ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

This review summarizes the use of hypnotic inductions while the subject is physically active, open-eyed and focused on the external environment. Research cited from several sources documents that traditional and alert inductions produce similar hypnotic susceptibility scores, but after an alert induction, subjects may report feeling more alert and in control. A case is reported of a client who was able to use such an induction to stay in alert hypnosis for an extended time, and reduce the long-standing anxiety effects of past failure. Finally, a systematic way is discussed to generate inductions that may expand the use of hypnosis to new applications.
291
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis Copyright 2006 by the American Society of Clinical Hypnosis
48:4, April 2006
Alert Hypnosis: A Review and Case Report
David M. Wark
University of Minnesota
Abstract
This review summarizes the use of hypnotic inductions while the subject is
physically active, open-eyed and focused on the external environment. Research
cited from several sources documents that traditional and alert inductions produce
similar hypnotic susceptibility scores, but after an alert induction, subjects may
report feeling more alert and in control. A case is reported of a client who was
able to use such an induction to stay in alert hypnosis for an extended time, and
reduce the long-standing anxiety effects of past failure. Finally, a systematic way
is discussed to generate inductions that may expand the use of hypnosis to new
applications.
Key words: Alert hypnosis, active hypnosis, hypnotic inductions, test
anxiety
Address correspondences and reprint requests to:
David M. Wark, Ph.D.
1313 5
th
St. SE.
Minneapolis MN, 55414
Email: wark@umn.edu
292
Alert Hypnosis Review
Alert Hypnosis: A Review and Case Report
This paper reviews a development in the understanding of hypnosis. A method of
hypnotic induction is showing up in the literature, referred to by several different titles,
usually including the term “alert” as in “hyperalert hypnosis” (Ludwig & Lyle, 1964), “active-
alert hypnosis” (Bányai & Hilgard, 1976; Robazza & Bortoli, 1995; Cardeña, E., Alarcon,
A., Capafons, A., & Bayot, A., 1998), “alert hypnosis” (Wark, 1998), “awake-alert” (Iglesias
& Iglesias, 2005) or “waking hypnosis” (Capafons, 2004). Regardless of the designation,
the technique is distinct in two significant respects. First, a subject does it with open eyes.
The operator does not use phrases like “You are getting more and more sleepy and drowsy.
Your eyelids are becoming heavier, more and more tired and heavy.” as found in classic
inductions such as the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scales (Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard,
1959). Second, the subject can be moderately to vigorously active. There is no need for
catalepsy or even relaxation. (Ludwig & Lyle, 1964; Bányai, É., Meszaros, I., & Greguss,
A.C., 1983). In spite of these differences, the response to suggestions is practically the same
as for more traditional techniques (Wark, 1998).
This review makes a distinction between the suggestions used for induction and
those used for treatment. Of course, the distinction is purely heuristic. Inductions are
suggestions formalized into an arbitrary procedure or ritual. But it is helpful to make a
distinction between the suggestions used early for the induction of hypnosis and the
suggestions used later for treatment or change.
Applications of Alert or “Waking” Hypnosis
The earliest contemporary report of “waking” hypnosis was an educational
application. In the early 20th century, hypnosis was popularly considered to be a type of
sleep, in part a carry over from the historical writings of Braid (1843) and in part agreement
with the more contemporary work of Pavlov, who considered hypnosis a type of cerebral
inhibition (Windholz, 1996). Wells (1924) was interested in demonstrating hypnosis to his
classes in psychology, and countering the false idea that it was a form of sleep.
Wells used a technique he called “waking hypnosis”. He wrote that waking hypnosis
was less mysterious, took less time, was easier to use and succeeded with more students.
Instead of sleep or drowsiness, his preinduction talk covered the concept of normal
dissociation of mind and body, while awake. His examples included lapses of memory, and
absent minded acts. Following the lecture he gave a direct suggestion for eye closure or
finger lock. After that, he gave more suggestions, depending on how the subject responded,
and what he wanted to demonstrate. He reported effective awake suggestions for hypnotic
phenomena such as amnesia, automatic writing, recalling forgotten information, complex
mental computation and anesthesia. In every case, Wells said, the subject was awake and
alert. Wells’ evidence for the application was anecdotal, but his enthusiasm for waking
hypnosis was very strong.
A major demonstration of highly active hypnosis developed from an observation in
social science. Ludwig and Lyle (1964) introduced the concept of “hyperalert” hypnosis to
describe people who seemed to be following suggestions in a hypnotized way, while physically
very active. As examples of this “natural hypnosis” they cited tribal dances, religious revivals
and political rallies. To demonstrate the process experimentally, they used 9 prison inmates
who were told they were participating in a new treatment program. The investigators use a
293
Wark
variety of suggestions to increase tension, alertness and stimulation
while diminishing comfort and concentration. For one example, subjects were told to spin
around while sweeping the room with their eyes. They were told that they would become so
dizzy they would be forced to sit down and enter a very tense hyperalert state. After 5 to 25
minutes of this and other active tension inductions, subjects appeared to be awake, but their
faces had a flat “trance-like” quality. The subjects’ responses to 11 standard hypnotic challenge
items (arm rigidity, hand anesthesia, negative hallucination, posttrance amnesia, etc) were
tested after both the active and traditional eye closure and relaxation inductions. The
investigators found no significant differences between response to suggestions following
hyperalert or traditional inductions (Ludwig & Lyle, 1964).
