ArticlePDF Available

Alzheimer's Quick Test cognitive screening criteria for West African speakers of Krio

Authors:

Abstract

To obtain normative data for Alzheimer's Quick Test (AQT) measures of perceptual and cognitive speed from West African speakers of Krio. Normal adults, who were functionally independent, from Sierra Leone (n = 164) aged 25-79 years. Perceptual and cognitive speed were measured with AQT single- and dual-dimension colour-number (C-N) and colour-animal (C-A) naming tasks. Tests were administered individually in the participants' communities. Men and women performed similarly (P>0.05), whereas literate speakers used significantly less time than preliterate peers (P<0.01). Correlations between age and colour naming were low (P<0.01) and speed decreased by <0.1 s per year. Dual-dimension naming remained stable across ages. Correlations with years of education were low for dual-dimension naming (P<0.01) and speed increased approximately 0.4 s per added year. Correlations between age and education and AQT naming were non-significant for literate participants. Criterion time cut-offs (seconds) for screening were developed for preliterate and literate speakers of Krio for typical (<+1 SD), slower-than-typical (between +1 and +2 SD) and atypical (>+2 SD) performance. AQT C-N and C-A naming are time efficient (3-5 min each), objective and reliable and can be administered in Krio to West African adults in Africa, Europe or North America to screen for cognitive impairments and facilitate referral for medical workup.
Age and Ageing © The Author 2006. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the British Geriatrics Society.
doi:10.1093/ageing/afl058 All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org
Page 1 of 5
Alzheimer’s Quick Test cognitive screening
criteria for West African speakers of Krio
NIELS PETER NIELSEN
1
, ELISABETH H. WIIG
2
1
Department of Psychiatry, Hvidovre Hospital, Vesterbrogade 31, Copenhagen, Denmark
2
Knowledge Research Institute, Inc., 7101 Lake Powell Drive, Arlington, TX 776016-3517, USA
Address correspondence to: Elisabeth H. Wiig. Tel: (+817) 572 6254. Fax: (+817) 478 1048. Email: ehwiig@krii.com
Abstract
Objectives: to obtain normative data for Alzheimer’s Quick Test (AQT) measures of perceptual and cognitive speed from
West African speakers of Krio.
Subjects: normal adults, who were functionally independent, from Sierra Leone (n = 164) aged 25–79 years.
Methods: perceptual and cognitive speed were measured with AQT single- and dual-dimension colour–number (C–N) and
colour–animal (C–A) naming tasks. Tests were administered individually in the participants’ communities.
Results: men and women performed similarly (P>0.05), whereas literate speakers used significantly less time than preliterate
peers (P<0.01). Correlations between age and colour naming were low (P<0.01) and speed decreased by <0.1 s per year.
Dual-dimension naming remained stable across ages. Correlations with years of education were low for dual-dimension naming
(P<0.01) and speed increased 0.4 s per added year. Correlations between age and education and AQT naming were non-sig-
nificant for literate participants. Criterion time cut-offs (seconds) for screening were developed for preliterate and literate
speakers of Krio for typical (<+1 SD), slower-than-typical (between +1 and +2 SD) and atypical (>+2 SD) performance.
Conclusion: AQT C–N and C–A naming are time efficient (3–5 min each), objective and reliable and can be administered in
Krio to West African adults in Africa, Europe or North America to screen for cognitive impairments and facilitate referral
for medical workup.
Keywords: cognitive processing speed, attention/executive, set shifting, screening criteria, elderly
Introduction
With rapid global growth in elderly populations, it is import-
ant to find behavioural measures to differentiate normal
ageing from cognitive impairments caused by disease proc-
esses. This is challenging, as normal performance varies
because of hereditary, developmental, educational or envi-
ronmental factors [1, 2]. There is limited information about
normal cognitive ageing [3–7] and none for African adults.
This is in spite of the estimates that the African population
over the age of 65 years would grow from 17.7 to 37.9 mil-
lion from 1997 to 2025 [8]. This study provides measures of
normal cognitive ageing in West African Krio speakers,
obtained with simple, objective and reliable processing-
speed tests with broad cultural/linguistic application [9].
Tests of content are typically used to screen for cogni-
tive impairment and decline associated with neurological
conditions. However, they introduce cultural, linguistic
and/or educational biases, when used outside Western cul-
tures for which they were developed. The common meas-
ure, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [10, 11],
shows limitations in differentiating normal function from
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [12, 13]. Thus, MMSE showed spe-
cificity of 100% and sensitivity of 84%, whereas Alzheimer’s
Quick Test (AQT) colour–form (C–F) naming [9] showed
specificity of 97% and sensitivity of 97%. Educated West-
erners readily perform the MMSE tasks, but abilities
required for subtraction, reading and writing might not be
developed in societies where literacy is not generally estab-
lished. It is, therefore, of importance for the developing
societies to identify cognitive-screening measures that are
quick, reliable, objective and cross-culturally applicable, and
can be administered with minimal training. These consider-
ations prompted the collection of normative data for AQT
colour–number (C–N) and colour–animal (C–A) naming [9]
from functionally intact West African speakers of Krio.
AQT assesses processing speed (i.e. time to complete a
controlled-input task) with rapid naming. Processing-speed
tests are sensitive to small changes in time, and older adults
typically use longer time than younger adults [14–16]. Simi-
larly, AQT dual-dimension (C–N and C–A) naming shows
significant, but small, time increases with age for American
adults below the age of 60 years (1 s per decade) and slightly
larger increases after age 60 (1 s per seven years) [5]. Nam-
ing times are not affected by gender [9, 17].
The AQT naming tasks are criterion-referenced for nor-
mal adults in the US, Sweden and Greece, and cut-offs
Age and Ageing Advance Access published June 28, 2006
by guest on June 7, 2013http://ageing.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
N. P. Nielsen and E. H. Wiig
Page 2 of 5
(seconds) for typical, slower-than-typical and atypical nam-
ing speeds are reported [9, 17, 18]. Test–retest reliability is
high, and effects of age on naming are similar for American
and Swedish speakers [9, 17]. Speakers of Greek require a
higher cut-off point (100 s) for atypical C–F naming than
American and Swedish speakers (+30 s) [18].
Sustained naming causes redistribution of cerebral blood
flow. During AQT C–F naming, normal adults show
increased blood flow to temporal–parietal brain regions
bilaterally and suppressed flow to frontal regions compared
with the rest [9, 17, 19, 20]. C–F naming shows high sensi-
tivity (93.1–98.7%) and specificity (96.6–99.9%) and differ-
entiates Swedish and Greek adults with mild-to-moderate
AD from cognitively normal peers [17, 18], and in differen-
tiating AD from dementia with Lewy bodies [21]. AQT
dual-dimension naming assesses attention/executive func-
tions mediated by temporal–parietal activation [22––24].
