ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Magnetic material in the body parts of the stingless bee Schwarziana quadripunctata, heads, pairs of antennae, thorax and abdomens, were investigated by SQUID magnetometry and Ferromagnetic Resonance (FMR). The saturation, J s and remanent, J r, magnetizations and coercive field H c are determined from the hysteresis curves. From H c and J r/J s the magnetic particle sizes are estimated. The J s and the FMR spectral absorption areas yield 23±3%, 45±5%, 15±2% and 19±4% magnetic material contributions of head, pair of antennae, thorax and abdomen, respectively, similar to those observed in the migratory ant Pachycondyla marginata. This result is discussed in light of the hypothesis of antennae as a magnetosensor structure.
Content may be subject to copyright.
UNCORRECTED
PROOF
2 Stingless bee antennae: A magnetic sensory organ?
3
4
M. J. Lucano, G. Cernicchiaro, E. Wajnberg & D. M. S. Esquivel*
5 Centro Brasileiro de Pesquisas
´
sicas, R Xavier Sigaud 150, Rio de Janeiro, RJ 22290-180, Brazil; *Author
6 for correspondence (E-mail: darci@cbpf.br)
7
Received 14 April 2005; accepted 05 July 2005
8 Key words: stingless bee, pair of antennae, SQUID, FMR
9 Abstract
10 Magnetic material in the body parts of the stingless bee Schwarziana quadripunctata, heads, pairs of
11 antennae, thorax and abdomens, were investigated by SQUID magnetometry and Ferromagnetic Reso-
12 nance (FMR). The saturation, J
s
and remanent, J
r
, magnetizations and coercive field H
c
are determined
13 from the hysteresis curves. From H
c
and J
r
/J
s
the magnetic particle sizes are estimated. The J
s
and the FMR
14 spectral absorption areas yield 23±3%, 45±5%, 15±2% and 19±4% magnetic material contributions of
15 head, pair of antennae, thorax and abdomen, respectively, similar to those observed in the migratory ant
16 Pachycondyla marginata. This result is discussed in light of the hypothesis of antennae as a magnetosensor
17 structure.
18
19
20 Introduction
21 For the last 30 years, since the evidence of mag-
22 netotactic bacteria magnetosomes containing
23 magnetite biomineralized nanoparticles (Blake-
24 more 1975), several works on different fields have
25 been developed in order to understand geomag-
26 netic orientation in organisms. Behavioural exper-
27 iments were performed involving several species of
28 animals (Wiltschko & Wiltschko 1995; Va
´
cha &
29 Soukopova
´
2004; Wiltschko et al. 2004) and pur-
30 suing the comprehension of the mechanism
31 underneath this phenomenon. In particular,
32 extensive studies on insects have been focused on
33 the honeybee Apis mellifera. The correlation
34 between honeybee behaviour and the geomagnetic
35 field was firstly proved in 1968 (Lindauer & Mar-
36 tin 1968). Later on, magnetic material was
37 detected in their body using superconducting
38 magnetometers and pointing to a putative mech-
39 anism made of minute particles acting as a mag-
40 netic sensor (Gould et al. 1978). Iron-containing
41 trophocytes were found within the fat body of this
42 adult honeybee (Kuterbach & Walcott 1986),
43 identified as superparamagnet ic (SPM) magnetite
44particles (Hsu & Li 1994), although this result was
45not reproduced. Electron-dense material found in
46the hairs of honeybee abdomens or near the cutex
47was proposed as single domain or SPM magnetite
48(Schiff 1991) and a hypothesis was developed for
49associative learning of visual and magnetic stimuli
50(Schiff & Canal 1993). The presence of iron par-
51ticles were also observed by optical and electron
52microscopy in the trophocytes of adult Scapto-
53trigona postica, a stingless honeybee (Cunha et al.
541987). More recently, iron- rich granules found in
55the fat body of queen honeybees A. mellifera and
56S. postica, were proposed to be formed by holof-
57erritin molecules with inorganic phosphate and
58calcium (and magnesium in S. postica ) with
59diameters smaller than those previously described
60in the literature (Keim et al. 2002).
61A motivation for searching such a sensor would
62be the confirmation that the species behaviour is
63sensitive to the geomagnetic field. The first steps
64are to detect and localize magnetic nanoparticles
65as candidates for magnetic receptors, determining
66their magnetic properties. The following step,
67more complex, is to understand the physiological
68process that is involved in the magnetoreception
BioMetals (2005) 00:1–6 Springer 2005
DOI 10.1007/s10534-005-0520-4
Journal : BIOM Dispatch : 21-7-2005 Pages : 6
CMS No. : DO00020520
h LE h TYPESET
MS Code : BIOM128R1 h CP h DISK
44
AUTHOR’S PROOF!
PDF-OUTPUT
UNCORRECTED
PROOF
69 mechanism. This seems to be the case of the
70 Schwarziana quadripunctata bee for which the
71 magnetic field effect was observed in the frequency
72 of nest exiting (Nascimento et al. 2001).
73 In this report we present room temperature (RT)
74 SQUID magnetic measurements and ferromagnetic
75 resonance technique (FMR) results for magnetic
76 material in the body parts of the S. quadripunctata
77 bee, aiming to existence of a magnetoreceptor.
78 Methods and materials
79 The meliponini stingless bee S. quadripunctata,
80 native of the Atlantic Mata Forest, was found in
81 an underground nest located at Tereso
´
polis, Rio
82 de Janeiro-Brazil, at 1000 m above the sea level
83 and geomagnetic field intensity 0.238 Oe, inclina-
84 tion )32 and declination )2030¢. Adult foragers
85 were collected in the summer between 8–13 h, a
86 period of maximum foraging activity within the
87 optimal flying temperature range of 21–26 C
88 (Imperatriz-Fons eca & Darakjian 1994). Bees were
89 collected still alive, put in a refrigerator and after a
90 week transferred to cacodylate buffer 0.1 M
91 pH 7.4. Ten individuals were used without tho-
92 raxical members. Two groups of four bees each
93 were separated in four parts: head, pair of anten-
94 nae, thorax and abdomen, for SQUID and FMR
95 experiments. To minimize contamination, stain-
96 less-steel instruments were used. Two whole bees
97 were kept for control. The SQUID sample holder
98 does not fit more than two individu als.
99 Just before measurements, samples were dried
100 at 50 C for 1 h. Four units of each body part were
101 oriented one unit close to each other fixed on a
102kapton tape and on a Teflon sample holder for
103SQUID and FMR measurements, respectively. X-
104band FMR spectra (Bruker ESP 300E) at 4 mW
105microwave power, with 210
4
receiver gain and
1062.018 Oe field modulation amplitude and hystere-
107sis curves (MPMS-XL Quantum Design SQUID
108magnetometer) were obtained at room tempera-
109ture with the magnetic fie ld applied parallel to the
110long body axis of the insect, as shown in Figure 1.