If alert hypnosis was ever to be useful, Vingoe (1968) realized that there should be
a measure of hypnotic response using the eyes open induction. He developed a Group Alert
Trance (GAT) hypnosis scale. For induction, the subject gazes fixedly at the left hand, while
receiving suggestions for physical relaxation and mental alertness and readiness to carry out
any suggestion. The patter is similar to the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale: Form B
(SHSS:B) (Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1959), but with the suggestions for sleep or drowsiness
removed, and instructions for mental alertness inserted. Following the induction, the subject
receives suggestions for five tasks [head fall, alert state, communications inhibition, post
hypnotic suggestion and posthypnotic amnesia]. The results for these test items on which
they can be compared are no different from the percent passing on the Harvard Group Scale
of Hypnotic Susceptibility: Form A (HGSHS:A) by Shor and Orne (1962). In a later study,
Vingoe (1973) compared 186 undergraduates on the HGSHS:A and the GAT. The correlation
was r = .68. The author suggests that researchers and clinicians can use different inductions
depending on whether an arousal increase (alert induction) or decrease (traditional induction)
is more appropriate.
The early reports of alert hypnosis may have stimulated some new ways to think
about hypnosis. Working from a sociocognitive perspective, Gibbons (1979) theorized that
hypnosis happens when a subject is given some expectations for a change in awareness, the
operator specifies the changes, and provides a cue for the change to happen. From that
perspective, suggestions of sleep would lead to counter productive reduction of awareness.
Gibbons developed his hyperempiria induction with suggestions for increased alertness,
exhilaration, mind expansion, and increased sensitivity. He talks about relaxation, but never
uses the metaphor of drowsiness or sleep. In a typical induction, (Gibbons, 1974) the operator
would ask the subject to imagine a warm sandy beach, alertly notice the tangy salt air, feel an
exhilarating sense of arousal and responsiveness. When the hyperempiria and traditional
inductions are compared using standard susceptibility scales, the results were very similar.
Gibbons’ contribution is an expanded notion of how to manage an induction to hypnosis.
Éve Bányai was convinced that hypnosis had nothing to do with sleep, and so
could be induced alertly. Her understanding was driven in part by a theoretical disagreement
with Pavlov’s dogma that hypnosis was a type of cerebral inhibition (Windholz, 1996) and
in part by her work on the differences of EEG in hypnosis and sleep (Bányai, É., Meszaros,
I., & Greguss, A.C., 1983). She and Hilgard (Bányai & Hilgard, 1976) described “active-
alert hypnosis”. They developed a standardized induction that involved open eyes, no mention
of relaxation or sleep, and measurable physical activity. During the induction their subjects
pumped a stationary bike under heavy load. As script for the induction, Bányai used the
SHSC:B (Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1959) with suggestions for alertness, attentiveness and
freshness instead of sleep and drowsiness. The same students also were given a traditional
294
Alert Hypnosis Review
induction. In each condition, after the induction, the students were tested with eight items
from the SHSC: Form A or B (Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1959). For the active-alert condition,
the 50 subjects achieved a M = 4.94 (SD = 2.18) and for the counterbalanced traditional
induction the same subjects achieved M = 5.16 (SD = 2.06), a non-significant difference.
Later Bányai (Bányai, E., Zseni, A., & Tury, F.T., 1993) report on the use of active alert
inductions in psychotherapy. The technique was helpful when treating 200 patients with
depression, generalized anxiety, panic, conversion reactions, and phobia.
There were other applications for Bányai’s innovation. Miller (Miller, M.F.,
Barabasz, A.F., & Barabasz, M., 1991) demonstrated that active alert bicycle induction and
suggestion could be used to develop cold pressor anesthesia. Both types of induction, active
alert or traditional, were equally effective. High hypnotizables were better at pain control
then lows, but the type of induction made no difference.
The positive effect of a waking induction and suggestions for alertness seemed
reliable. Wark wanted to show his college students how to use hypnotic techniques in a
practical education application. Since closed eyes would make study impossible, some from
of alert, eyes open induction was mandatory. He taught them to use “alert self-hypnosis” to
improve their skills to read, listen, take notes, and write exams. Students were randomized
into alert hypnosis treatment or waiting list groups. Following training in an eyes-open
induction, treated subjects were coached to create and give themselves suggestions for better
comprehension while reading in hypnosis. After training, all subjects were tested on a
standardized reading comprehension test (Raygor, 1970). Students in the control group scored
at the 52 percentile, but student in the trained group reading in alert hypnosis scored at the
73 percentile, p < 01 (Wark & laPlante, 1991). In a later study, students in an actual college
courses learned to listen, read, and take tests in alert hypnosis. Using grade point averages
(GPA) calculated from their transcripts, students made an average gain almost one half a
grade from the quarter before to the quarter after training (M GPA = .448, SD = 1.024, p =
.005). Interestingly, the more hypnotizable students, who scored in the highest third on the
Creative Imagination Scale (Wilson & Barber, 1978), made an average gain of over one whole
grade (M GPA = 1.03, SD = .846, p <. 01).
In an innovative development based on sociocognitive principles, Amigó, Capafons
and their colleagues (Amigó, 1994; Capafons & Amigó, 1995) developed a technique of
alert hypnosis called Emotional Self Regulation Therapy (ESRT). There are several variations
of their procedure, but all the methods share an innovative three phase structure. In the first
phase the subject is presented with stimuli (cold water, heavy book, lemon juice) to produce
a recallable response (numbness, fatigue, unpleasant taste). The subject is asked to associate
the responses with a simple cue (word, image, sound). In the second phase, the subject
recalls the response to the cue, but without the physical stimulus. The therapist may tap a
bell and ask the subject to remember the sensation of numbness. These exercises are repeated
with various cues until the response is produced with a feeling of automaticity. For the final
phase, the subject is told that their nervous system has been made more active and receptive
by the exercise. It is now possible for the subject to experience any suggested response
without new training. They can experience, for example, a suggested sense of warmth or
energy.