This study was descriptive and objectives were prag-
matic. They were as follows: (i) to obtain normative data for
AQT C–N and C–A naming from West African speakers of
Krio, (ii) to compare naming times and effects of ageing for
Krio and American speakers and (iii) to develop culturally/
linguistically appropriate criterion cut-offs (seconds) for
typical, slower-than-typical and atypical performance.
Methods
Participants
There were 59 women and 105 men (mean age 44.6 years, SD
9.5). All gave informed consent in accordance with the Decla-
ration of Helsinki. A well-being questionnaire about patterns
of eating, sleeping, working and mood and absence of fever,
neurological disorders, diabetes and HIV screened partici-
pants. Adults with neurological disorders or HIV, or who did
not meet the criteria for well-being, were excluded. Partici-
pants were recruited from the members of the Lutheran com-
munity, and each was paid $5.00 by the authors. All were
natives of Sierra Leone, equally distributed between urban
(Freetown) and rural areas. Participants completed between 2
and 17 years of education (M 10 years, SD 4 years). A total of
45 participants had recurring malaria and 119 were malaria
free. Participants included housewives, day workers, traders
and professionals, who led functionally independent lives in
which they shopped, held jobs and performed societal
responsibilities. Owing to their roles in the society, all were
presumed to function within the normal cognitive range and
to be free of cognitive impairments or dementia that would
interfere with their work or societal functions.
Materials and administration
A trained examiner, native of Sierra Leone, administered the
tests individually in Krio, the lingua franca for trading and
social interactions, which blends English, French and Por-
tuguese. Participants completed testing in one session in
their community settings, and short trials established ade-
quacy of naming test stimuli.
AQT C–N and C–A each consists of three tests. Test 1
requires naming of 40 squares (e.g. black, blue, red or
yellow) and Test 2 naming either 40 numbers (e.g. 2, 4, 5 and
7) or animals (e.g. bird, cat, fish, rat, snake and spider) ren-
dered in black. Tests 1 and 2 measure perceptual speed (i.e.
perception + response time). Test 3 requires naming of 40 C–
N (e.g. red circle and blue square) or C–A (e.g. yellow fish and
black spider) combinations and measures cognitive speed (i.e.
perceptual speed + cognitive overhead). Test–retest reliability
(r) is high for C–N (0.91) and C–A naming (0.96).
Naming times for each test were measured digitally,
beginning at voice onset, and recorded in seconds and in
fractions of seconds. Data from 144 American speakers
with a mean age of 39.6 years (SD 15.9) were used for com-
parison. One-way ANOVA indicated no significant differ-
ences in age between Krio and American speakers (F =
3.20; P>0.05).
Statistical analyses
Naming times (seconds) were used in the analyses for the
total group (n = 164), men (n = 105) and women (n = 59),
preliterate (n = 41) and literate (n = 123) speakers and
speakers with (n = 45) and without (n = 119) malaria. The
analyses (SPSS Version 11.0.2 for Macintosh) produced
descriptive statistics, one-way and univariate ANOVA com-
parisons, correlations (r) and linear regression coefficients
(b). Measures of variability (SD) and cumulative frequencies
were used to establish criterion cut-off times (seconds).
Results
Background variables
AQT naming times (seconds) for Krio speakers are summa-
rised in Table 1. Single-dimension naming times are about
one-third shorter than the dual-dimension times. Means for
men and women did not differ significantly for colour,
number, animal or C–A naming (F = 0.96–2.37; P>0.05).
C–N naming was significantly faster among women than
men (F = 6.55; P<0.01).
Mean time differences between (i) preliterate (n = 41)
and literate adults (n = 123) and (ii) adults with (n = 45) and
without malaria (n = 119) were tested. One-way ANOVA
with literacy as a factor (i.e. education above age 15/Grade
8) [25] indicates significantly shorter C–N (M = 55.30, SD
20.37) and C–A (M = 76.14, SD 23.52) times for literate
than for preliterate speakers (M = 73.10, SD 17.67; 82.98,
SD 14.36) (F = 8.19 and 12.01; P<0.01). Naming times by
literate Krio (n = 123) and American speakers (n = 144) were
compared. Means (seconds) for Americans were as follows:
colour—20.91 (SD 3.31), number—14.26 (SD 2.72), C–
N—41.04 (SD 6.75), animal—25.74 (SD 3.75) and C–A—
48.29 (SD 7.18). All differences were significant (P<0.01)
(F = 43.09, 21.65, 11.08, 44.30 and 36.85). Univariate
ANOVA with age and years of education as independent
variables tested interaction effects. For C–N, age was not
significant (F = 1.32), whereas education (F = 2.24;
P<0.05) and the age–education interaction were significant
(F = 0.81; P>0.05). For C–A, the effects for age (F = 1.52),
education (F = 1.23) and interaction (F = 1.11) were all
non-significant (P>0.05).
by guest on June 7, 2013http://ageing.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
AQT cognitive screening criteria
Page 3 of 5
Mean differences between speakers with (n = 45) and
without (n = 119) malaria were significant (P<0.05) for col-
our, C–N and C–A naming (F = 5.66, 20.63 and 9.50).
Adults with malaria completed 12.0 (SD 2.5) and adults
without malaria completed 9.8 (SD 3.7) years of education,
which is a significant difference (F = 11.67; P<0.01).
Age and education effects
In the total group (n = 164), correlations (r) between age
and colour and animal naming were significant (0.23
and 0.17; P<0.05), whereas correlations with number,
C–N and C–A were not (0.09, 0.15 and 0.03; P>0.05).
Significant relationships between age and naming times
were analysed using linear regression (Table 2). Coeffi-
cients for age effects for colour and animal naming indi-
cated increases (slowing of speed) of 0.05 and 0.08 s per
year. Age accounted for 5% of the variance in colour and
3% in animal naming time.
Correlations (r) were significant between education and
colour, C–N and C–A naming (0.19, 0.38 and 0.28;
P<0.05), whereas correlations with number and animal nam-
ing were not (0.03 and 0.13; P>0.05). Linear regression
(Table 2) indicated decreases in time (increased speed) of 0.14
s for colour, 0.43 s for C–N and 0.46 s for C–A naming per
added year of education. The relationships were described by
the following equations: C–N = 82.96 + (2.25) (education)
and C–A = 94.97 + (1.67) (education). Education
accounted for 3% of the variance in colour, 14% in C–N and
8% in C–A naming time. In the previous research, education
after the age of 15 years (Grade 8) did not affect AQT dual-
dimension naming times [1, 19]. Therefore, correlations (r)
were calculated between years of education and C–N and C–
A naming for literate Krio speakers (n =123). There was no
significant correlation for C–N (r = 0.14; P>0.05), but there
was for C–A naming (r = 0.29; P<0.01).