111The FMR absorption spectra areas (second inte-
112gral of the derivative spectra) were calculated with
113a software developed using the graphic language
114LabVIEW
, starting at the high field values where
115the baseline is better defined.
116Results
117Hysteresis curves present a straight line with po-
118sitive or negative slope at very strong fields due to
119paramagnetic or diamagnetic contributions, respec-
120tively. Bee, head, thorax and abdomen present a
121diamagnetic contribution (figure not shown), while
Figure 1. Insect orientation relative to the magnetic field.
Table 1. Magnetic parameters of one S. quadripunctata bee
a
and body parts
b
.
Whole bee Head Antennae Thorax Abdomen
J
s
(10
)6
emu) 3.3±0.4 1.1±0.3 2.1±0.3 0.7±0.3 0.9±0.5
H
c
(Oe) 43±15 32±8 130±5 44±18 90±20
J
r
(10
)7
emu) 2.0±0.8 1.4±0.4 5±0.5 0.8±0.1 0.8±0.4
v (10
)9
emu/Oe) )4.2±0.5 )2±0.2 +0.4±0.1 )3.6±0.2 )1.6±0.2
J
r
/J
s
0.06±0.03 0.12±0.06 0.24±0.03 0.12±0.03 0.09±0.03
Magnetic (%) 23±3 44±4 15±2 19±4
c
S (10
8
a. u.) 2.1±0.1 5±0.2 1.8±0.1 1.7±0.1
FMR (%) 20±1 47±3 16±1 \16±1
a
Two bees average values.
b
Four bees parts average values.
c
Taking the control bee J
s
value it increases to 30%.
2
Journal : BIOM Dispatch : 21-7-2005 Pages : 6
CMS No. : DO00020520
h LE h TYPESET
MS Code : BIOM128R1 h CP h DISK
44
UNCORRECTED
PROOF
137137the antennae a paramagnetic one . The dia/para-
138 magnetic susceptibilities (Table 1) are obtained by
139 a linear fit of the curve at magnetic fields higher
140 than that where ferromagnetic saturation is
141 achieved and their contributions subtracted. Fig-
142 ure 2 presents the RT hysteresis curves normalized
143 to one pa rt and one individual, with the highest
144 magnetic contribution coming from the antennae
145 part. For clearness, thorax and head loops are not
146 shown and only one branch of the abdomen and
147 antennae loop were measured. The magnetic
148 parameters: saturation magnetization, J
s
, rema-
149 nent magnetization, J
r
and coercive field H
c
, ob-
150 tained for each body part and for one bee are given
151 in Table 1 , including the J
r
/J
s
ratio. The J
s
sum of
152 each body part average, 4.8±1.410
)6
emu, is
153 taken to calculate the percentual contributions to
154 J
s
as 44±4%, 23±3%, 15±2%, 19±4% for an-
155 tenna, head, thorax and abdomen, respectively.
156 Considering the magnetic material differences
157 content among individuals an d the error bars, the
158 total J
s
is in good agreement with the average J
s
of
159 the two bees used as control.
160 The low field region of the head and antennae
161 hysteresis curves in Figure 2, normalized to their
162 J
s
values, are given in the insert. The antennae
163present the highest H
c
value (130 Oe) and J
r
/J
s
164ratio (0.24), comparatively to the H
c
(32–90 Oe)
165and J
r
/J
s
(0.09– 0.12) values of other parts. Con-
166sidering magnetite as the magnetic particles
167material, the antennae particle sizes fall between
1680.037 and 0.10 lm while the other body part par-
169ticles are about 0.22 l m (Ozdemir et al. 2002).
170Figure 3 shows the FMR spectra of the bee
171body parts with the magnetic field oriented parallel
172to the long body axis. Diamagnetism does not
173contribute to the FMR spectra while paramagne-
174tism does and was not subtracted, as in the hys-
175teresis curves. The four parts spectra present a
176broad (linewidth 550–900 Oe) component at high
177field, HF, centred at about 3000 Oe, with the
178antennae HF line intensity higher than the other
179ones. Only the antennae spectrum clearly presents
180another component at low field, LF, at about
1811300 Oe. The values of the absorption areas S,
182(the second integral of the FMR derivative spec-
183tra) of the parts of the S. quadripunctata bee are
184given in Table 1. S calculated with the WINEPR
185(Bruker) software is not accurate when a compo-
186nent spreads out to zero field, as in the antennae
187case. The specially developed software used in this
188paper, corrects the assumption of zero intensity at
189the first spectrum field value by integrating from
190high to low field values. Even so, the antenna S
191value is a low limit value because the LF line is
192incomplete and the respective contribution cannot
Figure 2. RT Hysteresis curves of S. quadripunctata whole bee,
pair of antennae and abdomen, oriented parallel to the mag-
netic field, normalized to one individual and part. Insert low
field region of head (dashed line) and antennae (solid line)
normalized hysteresis curves.
0 2000 4000 6000
LF
HF
abdomen
pair of antennae
head
thorax
H (Oe)
Figure 3. RT X-band ferromagnetic resonance spectra of
S. quadripunctata body parts. Lines are guide to the eyes.
3
Journal : BIOM Dispatch : 21-7-2005 Pages : 6
CMS No. : DO00020520
h LE h TYPESET
MS Code : BIOM128R1 h CP h DISK
44
UNCORRECTED
PROOF
193 be fully calculated. S values of the HF at RT are
194 related to the magnetic material amou nt, as shown
195 by its linear relation to the saturation magnetiza-
196 tion in termites (Oliveira et al. 2005). Correlation
197 between integrated FMR intensity and the mag-
198 netization was also observed in Si doping of fer-
199 rihydrite nanoparticles (Seehra et al. 2001).
200 Taking S as proportional to the number of reso-
201 nant spins in the sample, the magnetic mate rial
202 percentages in each body part are: 47±3%,20±1%,
203 16±1% and 16±1% in the antennae, head, thorax
204 and abdomen, respectively. These values are in
205 very good agreement with those above, obtained
206 by SQUID magnetometry.
