ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

The fine motor abilities of children who participated in two years of piano instruction and those who had never received formal music training were compared before and after the instruction. A significant improvement in fine motor skills was found only for the children who received the lessons, and a significant difference in the speed of response was found between the two groups at the end of the two years of instruction. The innumerable opportunities to assess, refine, and time their motor responses to specific stimuli during musical practice and the availability of constant evaluative feedback (i.e., sound) may allow musicians to improve the accuracy and speed of perceiving and responding to relevant stimuli.
262
Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1060: 262–264 (2005). © 2005 New York Academy of Sciences.
doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.053
Does Music Instruction Improve
Fine Motor Abilities?
EUGENIA COSTA-GIOMI
University of Texas–Austin, Center for Music Learning, Austin, Texas 78712-0435, USA
ABSTRACT: The fine motor abilities of children who participated in two years
of piano instruction and those who had never received formal music training
were compared before and after the instruction. A significant improvement in
fine motor skills was found only for the children who received the lessons, and
a significant difference in the speed of response was found between the two
groups at the end of the two years of instruction. The innumerable opportuni-
ties to assess, refine, and time their motor responses to specific stimuli during
musical practice and the availability of constant evaluative feedback (i.e.,
sound) may allow musicians to improve the accuracy and speed of perceiving
and responding to relevant stimuli.
KEYWORDS: children; motor response; music instruction; music education;
music practice
INTRODUCTION
Musicians outperform nonmusicians in certain perception tasks that require an
accurate and immediate motor response to a visual stimulus.1 These results have
been taken as evidence that music training increases the speed and accuracy of the
visual–motor association. Similarly, the anatomical differences in the sensorimotor
cortex found between musicians and nonmusicians suggest that extensive music
practice affects the organization of this cortical area.2–5 Studies that focused on the
cortical representation of hand fingers during intensive keyboard practice sessions
have indeed shown clear changes over periods as short as five days and as long as
two weeks.5 No longitudinal research exists on the neurological changes that occur
over longer periods of musical practice.
Although a few studies conducted with young children suggest that early music
instruction improves performance in visual–motor tasks,6,7 others showed no signif-
icant fine motor skills improvements after five months of violin instruction.8 The ef-
fects of instrumental practice on fine motor skills in children have not been studied
systematically despite the strong belief among parents and educators that learning to
play an instrument improves dexterity and motor control.
Address for correspondence: Eugenia Costa-Giomi, University of Texas–Austin, Center for
Music Learning, 1 University Station E3100, Austin, TX 78712-0435. Voice: 512-471-2495.
costagiomi@mail.utexas.edu
263COSTA-GIOMI: MUSIC INSTRUCTION AND FINE MOTOR ABILITIES
METHOD
Children (n = 117) attending 16 public schools who had no formal music instruc-
tion, no piano at home, and family annual incomes below $30,000 were randomly
assigned to either the experimental group receiving two years of individual piano
lessons weekly or the control group receiving no formal music instruction. Children
in the experimental group received acoustic pianos and practiced, as an average, up
to 3.5 hours weekly.9 The cognitive abilities, academic achievement, musical abili-
ties, self-esteem, and motor proficiency of the two groups were comparable at the
start of the project as determined by standardized tests.10
Fifty-one children in the experimental group and 39 children in the control group
completed the fine motor components of the Bruinsky-Oseretsky Motor Proficiency
Test before and after the two years of instruction. The total scores of the fine motor
test and the scores in subtest 6: Response Speed, subtest 7: Visual-Motor Control,
and subtest 8: Upper-Limb Speed and Dexterity were analyzed through ANOVAs
with repeated measures.
RESULTS
Significant group (piano/non-piano group) × instruction (pre/posttest, repeated
measures) interactions were found for the total fine motor scores and the response
speed subtest scores F(1,88) = 4.01 P < .05 and F(1,88) = 13.61 P < .001, respec-
tively. The fine motor skills of the piano group improved significantly more during
the two years of the project than did those of the control group, and the differences
in Subtest 6 scores between the two groups of children were significant only after
the two years of piano instruction. These results suggest that the improvement in mo-
tor proficiency was mainly caused by differential scores in the speed subtest which
required children to react quickly to catch a rod that was sliding down against a wall.