ESRT was reported in several treatment studies. In one, the technique was used to
treat depression (Amigó, 1994). A patient was given 20 mg of ephedrine and trained to
recall the energized feeling. Using a cough drop as a cue, the patient was able to bring back
the sensations of energy and scored lower on measures of depression and anxiety. In another
295
Wark
series, a variation of the basic method was used to help clients stop smoking. Results were
mixed, but generally positive (Capafons & Amigó, 1995). Capafons (1998) also developed
a three-step variant of ESRT called Rapid Self Hypnosis.
Breaking new ground, Barabasz (Barabasz & Barabasz, 1996) used alert hypnosis
in conjunction with brain wave neurofeedback. The goal was to teach attention deficit
disordered (ADD) patients to read more effectively. When ADD patients read text, their
brain waves shift to slow 4 to 8 Hz activity. When non-disordered students read, they shift to
faster 14 to 32 Hz pattern, and show increased response in the right frontal lobe.
Neurofeedback treatment involves allowing the ADD subject to monitor their brain waves
while reading, and shift toward the faster response. To enhance the effect of the biofeedback,
the subjects were trained by alert induction and hypnosis to attend to the meter. Following
the induction, subjects received suggestions to “focus your attention and concentrate as
completely as you desire”. Subjects who received the alert induction plus suggestions to
attend reached the faster EEG criteria in fewer trials. They were in active hypnosis during
the treatment.
Cardeña (Cardeña, E., Alarcon, A., Capafons, A., & Bayot, A., 1998) used a set of
energizing exercises to prepare subjects for an “active hand” induction. In the preinduction
exercises, the subject was asked to recall or imagine experiences designed to increase heart
beat, respiration, wide visual expansion. The actual induction involved moving the right
hand vertically up and down rapidly and then suggesting the increased heart beat, respiration
and alertness. The authors found that subjects were significantly more responsive and more
likely to stay in the program than when using Bányai’s bicycles induction.
Athletic achievements can be increased with alert hypnosis. Robazza applied the
idea of alert induction to raise an archers precision. In his model (Robazza & Bortoli,
1994), the athlete in alert hypnosis receives suggestions for body awareness, imagery
rehearsal, focus on relevant cues for the event, and smooth automatic execution. The impact
of all these suggestion is enhanced by alert hypnotic induction and suggestion during practice,
followed by a total review later, in traditional hypnosis. In an uncontrolled N = 1 case study,
the authors (Robazza & Bortoli, 1995) reported an experienced archer seen one day a week
for 20 weeks. Mean scores and mean errors were tabulated for two base sessions before
training and six tests during. The data showed solid improvement by an athlete with 17 years
of practice and experience.
Alert induction can be part of a covert, patient controlled treatment. Iglesias and
Iglesias (2005) used awake-alert self-hypnosis to teach a client panic control in social
situations. The client was a recognized philanthropic leader, with self-esteem organized
around public recognition and acceptance. The panic attacks, with heart rate and respiration
increase, stomach upset and sweating occurring in public situations, were very embarrassing.
The client needed a covert way to deal with the panic since traditional self-hypnosis, eyes
closed and body relaxed, would not be acceptable. The authors developed an eyes open
alert induction. The signal for the alert induction was to gaze at or just imagine an expensive
and beautiful Waterford crystal goblet. The client learned to enter eyes open hypnosis and
suggest that her body was “woodsy” and numb, that the physiological symptoms were
gone. The patient reported satisfactory control of the panic attacks.
How do traditional and alert inductions compare?
The effects of an alert induction are similar to those produced by traditional
296
Alert Hypnosis Review
techniques. There are 8 controlled studies reporting a total of 52 different responses. The
responses include subjective measures such as reports of self-awareness (Vingoe, F.J., Hobro,
N., & Milner-Whitaker, I., 1993; Bányai & Hilgard, 1976; Fellows & Richardson, 1993) or
facial appearance, (Ludwig & Lyle,1964), behavioral measures such as finger lock and arm
rigidity (Malott, 1984); and autonomic or physiological effects such as enhanced or diminished
cerebral hemisphere response (Cikurel & Gruzelier, 1990) and cold pressor pain (Miller,
M.F., Barabasz, A.F., & Barabasz, M., 1991). For 39 measures there are no statistical or
practical differences between the responses to suggestions following traditional or alert
inductions. For a detailed review, beyond the needs of this paper, see Wark (1998).
A few significant differences appeared on 13 response measures. Traditional
inductions were more effective in producing a feeling of relaxation (Fellows & Richardson,
1993; Vingoe, F.J., Hobro, N., & Milner-Whitaker, I., 1993) verbal inhibition and calmness
(Bányai & Hilgard, 1976), and hallucinated sound (Bányai, 1980). On the other hand, alert
hypnosis produced greater effect on these variables: discomfort and spontaneous movement
(Ludwig & Lyle, 1964), edginess, (Fellows & Richardson, 1993), movement, and feelings
of alertness (Bányai & Hilgard, 1976). Interestingly, alert inductions were significantly
associated with reports of joyful dreams and active participation (Bányai, 1980).
What can we conclude about the techniques of alert induction compared to the
traditional procedures? Regardless of the type of induction, the effects of hypnotic suggestion
are generally similar. There are some differences associated with the more active induction:
some discomfort, a greater sense of activity and joy of participation.