207 Discussion
208 Magnetoreception is a mechanism of magnetic
209 field perception and transduction used for an
210 organism’s orientation. Two hypotheses have
211 arisen to explain its basis: one considering bio-
212 chemical reactions modulated by magnetic field,
213 and another the presence of biogenic magnetic
214 particles as magnetosensors. For now, much of
215 what is known about this mechanism has been
216 accumulated from behavioural experiments, theo-
217 retical proposals and a few electrophysiological
218 and anatomical studies (Lohmann & Johnsen
219 2000). Recent results suggested the involvement of
220 at least two types of receptors in obtaining mag-
221 netic compass information, with the specific
222 interaction of these receptors being rather complex
223 (Wiltschko et al. 2004). Biogenic magnetic parti-
224 cles have gained relevance as they have been
225 reported in several species (Wiltschko & Wiltschko
226 1995; Safarik & Safarikova 2002), but their con-
227 nections to nervous structures still need to be
228 proved. Despite the difficulty of locating tiny
229 magnetoreceptors, that might be dispersed any-
230 where within the animal body, FMR or SQUID
231magnetometry can be used to characterize their
232properties present in some social insects (Wajnberg
233et al. 2000; El-Jaick et al. 2001; Esquivel et al.
2342002; Alves et al. 2004; Esquivel et al. 2004
1;
235Wajnberg et al. 2004; Oliveira et al. 2005a). In this
236paper, both techniques were used to study the
237body parts of S. quadripunctata bees. The HF and
238LF FMR components presen t in this bee body
239parts have already been observed in the abdomen
240of A. mellifera and P. marginata and associated to
241isolated and aggregated magnetite nanoparticles,
242respectively (Wajnberg et al. 2000; El-Jaick et al.
2432001). Moreover, the relative amounts of magnetic
244material obtained from J
s
and S strongly agree,
245confirming the usefulness of the latter in compar-
246ing amounts of magnetic materials at RT. The
247joint analysis of the magnetic material with both
248techniques in all body parts results as 23±3%,
24945±5%, 15±2% and 19±4% magnetic material
250contributions of head, antennae, thorax and
251abdomen, respectively. It agrees on the stingless
252bee antennae containing the highest amount. As
253far as we know, this is the first study on magnetic
254material in all body parts of a honeybee other than
255Apis mellifera, the most studied one, besides opti-
256cal and Electron Microscopy observations on S.
257postica abdomens (Cunha et al. 1987; Keim et al.
2582002). A few previous FMR results (Takagi 1995;
259El-Jaick et al. 2001) confirmed the presence of
260ferromagnetic and paramagnetic material in A.
261mellifera abdomens, without measuring the other
262body parts. On the other hand, magnetic mea-
263surements of whole A. mellifera (Oliveira et al.
2642005a), body parts (Takagi 1995) and particularly
265abdomens (Esquivel et al. 2002) have shown the
266presence of superparamagnetic and larger mag-
267netic particles or aggregates in this body part.
268Hysteresis parameters of whole honeybees and
269respective abdomens are compared in Table 2.
270Honeybees A. mellifera and S. quadripunctata
271present very different magnetic material properties,
Table 2. A. mellifera and S. quadripunctata magnetic parameters.
S. quadripunctata A. mellifera S. quadripunctata abdomen A. mellifera abdomen
J
s
(10
)6
emu) 3.3±0.4 39±4 0.9±0.5 2.5
H
c
(Oe) 43±15 93±10 90±20 44
J
r
(10
)7
emu) 2.0±0.8 46±5 0.8±0.4 2.4
v (10
)9
emu/Oe) )4.2±0.5 )1.6±0.2
J
r
/J
s
0.06±0.03 0.11±0.03 0.09±0.03 0.09
4
Journal : BIOM Dispatch : 21-7-2005 Pages : 6
CMS No. : DO00020520
h LE h TYPESET
MS Code : BIOM128R1 h CP h DISK
44
UNCORRECTED
PROOF
272 except for the J
r
/J
s
ratio. The amount of magnetic
273 material in S. quadripunctata is approxim ately 10
274 times lower than in A. mellifera, and almost three
275 times lower in the abdomens as observed from the J
s
276 values. For comparison, A. mellifera workers are
277 about 12 mm long while S. quadripunctata about
278 6 mm, and the abdomens present the same length
279 ratio.
280 The magnetic fraction present in the S. quad-
281 ripunctata abdomen (19% Table 2) is higher than
282 in A. mellifera (6%). Even considering the differ-
283 ences in magnetic material among individuals of
284 the same species, this J
s
fraction calculated based
285 on the control bee J
s
value (30%) evidences even
286 more the honeybee differences. The estimated size
287 of the particles in S. quadripunctata abdomens
288 (220 nm) is much larger than 13 nm of the A.
289 mellifera estimated from FMR experiments. This
290 difference can be related to: genus specificity,
291 technique sensitivity (SQUID and FMR), sample
292 preparations and environment conditions. The
293 large size is in good agreement with 40–160 nm
294 size range of the iron granules found in another
295 stingless bee S. postica (Cunha et al. 1987), al-
296 though ferritin-like granules were observed as
297 electron-dense particles measuring 2.1±0.5 nm in
298 their abdomen (Keim et al. 2002). Stress should be
299 given to the ingested magnetic material contribu-
300 tion in the thorax and abdomen, which is not
301 biomineralized, and could be the cause of the dif-
302 ferent nanoparticle size and concentrations in
303 abdomens. On the other hand, the head and
304 antennae material can only be the result of a bio-
305 mineralization process, which from an evolution-
306 ary point of view can produce a specific and
307 efficient size and geometry. It is interesting to note
308 that the Pachycondyla marginata ant, which
309 migratory behaviour was related to the geomag-
310 netic field (Acosta-Avalos et al. 2001), shows a
311 similar result, with 42±3% of the magnetic
312 material in the antennae (Wajnberg et al. 2004).
313 As far as we know, no experiments have been
314 carried out concerning the antennae as a magne-
315 toreceptor for orientation; however, the sensitivity
316 of beetle and bug antennae to non-uniform
317 microwave electromagnetic fields was studied,
318 indicating that they can detect and respond to the
319 radiation (Ondracek et al. 1976). Although no
320 obvious organ or structure devoted to magneto-
321 reception necessarily exists, bees possess complex
322 sensory organs, as antennae and eyes, which
323should be considered. The antennae are composed
324of thousands of sensilla, which are con nected to
325the central nervous system (Dade 1994). More
326than one decade ago, magnetite particles found in
327A. mellifera bee abdomens were suggested for
328magnetic orientation (Kirschvink & Walker 1985);
329nevertheless, the high fraction and size of this
330biomineralized magnetic material in the S. quad-
331ripunctata antennae led us to speculate that this
332part may be a magnetosensor organ. These pre-
333liminary findings should be corroborated with
334further behavioural studies and complementary
335physical characterization techniques to compare to
336other insect species, whose orientation behaviour
337is known to be influenced by the geomagnetic field.
338Acknowledgements
339We are grateful to R. Eizemberg for samples
340supply, Dr M. Castro for taxonomic information
341and to Dr O.C. Alves, Dr H.G.P. Lins de Barros
342for helpful discussion and Dr D. Guenzburger for
343carefully reading. MJL thanks CLAF-CNPq and
344EW thanks CNPq for financial support.
