ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

A stratified probability sample survey of the British general population, aged 16 to 44 years, was conducted from 1999 to 2001 (N = 11,161) using face-to-face interviewing and computer-assisted self-interviewing. We used these data to estimate the population prevalence of masturbation, and to identify sociodemographic, sexual behavioral, and attitudinal factors associated with reporting this behavior. Seventy-three percent of men and 36.8% of women reported masturbating in the 4 weeks prior to interview (95% confidence interval 71.5%-74.4% and 35.4%-38.2%, respectively). A number of sociodemographic and behavioral factors were associated with reporting masturbation. Among both men and women, reporting masturbation increased with higher levels of education and social class and was more common among those reporting sexual function problems. For women, masturbation was more likely among those who reported more frequent vaginal sex in the last four weeks, a greater repertoire of sexual activity (such as reporting oral and anal sex), and more sexual partners in the last year. In contrast, the prevalence of masturbation was lower among men reporting more frequent vaginal sex. Both men and women reporting same-sex partner(s) were significantly more likely to report masturbation. Masturbation is a common sexual practice with significant variations in reporting between men and women.
Arch Sex Behav
DOI 10.1007/s10508-006-9123-6
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Prevalence of Masturbation and Associated Factors in a British
National Probability Survey
Makeda Gerressu · Catherine H. Mercer ·
Cynthia A. Graham · Kaye Wellings · Anne M. Johnson
Received: 18 April 2006 / Revised: 19 September 2006 / Accepted: 19 September 2006
C
Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007
Abstract A stratified probability sample survey of the
British general population, aged 16 to 44 years, was con-
ducted from 1999 to 2001 (N = 11,161) using face-to-face
interviewing and computer-assisted self-interviewing. We
used these data to estimate the population prevalence of
masturbation, and to identify sociodemographic, sexual be-
havioral, and attitudinal factors associated with reporting this
behavior. Seventy-three percent of men and 36.8% of women
reported masturbating in the 4 weeks prior to interview (95%
confidence interval 71.5%–74.4% and 35.4%–38.2%, re-
spectively). A number of sociodemographic and behavioral
factors were associated with reporting masturbation. Among
both men and women, reporting masturbation increased
with higher levels of education and social class and was
more common among those reporting sexual function
M. Gerressu · C. H. Mercer · A. M. Johnson
Centre for Sexual Health and HIV Research,
Department of Primary Care and Population Sciences,
University College London,
Mortimer Market Centre, off Capper Street, London, England
C. A. Graham
Oxford Doctoral Course in Clinical Psychology, University of
Oxford, Warneford Hospital,
Oxford, England
K. Wellings
Centre for Sexual and Reproductive Health Research, London
School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine,
London, England
M. Gerressu (
)
Centre for Sexual Health and HIV Research,
Department of Primary Care and Population Sciences,
University College London,
Mortimer Market Centre, off Capper Street,
London WC1E 6JB, England
e-mail: mgerressu@gum.ucl.ac.uk
problems. For women, masturbation was more likely among
those who reported more frequent vaginal sex in the last
four weeks, a greater repertoire of sexual activity (such as
reporting oral and anal sex), and more sexual partners in
the last year. In contrast, the prevalence of masturbation
was lower among men reporting more frequent vaginal
sex. Both men and women reporting same-sex partner(s)
were significantly more likely to report masturbation.
Masturbation is a common sexual practice with significant
variations in reporting between men and women.
Keywords Masturbation
.
Sexual behavior
.
Sex survey
.
Gender differences
Introduction
From a public health perspective, masturbation is consid-
ered a safe sexual activity. It has been promoted as a safe
alternative to higher risk practices by both researchers and
clinicians (Pinkerton, Bogart, Cecil, & Abramson, 2002).
It has been recommended as a way for people to familiar-
ize themselves with their body and their sexual responses
(Zamboni & Crawford, 2002) and used as a treatment for
premature ejaculation and orgasmic disorders in women
(Heiman & LoPiccolo, 1988). Masturbation has also been
presented as a way people in later life might fulfil their
sexual needs if experiencing difficulty finding new sexual
partners (Kontula & Haavio-Mannila, 2002).
Despite its sexual health potential, masturbation has been
stigmatized over the centuries. Held as a moral sin by
Judaism, Islam, and Christianity, masturbation became per-
ceived as a mortal threat to health in the 18th century. It was
renamed “onanism” and for two hundred years physicians
warned patients of the multitude of illnesses associated with
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
masturbation (Laqueur, 2003). It was claimed that mastur-
bation would lead inter alia to insanity, fits, blindness, and
impotence (Bullough, 2002). Many “cures” were developed,
ranging from circumcision for both men and women to cas-
tration (Darby, 2003). According to Laqueur (2003), this
anxiety stemmed from the belief at the time, that allowing
people to withdraw into their isolated private sexual fantasy
world would be socially disruptive.
