Article

The End of the End of Ideology

Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003, USA.
American Psychologist (Impact Factor: 6.87). 11/2006; 61(7):651-70. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.651
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT

The "end of ideology" was declared by social scientists in the aftermath of World War II. They argued that (a) ordinary citizens' political attitudes lack the kind of stability, consistency, and constraint that ideology requires; (b) ideological constructs such as liberalism and conservatism lack motivational potency and behavioral significance; (c) there are no major differences in content (or substance) between liberal and conservative points of view; and (d) there are few important differences in psychological processes (or styles) that underlie liberal versus conservative orientations. The end-of-ideologists were so influential that researchers ignored the topic of ideology for many years. However, current political realities, recent data from the American National Election Studies, and results from an emerging psychological paradigm provide strong grounds for returning to the study of ideology. Studies reveal that there are indeed meaningful political and psychological differences that covary with ideological self-placement. Situational variables--including system threat and mortality salience--and dispositional variables--including openness and conscientiousness--affect the degree to which an individual is drawn to liberal versus conservative leaders, parties, and opinions. A psychological analysis is also useful for understanding the political divide between "red states" and "blue states."

Download full-text

Full-text

Available from: John T Jost
    • "RWA, THREAT, AND IMMIGRANTS' DEVIANT BEHAVIOR Right-wing authoritarianism can be regarded as a social attitude expressing a threat-driven motivation for conformity and security (Duckitt, 2001). It has been shown to be a valid predictor of conservative values (Duckitt & Fisher, 2003; Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003), resistance to social change (Jost, 2006), and negative attitudes and behaviors towards outgroups (Altemeyer, 1996, 1998; Duckitt, 2001)—especially towards such outgroups that show behavior that deviates from normative ingroup behavior (Duckitt, 1989) and therefore pose a threat to social security and ingroup norms (Asbrock, Sibley, & Duckitt, 2010; Cohrs & Asbrock, 2009). Because of an underlying perception of the world as a dangerous and threatening place, individuals high in RWA are especially sensitive for potentially threatening situations (such as deviant behavior) and show stronger reactions (e.g., ethnocentrism) when confronted with such situations (Duckitt & Sibley, 2010). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: In two studies we tested the relationship between non-immigrant individuals’ perceptions of deviant behavior carried out by Muslims and foreigners and discriminatory intentions towards these outgroups. Based on a longitudinal and a representative cross-sectional sample, we showed that two different types of perceived deviant behavior (Study 1: Muslims’ unwillingness to integrate; Study 2: foreigners’ hostility towards the non-immigrant majority group) are related to increased intergroup threat, which in turn is related to increased intentions to show passive discrimination (i.e. avoidance) towards these outgroups. In line with theorizing about an increased sensitivity for threat in authoritarian individuals, the relationship between perceptions of deviant behavior and threat was especially strong amongst high authoritarian individuals. Theoretical and practical implications of our results are discussed.
    No preview · Article · Dec 2016 · European Journal of Social Psychology
  • Source
    • "This analysis was based on responses from the 5,410 registered voters in Wave I of this national probability sample who provided complete information for our variables of interest. 15 Along with items assessing the Dark Duo ideologies, we included a host of demographic covariates (including ethnicity, socioeconomic status, education, and employment), measures of Big-Six personality (using the Mini-IPIP6 scale presented inSibley, Luyten, et al., 2011), political orientation (using the liberal-conservative self-placement item fromJost, 2006), and short-form scales assessing support for color-blind ideology (), system justification (Kay & Jost, 2003), and patriotism and nationalism (Kosterman & Feshbach, 1989). Controlling for these demographic variables provides a conservative test of our hypotheses, as research indicates that education and personality traits explain unique variance in people's attitudes toward sociopolitical issues (e.g.,Osborne, Dufresne, Eady, Lees-Marshment, & van der Linden, 2012;). "

    Full-text · Article · Feb 2016 · Political Psychology
  • Source
    • "On the other end of the spectrum, people who adhere to left-wing ideology oppose hierarchy (Pratto et al., 1994), and favor social practices aimed at achieving social and economic equality (Evans, Heath, & Lalljee, 1996; Feldman, 1988; Glaser, 2005; Graetz & Shapiro, 2005; Jacoby, 1991). Furthermore, they are less likely to hold prejudicial attitudes toward minority groups (Jost, 2006). "
    [Show description] [Hide description]
    DESCRIPTION: Imagine yourself facing someone who might attack your group – if you could control your emotions, how would you want to feel toward that person? We argue that the goals people have for their group dictate how they want to feel on behalf of their group. We further propose that these group-based emotional preferences, in turn, influence how people actually feel as group members and how they react to political events. We conducted nine studies to test our proposed model. In a pilot study, we showed that political ideology is related to how people want to feel toward outgroup members, even when controlling for how they want to feel in general, or how they actually feel toward outgroup members. In Studies A1-A3, we demonstrated that group-based emotional preferences are linked to emotional experience and that both mediate links between political ideology and political reactions. In Study A4, we showed that political ideology influences emotional preferences, emotional experiences and political reactions. Next, in Studies B1-B4, we demonstrated that changing group-based emotional preferences can shape group-based emotional experiences and consequently influence political reactions. By suggesting that group-based emotions are motivated, our findings point to new directions for advancing conflict resolution.
    Full-text · Research · Jan 2016
Show more