345References
346
Acosta-Avalos D, Esquivel DMS, Wajnberg E, Lins de Barros
347HGP, Oliveira PS, Leal I. 2001 Seasonal patterns in the
348orientation system of the migratory ant Pachycondyla mar-
349ginata. Naturwissenschaften 88, 343–346.
350Alves OC, Wajnberg E, Oliveira JF, Esquivel DMS. 2004
351Magnetic material arrangement in oriented termites: A
352magnetic resonance study. J Magn Res 168, 246–251.
353Blakemore R. 1975 Magnetotactic Bacteria. Science 190,
354377–379.
355Cunha MAS, Walcott B, Sesso A. 1987 Iron-containing cells in
356the stingless bee Scaptotrigona postica Latreille (Hymenop-
357tera: Apidae). Morphology and ultrastructure. In: Eder J,
358Rembold H, eds., Chemistry and Biology of Social Insects.
359Verlag; Munchen: pp. 91.
360Dade HA. 1994 Anatomy and Dissection of the Honeybee.
361International Bee Research Association:Cardiff.
362El-Jaick LJ, Acosta-Avalos D, Esquivel DMS, Wajnberg E,
363Linhares MP. 2001 Electron paramagnetic resonance study
364of honeybee Apis mellifera abdomens. Eur Biophys J 29,
365579–586.
366Esquivel DMS, Wajnberg E, Cernicchiaro G, Garcia BE,
367Acosta -Avalos D. 2002 Magnetic material arrangement in
368Apis mellifera abdomens. MRS Symposium Proceedings
369Series 724, N7.2.1.
370Gould JL, Kirschvink JL, Deffeyes KS. 1978 Bees have mag-
371netic remanence. Science 201, 1026–1028.
372Hsu C-Y, Li C-W. 1994 Magnetoreception in honeybees. Sci-
373ence 265, 95–96.
374Imperatriz-Fonseca VL, Darakjian P. 1994 Flight activity of
375Schwarziana quadripunctata quadripunctata (Apidea, Melip-
5
Journal : BIOM Dispatch : 21-7-2005 Pages : 6
CMS No. : DO00020520
h LE h TYPESET
MS Code : BIOM128R1 h CP h DISK
44
UNCORRECTED
PROOF
376 oninae): influence of environmental factors. Abstract. In:
377 International Behaviour Ecology Congress, Nottingham
378 (UK); 86.
379 Keim CN, Cruz-Landim C, Carneiro FG, Farina M. 2002
380 Ferritin in iron containing granules from the fat body of the
381 honeybees Apis mellifera and Scaptotrigona postica. Micron
382 33, 53–59.
383 Kirschvink JL, Walker MM. 1985 Particle-size considerations
384 for magnetite-based magnetoreceptors. In: Kirschvink JL,
385 Jones DS, MacFadden BJ, eds., Magnetite Biomineralization
386 and Magnetoreception in Organism. A New Biomagnetism.
387 Plenum Press; New York: pp. 243–254.
388 Kuterbach DA, Walcott B. 1986 Iron containing cells in the
389 honeybee (Apis mellifera). I. Adult morphology and physi-
390 ology. J Exp Biol 126, 375–387.
391 Lindauer M, Martin H. 1968 Die Schwereorientierung der
392 Biene unter dem Einfluss des Erdmagnetfeldes. Z Vergl
393 Physiol 60, 219–243.
394 Lohmann KJ, Johnsen S. 2000 The neurobiology of magneto-
395 reception in vertebrate animals. Trends Neurosc 23(4),
396 153–169.
397 Nascimento FS, Barbosa MA, Eizemberg R, Wajnberg E,
398 Esquivel DMS. 2001 Efeitos do campo geogmagne
´
tico no
399 comportamento de abelhas nativas da Mata Atlaˆ ntica.
400 Abstract. In: XIX Congresso Brasileiro de Etologia, Juiz de
401 Fora (Br).
402 Ondracek J, Zdarek J, Landa V, Datlov I. 1976 Importance of
403 antennae for orientation of insects in a non-uniform micro-
404 wave eletromagnetic field. Nature 260, 522–523.
405 Oliveira JF, Wajnberg E, Esquivel DMS, Alves OC. 2005
406 Magnetic resonance as a technique to magnetic biosensors
407 characterization in Neocapritermes opacus termites. J Magn
408 Magn Mater 294, e171–e174.
409 Oliveira JF, Cernicchiaro GR, Winklhofer M, Dutra H,
410 Oliveira PS, Esquivel DMS, Wajnberg E. 2005a Compara-
411tive magnetic measurements in social insects. J Magn Magn
412Mater 289C, 442–444.
413Ozdemir O, Dunlop DJ, Moskowitz BM. 2002 Changes in
414remanence, coercivity and domain state at low temperature
415in magnetite. Earth Planet Sci Lett 194, 343–358.
416Safarik I, Safarikova M. 2002 Magnetic nanoparticles and
417biosciences. Monatsh Chem 133, 737–759.
418Schiff H. 1991 Modulation of spike frequencies by varying the
419ambient magnetic field and magnetite candidates in bees
420(Apis mellifera). Comp Biochem Physiol 100A(4), 975–985.
421Schiff H, Canal G. 1993 The magnetic and electric field induced
422by superparamagnetic magnetite in honeybees. Biol Cybern
42369, 7–17.
424Seehra MS, Punoose A, Roy P, Manivannan A. 2001 Effect of
425Si doping on the electron spin resonance properties of fer-
426rihydrite nanoparticles. IEEE Trans Magn 37(4), 2207–2209.
427Takagi S. 1995 Paramagnetism of honeybees. J Phys Soc Jpn
42864(11), 4378–4381.
429Va
´
cha M, Soukopova
´
H. 2004 Magnetic orientation in the
430mealworm beetle Tenebrio and the effect of light. J Exp Biol
431207, 1241–1248.
432Wajnberg E, Cernicchiaro G, Esquivel DMS. 2004 Antennae:
433The strongest magnetic part of the migratory ant. Biometals
43417, 467–470.
435Wajnberg E, Acosta-Avalos D, El-Jaick LJ, Abrac
ˇ
ado L,
436Coelho JLA, Bazukis AF, Morais PC, Esquivel DMS. 2000
437Electron paramagnetic resonance study of the migratory ant
438Pachycondyla marginata abdomens. Biophys J 78,
4391018–1023.
440Wiltschko W, Wiltschko R. 1995 Magnetic Orientation in
441Animals. Springer-Verlag:Berlin.