In the 20th century, with better understanding of the causes
of sexually transmitted infections and their symptoms, and
the influence of psychoanalysts, artists, the feminist and gay
rights movements, views of masturbation changed (Laqueur,
2003). The topic remains one that tends not to be openly
discussed and negative attitudes persist (Coleman, 2002)but
it is also a frequently raised subject on sex and relation-
ship phone lines and Internet sites in a number of countries
(Barbey, 1991; Cardamakis, Vinakos, Lambou, & Papathana-
siou, 1993; International Planned Parenthood Federation,
1996; Kitamura, 1990; Kuriansky, 1996).
Kinsey’s sexual behavior studies in the 1940s and 1950s
first provided evidence on the frequency and distribution of
masturbation (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1953;Kinsey,
Pomeroy, Martin, & Gebhard, 1948). For all the negative
views surrounding the practice, it was found to be more
common than previously thought. A substantial gender
difference in the incidence of masturbation was reported,
and associations between masturbation and various sociode-
mographic variables, such as education and religion, were
identified. Some 40 years later, masturbation was excluded
altogether from the first National Survey of Sexual Attitudes
and Lifestyles in Britain (Natsal 1990). This was a decision
prompted partly by its low risk status (Editorial, 1994)
and partly as a result of developmental qualitative research
findings that masturbation elicited a level of “awkwardness
and embarrassment” (Spencer, Faulkner, & Keegan, 1988).
More recent national surveys of sexual behavior in several
countries have included questions on masturbation (B
´
ejin,
1996; Kontula & Haavio-Mannila, 2002; Laumann, Gagnon,
Michael, & Michaels, 1994; Spira, Bajos, & ACSF Group,
1994). The reported rates of masturbation varied widely, in
part reflecting methodological differences in terms of survey
design, question wording, and method of questioning. One
consistency across studies was the striking gender differ-
ence in the prevalence of masturbation (Kontula & Haavio-
Mannila, 2002; Laumann et al., 1994; Oliver & Hyde, 1993;
Spira et al., 1994). Gender differences in many other sex-
ual behaviors have narrowed, but the gender gap in reported
masturbation remains substantial (Hyde, 2005).
Laumann et al. (1994) found that the largest proportion
of their participants chose “to release sexual tension” and to
obtain “physical pleasure” as their reasons for masturbating
but other reasons given were “to relax,” to “get to sleep”
or because their current partner did not want sex (the latter
was reported by three times as many men as women). The
most common reasons were perhaps the most obvious; how-
ever, there has been debate around whether masturbation is
a substitute for sex with a partner (Dekker & Schmidt, 2002;
Kontula & Haavio-Mannila, 2002
; Pinkerton et al., 2002)or
a behavior that is part of a wide repertoire of acts aimed at
sexual expression and satisfaction, irrespective of partner-
ship status (Laumann et al., 1994; Pinkerton et al., 2002).
Some studies report that while frequency of intercourse in
the last four weeks decreased with the increased duration
of the relationship, frequency of masturbation actually in-
creased with time in long lasting unions (Dekker & Schmidt,
2002; Kontula & Haavio-Mannila, 2002).
Most studies of masturbation have used convenience sam-
ples in clinics, universities, or community settings and have
explored associations between masturbation frequency and
variables such as relationship status, other sexual activities,
contraceptive use, and attitudes such as guilt (Dekker &
Schmidt, 2002; Pinkerton et al., 2002; Robinson, Bockting,
& Harrell, 2002). However, findings from convenience sam-
ples are not generalizable, and there are few robust data inter-
nationally that permit us to measure prevalence and identify
correlates of masturbation from representative, general pop-
ulation samples.
Britain’s second National Survey of Sexual Attitudes and
Lifestyles (Natsal 2000), conducted between 1999 and 2001,
included for the first time a question on masturbation (Erens
et al., 2001; Johnson et al., 2001). In this article, we used
these nationally representative, contemporary data to explore
the prevalence of reporting masturbation and to identify asso-
ciated sociodemographic, sexual behavioral, and attitudinal
factors.
Method
Participants
Natsal 2000 was a stratified probability sample survey of the
general population aged 16 to 44 years, resident in Britain.
In total, 11,161 people (4762 men and 6399 women) were
interviewed between May 1999 and February 2001. Details
of the methodology and question wording were published
elsewhere (Erens et al., 2001; Johnson et al., 2001). Briefly,
a sample of 40,523 addresses was selected from the small-
user Postcode Address File for Britain with a multistage
probability cluster design, with over-sampling in Greater
London.