Criterion cut-offs
Normative ranges for typical, slower-than-typical and atypi-
cal naming times for literate (n = 123) and preliterate (n =
41) Krio speakers were developed by previously employed
methods [9, 17] (Table 3). Times (seconds) shorter than +1
SD above the mean indicated typical, longer than +2 SD
above the mean indicated atypical (potentially pathological)
and between +1 and +2 SD indicated slower-than-typical
performance. Cut-off times were adjusted to lend parsi-
mony and ease of reference for screening.
Cut-off criteria for literate Krio speakers were compared
with criteria for literate American and Swedish speakers for
C–N (typical <50 s; slower 51–59 s; atypical >60 s) and C–
A naming (typical <60 s; slower 61–69 s; atypical <70 s) [9,
17]. This indicated a difference in cut-off criteria of 25–30 s
for C–N and 40–50 s for C–A naming.
Discussion
Normative naming times were obtained from Krio speakers
for AQT C–N and C–A naming, and criterion cut-offs were
developed for potential cognitive screening. C–F naming,
explored in clinical research [9, 13, 17–20], was not adminis-
tered because of rare use of names for geometric forms in West
African daily life. The study assumed a macro-perspective
Table 1. AQT means and standard deviations for the total group of West African speakers of Krio and subgroups based on
gender, literacy and malaria
*One-way ANOVA F values for the comparison significant at P<0.01. Statistical differences between subgroups and F values are reported in the results section.
....................................................................................................................................
.
Measure [Mean (SD)]
Groups Colour Number Colour–number Animal Colour–animal
.................................................................................................................................................................................
T
otal (n = 164) 30.73 (6.09) 19.48 (5.72) 59.39 (21.06) 37.60 (9.20) 77.80 (21.72)
Men (n = 105) 30.99 (5.75) 19.06 (5.81) 62.56* (21.29) 38.25 (9.80) 79.22 (20.73)
W
omen (n = 59) 29.98 (7.10) 20.24 (5.54) 54.00* (19.84) 36.12 (7.62) 74.51 (23.62)
Preliterate (n = 41) 30.71 (5.83) 17.59 (3.57) 73.10* (17.67) 39.83 (10.65) 82.98* (14.36)
Literate (n = 123) 30.49 (6.50) 20.05 (6.18) 55.30* (20.37) 37.07 (8.74) 76.14* (23.52)
Malaria (n = 45) 28.76 (7.28) 19.38 (6.71) 48.04* (12.84) 36.18 (8.18) 69.27* (26.63)
Free from malaria (n = 119) 31.32 (5.69) 19.50 (5.32) 63.87* (21.96) 38.12 (9.51) 80.74* (18.89)
Table 2. Linear relationships between age and years of education and AQT naming times (seconds) for Krio speakers (n = 164)
V
ariable Naming time Constant B
0
Coefficient B
1
P r r-squared
.................................................................................................................................................................................
A
ge Color 23.97 0.15 <0.01 0.23 0.05
Animal 45.10 0.17 <0.05 0.17 0.03
Education Color 34.28 0.34 <0.05 0.19 0.03
Colour–number 82.96 2.25 <0.01 0.38 0.15
Colour–animal 94.97 1.67 <0.01 0.28 0.08
Table 3. Criterion cut-off times for typical, slower-than-
typical and atypical performance for preliterate and literate
adult speakers of Krio
Group AQT test Typical Slower-than-typical Atypical
.....................................................................................
Preliterate Colour–number <90 s 91–109 s >110 s
Colour–animal <110 s 111–129 s >130 s
Literate Colour–number <75 s 76–94 s >95 s
Colour–animal <100 s 101–124 s >125 s
by guest on June 7, 2013http://ageing.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
N. P. Nielsen and E. H. Wiig
Page 4 of 5
where dual-dimension naming times were considered sympto-
matic of broader attention/executive functions (e.g. attention,
working memory and set shifting) [20, 22–24].
Women named the C–N stimuli on average 10 s faster
than men, a counterintuitive finding, as men tend to be the
traders. More importantly, men and women used similar
times for dual-dimension naming, concurring with the find-
ings for American and Swedish speakers [1, 5, 17]. Age did
not significantly affect dual-dimension naming either.
Education (literacy) influenced C–N, but not C–A nam-
ing speed. This seems appropriate, as colour naming and
number naming are part of the curriculum in West African
schools, and longer exposure should result in greater cogni-
tive speed. Interactions between age and education were not
significant for dual-dimension naming. As a group, Krio
speakers were significantly slower at naming both single-
and dual-dimension stimuli than American speakers [5].
Even literate Krio speakers used considerably longer time
than literate Americans to name stimulus combinations.
Age and education
Associations between age and naming times were low and
only reached significance for perceptual speed measures.
Age accounted for less variance in colour naming for Krio
(5%) than for American speakers (8%) [5]. Associations
between years of education and dual-dimension naming
were low, but significant, and speed increased by about 2 s
for each added year of education. Education accounted for
relatively large proportions of the variance in C–N and C–A
naming (15 and 8%, respectively).
The association between education and naming time was
evident in adults with and without malaria. Adults with
malaria, and more advanced education, used on average 10 s
less for C–N and 20 s less for C–A naming than malaria-free
adults. This supports that achievement of literacy results in
greater cognitive flexibility. In the literate group, there was
no significant association between education and C–N nam-
ing. This concurs with the findings that education past
established literacy has little effect on dual-dimension nam-
ing among American and Swedish adults [9, 17].
Implications for screening
Criterion cut-offs for AQT naming are identical for Ameri-
can and Swedish speakers [9, 17, 18, 20]. Because Krio
shares features of English in content and structure, similari-
ties in naming times would be expected. However, criterion
cut-offs for the lower limits of atypical performance for
Krio speakers are more than 30 s longer than cut-offs for
American speakers [9]. The difference may reflect cultural
traditions for rate of speech in West Africa or other uncon-
trolled factors.
Speed, ease of administration and equity of access are
desirable screening-test qualities in societies with limited
education. The advantages of using C–N over C–A naming
with West African Krio speakers were therefore considered
based on variability, biases and expressed preferences. C–N
and C–A naming showed similar variability, but the majority
preferred C–N to C–A naming. Although C–N or C–A
naming may be used alone for screening, best clinical
practice suggests administering both the tasks (5–7 min total)
until clinical utility data become available. We also suggest
repeating administration of dual-dimension tests with atypi-
cal naming times (>+2 SD) for consistency and validation.