442Wiltschko W, Gesson M, Stapput K, Wiltschko R. 2004 Light
443dependent magnetoreception in birds: Interaction of at least
444two different receptors. Naturwissenschaften 91, 130–134.
445
6
Journal : BIOM Dispatch : 21-7-2005 Pages : 6
CMS No. : DO00020520
h LE h TYPESET
MS Code : BIOM128R1 h CP h DISK
44
... The main goal of the present manuscript is to seek magnetite in the intact body parts (abdomen, thorax, head and antennae) of A. mellifera honeybees, using magnetometry and FMR techniques at room temperature. In other social insects, as ants and stingless bees, it has been observed a strong magnetism in the antennae or the head [28][29][30][31]. Those reports stimulated us to study the magnetism in the antennae and the head of A. mellifera honeybees to observe if their magnetizations are stronger than that of the other honeybee parts. ...
... Table 1 show differences among both samples with the abdomen being the part with higher saturation magnetization in both samples. Different from other insects, as the stingless bee Schwarziana quadripunctata [30] and the migratory ant Pachycondyla marginata [25], the antenna does not show the strongest magnetization. Table 1 shows that both samples present very different values for M S , M R , H C , and H CR . ...
... The antennae have been reported to present magnetism in other insects, as the ant Pachycondyla marginata [31], the ant Solenopsis interrupta [28], the neotropical ant Atta colombica [29], and the stingless bee Schwarziana quadripunctata [30]. Also, the antennae have been proposed as the site for magnetoreception in Monarch butterflies [12] and migratory ants [31], being the magnetic material associated to the magnetosensor not necessarily magnetite, as proposed by Wajnberg et al. [60] where they report the presence of titanium iron oxides in the antennae of migratory ants. ...
Article
Full-text available
The detection of magnetic fields by animals is known as magnetoreception. The ferromagnetic hypothesis explains magnetoreception assuming that magnetic nanoparticles are used as magnetic field transducers. Magnetite nanoparticles in the abdomen of Apis mellifera honeybees have been proposed in the literature as the magnetic field transducer. However, studies with ants and stingless bees have shown that the whole body of the insect contain magnetic material, and that the largest magnetization is in the antennae. The aim of the present study is to investigate the magnetization of all the body parts of honeybees as has been done with ants and stingless bees. To do that, the head without antennae, antennae, thorax, and abdomen obtained from Apis mellifera honeybees were analyzed using magnetometry and Ferromagnetic Resonance (FMR) techniques. The magnetometry and FMR measurements show the presence of magnetic material in all honeybee body parts. Our results present evidence of the presence of biomineralized magnetite nanoparticles in the honeybee abdomen and, for the first time, magnetite in the antennae. FMR measurements permit to identify the magnetite in the abdomen as biomineralized. As behavioral experiments reported in the literature have shown that the abdomen is involved in magnetoreception, new experimental approaches must be done to confirm or discard the involvement of the antennae in magnetoreception.
... With this newly gained knowledge, we asked where the magnetic sensor in the ants might be located. Previous studies had suggested that the insect antennae might be involved in magnetoreception (Abraçado et al., 2008;de Oliveira et al., 2010;Guerra et al., 2014;Lucano et al., 2006;Wajnberg et al., 2010Wajnberg et al., , 2017. The ant antennae is involved in several tasks related to orientation behavior, including olfaction (Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990), wind compass orientation (Müller and Wehner, 2007;Sane et al., 2007), or graviception (Vowles, 1954b). ...
... Additionally, in the antennae of stingless bees and ants a sufficient amount of magnetic material has been found to potentially enable magnetoreception Lucano et al., 2006). Another indication for the involvement of the Johnston's organ in magnetoreception is, that the magnetic sense of Hymenoptera seems to be entangled with the gravitational sense (Lindauer and Martin, 1968;Martin and Lindauer, 1977). ...
... The multimodal nature of the JO makes it a suitable candidate to play a crucial role during learning walks. In addition, the insect antennae have been suggested as a potential site for magnetoreceptionGuerra, Gegear, & Reppert, 2014;Lucano, Cernicchiaro, Wajnberg, & Esquivel, 2006), which renders the antenna and potentially the JO as one candidate in the search for the insect magnetic compass(Fleischmann, Grob, & Rössler, 2020).Future combinations of behavioral manipulations with physiologicaland anatomical studies in Cataglyphis ants are highly promising to further elucidate the roles of this fascinating multisensory organ in navigation and the respective processing areas in the central brain of the ant.ACKNOWLEDGMENTSThe authors want to thank the Greek government and the management boards of the Schinias and Strofylia National Parks for permissions to excavate ant nests. Special thanks go to Claudia Gehrig for sectioning the antennae and Sebastian Britz for introducing us to TrakEM2. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Humans and animals alike use the sun, the moon, and the stars to guide their ways. However, the position of celestial cues changes depending on daytime, season, and place on earth. To use these celestial cues for reliable navigation, the rotation of the sky has to be compensated. While humans invented complicated mechanisms like the Antikythera mechanism to keep track of celestial movements, animals can only rely on their brains. The desert ant Cataglyphis is a prime example of an animal using celestial cues for navigation. Using the sun and the related skylight polarization pattern as a compass, and a step integrator for distance measurements, it can determine a vector always pointing homewards. This mechanism is called path integration. Since the sun’s position and, therefore, also the polarization pattern changes throughout the day, Cataglyphis have to correct this movement. If they did not compensate for time, the ants’ compass would direct them in different directions in the morning and the evening. Thus, the ants have to learn the solar ephemeris before their far-reaching foraging trips. To do so, Cataglyphis ants perform a well-structured learning-walk behavior during the transition phase from indoor worker to outdoor forager. While walking in small loops around the nest entrance, the ants repeatedly stop their forward movements to perform turns. These can be small walked circles (voltes) or tight turns about the ants’ body axes (pirouettes). During pirouettes, the ants gaze back to their nest entrance during stopping phases. These look backs provide a behavioral read-out for the state of the path integrator. The ants “tell” the observer where they think their nest is, by looking back to it. Pirouettes are only performed by Cataglyphis ants inhabiting an environment with a prominent visual panorama. This indicates, that pirouettes are performed to learn the visual panorama. Voltes, on the other hand, might be used for calibrating the celestial compass of the ants. In my doctoral thesis, I employed a wide range of state-of-the-art techniques from different disciplines in biology to gain a deeper understanding of how navigational information is acquired, memorized, used, and calibrated during the transition phase from interior worker to outdoor forager. I could show, that celestial orientation cues that provide the main compass during foraging, do not guide the ants during the look-back behavior of initial learning walks. Instead Cataglyphis nodus relies on the earth’s magnetic field as a compass during this early learning phase. While not guiding the ants during their first walks outside of the nest, excluding the ants from perceiving the natural polarization pattern of the skylight has significant consequences on learning-related plasticity in the ants’ brain. Only if the ants are able to perform their learning-walk behavior under a skylight polarization pattern that changes throughout the day, plastic neuronal changes in high-order integration centers are induced. Especially the mushroom body collar, a center for learning and memory, and the central complex, a center for orientation and motor control, showed an increase in volume after learning walks. This underlines the importance of learning walks for calibrating the celestial compass. The magnetic compass might provide the necessary stable reference system for the ants to calibrate their celestial compass and learn the position of landmark information. In the ant brain, visual information from the polarization-sensitive ocelli converge in tight apposition with neuronal afferents of the mechanosensitive Johnston’s organ in the ant’s antennae. This makes the ants’ antennae an interesting candidate for studying the sensory bases of compass calibration in Cataglyphis ants. The brain of the desert navigators is well adapted to successfully accomplish their navigational needs. Females (gynes and workers) have voluminous mushroom bodies, and the synaptic complexity to store large amount of view-based navigational information, which they acquire during initial learning walks. The male Cataglyphis brain is better suited for innate behaviors that support finding a mate. The results of my thesis show that the well adapted brains of C. nodus ants undergoes massive structural changes during leaning walks, dependent on a changing celestial polarization pattern. This underlies the essential role of learning walks in the calibration of orientation systems in desert ants.