Scores in tasks that measured eye–hand coordination and dexterity were not affected
by the lessons.
CONCLUSIONS
Music performance requires accurate and quick motor reaction to visual, aural,
and kinesthetic stimuli. When performers practice their instruments, they receive
immediate and consistent aural feedback about their motor response to such stimuli.
The innumerable opportunities to assess, refine, and time their motor responses to
specific stimuli during musical practice and the availability of constant evaluative
feedback (i.e., sound) may allow musicians to improve the accuracy and speed in
perceiving the stimuli and responding to it.
[Competing interests: The authors declare that they have no competing financial
interests.]
264 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES
REFERENCES
1. BROCHARD, R. et al. 2004. Effect of musical expertise on visuospatial abilities: evi-
dence from reaction times and mental imagery. Brain Cogn. 54: 103–109.
2. ELBERT, T., C. PANTEV, C. WIENDBRUCH, et al. 1995. Increased cortical representation
of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science 270: 305–307.
3. JÄNCKE, L., N.J. SHAH & M. PETERS. 2000. Cortical activations in primary and second-
ary motor areas for complex bimanual movements in professional pianists. Brain
Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 10: 177–183.
4. PANTEV, C., A. ENGELIEN, V. CANDIA & T. ELBERT. 2001. Representational cortex in
musicians. Plastic alterations in response to musical practice. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.
930: 300–314.
5. PASCUAL-LEONE, A. 2001. The brain that plays music and is changed by it. Ann. N. Y.
Acad. Sci 930: 315–332.
6. BROWN, J. et al. 1981. Effects of an integrated physical education/music program in
changing early childhood perceptual–motor performance. Percept. Mot. Skills 53:
151–154.
7. ORSMOND, G.I. & L.K. MILLER. 1999. Cognitive, musical, and environmental correlates
of early music instruction. Psychol. Mus. 27: 18–37.
8. ESSMA, S. 2005. Does music instruction increase children’s fine motor abilities?
University of Texas Austin. Unpublished manuscript.
9. COSTA-GIOMI, E. 2005. “I do not want to study piano!” Early predictors of student
dropout behavior. Bull. Coun. Res. Music Educ. 161/162: 57–64.
10. COSTA-GIOMI, E. 1999. The effects of three years of piano instruction on children’s
cognitive development. J. Res. Music Educ. 47: 198–212.
... It is also associated with enhanced motor skills, such as the movements of the fingers, hands, and arms [32][33][34][35][36] . Several longitudinal studies have corroborated the causal nature of this relationship for rhythm 37,38,39 , as well as for motor skills 38,40,41,42 . Concordant with behavioral findings, cross-sectional neuroimaging studies have shown that musicians differ from non-musicians in the brain organization of the auditory-motor system 33,43-51; for a review, see 25,52 . ...
... In this study, we focus on the link between rhythm and motor processes. Rhythm and motor processes have been extensively examined in relation to music experience and cognition, typically regarding differences in performance between musicians and non-musicians 26,30,33 and training-related improvements in one or the other 38,40,42 . Less is known about the rhythm-motor interplay during music training, namely its neural underpinnings and its potential impact on training-related effects. ...
... At a purely behavioral level, we found that children who had music training improved their rhythm and motor skills more than those in the sports and control groups. Considering that rhythm and motor processes are directly implicated in music performance, these findings are hardly unexpected, and indeed they match extant evidence on the benefits of music training for rhythm skills 38,39 and motor performance 38,40 . Importantly, a better ability to discriminate rhythm before training was associated with greater improvement in the motor performance of the preferred hand and both hands in the music group, but not in sports or control groups. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Rhythm and motor function are intrinsically linked to each other and to music, but the motor-rhythm interplay during music training, and the corresponding brain mechanisms, are underexplored. In a longitudinal training study with children, we inspected the role of rhythm skills in the fine motor improvements arising from music training, and which brain regions were implicated. Fifty-seven 8-year-olds were assigned to a 6-month music training (n = 21), or sports training (n = 18), or a control group (n = 18). They performed motor and rhythm tasks, and underwent structural brain scans before and after training. Better ability to perceive rhythm before training was associated with less gray matter volume in regions of the cerebellum, fusiform gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, ventral diencephalon, inferior/middle temporal gyri, superior frontal gyrus, and motor area. Motor performance improved with music training, greater improvements with better pre-training rhythm discrimination. Music training induced a loss of gray matter volume in the left cerebellum and fusiform gyrus, and volume loss correlated with greater motor gains. No such effects were found in the sports and control groups. So, children with fine-tuned rhythm perception were more prone to fine motor improvements through music training, and this motor-rhythm link was handled by the left cerebellum and fusiform gyrus. These findings have implications for models on plasticity and music/rhythm cognition, and for programs targeting motor function.