Case Report
The following case illustrates the use of alert hypnosis to deal with an extreme,
long standing case of anxiety. Lynn was a youthful looking 51-year-old woman, very
intelligent, lively and quick in conversation. Her movements were sure and confident. It was
no surprise that she reported enjoying skiing, volunteering for social action causes and did
competitive dancing. When she described her achievements in athletics, community
organizing and competition, she relaxed and smiled warmly.
She presented because although she graduated from law school 18 years ago and
failed her law board three times, she now wants to try again. In the intake interview, whenever
she talked about reading for or taking an exam, her breathing got labored and she was in
frank respiratory distress. Her reaction to reading seemed unconnected to the rest of her life.
Lynn reported a lifetime of health challenges, including asthma, allergies, arthritis,
temporary paralysis, and sensitivity to chemicals and smoke. She said that her respiratory
allergies were the cause of her failed law board exams. She could not breathe in the smoke
polluted room. She became totally disabled in 1987, a few years after graduation from law
school. In 1995, she had recovered enough to enroll in a professional healthcare training
program.
On the Test Anxiety Survey (Sarason & Ganzer, 1962) she scored 11 indicating
significant reading test anxiety (Wark, D.M., Bennett, J.M., & Wolinski, J, 1980; Wark,
D.M., Bennett, J.M., Emerson, N.M., & Ottenheimer, H., 1981; Wark & Bennett, 1981).
The item responses indicate concern about both the past [consequences of failing] and the
future, [how badly she would compare to other students].
297
Wark
Treatment plan
As long as Lynn focused on the present time, or something pleasant, she maintained
good control. I wanted to teach her how to stay attentive while controlling her anxiety during
her reading, study and test taking. Desensitizing the law exam would reduce the anxiety, but
would she internalize that control? Suggestions in hypnosis could integrate the feeling of
control and her strong sense of social and physical competence. Alert eyes open hypnosis
seemed important so she could read and respond to the exam, maintaining her integrated
control.
I did an alert induction with strong “here-and-now” focus. I had her alertly attend
to the feelings of her body in the chair, sound of my voice, the pattern of her breathing, and
her sense of current control. Then I suggested an age regression to the last exam, but asked
her to hold an image of herself relaxing and breathing. My goal was to modify the recall of
the test, so as to disrupt the associations between the test, feelings of anxiety and labored
breathing. We repeated the exercise several times. At the end of the session, she said she
could recall the exam with less discomfort.
After she learned to induce eyes open alert hypnosis and to read while in hypnosis,
we turned our attention to the upcoming bar exams. At the next session, we talked about her
hypnotic suggestions. She said that for her, the positive suggestion was to feel her competence
and pleasure in her work and study. She did an alert induction and I suggested she experience
that joy. I could see her smile increase as her mouth relaxed and her lips turned up.
At the next session, we reviewed her pattern using alert hypnosis. She said she was
able to enter hypnosis and give herself supportive suggestion. But any image or suggestion
associated with past negative experience was disruptive. During the session when I said,
“You are doing well”, she popped out of hypnosis. At my words, she had flashed on her
fathers attempt to manipulate and control her.
We discussed her transference response. She had not yet developed a mature
mechanism to deal with associations from the past while in present alert hypnosis. So we
built that new strategy. Whenever she felt an intrusion from me or any other source, instead
of fighting it, she was to stop and do a new alert induction focusing on the here and now.
This was designed to “clear the screen” and momentarily erase the past negative message.
Then she was to give herself supportive and positive suggestions. She practiced several
times in the office.
On one occasion between sessions, Lynn called to say her shoulders hurt. I asked
her to associate to the feeling. She said she felt a lot of burdensome guilt from her past
failures. I asked her to do what we had planned: stop, alert focus on the now, and relax the
shoulders. Insightfully, she said she realized the whole process is confronting demons from
the past.
Session before bar exam.
I suggested that her goal for the 2 days of test taking was to read questions while in
alert hypnosis and stay calm. She said her goal was to pass the bar, but that she didn’t feel
she’d had enough time to do a thorough enough study. I asked if she could change the goal.
She then said her goal would be simply to stay calm and clear-minded during the exam.
Session after the bar exam.
We processed her experience taking the test. She said that during the 2 days of 6 hour
exams she was able to stay in alert hypnosis. She needed her inhaler only once during the 2 days.
298
Alert Hypnosis Review
Most importantly, she was able to stay with the current exam. She did not regress to the past, but
stayed alert and in the moment. She passed the exam and has an active legal practice.
Discussion
This review leads to an interesting observation. The inductions reviewed here can
be arranged along an ordinal dimension from relaxation to activity. Well’s (1924) lecture
induction is characterized by very little physical activity. Inductions by Vingoe (1968), Iglesias
& Iglesias (2005) and Gibbons (1974) all involve a slight increase in physical activity. They
use a traditional relaxation induction, but without any mention of sleep or drowsiness.
Arguably a bit more physical activity is part of Barabasz & Barabasz (1996) induction. The
subject is asked to roll eyes up, hold the tension, and then relax. Wark’s (1996) induction
involves structured physical exercises. The subject is asked to change posture, inhale and
hold, then exhale and relax. Cardeña’s (1998) active hand falls a bit further toward the
physical end of the scale. Robazza’s (1995) archery practice and Bányai and Hilgard’s (1976)
exercise bicycle are clearly physical and energetic inductions. Ludwig and Lyles’s (1964)
very strenuous physical exercises in a prison courtyard are probably the current anchor
point at the energetic end of the dimension.