... Such studies allowed the identification of the antennae as the location of the magnetoreceptor. For other insects, such as Atta colombica (Guérin-Méneville, 1844) and Schwarziana quadripunctata (Lepeletier, 1836) (Lucano et al., 2006;Alves et al., 2014), similar studies identify the antennae as the body part with a higher magnetic signal. ...
... Magnetometry studies done in other ants have shown that all parts of the ant body are magnetic , but the abdomen and the head show different magnetic properties. Those studies led to the proposal that the magnetosensor must be in the antennae for Pachycondyla marginata, Atta colombica, and Schwarziana quadripunctata (Wajnberg et al., 2004;Lucano et al., 2006;Alves et al., 2014). Our results show that the abdomen and the head have different magnetic properties, and both can host the magnetosensor responsible for the observed magnetosensibility. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of the present paper is to study magnetosensibility and to seek for magnetic nanoparticles in ants. The social insects, by living in colonies, developed very efficient methods of nestmate recognition, being less tolerant towards individuals from other colonies. Therefore, any kind of strange behavior between nestmates and/or conspecifics, besides those present in their own behavioral repertoire, is not expected. The behavior study in the present paper analyze whether changes in the intensity of applied magnetic fields on Ectatomma brunneun (Smith) ants can cause changes in the normal pattern of interaction between conspecifics. A pair of coils generating a magnetic field was used to change the whole local geomagnetic field. Magnetometry studies were done on abdomens and head + antennae using a SQUID magnetometer. The results show that changes in the geomagnetic field affect the usual pattern of interactions between workers from different colonies. The magnetometry results show that abdomens present superparamagnetic nanoparticles and heads present magnetic single domain nanoparticles. Behavior experiments show for the first time that Ectatomma brunneun ants are magnetosensible. The change in nestmate recognition of Ectatomma ants observed while a magnetic field is applied can be associated to some kind of disturbance in a magnetosensor presented in the body based on magnetic nanoparticles.
... In Schwarziana quadripunctata the flight direction in daylight at the point of exit of underground hives is on record (Esquivel et al., 2007;. Experimental results in the presence of magnetic nanoparticles suggest that EMF serves as an orientation cue (Lucano et al., 2006). Only the reversed vertical field affects the inclination of the light trajectory. ...
Article
Full-text available
Few insects have the sensory ability to sense and use the earth’s magnetic field. Studies have revealed a wealth of information on the magnetic sense of some insects. However, the mechanism of sensing the earth’s magnetic field, called magnetoreception, is still enigmatic in insects. Magnetoreception studies in fruit flies, bees, and ants are well-documented. Of two hypothesized types of magnetoreception mechanisms in those insects, one is ferromagnetic, and the other is light-dependent. Although experimental results appear to be consistent with the proposed hypothesized mechanisms it is possible that there is still an unknown mechanism that would explain and confirm the experimental results. Thus, theories explaining magnetoreception in insects are yet to be come out. Magnetoreception plays a role in migration, orientation, as well as navigation in insects. Several sensory cues play significant role in migration. Moreover, our understanding of magnetoreception requires information from various branches of science, such as physics, behavioural biology, zoology, and environmental biology. The article attempts to update the account of magnetoreception in the said insects as well as to identify the gaps in our knowledge thereof.
... For the first time the FMR parameters A and g eff have been used in the study of magnetic material in insects and the results suggest that the magnetic nanoparticles in P. versicolor body and in the abdomen and head of P. paulista are biomineralized inside the body and that the magnetic nanoparticles in P. paulista antennae are similar to inorganic magnetic nanoparticles. Our results are similar to other results obtained in other insects, where it is shown the presence of magnetic nanoparticles in all the insect body (see for example: Acosta-Avalos et al. 1999, Lucano et al. 2006, Chambarelli et al. 2008, Pereira et al. 2019. Our results do not imply that magnetoreception using magnetic nanoparticles is not possible in insects, because it is not known the purpose of biomeralized magnetic nanoparticles in the body insects. ...
Article
Full-text available
The detection of the geomagnetic field by animals to use as a cue in homing and migration is known as magnetoreception. The ferromagnetic hypothesis explains magnetoreception assuming that magnetic nanoparticles in cellular structures are used as magnetic field transducers. Considering magnetoreception in social insects, the most studied has been the honeybee Apis mellifera and only in two wasp species (Vespa orientalis and Polybia paulista) have been shown a magnetosensitive behavior. In the present report the body parts (abdomen, head and antennae) of Polistes versicolor and Polybia paulista wasps were studied aiming to find biomineralized magnetic nanoparticles, using magnetometry measurements and ferromagnetic resonance spectroscopy. The magnetometry measurements show the presence of magnetic nanoparticles in all body parts, being characterized as mixtures of superparamagnetic, single domain and pseudo-single domain nanoparticles. From the ferromagnetic resonance spectra were obtained the asymmetry ratio A and the effective g factor geff, and those parameters are consistent with the presence of biomineralized magnetic nanoparticles in both wasps. In the case of Polybia paulista, the magnetic nanoparticles can be associated with some sort of magnetosensor once this wasp is magnetosensitive. For Polistes versicolor, the results indicate that this wasp can be magnetosensitive as Polybia paulista once their magnetic nanoparticles are biomineralized in the body. Behavioral studies with Polistes versicolor wasps deserve to be performed.