Results suggest that AQT C–N and C–A can be used for
first-line screening of Krio speakers in West Africa, Europe
or North America to identify adults, who may require fur-
ther evaluation for cognitive impairment and/or dementia.
The tests are not intended for diagnostic purposes, because
they are sensitive to cognitive impairments resulting from a
range of neurological and psychiatric disorders. If naming
time consistently falls in the atypical range, referral for med-
ical workup is recommended. If naming time falls within the
slower-than-typical range, periodic screening is recom-
mended to identify if significant reductions in cognitive
speed (+15 s or more), which may be indicative of a pro-
gressive disorder, have occurred.
Current health priorities in West Africa concern individ-
uals who are HIV positive. Growth in the elderly popula-
tion will, however, demand attention to detecting cognitive
impairments, resulting from dementia or other causes.
While prescription medications for AD are unavailable to
most West Africans, the cultures have long histories of
treating illness, including memory loss, with herbal constitu-
ents and compounds, justifying early identification.
Key points
Normative data for AQT C–N and C–A naming were
collected from 164 functionally intact speakers of Krio in
Sierra Leone.
Naming speed was significantly faster for literate than for
preliterate speakers.
Age had no significant effect on dual-dimension naming
times.
Years of education affected dual-dimension naming
times in the total group, but not in the group of literate
speakers.
Criterion cut-offs were developed separately for screen-
ing literate and preliterate West African speakers of Krio.
Conflicts of interest
None.
Acknowledgements
We express our gratitude to the Evangelical Lutheran
Church of Sierra Leone, its staff and members. We are espe-
cially indebted to Bishop Tom Barnett for supporting the
study and to the Reverend Marie Barnett for collecting the
data. This research received no third party funding.
References
1. Institute for the Study of Aging & International Longevity
Center-USA. Achieving and maintaining cognitive vitality with
aging: workshop report. New York: ISOA & ILC-USA, 2001.
by guest on June 7, 2013http://ageing.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
AQT cognitive screening criteria
Page 5 of 5
2. Salthouse TA. Theoretical Perspectives on Cognitive Aging.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1991.
3. Christensen H, Kumar R. Cognitive changes and the ageing
brain. In: Sachdev PS, ed. The Ageing Brain: The Neurobiol-
ogy and Neuropsychiatry of Ageing. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger;
2003, 75–95.
4. Fleischman DA, Wilson RS, Gabrieli JDE et al. A longitudinal
study of implicit and explicit memory in old persons. Psychol
Aging 2004; 4: 617–25.
5. Jacobson JM, Nielsen NP, Minthon L et al. Multiple rapid
automatic naming measures of cognition: normal performance
and effects of aging. Percept Mot Skills 2004; 98: 739–53.
6. Price L, Said K, Haaland KY. Age-associated memory impair-
ment of logical memory and visual reproduction. J Clin Exp
Neuropsychol 2004; 26: 531–38.
7. Small SA, Stern Y, Tang M et al. Selective decline in mem-
ory function among healthy elderly. Neurology 1999; 52:
1392–6.
8. Cumings JL.The Neuropsychiatry of Alzheimer’s Disease and
Related Dementias. London: Taylor and Francis, 2003.
9. Wiig EH, Nielsen NP, Minthon L, Warkentin S. Alzheimer’s
Quick Test: Assessment of Parietal Function (AQT). San
Antonio (TX): Harcourt/PsychCorp, 2002.
10. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a
practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for
the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12: 189–98.
11. Molloy D, Alemayehu E, Roberts R. Reliability of a standard-
ized mini-mental state examination compared with the tradi-
tional mini-mental state examination. Am J Psychiatry 1991;
148: 102–5.
12. Burns A, Lawlor B, Craig S. Rating scales in old age psychiatry.
Br J Psychiatry 2002; 180: 161–7.
13. Nielsen NP, Wiig EH, Minthon L, Warkentin S. Clinical utility
of colour-form naming in Alzheimer’s disease: Preliminary
evidence. Percept Mot Skills 2004; 99: 1201–4.
14. Goetz DW, Jacobson JM, Murnane JE et al. (1989). Prolonga-
tion of simple and choice reaction times in a double-blind
comparison of twice-daily hydroxyzine versus terfenadine. J
Allergy Clin Immunol 1989; 84: 316–22.
15. Strauss E, Loring D, Chelune G et al. Predicting cognitive
impairment in epilepsy: Findings from the Bozeman epilepsy
consortium. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1995; 17: 909–17.
16. Vendrell P, Junque C, Pujol J et al. The role of prefrontal
regions in the Stroop task. Neuropsychologia 1995; 33: 341–52.
17. Wiig EH, Nielsen NP, Minthon L, Warkentin S. Alzheimer’s
Quick Test: Assessment of Parietal Function. Svensk Version &
Norsk Versjon. Stockholm, Sweden: Psykologiførlaget, 2003.
18. Warkentin S, Tsantali E, Kiosseoglou G et al. The AQT as a
useful short screening test for dementia: Evidence from two
European cultures. Alzheimer’s Assoc Int Conf on Prevention
of Dementia. Washington, DC, 2005.
19. Warkentin S.
AQS assessment of temporo–parietal lobe func-
tion. Scientific Presentation. Euro Union Consortium on
Alzheimer’s Disease. Liege, Belgium, 2003.
20. Wiig EH, Nielsen NP, Minthon L et al. Parietal lobe activation
in rapid, automatized naming by adults. Percept Mot Skills
2002; 94: 1230–44.
21. Londos E, Warkentin S, Minthon L. “AQT” – a measure of
cognitive speed – is a useful tool in the diagnosis of dementia
with Lewy bodies. 9th Internat Psychogeriat Assoc Conf.
Stockholm, Sweden, 2005.
22. Baddeley A, Bressi S, Della Sala S et al. The decline of working
memory in Alzheimer’s disease: a longitudinal study. Brain
1991; 114: 2521–42.
23. Fry AF, Hale S. Processing speed, working memory, and fluid
intelligence: evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychol Sci
1996; 7: 237–41.
24. Fockert JW, Rees G, Rith CD et al. The role of working mem-
ory in selective visual attention. Science 2001; 291: 1803–6.
25. The World Factbook 2003. Superintendent of Documents,
Pittsburgh, PA.