Arranging these inductions along the dimension of relaxed to energetic offers a
fruitful way to conceptualize hypnotic inductions. Many kinds of active response can be
used as an induction and set the stage for increased suggestibility. Focusing while walking
down a long office corridor to a difficult meeting could be preparation for self-hypnotic
suggestions to be a good listener or an assertive speaker. Gentle, slow, repetitive romantic
touching could be an active, arousing induction for suggestions of pleasure and better sexual
performance. Stretching and warm up exercises for athletes can be an induction that facilitate
the use of suggestions for increased endurance in a workout or increased concentration in a
competition. The suggestions can come from the athlete or a coach. In the case report,
sitting up right, gazing at a point on the floor and breathing deeply set the stage for Lynn’s
suggestions of control and presence in a long examination.
This review demonstrates that there are ways to induce hypnosis beyond the
traditional eye fixation and relaxation. Thus, the terms “alert” or “waking” are terms of
encouragement to expand the ways hypnotic interventions can be planned and carried out.
References
Amigó, S. (1994). Self-regulation therapy and the voluntary reproduction of stimulant
effects of ephedrine: Possible therapeutic applications. Contemporary Hypnosis,
11(3), 108-120.
Barabasz, A., & Barabasz, M. (1996). Neurotherapy and alert hypnosis in the treatment
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In S. J. Lynn, I. Kirsch, & J. Rhue
(Eds.), Casebook of clinical hypnosis. (pp. 217-292). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Braid, J. (1843). Neurypnology: Or the Rationale of Nervous Sleep. London: John
Churchill.
Bányai, E., Zseni, A., & Tury, F.T. (1993). Active-alert hypnosis in psychotherapy. In
J. Rhue, S.J. Lynn, & I. Kirsch (Eds.), Handbook of clinical hypnosis.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
299
Wark
Bányai, É., & Hilgard, E. (1976). A comparison of active-alert hypnotic induction with
traditional relaxation induction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85(2), 218-224.
Bányai, É. (1980). A new way to induce a hypnotic-like alert state of consciousness:
Active-alert induction. In L. Kardos and C. Pleth (Eds.), Problems of the
regulation of activity. (pp. 261-273). Budapest: Akademiai Kiado.
Bányai, É., Meszaros, I., & Greguss, A.C. (1983). Psychophysiological comparison of
active-alert and traditional hypnosis. In R. Sinz and M. R. Rosenzweig (Eds.),
Psychophysiology. (pp. 225-230). Jena (GDR) and Amsterdam (Netherlands):
VEB Gustav Fischer Verlag and Elsevier Biomedial Press.
Capafons, A. (1998). Rapid self-hypnosis: A suggestion method for self-control.
Psicothema, 10, 571-581.
Capafons, A. (2004). Waking hypnosis for waking people: Why in Valencia? Contemporary
Hypnosis, 21(3), 136-145.
Capafons, A., & Amigó, S. (1995). Emotional self-regulation therapy for smoking
reduction: Description and initial empirical data. International Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 43(1), 7-19.
Cardeña, E., Alarcon, A., Capafons, A., & Bayot, A. (1998). Effects on suggestibility of a
new method of active-alert hypnosis: Alert hand. International Journal of
Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis , 46(3) , 280-294.
Cikurel, K., & Gruzelier, J. (1990). The effect of an active-alert hypnotic induction on
lateral asymmetry in haptic processing. British Journal of Experimental and
Clinical Hypnosis, 7(1), 17-25.
Fellows, B.J., & Richardson, J. (1993). Relaxed and alert hypnosis: An experiential
comparison. Contemporary Hypnosis, 10(1), 49-54.
Gibbons, D. (1974). Hyperempiria: A new “altered state of consciousness” induced by
suggestion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 47-53.
Gibbons, D. (1979). Applied hypnosis and hyperempiria. New York: Plenum Press.
Iglesias, A., & Iglesias, A. (2005). Awake-alert hypnosis in the treatment of panic
Disorder: A Case Report. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 47(4), 249-257.
Ludwig, A.M., & Lyle, W.H. (1964). Tension induction and the hyperalert state. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69(1), 70-76.
Malott, J.M. (1984). Active-alert hypnosis: Replication and extension of previous
research. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93(2), 246-249.
Miller, M.F., Barabasz, A.F., & Barabasz, M. (1991). Effects of active alert and relaxation
hypnotic inductions on cold pressor pain. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
100(2), 223-226.
Raygor, A. L. (1970). McGraw Hill Basic Skills System Reading Test. Monterey, CA:
McGraw Hill Publishing Co.
Robazza, C., & Bortoli, L. (1994). Hypnosis in sport: An isomorphic model. Perceptual
and Motor Skills, 79(2), 963-973.
Robazza, C., & Bortoli, L. (1995). A case study of improved performance in archery
using hypnosis. Perceptual and Motor Skills , 81(3, Pt 2), 1364-1366 .
Sarason, I., & Ganzer, V.J. (1962). Anxiety, reinforcement, and experimental instructions
in a free verbal situation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 300-307.
Shor, R. E. & Orne, E. C. (1962). Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility. Palo
Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
300
Alert Hypnosis Review
Vingoe, F.J. (1973). Comparison of the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility,
Form A and the Group Alert Trance Scale in a university population.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 21(3), 169-179.
Vingoe, F. (1968). The development of a group alert-trance scale. International Journal
of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 16(2), 120-132.
Vingoe, F.J., Hobro, N., & Milner-Whitaker, I. (1993). Response to a modified Hypnosis
Experience Questionnaire (HEQ-44) following alert and relaxation hypnotic
procedures. Contemporary Hypnosis, 10(3), 159-165.