... The multimodal nature of the JO makes it a suitable candidate to play a crucial role during learning walks. In addition, the insect antennae have been suggested as a potential site for magnetoreception (de Oliveira et al., 2010;Guerra, Gegear, & Reppert, 2014;Lucano, Cernicchiaro, Wajnberg, & Esquivel, 2006), which renders the antenna and potentially the JO as one candidate in the search for the insect magnetic compass (Fleischmann, Grob, & Rössler, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
The Johnston’s organ (JO) in the insect antenna is a multisensory organ involved in several navigational tasks including wind-compass orientation, flight control, graviception, and, possibly, magnetoreception. Here we investigate the three dimensional anatomy of the JO and its neuronal projections into the brain of the desert ant Cataglyphis, a marvelous longdistance navigator. The JO of C. nodus workers consists of 40 scolopidia comprising three sensory neurons each. The numbers of scolopidia slightly vary between different sexes (female/male) and castes (worker/queen). Individual scolopidia attach to the intersegmental membrane between pedicel and flagellum of the antenna and line up in a ring-like organization. Three JO nerves project along the two antennal nerve branches into the brain. Anterograde double staining of the antennal afferents revealed that JO receptor neurons project to several distinct neuropils in the central brain. The T5 tract projects into the antennal mechanosensory and motor center (AMMC), while the T6 tract bypasses the AMMC via the saddle and forms collaterals terminating in the posterior slope (PS) (T6I), the ventral complex (T6II), and the ventrolateral protocerebrum (T6III). Double labeling of JO and ocellar afferents revealed that input from the JO and visual information from the ocelli converge in tight apposition in the PS. The general JO anatomy and its central projection patterns resemble situations in honeybees and Drosophila. The multisensory nature of the JO together with its projections to multisensory neuropils in the ant brain likely serves synchronization and calibration of different sensory modalities during the ontogeny of navigation in Cataglyphis.
... It has remained an open question whether the honeybee abdomen actually plays a crucial role in magnetoreception or has a function as waste storage for dietary iron (Shaw et al. 2018). Based on the studies offering indirect evidence for the location of the magnetic compass, several studies have pointed at the hymenopteran antenna as a potential location, particularly in ants (Abraçado et al. 2008;de Oliveira et al. 2010;Wajnberg et al. 2017) and stingless bees (Lucano et al. 2006). Even though the mere existence of ferromagnetic material somewhere in the animal does not prove a magneto-sensitive organ, the hymenopteran antenna is a promising candidate for the magnetic compass. ...
Article
Full-text available
The use of information provided by the geomagnetic field (GMF) for navigation is widespread across the animal kingdom. At the same time, the magnetic sense is one of the least understood senses. Here, we review evidence for magnetoreception in Hymenoptera. We focus on experiments aiming to shed light on the role of the GMF for navigation. Both honeybees and desert ants are well-studied experimental models for navigation, and both use the GMF for specific navigational tasks under certain conditions. Cataglyphis desert ants use the GMF as a compass cue for path integration during their initial learning walks to align their gaze directions towards the nest entrance. This represents the first example for the use of the GMF in an insect species for a genuine navigational task under natural conditions and with all other navigational cues available. We argue that the recently described magnetic compass in Cataglyphis opens up a new integrative approach to understand the mechanisms underlying magnetoreception in Hymenoptera on different biological levels.
... The ferromagnetic hypothesis is well accepted. The presence of magnetic nanoparticles has been shown in different body parts and in the antennae of ants and bees Lucano et al. 2006) where they are thought to function as magnetosensors. FMR is mostly used to verify the presence of these magnetic nanoparticles. ...
Article
Ferritin has been studied in many animals, plants and bacteria. The main functions of ferritin in mammals are iron concentration and stabilization, protection against oxidants and iron storage for later developmental or iron-dependent activities. Although insect ferritin plays a key role in iron transport, only a few studies to date have examined its properties and function. Ferritin isolation from the haemolymph of adult Camponotus sericeiventris ants involved heating at 75 °C, followed by protein fractionation with 3.2 M KBr gradients and ferritin sedimentation with KBr. Protein identification was performed using high-resolution proteomics techniques. SDS-PAGE revealed three subunits with molecular weights (MW) of 26, 28 and 31 kDa. Native PAGE indicated a MW higher than 669 kDa. Proteomic analysis strongly suggested the 26 and 31 kDa bands as F2LCH and F1HCH subunits of ferritin, respectively. Ferromagnetic resonance (FMR) at 100 K showed, at low field, a characteristic broad component of the ferritin iron core, suggesting that its distribution was shifted to values greater than 3000, a higher content than in mammals. The protein yield and MW were comparable to those reported in other studies of insects. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report on ferritin extracted from adult ants to date. These results are discussed on the basis of the protein structure–function relation of secreted insect and mammal ferritins. This purification method will allow the use of magnetic techniques, which are relevant for understanding the role of ferritin in the biomineralization of magnetic nanoparticles in insects.
... In P. marginata and Solenopsis interrupta ants, and S. quadripunctata bees (Wajnberg et al. 2004;Lucano et al. 2006;Abraçado et al. 2009), consistent observations of the highest amount of magnetic material in the head and antennae, that are free of recently ingested material, have indicated them as magnetic sensor organs. Although ingested material passes through the insect mouth the amount of ingested material in the head would be negligible, and there should be none of it inside the antenna. ...
Article
Full-text available
The most accepted hypothesis of magnetoreception for social insects is the ferromagnetic hypothesis which assumes the presence of magnetic material as a sensor coupled to sensitive structures that transmit the geomagnetic field information to the nervous system. As magnetite is the most common magnetic material observed in living beings, it has been suggested as basic constituent of the magnetoreception system. Antennae and head have been pointed as possible magnetosensor organs in social insects as ants, bees and termites. Samples of three antenna joints: head-scape, scape-pedicel and pedicel-third segment joints were embedded in epoxi resin, ultrathin sectioned and analyzed by transmission electron microscopy. Selected area electron diffraction patterns and X-ray energy dispersive spectroscopy were obtained to identify the nanoparticle compound. Besides iron oxides, for the first time, nanoparticles containing titanium have been identified surrounded by tissue in the antennae of ants. Given their dimension and related magnetic characteristics, these nanoparticles are discussed as being part of the magnetosensor system.