Received 2 December 2005; accepted in revised form 25 April
2006
by guest on June 7, 2013http://ageing.oxfordjournals.org/Downloaded from
... A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT) was later designed by Wiig et al. to compare processing speed in adults with clinical diagnoses of dementia and neurotypical age peers [18][19][20][21]. AQT is a visual-verbal processing speed test that evaluates aspects of executive function and can be used in a variety of languages and cultures [5,16,22]. AQT measures the speed of perception, retrieval, and naming of basic colors and forms in single-dimension naming and cognitive speed associated with central executive functions (attention, working memory, and set shifting) in dual-dimension naming of color-form combinations. The study showed that a decline in the speed of perception and cognition precedes a decline in linguistic-cognitive abilities in mild to moderate severity of AD [10]. ...
... Gender and education do not affect AQT. Nielsen et al. (2006) indicated no difference in time measures between the men and women, but the AQT time measures were shorter for literate than for illiterate old people [22]. In this study, AQT time was faster in higher education but was not statistically significant. ...
... Gender and education do not affect AQT. Nielsen et al. (2006) indicated no difference in time measures between the men and women, but the AQT time measures were shorter for literate than for illiterate old people [22]. In this study, AQT time was faster in higher education but was not statistically significant. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Cognitive disorders are one of the important issues in old age. There are many cognitive tests, but some variables affect their results (e.g., age and education). This study aimed to evaluate the reliability and validity of A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT) in screening for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia. Methods This is a psychometric properties study. 115 older adults participated in the study and were divided into three groups (46 with MCI, 24 with dementia, and 45 control) based on the diagnosis of two geriatric psychiatrists. Participants were assessed by AQT and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Data were analyzed using Pearson correlation, independent t-test, and ROC curve by SPSS v.23. Results There was no significant correlation between AQT subscales and age and no significant difference between the AQT subscales in sex, educational levels. The test-retest correlations ranges were 0.84 from 097. Concurrent validity was significant between MMSE and AQT. Its correlation was with Color − 0.78, Form − 0.71, and Color-Form − 0.72. The cut-off point for Color was 43.50 s, Form 52 s, and Color-Form 89 s were based on sensitivity and specificity for differentiating older patients with MCI with controls. The cut-off point for Color was 62.50 s, for Form 111 s, and Color-Form 197.50 s based on sensitivity and specificity measures for differentiating older patients with dementia and MCI. Conclusion The findings showed that AQT is a suitable tool for screening cognitive function in older adults.
... There are virtually no AQT practice effects, test-retest reliability is high (Humes et al., 2013) and cultural bias reportedly low (Dsurney, 2007;Takahashi et al., 2012;Wiig et al., 2002a) although differences in syllable length across languages, and thereby naming speeds, motivate language-specific norms (Wiig & Al-Halees, 2013). Literacy is not required even if it facilitates performance (Nielsen & Wiig, 2006) and correlations with educational levels beyond 8 years of schooling are typically low, but not in all samples (Nielsen & Wiig, 2006;Petrazzuoli et al., 2014;Takahashi et al., 2012;Wiig & Al-Halees, 2013). ...
... There are virtually no AQT practice effects, test-retest reliability is high (Humes et al., 2013) and cultural bias reportedly low (Dsurney, 2007;Takahashi et al., 2012;Wiig et al., 2002a) although differences in syllable length across languages, and thereby naming speeds, motivate language-specific norms (Wiig & Al-Halees, 2013). Literacy is not required even if it facilitates performance (Nielsen & Wiig, 2006) and correlations with educational levels beyond 8 years of schooling are typically low, but not in all samples (Nielsen & Wiig, 2006;Petrazzuoli et al., 2014;Takahashi et al., 2012;Wiig & Al-Halees, 2013). ...
... No gender effects have been reported (Nielsen & Wiig, 2006;Petrazzuoli et al., 2014;Subirana-Mirete et al., 2014;Wiig et al., 2007) but age has consistently been associated with decreasing AQT performance, as would be expected in a measure of processing speed. The effect has been described as small, in the range of 1 to 6 s/decade depending on task and sample (Nielsen & Wiig, 2006;Subirana-Mirete et al., 2014;Wiig et al., 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
Slowed processing speed is part of normal aging but also a symptom of many diseases, including dementia. A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT) consists of three conditions: color naming (AQT1), form naming (AQT2) and dual color-form naming (AQT3) and offers a user-friendly assessment of processing speed that is used internationally to identify cognitive impairment in elderly patients. Appropriate age-norms have however been lacking. This study provides regression-based norms derived from a Swedish sample of 158 cognitively healthy 80 to 94-year olds. The results show age effects in all three conditions, a non-linear education effect in AQT1, and age by gender interactions in AQT2 and AQT3: men performed worse with increasing age, but women remained on a par. However, irrespective of age and gender, AQT2 and AQT3 mean raw and predicted scores were slower than the hitherto recommended cutoff criteria for suspected cognitive impairment.
... A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed has shown a high test-retest reliability (r = 0.84 to 0.96) with no significant gender differences or differences caused by years of education after achieving literacy (grades 8 and above; Wiig et al., 2002;Wiig et al., 2003;Jacobson et al., 2004;Nielsen and Wiig, 2006). Previous studies have shown that AQT times correlate positively with age, which is in agreement with the fact that aging is associated with slower cognitive speed (Jacobson et al., 2004;Nielsen and Wiig, 2006;Wiig et al., 2007). ...
... A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed has shown a high test-retest reliability (r = 0.84 to 0.96) with no significant gender differences or differences caused by years of education after achieving literacy (grades 8 and above; Wiig et al., 2002;Wiig et al., 2003;Jacobson et al., 2004;Nielsen and Wiig, 2006). Previous studies have shown that AQT times correlate positively with age, which is in agreement with the fact that aging is associated with slower cognitive speed (Jacobson et al., 2004;Nielsen and Wiig, 2006;Wiig et al., 2007). Criterion cut-off times (in seconds) for typical (less than +1.0 SD), slower-than-typical (between +1.0 and +2.0 SD), and atypical/pathological performance (greater than +2 SD) were identical for English and Swedish languages (Wiig et al., 2002;2003). ...