Wark, D.M. (1996). Teaching college students better learning using self-hypnosis.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 38(4), 277-287.
Wark, D.M. (1998). Alert hypnosis: History and applications. In W. J. Matthews
and J.H. Edgette (Eds.), Creative thinking and research in brief therapy:
Solutions, strategies, narratives, Volume 2. (pp. 287-306). Philadelphia, PA:
Brunner/Mazel.
Wark, D.M., & Bennett, J.M. (1981). The measurement of test anxiety in a reading class.
Reading World, 20, 215-222.
Wark, D.M., Bennett, J.M., Emerson, N.M., & Ottenheimer, H. (1981). Reducing test
anxiety effects of reading comprehension of college students. In G. H. Mc Ninch
(Ed.), Comprehension: Process and produce. First Yearbook of the American
Reading Forum. (pp. 60-62). Hattiesburg, MS: American Reading Forum.
Wark, D.M., Bennett, J.M., & Wolinski, J. (1980). The effects of anxiety on reading
comprehension. In J. A. Fisher (Ed.), College and Adult Reading. Tenth
Yearbook of the North Central Reading Association. (pp. 1-16). Minneapolis, MN:
North Central Reading Association.
Wark, D.M., & laPlante, P.M. (1991). Reading in alert trance: Effects on comprehension.
Hypnos, 17(2), 90-97.
Weitzenhoffer, A. M. & Hilgard, E. (1959). Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale
Forms A and B. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Wells, W.R. (1924). Experiments in waking hypnosis for instructional purposes. Journal
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 18, 389-404.
Wilson, S.C., & Barber, T.X. (1978). The Creative Imagination Scale as a measure of
hypnotic responsiveness: Application to experimental and clinical hypnosis.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 20(4), 235-249.
Windholz, G. (1996). Hypnosis and inhibition as viewed by Heidenhain and Pavlov.
Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 31(2), 155-162.
... Bien que certains chercheurs aient attribué les effets hypnotiques à la relaxation, en postulant que celle-ci pourrait améliorer la réponse hypnotique en inhibant l'excitation nerveuse autonome et centrale [111][112][113][114]118], la relaxation n'est pas indispensable aux réponses hypnotiques (Tableau 1). Les inductions impliquant une activité mentale et physique se sont avérées aussi efficaces que celles favorisant la relaxation [84,119,120]. De plus, le nombre d'études comparant les inductions basées sur la relaxation à celles plus actives est limité [10,84]. Une étude a même révélé que les instructions de relaxation préalables à l'induction étaient moins efficaces que les instructions visant à réduire la pensée critique [121]. ...
Article
Full-text available
RÉSUMÉ L’hypnose est une technique ayant une longue histoire d’utilisation dans des cadres médicaux et psychothérapeutiques. Au cours de la dernière décennie, de nombreuses recherches ont indiqué l’efficacité de l’hypnose pour divers maux physiologiques et psychologiques, notamment la douleur, la détresse et les troubles psychosomatiques. Cependant, l’adoption et l’acceptation de l’hypnose sont encore entravées par les mythes et les fausses idées qui persistent parmi le grand public et les cliniciens. Compte tenu du rôle important des cliniciens dans l’administration et l’acceptation des traitements médicaux et psychothérapeutiques, les cliniciens doivent être informés sur l’hypnose pour améliorer leurs points de vue et promouvoir leur adoption de cette intervention. Cette revue retrace les racines historiques des mythes entourant l’hypnose, tout en démêlant les idées erronées qui ont entravé son utilisation dans les milieux cliniques et de recherche. Nous espérons que cette revue pourra améliorer les connaissances des cliniciens et du public sur l’hypnose clinique et orienter la recherche vers une pratique de l’hypnose fondée sur des preuves.
... Although, Knight (2005) gives a definition of hypnosis through relaxation, "Hypnosis is like daydreaming: a form of relaxed concentration." Other researchers such as Wark (2006) found that sometimes relaxation is not a required factor, "Hypnosis can be carried out with the individual being physically active, open-eyed, focusing on the external environment and with no suggestions of relaxation". ...
Research
Full-text available
Pure and blended coaching are gaining popularity because they are effective in helping individuals and teams to solve problems, achieve goals, improve performance, and develop skills. Nevertheless, often coaching remains inefficient in dealing with incongruent clients because leaders and coaches mostly coach grievously limited conscious minds. Hypnosis is not widespread in business yet but enables utilizing wise unconscious minds to solve problems more congruently, effectively, and efficiently. Researchers discuss adapting temporary progression, age regression, and only a few other hypnotic techniques. This research advances “Hypno-coaching” by applying qualitative patterns, concepts, methods, and values from Ericksonian hypnotherapy to coaching and by discussing the phenomenology of trance states. The author conducted online Hypno-coaching sessions with directors, managers, leaders, workers, and professionals. Quantitative data obtained from surveys of coachees about their experience after the sessions helped to infer: a) hypnosis is consistent with the core coaching competencies; b) hypnosis improves the effectiveness of coaching. Other findings support the ‘special state’ theory for the nature of hypnosis. Coachees became highly motivated, deeply relaxed, and likely to recommend Hypno-coaching to friends and colleagues.