... Executive functions are defined as higher-level cognitive functions that are closely related to complex purposeful behaviors in all areas of life (Fogel et al., 2021). Recently, studies on neuroimaging have shown a reduction in the thickness of the right medial orbitofrontal cortex and the alteration of brain activation patterns across functional networks including prefrontal, parietal, and cerebellar regions in DCD children (Costa-Giomi, 2005;Geuze et al. 2001;Lazăr, 2018;Wilson et al. 2017). Moreover, it is proved that DCD children are facing problems in the reticular activating system, basal ganglia and frontal and prefrontal cortices that play a crucial role in executive functions (Sartori et al. 2020). ...
... Performing actions in harmony with music requires correct and quick responses to visual, auditory and movement stimuli. When producing sound with musical instruments, a person immediately receives continuous auditory feedback from his movements; therefore, he can improve the speed of responding to auditory stimuli or his performance quality (Costa-Giomi, 2005;Da Silva Borges et al. 2012;Diamond, 2000;2013), and this leads to the development of excellent cognitive abilities, especially attention and response inhibition. The findings of this research show that performing movements with music stimulates different areas of the brain in both hemispheres related to motor aspects and cognitive functions like planning and attention. ...
Article
Full-text available
This research aims to study the effect of Bodypercussion exercises on balance and executive functions of children with DCD. Thirty children with DCD between seven and nine years of age were selected from girls' elementary schools. The subjects were randomly divided into two groups: experimental and control (15 subjects in each group). For eight weeks, the experimental group participated in group BAPNE neuromotor sessions. Data were analyzed with descriptive statistics and mixed ANOVA test with repeated measures and Bonferroni's post hoc test. The results showed that the result of BAPNE body percussion exercises on static balance (P=0.001), dynamic balance (P=0.001), sustained attention (P=0.001) and response inhibition (P=0.001) is significant and participants in the experimental group performed better at post-test (P=0.001) and follow-up (P=0.001) than the control group.
... In this study, we focus on the link between rhythm, motor processes, and music. Rhythm and motor processes have been extensively examined in relation to music experience and cognition, typically regarding differences between musicians and non-musicians 24,28,31 and training-related improvements in one or the other 36,38,40 . Less is known about the rhythm-motor interplay during music training proper, its neural underpinning, and impact on training-related effects. ...
... At the behavioral level, we found that children who had music training improved their rhythm and motor skills more than those in the sports and control groups. Considering that rhythm and motor processes are directly implicated in music performance, these findings are hardly unexpected, and indeed they match extant evidence on the benefits of music training for rhythm skills 36,37 and motor performance 36,38 . Importantly, better ability to discriminate rhythm before training was associated with greater improvement in motor performance with the preferred hand and with both hands in the music group, but not in sports or control groups. ...