... In this study, we obtained normative namingtimes for AQT processing-speed tests used to assess cognitive function from a culturally and linguistically representative sample of cognitively healthy Italian primary care patients. Age proved to have a significant effect on processing speed and this concurs with previous findings (Jacobson et al., 2004;Nielsen and Wiig, 2006;Wiig et al., 2007). Years of education also correlated significantly but weakly with AQT naming times, a finding previously unreported. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT) is a brief test that can identify cognitive impairment. AQT has been validated in Arabic, English, Greek, Japanese, Norwegian, Spanish, and Swedish. The aim of this study was to develop Italian criterion-referenced norms for AQT. Methods AQT consists of three test plates where the patient shall rapidly name (1) the color of 40 blue, red, yellow, or black squares (AQT color), (2) the form of 40 black figures (circles, squares, triangles, or rectangles; AQT form), (3) the color and form of 40 figures (consisting of previous colors and forms; AQT color–form). The AQT test was administered to 121 Italian cognitively healthy primary care patients (age range: 45–90 years). Their mean Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score was 28.8 ± 0.9 points (range 26–30 points). AQT naming times in seconds were used for developing preliminary criterion cut-off times for different age groups. Results Age was found to have a significant moderate positive correlation with AQT naming times color (r = 0.65, p < 0.001), form (r = 0.53, p < 0.001), color–form (r = 0.63, p < 0.001) and a moderate negative correlation with MMSE score (r = –0.44, p < 0.001) and AQT naming times differed significantly between younger (45–55 years old), older (56–70 years old), and the oldest (71–90 years old) participants. Years of education correlated positively but weakly with MMSE score (r = 0.27, p = 0.003) and negatively but weakly with AQT color (r = –0.16, p = ns), form (r = –0.24, p = 0.007), and color–form (r = –0.19, p = 0.005). We established preliminary cut-off times for the AQT test based on +1 and +2 standard deviations according to the approach in other languages and settings. Conclusions This is the first Italian normative AQT study. Future studies of AQT – a test useful for dementia screening in primary care – will eventually refine cut-off times for normality balancing sensitivity and specificity in cognitive diagnostics.
... A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT) is a user-friendly and quickly administered test of processing speed, attention and executive function. 12 AQT is not affected by education and minimally affected by age, 13 and could thus serve as a complement to MMSE during the preoperative evaluation. In addition, depression has recently been suggested to be associated with delirium after cardiac surgery, 14 and screening for preoperative depressive symptoms can be done by The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS). ...
... 12 AQT's age effects are limited and AQT is not affected by education level. 13 Our results showed that patients with postoperative delirium indeed had a slower preoperative test performance and that AQT color and form >70 seconds was independently associated with a higher risk of postoperative delirium. However, the ROC curve showed an AUC of 0.605, meaning that AQT did not provide a greater accuracy in predicting postoperative delirium compared to MMSE. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: To evaluate if preoperative assessment with A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT) could increase the accuracy of predicting delirium after cardiac surgery compared to Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), and examine if a composite of variables, including cognitive function and depressive symptoms, could be useful to predict delirium. Patients and methods: Cardiac surgery was performed in 218 patients (mean age 72 years). Preoperative evaluation involved AQT, MMSE and Hospital Anxiety And Depression Scale (HADS). Postoperative delirium was assessed using Nursing Delirium Screening Scale (Nu-DESC) and Confusion Assessment Method-ICU (CAM-ICU). Logistic regression was performed to detect predictors of postoperative delirium and receiver operator characteristic curves (ROC) with area under the curve (AUC) to determine the accuracy. Results: Postoperative delirium occurred in 47 patients (22%) who had lower MMSE scores (median (range), 27 (19-30) vs 28 (20-30), p=0.009) and slower AQT (median (range), 76 (48-181) vs 70 (40-182) seconds, p=0.030) than patients without delirium. Predictive power measured as AUC (95% CI) was 0.605 (0.51-0.70) for AQT and 0.623 (0.53-0.72) for MMSE. Logistic regression (OR, 95% CI) showed MMSE <27 points (2.72, 1.27-5.86), AQT >70 sec (2.26, 1.03-4.95), HADS-D >4 points (2.60, 1.21-5.58) and longer cardiopulmonary bypass-time (1.007, 1.002-1.013) to be associated with postoperative delirium. Combining these parameters yielded an AUC of 0.736 (0.65-0.82). Conclusion: The ability of predicting delirium using AQT was similar to MMSE, and only slightly higher by combining AQT and MMSE. Adding HADS-D and cardiopulmonary bypass-time to MMSE and AQT increased the predictive power to a borderline acceptable discriminatory value. Preoperative cognitive tests and screening for depressive symptoms may help identify patients at risk of postoperative delirium. Yet, there is still a need to establish useful preoperative tests.
... The test is not affected by gender and only weakly with education (109). AQT has been shown to be affected by age to a small extent in normative studies, with longer time correlated with older age (110,111). In one study linear regression indicated a prolonged speed processing time with 2 to 4 seconds (depending on the task) per decade (112). ...
... MMSE is the most widely used diagnostic test but the test results are influenced by age, education and ethnicity, and the accuracy has been questioned (125,126). The brief test AQT is affected minimally by age and not at all by gender or education (111). AQT is relevant for PHC dementia diagnostic assessment, and so far, PHC data is scarce. ...
... The only signi cant correlation between age and AQT measures occurred for Form naming among old patients with dementia. In comparison, a study by Nielsen et al. (2006) indicated no difference in time measures between the two sexes, but the AQT time measures were shorter for literate than for illiterate old people [18]. In a study that evaluated the relationship between the AQT measures and neuropsychological test scores, no relationship was found between age and AQT naming time [8]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: Cognitive disorders are one of the most important issues in old age. They may remain hidden in the early stages. There are many cognitive tests, but some variables affect their results (e.g., age and education.) This study aimed to evaluate the reliability and validity of A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT) in Iranian older adults. Methods: This study aimed to test the psychometric properties of AQT. 114 older adults participated in the study and were divided into three groups (46 with mild cognitive impairments (MCI), 24 with dementia, and 45 without MCI and dementia) based on the diagnosis of two geriatric psychiatrists. Participants were assessed by AQT and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Data were analyzed using Pearson correlation, independent t-test, and ROC curve by SPSS v.23. Results: There was no significant correlation between AQT subscales and age and no significant difference between the AQT subscales in male and female, educational levels, and marital status. The test-retest correlations (r) were significant for Color (C) 0.84, Form (F) 0.91 and Color-Form (CF) 0.94. Convergent validity was significant between MMSE and AQT. Its correlation was with Color -0.78, Form -0.71, and Color-Form -0.72. The cut-off point for Color was 43.50 s, Form 52 s, and Color-Form 89 s were based on sensitivity and specificity for differentiating older patients with MCI with controls. The cut-off point for Color was 62.50 s, for Form 111 s, and Color-Form 197.50 s based on sensitivity and specificity measures for differentiating older patients with dementia and MCI. Conclusion: The findings of this study showed that A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT) is a suitable tool for assessing cognitive function in older adults.