Article
Many animals use the geomagnetic field to orient. Among the mechanisms proposed for magnetoreception, the ferromagnetic hypothesis assumes a magnetosensor based on magnetic particles. In this study, magnetic resonance (MR) is applied to 11 Lepidoptera species separated into four body parts: antennae, head, thorax, and abdomen. For the first time, magnetic characteristics of the parts are compared between migratory Urania fulgens and Aphrissa. statira and non-migratory Heliconius ethilla, Anartia amathea, An. fatima and Actinote thalia, species for which we had sufficient specimens for statistical analyses. Spectral characteristics of the magnetic material include the geff factor, linewidth and the high field (HF) component area, as well as, the HF area ratio of these body parts. The broad HF and low field (LF) components, commonly observed in social insect spectra, are present in the Lepidoptera body part spectra. Other unusual narrow lines are superimposed mainly to the HF component, the narrow component at geff ∼2.05 associated with very small Fe aggregates and the extra component at geff values from 2.13 to 3.03. The relative amount of magnetic material in the body parts are derived from the HF area. The results indicate that only antenna/head ratio of magnetic material amount and the spectral components distinguish migrant from non-migratory Lepidoptera. The extra component showing angular dependence and previously observed in spectra of honeybee abdomens and leaf-cutter ant antennae was observed in the antennae of non-migratory species but not in the migratory ones. Similarities of these attributes with other homing insects suggest that global orientation is as important for butterflies that occupy home ranges, defend territories, and trap-line flowers as it is for long-distance migrating Lepidoptera.
Book
Full-text available
The unusual capacity of some tropical freshwater fishes (of the dominating subgroup Teleostei) to generate and sense electric signals, the discharges of their weak electric organs, was discovered by Hans Lissmann (1951, 1958) of the University of Cambridge. He demonstrated the function of an active elec-trolocation system, but, along with others, also proposed a second function, that of communication. Studies in electrical communication were pioneered by Patricia Black-Cleworth (1970), then in the laboratory of T. Bullock at the University of California in La Jolla, and Peter Moller (1970), then in the laboratory of T. Szabo at the CNRS research institute and the Collège de France in Paris.
Article
Full-text available
Honeybees are the most studied insects in the magnetic orientation research field. Experiments on the magnetic remanence of honeybees have shown the presence of magnetite nanoparticles, aligned transversely to the body axis on the anterodorsal abdomen horizontal plane. These results support the hypothesis of ferromagnetic sensors for the magnetoreception mechanism. An Electron Paramagnetic Resonance (EPR) study identified isolated magnetite nanoparticles and aggregates of these particles with a low temperature transition (52 K - 91 K). Hysteresis curves of Apis mellifera abdomens organized parallel and perpendicular to the applied magnetic field were obtained from 5K to 310K. At low temperatures, the hysteresis curves indicate a preferential orientation of the magnetic easy axis parallel to the body axis. The saturation (J s) and remanent (J r) magnetizations, coercive field (H c) and initial susceptibility (χ) were obtained. Results were interpreted based on the presence of magnetite nanoparticles with 50 K and 120 K mean blocking temperatures.
Article
Full-text available
Starting from our magnetic field model for magnetic clouds (MCs), which topologically considers them as cylinders with elliptical cross sections, we present a first attempt in the study of the electric field induced by the movements of magnetic clouds in the interplanetary medium and the expansions of their cross sections. These expansions are included in the model assuming linear time dependence in all the components of the plasma current density. In a previous paper we already determined the magnetic field and current density of our MCs model, and in its development we established that to get it physically consistent, the induced electric field has to be independent of time. In the present work we calculate the expressions for the components of this electric field and fit them to the corresponding experimental data determined from the measurements of the plasma velocity and magnetic field components through the expression E\vec E = -v\vec vSW × B\vec B. To test the model, we have selected three intense and well-defined magnetic clouds observed in July 2000, November 2003, and May 2005. Until now we think it is one of the first attempts to incorporate this induced electric field in the context of analytical models for the study of MCs.
Article
Full-text available
Animals use the geomagnetic field in many ways: the magnetic vector provides a compass; magnetic intensity and/or inclination play a role as a component of the navigational 'map', and magnetic conditions of certain regions act as 'sign posts' or triggers, eliciting specific responses. A magnetic compass is widespread among animals, magnetic navigation is indicated e.g. in birds, marine turtles and spiny lobsters and the use of magnetic 'sign posts' has been described for birds and marine turtles. For magnetoreception, two hypotheses are currently discussed, one proposing a chemical compass based on a radical pair mechanism, the other postulating processes involving magnetite particles. The available evidence suggests that birds use both mechanisms , with the radical pair mechanism in the right eye providing directional information and a magnetite-based mechanism in the upper beak providing information on position as component of the 'map'. Behavioral data from other animals indicate a light-dependent compass probably based on a radical pair mechanism in amphibians and a possibly magnetite-based mechanism in mammals. Histological and elec-trophysiological data suggest a magnetite-based mechanism in the nasal cavities of salmonid fish. Little is known about the parts of the brain where the respective information is processed.
Article
Bacteria with motility directed by the local geomagnetic field have been observed in marine sediments. These magnetotactic microorganisms possess flagella and contain novel structured particles, rich in iron, within intracytoplasmic membrane vesicles. Conceivably these particles impart to cells a magnetic moment. This could explain the observed migration of these organisms in fields as weak as 0.5 gauss.
Article
The magnetic properties of honeybees have been investigated bymeasuring their susceptibility, magnetization andelectron-spin-resonance (ESR) absorption. The abdomen of adult workerhoneybees shows typical paramagnetism with small magnetic remanencedown to 4.2 K, while the other parts of the honeybees are apparentlydiamagnetic. The paramagnetism of the abdomen seems to develop not inthe pupal stage but after coming out of comb. Drones show noparamagnetism, but a queen bee seems to have some paramagnetic speciesin her abdomen.
Article
Two new sulphur are reported in monocristalline rubidiumchloride doped with rubidium and sulphur, and irradiated with X-rays at room temperature. By an extensive comparison with other experimental data on chalcogen centres in alkali halides an interstitial RbCl:S and a substitutional RbCl:S 3 model is proposed for these paramagnetic defects. Theoretical calculations confirm the S ion model for the former.
Article
This experimental study quantitatively correlates the saturation magnetization obtained from hysteresis curves (SQUID measurements) to the second integral of the magnetic resonance (MR) spectra of Neocapritermes opacus termites. Termites were submitted to an iron private diet, feeding them with pure cellulose for up to four days. This diet cleans their guts of ingested detrital material, eliminating non-biogenic soil-derived magnetite from the ensuing analyses. A clear relation between total magnetic moment (emu) from SQUID measurements and the signal intensity (absorption area) from MR is given.
Article
The presence of a magnetic influence upon behavior now appears to be a fairly common trait among a wide variety of organisms, as outlined and discussed elsewhere in this volume. In a broad manner, these behavioral responses can be grouped into two categories, the first of which involves the use of a relatively insensitive “compass” to obtain directional (north/south) information, and a more sensitive system involved in the “map” sense of vertebrates and the time cue of insects.