Article
Full-text available
Rhythm and motor function are intrinsically linked to each other and to music, but the rhythm-motor interplay during music training, and the corresponding brain mechanisms, are underexplored. In a longitudinal training study with children, we examined the role of rhythm predisposition in the fine motor improvements arising from music training, and which brain regions would be implicated. Fifty-seven 8-year-olds were assigned to either a 6-month music training (n = 21), sports training (n = 18), or a control group (n = 18). They performed rhythm and motor tasks, and structural brain scans before and after training were collected. Better ability to perceive rhythm before training was related to less gray matter volume in regions of the cerebellum, fusiform gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, ventral diencephalon, amygdala, and inferior/middle temporal gyri. Music training improved motor performance, and greater improvements correlated with better pre-training rhythm discrimination. Music training also induced a loss of gray matter volume in the left cerebellum and fusiform gyrus, and volume loss correlated with higher motor gains. No such effects were found in the sports and control groups. In summary, children with finer-tuned rhythm perception abilities were prone to finer motor improvements through music training, and this rhythm-motor link was to some extent subserved by the left cerebellum and fusiform gyrus. These findings have implications for models on music-related plasticity and rhythm cognition, and for programs targeting motor function.
... Accordingly, it could interrupt negative developmental cascades and promote a positive course of life in aging people. In fact, numerous experiments showed enhancement in attention, processing speed and working memory (Bugos, 2010;Bugos, 2019;Bugos et al., 2007;Degé & Kerkovius, 2018;Frischen et al., 2021;Roden, Könen, et al., 2014;Schellenberg, 2004), as well as improvements in children (Costa-Giomi, 2005;Forgeard et al., 2008;Hyde et al., 2009;James, Zuber, et al., 2020;Martins et al., 2018) and older adults' fine motor abilities following musical training (Bugos, 2019). But in general, research in this area has yielded conflicting results (e.g., Seinfeld et al., 2013;Steele, 2005), including metaanalyses examining music-related far transfer effects on non-musical cognitive functions with unconvincing results (Sala & Gobet, 2017. ...
Article
Full-text available
Musical training can improve fine motor skills, cognitive abilities and induce macrostructural brain changes. However, it is not clear whether the changes in motor skills occur simultaneously with changes in cognitive and neurophysiological parameters. In this study, 156 healthy, musically naïve, and right-handed older adults were recruited and randomly assigned to a piano training or a music listening group. Before, after six and twelve months participants were scanned using MRI and assessed for fine motor skills, auditory working memory and processing speed. A Bayesian multilevel modeling approach was used to examine behavioral and neurophysiological group differences. The relationships between motor and cognitive and between motor and neurophysiological parameters were determined using latent change score models. Compared to music listening, practicing piano resulted in greater improvement in fine motor skills and probably working memory. Only in the piano group, unimanual fine motor skills and gray matter volume of the contralateral M1 changed together during the 6-12-month period. Additionally, M1 co-developed with ipsilateral putamen and thalamus. Playing piano induced more prevalent coupling between the motor and cognitive domain. However, there is little evidence that fine motor control develops concurrently with cognitive functions. Playing an instrument promotes motor, cognitive and neural development into older age. During the learning process, the consolidation of piano skills appears to take place in sensorimotor networks, enabling musicians to perform untrained motor tasks with higher acuity. Relationships between the development of motor acuity and cognition were bidirectional and can be explained by a common cause as well as by shared resources with compensatory mechanisms.
... Fine motor skills include the use of small muscles -hands, fingers and wrists -in synchronisation with the eyes. Learning a musical instrument inevitably gets a child regularly practising fine motor coordination and dexterity for performing complex patterns (Costa-Giomi, 2005;Schlaug et al., 2005). Indeed, evidence suggests that children who learn to play a musical instrument have better fine motor skills than children who are not musically trained Schlaug et al., 2005). ...
Chapter
Musical care has multiple roles in the nurturing of a child. Music can have a powerful influence on children’s emotions, behaviour, and cognition. The chapter focuses on the ages 5 to 12 years: a period of childhood that typically encompasses rapid and vast developmental change. Drawing together research and clinical perspectives from the authors’ disciplines of music psychology, music education, and music therapy, the chapter frames itself around the three main areas of childhood development: cognitive, physiological/physical, and social-emotional. Research on the cognitive outcomes of musical care for children range from improved spatial abilities and IQ to enhanced memory and verbal intelligence. The physiological outcomes include pain reduction, relaxation, and reduced secretion of the stress hormone cortisol. The physical outcomes include improved gross and fine motor skills, temporal coordination, strength, and greater ability to synchronize with others. Social-emotional outcomes ranges from changes in intrapersonal capacities such as self-esteem, confidence, emotional regulation, and stability to changes in interpersonal capacities, including empathy, social relationships and bonding, shared attention, and communication skills. The chapter discusses the various methodologies used in research studies on music and children and suggests ways towards interdisciplinary collaboration. Then further research directions for musical care in childhood are presented. In conclusion, the authors argue that music is an act of caregiving that can influence the quality of a child’s development and therefore offers one way to nurture children’s cognitive, physical, and social-emotional capacities in preparation for the next stage of life.