... The possible effects of education became relevant in a study of West-African speakers of Krio due to the wide variation in years of education (range 2 to 17 years) in the sample (Nielsen & Wiig, 2006) (Table 3). ANOVA with literacy as a factor (i.e. , education above age IS/Grade 8) indicated significantly shorter naming times for color-number and color-animal naming by the literate (n = 123) than by the preliterate or illiterate adults (n = 41) (p< .01). ...
Article
Cognitive speed, defined as perceptual speed plus cognitive overhead resulting from demands on attention, working memory, and set shifting, is required for real-time performance in many domains, including reading and driving. Societal and technological changes and globalization place increasingly greater demands on real-time performance as part of societal expectations. This chapter introduces A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT), a cognitive processing-speed test for adolescents and adults and integrates the clinical research completed to date. The design of AQT is first compared with that of other commonly used processingspeed tests (e.g., Stroop Color-Word Test, Trail-Making-Test) to delineate differences in design, outcomes and applications. As a background for the clinical research with AQT, we introduce the theoretical bases for using processing speed to measure the cognitive status of adults, who are aging normally and adults with cognitive impairments that may indicate the onset of dementia. Relationships between AQT cognitive-speed measures and cortical and subcortical activation registered by regional cortical blood flow (rCBF) and functional MRI imaging are presented for normal adults. In normally aging adults, functional imagining during AQT color-form naming indicates that the occipitaltemporal- parietal regions are activated bilaterally with concurrent suppression of activation in the frontal regions of the brain. Regional cortical blood flow (rCBF) images are also presented for adults with mild-to-moderate Alzheimer's disease. In adults with mild-moderate Alzheimer's disease (AD), cognitive speed is significantly reduced and temporal-parietal cortical activation suppressed bilaterally. In normal child development, cognitive speed increases monotonically with age between 5 and 13-15 years of age, when there is a leveling off. Cognitive speed is reduced in developmental disorders such as specific language impairments (SLI) and ADHD. In normal aging cognitive speed is remarkably well preserved between ages 15 and 95 and decreases by only about 1 second per decade. The statistical characteristics of the AQT color, form and color-form naming tests indicate high test-retest reliability and high sensitivity and specificity values. Performances on the color-form naming tests are moderately associated with WAIS-III Performance IQ and MMSE in neuropsychiatric patients. The AQT processing-speed measures differentiate Alzheimer's disease and dementia with Lewy bodies in the early stages and have been used to monitor treatment of Alzheimer's patients longitudinally. In conclusion, we introduce a screening model for establishing individual cognitive-speed baselines in midlife for potential lifetime monitoring of changes in cognitive status.
... The possible effects of education became relevant in a study of West-African speakers of Krio due to the wide variation in years of education (range 2 to 17 years) in the sample (Nielsen & Wiig, 2006) (Table 3). ANOVA with literacy as a factor (i.e., education above age 15/Grade 8) indicated significantly shorter naming times for color-number and color-animal naming by the literate (n = 123) than by the preliterate or illiterate adults (n = 41) (p< .01). ...
Article
Cognitive speed, defined as perceptual speed plus cognitive overhead resulting from demands on attention, working memory, and set shifting, is required for real-time performance in many domains, including reading and driving. Societal and technological changes and globalization place increasingly greater demands on real-time performance as part of societal expectations. This chapter introduces A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed (AQT), a cognitive processing-speed test for adolescents and adults and integrates the clinical research completed to date. The design of AQT is first compared with that of other commonly used processingspeed tests (e.g., Stroop Color-Word Test, Trail-Making-Test) to delineate differences in design, outcomes and applications. As a background for the clinical research with AQT, we introduce the theoretical bases for using processing speed to measure the cognitive status of adults, who are aging normally and adults with cognitive impairments that may indicate the onset of dementia. Relationships between AQT cognitive-speed measures and cortical and subcortical activation registered by regional cortical blood flow (rCBF) and functional MRI imaging are presented for normal adults. In normally aging adults, functional imagining during AQT color-form naming indicates that the occipitaltemporal-parietal regions are activated bilaterally with concurrent suppression of activation in the frontal regions of the brain. Regional cortical blood flow (rCBF) images are also presented for adults with mild-to-moderate Alzheimer's disease. In adults with mild-moderate Alzheimer's disease (AD), cognitive speed is significantly reduced and temporal-parietal cortical activation suppressed bilaterally. In normal child development, cognitive speed increases monotonically with age between 5 and 13-15 years of age, when there is a leveling off. Cognitive speed is reduced in developmental disorders such as specific language impairments (SLI) and ADHD. In normal aging cognitive speed is remarkably well preserved between ages 15 and 95 and decreases by only about 1 second per decade. The statistical characteristics of the AQT color, form and color-form naming tests indicate high test-retest reliability and high sensitivity and specificity values. Performances on the color-form naming tests are moderately associated with WAIS-III Performance IQ and MMSE in neuropsychiatric patients. The AQT processing-speed measures differentiate Alzheimer's disease and dementia with Lewy bodies in the early stages and have been used to monitor treatment of Alzheimer's patients longitudinally. In conclusion, we introduce a screening model for establishing individual cognitive-speed baselines in midlife for potential lifetime monitoring of changes in cognitive status.
... Data presented show that a larger proportion in each age group had completed less than eight years of formal education. Previous studies indicate that years of education is not a signifi cant factor after Grade 8 or attainment of basic literacy, but is a nifi cant factor among lesser educated adults ( Nielsen & Wiig, 2006 ; Wiig & Al-Halees, 2013). Cognitive normality was checked via the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE: Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975 ). ...
Article
Full-text available
-A Quick Test of Cognitive Speed was administered to 357 participants without cognitive impairment, aged 18 to 85 years, to explore the effects of age on processing speed variables in Spanish speakers and to provide normative data for the test adapted to this population. Results were consistent with previous findings: correlations between age and naming times were high and statistically significant. Linear regression indicated that cognitive processing speed on this test slows 2 to 4 sec. per decade, depending on the task. Normalized data were provided. The findings concur with several studies that have linked age-cognitive impairment with slowing processing speed. This study attempted to assess the importance of this relation, as information processing speed could be considered a measure of cognitive impairment in everyday clinical screening evaluations.
