA, 81% scored equal to or fewer than 30 points out of 80, compared with only 12% of controls. In Study 2 we carried out a study of n = 197 adults from a general population, to test for previously reported sex differences (female superiority) in empathy. This confirmed that women scored significantly higher than men. The EQ reveals both a sex difference in empathy in the general population and an empathy deficit in AS/HFA.
Wien, Med. F., Hab.-Schr., 1943 (Nicht f. d. Austausch).
Article
SYNOPSIS The clinical features, course, aetiology, epidemiology, differential diagnosis and management of Asperger's syndrome are described. Classification is discussed and reasons are given for including the syndrome, together with early childhood autism, in a wider group of conditions which have, in common, impairment of development of social interaction, communication and imagination.
Article
The authors investigated the validity of Asperger Syndrome (AS) by comparing the neuropsychological profiles in this condition and Higher-Functioning Autism (HFA). Diagnostic assignment followed a stringent procedure based on ICD-10 research criteria for the two disorders. The groups had comparable age and Full Scale IQ distributions. The groups differed significantly in 11 neuropsychological areas. The profile obtained for individuals with AS coincided closely with a cluster of neuropsychological assets and deficits captured by the term nonverbal learning disabilities, suggesting an empirical distinction from HFA.