... Music programs have been studied with a view to them potentially enhancing EF (Bowmer, Mason, Knight, & Welch, 2018;Habibi, Damasio, Ilari, Elliott Sachs, & Damasio, 2018;Moreno et al., 2011;Williams, 2018). Correlational and intervention studies of hearing children undergoing music training consistently show that they perform better in fine-motor skill, rhythm perception, and auditory discrimination (Besson, Schön, Moreno, Santos, & Magne, 2007;Costa-Giomi, 2005;Slater et al., 2015). The playing of music is highly complex and includes repetition and practice, and strong motivational and emotional rewards (Diamond, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Several studies have reported poor executive function (EF) development in deaf children with subsequent impacts on their social and academic attainment. This paper describes the results of a music-based EF intervention designed for deaf children and carried out in two sets of primary schools. This is the first classroom-based EF training study with deaf children, and it also incorporates a replication phase. The intervention was a within-subject crossover design, with 29 deaf children aged 7-11 years who participated in both an EF and an art class control activity, each lasting 10 hours over 5 weeks. Non-verbal EF skills were assessed at pre-test, the crossover point, and post-test. Findings indicated that the EF intervention led to an improvement in participants' working memory and inhibitory skills in comparison with their performance on the same tasks after the control activity. The findings were not uniform for all EFs targeted nor for all cognitive ability levels in the sample. We discuss the implications of our findings for deaf children with different ability levels and for how EF interventions can be further improved.
Article
Purpose This research seeks to extend previous research into student discipline and competitiveness, at the macro or national level, to the micro or individual level. The role of factors with the potential to impact individual competitiveness, namely the expectations of the mother and the father, the importance of school discipline played in primary and secondary schools, and past participation in sport and music were examined. Design/methodology/approach Data from a sample of current university students and recent graduates representing Western (the United States) and Asian regions (South Korea and China) were analysed using multiple regressions to establish the explanatory power of independent variables in the competitiveness model, including testing for moderation effects of country of birth. Results The study found that personal discipline is significantly associated with individual competitiveness. The importance placed on discipline in primary schools was found to predict individual competitiveness as were parents’ expectations, yet with nuances in terms of maternal and paternal expectations. Surprisingly, neither participation in music nor discipline at secondary school was found to significantly associate with individual competitiveness. At the same time, the study did find moderating effects of country of birth and the number of years students played sport in predicting competitiveness. Originality/value Guided by the Ecological Systems Theory and the notion of the Pygmalion effect, we develop a framework of factors that shape an individual's competitiveness. The results make several theoretical contributions by establishing new drivers of individual competitiveness, and as such illuminating the importance of discipline during schooling and how parental expectations drive performance. Implications for employers, educational institutions, and parents are outlined and directions for further research are offered.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Autism is a neurodevelopment disorder that usually appears in early years and its main characteristics are repeated problems in social interaction, abnormal verbal and nonverbal communication, and stereotyped patterns of behavior and interests. Two of the therapies that are currently being discussed are music therapy and play therapy. Music with play is one of the skills necessary for communication in children with autism who do not use language to communicate. As a result the experience of music and play and the interaction between them ends in improving communication which is one of the main problems in these children. Through the process of integrating music and plays, patients will move to reach some predetermined goals by the help of some specialists who are trained in such courses. These goals may include improving motor, social and interpersonal development, reduced stereotypical behaviors, achieving self-awareness, but they are not limited to these. Conclusion: In this article, the importance of music education and play and the effect of training and playing music in children with autism is discussed. It is concluded that music therapy should be useful in conjunction with play therapy
Article