... However, despite being attributed to relaxation, hypnotic effects are not merely due to relaxation [2]. Active inductions involving mental alertness have appeared to be as effective as relaxation-based inductions [117,149,150]. A study found that pre-induction instructions for relaxation were less effective than instructions for reduced critical thinking [151]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and purpose: Hypnosis, a mind-body treatment dating back to early human history, has regained attention in the last decade, with research suggesting its effectiveness for varied physiological and psychological ailments such as distress, pain, and psychosomatic disorders. However, myths and misconceptions have prevailed among the general public and clinicians, hindering the adoption and acceptance of hypnosis. It is important to distinguish myths from facts and discern what is hypnosis and what is not to enhance the understanding, acceptance, and adoption of hypnotic interventions. Methods: This narrative review traces the history of myths surrounding hypnosis in contrast to the evolution of hypnosis as a treatment modality. In addition to comparing hypnosis to other interventions with similar procedures and features, the review unravels misconceptions that have impeded the adoption and acceptance of hypnosis in clinical and research settings and presents evidence to demystify this intervention. Results: This review examines the roots of myths while presenting historical facts and evidence that support hypnosis as a treatment modality and alleviate misconceptions depicting it as mystical. Further, the review distinguishes hypnotic and non-hypnotic interventions with overlapping procedures and phenomenological features to enhance our understanding of hypnotic techniques and phenomena. Conclusion: This review enhances the understanding of hypnosis in historical, clinical, and research contexts by disproving related myths and misconceptions to promote the adoption of hypnosis in clinical and research contexts. Further, this review highlights knowledge gaps requiring further investigations to steer research toward an evidence-based practice of hypnosis and optimise multimodal therapies embedding hypnosis.
... A ressonância magnética funcional teve um papel preponderante na avaliação das alterações do fluxo sanguíneo cerebral regional, descritos pelos sujeitos como "consciência do eu". [62][63][64] A autoconsciência parece afetar as funções integrativas do cérebro e alterar a formação da experiência consciente. 61,65,66 Do ponto de vista neuropsiquiátrico, a autoconsciência 67-72 comporta um conjunto de alterações neurofisiológicas temporárias nas quais se manifestam processos psicológicos no comportamento, atribuídos a agentes externos. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Recently, neuropsychiatry has focused almost exclusively on the correction of pathophysiological changes, inevitably neglecting certain psychosomatic factors of chronic diseases in the community. It is thus necessary to develop and apply mental health that uses a more comprehensive approach in the treatment of disabling diseases and in primary health care. Aim: This study aims to review the effect of self-awareness and awareness of morbidity, as described subjectively by patients during psychotherapeutic interventions or by healthy adults in meditative practices. Materials and Methods: To assess the effect described, a conceptual analysis is proposed as a method. Reviewing the current literature on the «state of the art” of brain dynamics and the psychoneuroimmunological axis, the correlation between the patient’s subjective experience and affective and cognitive neuroscience was tested. As a research hypothesis, it was intended to highlight the scientific paradigms used to study the effect described above. Results: The current limitation on the integration of psychotherapeutic approaches in understanding well-being and mental health prevention is presented, as well as the need for its implementation in general and family medicine and in the community psychiatry. Self-awareness and awareness of morbidity promotes physical and mental well-being and contributes to the development of non-pathological emotional traits. Classical psychotherapy, as well as meditation, allows the individual to access his internal representations, providing the mental space necessary to work on affections and cognitions. The clinical access to these internal representations is important not only in the treatment of disabling diseases, but also indispensable in primary health care. Conclusion: The physician, by promoting the physical and mental health of the individual, his family and the community, has a predominant role in the adhesion to the assumptions of good clinical practices. The research hypothesis defended by this conceptual analysis presents ground for the debate of a community medicine more focused on inducing policies for mental wellbeing, for self-awareness and for morbidity awareness. These mental health policies in the community are currently considered to be objectives of the general and family medicine health program, namely the person-centered approach, which aims at training and empowering the patient or healthy individual as the prime agent responsible for his own health.
... We hypothesized that the psychological and behavioural changes associated with hypnosis are produced by a brain state in which the segregation/integration balance is different from normal waking consciousness: specifically, it is possible PCI should decrease as the subject enters hypnosis, reminiscent of other states where consciousness begins to fade (Casali et al. 2013) and brain activation becomes less complex. Such a prediction seemed sensible, given the relaxation techniques through which hypnotic induction is usually performed and the phenomenological relaxed experience often reported to accompany hypnosis (Banyai and Hilgard 1976;Rainville et al. 2002;Kahn and Hobson 2003;Wark 2006). It should be noted, however, that while experiential similarities have been cited, sleep and hypnosis are not typically considered to be similar to each other neurobiologically (Evans 1979). ...
Article
Full-text available
Can the brain be shifted into a different state using a simple social cue, as tests on highly hypnotizable subjects would sug-gest? Demonstrating an altered global brain state is difficult. Brain activation varies greatly during wakefulness and can be voluntarily influenced. We measured the complexity of electrophysiological response to transcranial magnetic stimulation in one 'hypnotic virtuoso'. Such a measure produces a response arguably outside the subject's voluntary control and has been proven adequate for discriminating conscious from unconscious brain states. We show that a single-word hypnotic induction robustly shifted global neural connectivity into a state where activity remained sustained but failed to ignite strong, coherent activity in frontoparietal cortices. Changes in perturbational complexity indicate a similar move towards a more segregated state. We interpret these findings to suggest a shift in the underlying state of the brain, likely moderating subsequent hypnotic responding.
... Across both inductions, highly suggestible participants reported that they achieved "altered states." More recently, Wark (2006) summarized the findings across multiple studies (e.g., Cardeña, 2005;Malott, 1984;Miller, Barabasz, & Barabasz, 1991) that substituted an alert or hyper-alert induction for a more traditional relaxation-based induction and concluded that there were practically no significant differences across suggestion types. Cardeña, Alarcon, Capafons, and Bayot (1998) conducted an interesting study that provided a creative demonstration of the value of induction preparation. ...