Article
Full-text available
A previous study (Baddeley et al., 1986) explored the hypothesis that patients suffering from dementia of the Alzheimer type (AD) are particularly impaired in the functioning of the central executive component of working memory. It showed that, when patients are required to perform 2 concurrent tasks simultaneously, the AD patients are particularly impaired, even when level of performance on the individual tasks is equated with that of age-matched controls. Although the results were clear, interpretation was still complicated by 2 issues: first, the question of comparability of performance on the separate tests between AD and control patients: secondly, the question of whether our results could be interpreted simply in terms of a limited general processing capacity being more taxed by more difficult dual tasks than by the individual tasks performed alone. The present study followed up the AD and control patients after 6 and 12 mths. We were able to allow for the problem of comparability of performance by using patients as their own control. Under these conditions, there is a very clear tendency for dual task performance to deteriorate while single task performance is maintained. A second experiment varied difficulty within a single task in which patients and controls were required to categorize words as belonging to 1, 2 or 4 semantic categories. There was a clear effect of number of categories on performance and a systematic decline in performance over time. There was, however, no interaction between task difficulty as measured by number of alternatives and rate of deterioration, suggesting that the progressive deterioration in performance shown by AD patients is a function of whether single or dual task performance is required, and is not dependent on simple level of task difficulty. Implications for the analysis of the central executive component of working memory are discus
Article
Processing speed, working memory capacity, and fluid intelligence were assessed in a large sample (N = 214) of children, adolescents, and young adults (ages 7 to 19 years) Results of path analyses revealed that almost half of the age-related increase in fluid intelligence was mediated by developmental changes in processing speed and working memory and nearly three fourths of the improvement in working memory was mediated by developmental changes in processing speed Moreover, even when age-related differences in speed, working memory and fluid intelligence were statistically controlled, individual differences in speed had a direct effect on working memory capacity which, in turn was a direct determinant of individual differences in fluid intelligence
Article
The standardization sample from the WMS-III (weighted N = 1250) (The Psychological Corporation, 1997; Tulsky & Ledbetter, 2000), which varied in age from 16 to 89, was used to determine whether storage of verbal and spatial information was affected by normal aging. A previous study with the same sample using an analysis of variance approach showed that the age-associated deterioration for Logical Memory (LM) and Visual Reproduction (VR) delayed recall and recognition were comparable and largely explained by poor immediate recall. This led to the conclusion that age-associated memory impairment was due primarily to encoding deficits. However, that study did not control for the age-associated deterioration in immediate recall in order to ensure that the small age-associated effects for delayed recall and recognition were not confounded by the fact that the elderly had less to remember on the delayed recall and recognition trials. The current study used covariance analysis to statistically adjust the dependent measures, LM and VR delayed recall and recognition, based on immediate recall scores. These analyses showed that the age-associated changes in these measures were statistically significant, but the effect sizes were very small. Partial eta(2) for the main effect of age for LM was 0.06 with weak effect sizes for the interaction of recall type and age (0.02). The partial eta(2) for the main effect of age for VR was 0.09, and the interaction of recall type and age (0.04). This pattern of findings shows that after adjustment for immediate recall, LM and VR recall and recognition demonstrated comparable, slight declines with age suggesting that normal aging produced minimal changes in the ability to store new information when age-associated changes in initial encoding and retrieval were statistically controlled.
Article
The objective of this study was to compare the reliability of the Mini-Mental State Examination with that of a new Standardized Mini-Mental State Examination, which has expanded guidelines for administration and scoring. The subjects were 32 stable elderly residents of a nursing home and 16 elderly residents of a chronic care hospital unit. Six raters administered the Folstein Mini-Mental State to 22 of these stable elderly subjects, and five raters administered the standardized version to 26 of these subjects. Each subject was tested on three different occasions 1 week apart. Each rater tested 4-6 subjects at the first and third weeks and 4-6 different subjects at the second week. The analytic technique used was one-way analysis of variance to estimate the interrater variance and the intrarater variance. The intrarater variance on all occasions was reduced by 86% and the interrater variance was reduced by 76% when the Standardized Mini-Mental State was used; the reductions in variance were significant (p less than 0.003). The intraclass correlation for the Mini-Mental State was 0.69; for the standardized version it was 0.90. It took less time to administer the Standardized Mini-Mental State than the Mini-Mental State. The Standardized Mini-Mental State had better reliability than the Mini-Mental State in this study group. Although the improved reliability of the Standardized Mini-Mental State was achieved by reducing measurement noise, this advantage would likely occur in a broad spectrum of patients.
Article
Newer, nonsedating antihistamines provide a therapeutic alternative for the patient with allergy whose work is impaired by the side effects of traditional H1 antihistamines. To assess the differential effect of these antihistamines on reaction times and subjective symptoms, we compared terfenadine, 60 mg twice daily, to hydroxyzine, 25 mg twice daily, in a double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study of 16 healthy, asymptomatic adults. Simple reaction time and choice reaction time were measured with a computer-based, eye-hand, reaction-time testing apparatus. Reaction times and symptom scores were assessed 90 minutes after the fourth and tenth doses of each drug. Hydroxyzine, but not terfenadine, significantly prolonged both simple and choice reaction time (p less than or equal to 0.0001). However, decision time, the time to process one bit of spatial information, was not prolonged by either antihistamine. Therefore, hydroxyzine prolonged the interpretation and response to stimuli of the central nervous system without increasing single-bit processing time. Although terfenadine was not different from placebo for any symptom assessed, hydroxyzine produced significant drowsiness (p = 0.001), dry mouth (p = 0.022), and irritability (p = 0.021). During the 5 days of hydroxyzine administration, neither objective nor subjective symptoms demonstrated the development of tolerance. No correlation was found between subjective symptoms and prolongation of reaction times by hydroxyzine, suggesting that side effect symptoms of traditional antihistamines are unreliable predictors of objective performance. Terfenadine provides a promising therapeutic alternative to traditional antihistamines for individuals performing critical tasks.
Article
The Stroop is a classical paradigm that presumably involves the inhibition of automatic responses and is frequently used to assess the frontal lobe functions. We investigated the effect of discrete prefrontal lesions in a Stroop task. A sample of 32 patients with frontal lesions were matched with normal controls by sex, age and years of education. Significant differences between patients and controls were found for errors but not for reaction time. Regression analysis showed that the region most related to errors was the right prefrontal lateral cortex. Left lobectomies did not impair the Stroop performance. Our results favour the role of the right prefrontal cortex in sustained attention, and disagree with the conception of the left prefrontal cortex having a role in the inhibition of verbal automatic responses.
Article
We examined the contribution of age of seizure onset, seizure duration, seizure laterality, seizure location, gender, handedness, and cerebral speech representation to cognitive attainment in 1,141 patients with medically refractory seizures. The combined influence of the predictor variables was modest. Age of seizure onset was the best single indicator of Full Scale IQ (partial r = .23) and General Memory (partial r = .20). Laterality and location of dysfunction, and cerebral speech dominance were also relevant and independent indicators of aspects of cognition. Except for age of onset of seizures (early onset was associated with poorer cognitive attainment), however, the magnitude of the effects was limited.