Résumé Les effets de la pratique et de l’écoute musicale sur le fonctionnement cognitif et cérébral ont été grandement étudiés depuis les 20 dernières années. Les études comportementales et de neuroimagerie ont mis en évidence une importante réactivité du cerveau aux stimulations musicales et de nombreux chevauchements anatomiques et fonctionnels entre les traitements musicaux et des compétences non musicales (linguistiques, motrices et émotionnelles notamment). Parallèlement à ces travaux, d’autres études ont cherché à montrer et à expliquer des effets bénéfiques de transfert de la pratique ou de l’écoute musicale sur la cognition et la motricité, chez des sujets sains comme dans des populations pathologiques. Cette revue de littérature vise à intégrer l’ensemble de ces données pour mieux comprendre pourquoi la musique constitue un outil de stimulation cognitive avantageux et comment elle peut être utilisée dans des perspectives thérapeutiques novatrices, tout particulièrement dans le champ de la neuropsychologie cognitive.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to identify early predictors of drop out behavior in children engaged in piano instruction. Fourth-grade children (33 boys, 34 girls) were provided, at no cost to their families, with three years of weekly private piano lessons and an acoustic piano. Twenty-four children dropped out of the lessons during the first two years of the project. Prior to the start of the lessons, children completed tests and questionnaires and, throughout the project, piano teachers completed weekly progress reports about the attendance, piano homework, and practice routines of the children. There were no differences in cognitive abilities, musical abilities, motor proficiency, self-esteem and demographic factors (i.e., sex, family income, parental employment, parental education, and family structure) between the children who dropped out and those who completed three years of lessons. Dropouts were less likely to have siblings, missed more lessons, practiced less, and completed less piano homework than did their peers. Piano teachers reported that the dropouts had achieved less in the first six weeks of lessons than had the other children. These findings suggest that the clearest indications that a student is likely to dropout of piano lessons are lowered motivation and diminished achievement.
Article
Full-text available
The relationship between music and cognitive abilities was studied by observing the cognitive development of children provided (n = 63) and not provided (n = 54) with individual piano lessons from fourth to sixth grade. There were no differences in cognitive abilities, musical abilities, motor proficiency, self-esteem, academic achievement, or interest in studying piano between the two groups of children at the beginning of the study. It was found that the treatment affected children's general and spatial cognitive development. The magnitude of such effects (omega squared) was small. Additional analyses showed that although the experimental group obtained higher spatial abilities scores in the Developing Cognitive Abilities Test after 1 and 2 years of instruction than did the control group, the groups did not differ in general or specific cognitive abilities after 3 years of instruction. The treatment did not affect the development of quantitative and verbal cognitive abilities.
Article
Full-text available
Magnetic source imaging revealed that the cortical representation of the digits of the left hand of string players was larger than that in controls. The effect was smallest for the left thumb, and no such differences were observed for the representations of the right hand digits. The amount of cortical reorganization in the representation of the fingering digits was correlated with the age at which the person had begun to play. These results suggest that the representation of different parts of the body in the primary somatosensory cortex of humans depends on use and changes to conform to the current needs and experiences of the individual.
Article
This study investigated the effects of early music instruction on music perception skills as well as specific areas of cognitive development (non-verbal/spatial abilities). Participants were 29 children enrolled in beginning music instruction, and 29 children matched in age and gender attending preschool, without formal music lessons. The children were assessed on a music perception task (melody recognition) and a variety of cognitive measures (receptive vocabulary, visual-motor integration, an embedded figures task, and puzzles), prior to beginning music instruction and four months later. The results indicated that preschool children taking music lessons may be a select group; they were more likely to have previous musical experience and additional extra-curricular activities. After four months of music lessons, children in the music group showed the greatest improvement on the music task. A transfer effect was observed primarily for visual-motor skills, with the music group improving significantly on the visual-motor integration task. The findings are discussed as relevant to previous research. Implications for music education are also discussed. © 1999 by the Society for Research in Psychology of Music and Music Education.