Article
Coaching psychology has not given much thought to hypnosis, despite its long history of applications in enhancing human abilities and potentials. Since a range of psychotherapeutic strategies have been successfully transitioned to coaching and are commonly practiced by coaches and coaching psychologists, it is suggested that hypnotic techniques and principles can also be successfully employed to meet coaching objectives. This paper mainly discusses: (a) the use of self-hypnosis for the coachee; (b) adapting age-progression and age-regression to fit coaching needs; and (c) the use of indirect suggestions in the coaching dialogue to enhance coachee receptivity. The term ‘coaching hypnosis’ is proposed when referring to the use of hypnosis in coaching as it will help to separate links with hypnotherapy and encourage it to build an identity of its own.
Article
In questo studio pilota, partendo dalle evidenze scientifiche relative ai diversi stati di coscienza modificata raggiunti con l'ipnosi vigile e dall'analisi delle somiglianze e delle differenze della stessa con l'ipnosi tradizionale, viene utilizzata l'Active Alert Hypnosis per ottenere un miglioramento della performance e dell'apprendimento in tempi brevi. Il protocollo ideato, che ha consentito il raggiungimento di uno o più obiettivi specifici di tipo prestazionale, può essere integrato anche a un percorso di psicoterapia più ampio.
Article
Full-text available
We contrasted relaxation and active alert hypnotic inductions with or without a specific suggestion for cold pressor pain analgesia. Groups of high (n = 38) and low (n = 27) hypnotizable subjects were tested; hypnotizability had been determined from results of the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form C. Cold pressor pain data were obtained after counterbalanced exposure to relaxation and active alert inductions. Highly hypnotizable subjects demonstrated lower pain scores than did low hypnotizable ones. Pain reports did not differ between induction conditions. Highly hypnotizable subjects given an analgesic suggestion showed lower pain scores than did those exposed only to hypnosis. The findings, conceptualized within E. R. Hilgard's (1977a) neodissociation theory, show that relaxation is not necessary for hypnotic analgesia.
Article
Full-text available
A structured self-hypnosis method -rapid self-hypnosis- is described. This method has been created from a cognitive-behavioral perspective, and has received empirical validation. Some clinical applications of rapid self-hypnosis are shown from a coping skills and self-control orientation. From this perspective, the use of the method in everyday activities are emphasized. Clients can use suggestions while keeping their eyes open and being active. Mention to altered states of consciousness, trance o esoteric ideas is absolutely avoided.
Article
Full-text available
This article presents a cultural analysis of the reasons that waking hypnosis is widely accepted in Spain. These reasons relate to several features of the Spanish people, including their attitude towards others, politics, religion, habits and even the climate in which they live. In general, Spaniards seem to perceive ‘waking’ hypnosis as a technique that better adapts to their lifestyle than more traditional forms of hypnosis. In addition, they may be able to have more confidence in ‘waking’ hypnosis as it appears less threatening, since it is differentiated from stage hypnosis as presented by the media. Therefore, although ‘waking’ hypnosis is American in origin, the particular reasons why it has developed in Spain can be easily understood. Copyright © 2004 British Society of Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis
Article
Recent research demonstrates a negative correlation between test anxiety and reading comprehension by college students. Seemingly, high anxious students get lower comprehension because they ignore or do not integrate cue's from different parts of the text. For the clinician, the problem is how to diagnose test anxiety. The Test Anxiety Scale (TAS) is shown to be a reliable and valid instrument for separating test anxious readers in a population of students. A cutting score is proposed that seems to work in the reading program. Suggestions for reducing reading anxiety are presented. The 16 item TAS is included in the article.
Article
The results obtained with the use of active-alert hypnosis, both with nonclinical volunteers and with patients seeking therapy, show that active-alert hypnosis deserves a prominent place in the array of treatment options available to hypnotherapists. At the same time, controlled outcome research is necessary to further evaluate the nature and magnitude of treatment-related gains. The positive emotional state achieved by recipients of active-alert hypnosis is consistent with a shift in emphasis in contemporary psychology from a focus on negative emotional states to the induction of positive emotions such as love and happiness. Moreover, the active participation of people in active-alert hypnosis is in harmony with current trends in psychotherapy to cultivate competence, self-actualization, the establishment of positive mental health based on mastery, ego activity, and an internal locus of control. By emphasizing positive goals in life and enhancing feelings of mastery, active-alert hypnotherapy can be a potent tool not only in correcting dysfunctions and in treating disorders, but also in enhancing self-development by actuating a cycle of growth-enhancing human potentials. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Compared bilateral haptic processing times before and during a traditional hypnotic relaxation procedure in which 6 high susceptible or 6 medium susceptible male medical students pedalled a bicycle ergometer. Instructions on mental alertness were incorporated with hypnosis. Results replicate previous evidence (J. H. Gruzelier et al; see record 1985-29970-001) suggesting a slowing of left hemispherical processing and a facilitation of right hemispherical processing in susceptible Ss and characterize high rather than medium susceptibles. These effects occurred with both induction procedures, whose influence on susceptibility was highly correlated (E. I. Banyai and E. R. Hilgard; see record 1976-21835-001). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The author discusses the theory of hypnotism and the possibility of employing it for experimental purposes in the class-room. He distinguishes between sleeping and waking hypnosis, justifying his use of the latter term with a wealth of historical and contemporary evidence. He reports the success of class experiments in both types of hypnosis, particularly the latter which he describes minutely. From Psych Bulletin 21:12:01185. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)