Article
The lifelong ability to adapt to environmental needs is based on the capacity of the central nervous system for plastic alterations. In a series of neurophysiological experiments, we studied the impact of music and musical training in musicians on the specific functional organization in auditory and somatosensory representational cortex. In one such study, subjects listened to music from which one specific spectral frequency was removed. This led to rapid and reversible adaptation of neuronal responses in auditory cortex. Further experimental evidence demonstrated that long years of practice and training by professional musicians to enable them to reach their capacity is associated with enlarged cortical representations in the somatosensory and auditory domains. This tuning of neuronal representations was specifically observed for musical tones and was absent when pure sinusoidal tones were used as stimuli. In the somatosensory cortex, plastic changes proved to be specific for the fingers frequently used and stimulated. These changes were not detected in the fingers of the hand that were not involved in playing the particular instrument. Neuroplastic alterations also may be driven into a domain where they may become maladaptive. The clinical syndrome of focal hand dystonia that may occur in musicians who engage in forceful practice may be one such consequence. We will discuss the possibilities of reversing maladaptive responses leading to the successful treatment of focal hand dystonia, which relies on basic research about cortical reorganization. This example elucidates how neuroscientific progress can guide the development of practice guidelines and therapeutic measures for the benefit of professional musicians.
Article
Playing a musical instrument demands extensive procedural and motor learning that results in plastic reorganization of the human brain. These plastic changes seem to include the rapid unmasking of existing connections and the establishment of new ones. Therefore, both functional and structural changes take place in the brain of instrumentalists as they learn to cope with the demands of their activity. Neuroimaging techniques allow documentation of these plastic changes in the human brain. These plastic changes are fundamental to the accomplishment of skillful playing, but they pose a risk for the development of motor control dysfunctions that may give rise to overuse syndromes and focal, task-specific dystonia.
Article
Two approaches to facilitating perceptual-motor development in children, ages 4 to 6 yr., were investigated. The experimental group (n = 15) received 24 sessions of integrated physical education/music instruction based upon concepts of Kodaly and Dalcroze. The control group (n = 15) received 24 sessions of movement exploration and self-testing instruction. Analysis of covariance indicated that significant improvement occurred only in the experimental group, with discharges changes in the motor, auditory, and language aspects of perceptual-motor performance as well as total score.
Article
Hemodynamic responses were measured applying functional magnetic resonance imaging in two professional piano players and two carefully matched non-musician control subjects during the performance of self-paced bimanual and unimanual tapping tasks. The bimanual tasks were chosen because they resemble typical movements pianists have to generate during piano exercises. The results showed that the primary and secondary motor areas (M1, SMA, pre-SMA, and CMA) were considerably activated to a much lesser degree in professional pianists than in non-musicians. This difference was strongest for the pre-SMA and CMA, where professional pianists showed very little activation. The results suggest that the long lasting extensive hand skill training of the pianists leads to greater efficiency which is reflected in a smaller number of active neurons needed to perform given finger movements. This in turn enlarges the possible control capacity for a wide range of movements because more movements, or more 'degrees of freedom', are controllable.
Article
The lifelong ability to adapt to environmental needs is based on the capacity of the central nervous system for plastic alterations. In a series of neurophysiological experiments, we studied the impact of music and musical training in musicians on the specific functional organization in auditory and somatosensory representational cortex. In one such study, subjects listened to music from which one specific spectral frequency was removed. This led to rapid and reversible adaptation of neuronal responses in auditory cortex. Further experimental evidence demonstrated that long years of practice and training by professional musicians to enable them to reach their capacity is associated with enlarged cortical representations in the somatosensory and auditory domains. This tuning of neuronal representations was specifically observed for musical tones and was absent when pure sinusoidal tones were used as stimuli. In the somatosensory cortex, plastic changes proved to be specific for the fingers frequently used and stimulated. These changes were not detected in the fingers of the hand that were not involved in playing the particular instrument. Neuroplastic alterations also may be driven into a domain where they may become maladaptive. The clinical syndrome of focal hand dystonia that may occur in musicians who engage in forceful practice may be one such consequence. We will discuss the possibilities of reversing maladaptive responses leading to the successful treatment of focal hand dystonia, which relies on basic research about cortical reorganization. This example elucidates how neuroscientific progress can guide the development of practice guidelines and therapeutic measures for the benefit of professional musicians.