Procedure
Interviewers visited all selected addresses and recorded the
number of residents aged 16 to 44 years. One resident from
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
every household was invited by random selection to partic-
ipate in the study. Natsal 2000 achieved a response rate of
65.4%, which is in line with other major surveys conducted
in Britain (Lynn & Clarke, 2002). Trained interviewers con-
ducted face-to-face interviews in participants’ homes, in-
cluding a computer-assisted self-interview (CASI). The face-
to-face interview included questions on sociodemographics,
attitudes, sexual attraction, and experience. Participants who
reported no sexual experience of any kind in the face-to-
face interview, and those aged 16 and 17 years with some
heterosexual experience but no heterosexual intercourse or
same-sex experience reported in screening questions, were
not given the CASI.
In the CASI, eligible participants were asked questions on
various sexual practices, including masturbation: “When, if
ever, was the last occasionyou masturbated? That is, aroused
yourself sexually?” Participants were given a choice of seven
response options: in the last 7 days; between 7 days and
4 weeks ago; between 4 weeks and 6 months ago; between
6 months and 1 year ago; between 1 year and 5 years ago;
longer than 5 years ago; never masturbated or aroused myself
sexually.
The study was approved by the University College Hos-
pital and North Thames Multi-Centre Research Ethics Com-
mittee and all the Local Research Ethics Committees in
Britain.
Data analysis
All analyses were performed using the survey analysis func-
tions of STATA 7.0 to account for stratification, clustering,
and weighting of the data. The data were weighted to correct
for unequal selection probabilities, including over-sampling
in Greater London, and to match Britain’s age/sex population
profile (Erens et al., 2001; Johnson et al., 2001).
As in previous publications (Fenton et al., 2005; Johnson
et al., 2001; Wellings et al., 2001), we used binary logistic
regression to obtain odds ratios (OR) to compare estimates
for participants who did and did not report masturbation in
the last 4 weeks. We also present ORs adjusting for selected
sociodemographic characteristics (see Table 2), numbers of
sexual partners in the last year, and whether or not partic-
ipants reported same-sex genital contact (ever). Statistical
significance was considered as p < .05 for all analyses.
Results
Prevalence of masturbation
Ninety-five percent of men and 71.2% of women reported
that they had masturbated at some point in their lives (Table
1). Seventy-three percent of men and 36.8% of women
reported masturbating in the four weeks before their inter-
view, while approximately half of the men (51.7%) and one
in six women (17.8%) reported masturbating in the previous
seven days. This gender difference in prevalence was highly
statistically significant, χ
2
(1) = 1861.15, p < .0001.
Sociodemographic factors
Table 2 shows that reporting masturbation in the last four
weeks (referred hereafter for brevity as “reporting mastur-
bation”) was significantly associated with age for men and
women, with prevalence highest among those aged 25–34.
Previously married and single men were significantly more
likely to report masturbation than married or cohabiting men.
This was true even after adjusting for sociodemographic fac-
tors. Among women, cohabiting, single, and previously mar-
ried women were more likely to report masturbation than
married women, an association which remained significant
after adjustment. Both men and women with children were
significantly less likely to report masturbation, even after
adjustment.
Higher educational achievement and higher social class
were both associated with high prevalence of reporting mas-
turbation, associations that remained after adjustment. Men
and women who self-reported their ethnicity as “white”
Table 1 Last occasion of
masturbation by gender
Men % Women %
(95% CI) (95% CI)
Denominator (unweighted, weighted) 4410, 5281 5999, 5132
Last occasion of masturbation
a
Last 7 days 51.7% (50.0%–53.3%) 17.8% (16.7%–18.8%)
Between 7 days and 4 weeks ago 21.3% (20.0%–22.7%) 19.0% (17.9%–20.1%)
Between 4 weeks and 6 months ago 8.3% (7.4%–9.2%) 13.7% (12.8%–14.7%)
Between 6 months and 1 year ago 4.3% (3.7%–5.0%) 6.5% (5.8%–7.2%)
Between 1 year and 5 years ago 4.6% (3.9%–5.3%) 7.7% (7.0%–8.5%)
Longer than 5 years ago 4.5% (3.9%–5.3%) 6.5% (5.8%–7.2%)
Never 5.4% (4.7%–6.2%) 28.8% (27.6%–30.1%)
p value for gender difference p < .0001
a
Participants were asked
“When, if ever, was the last
occasion you masturbated? That
is, aroused yourself sexually?”
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
Table 2 Selected sociodemographic factors associated with reporting masturbation in the last 4 weeks by gender
Denominator Denominator
Men (unweighted/ Women (unweighted/
Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted) Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted)
All 73.0% (71.5%–74.4%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 4410, 5281 36.8% (35.4%–38.2%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 5999, 5132
Age p < .0001 p = .0001 p = .0308 p < .0001
16–24 72.6% (69.3%–75.6%) 1.00 1.00 1033, 1272 33.9% (31.0%–36.9%) 1.00 1.00 1224, 1244
25–34 76.9% (74.6%–79.0%) 1.26 (1.03–1.53) 1.69 (1.32–2.16) 1708, 2037 38.8% (36.7%–41.0%) 1.24 (1.05–1.45) 1.53 (1.25–1.87) 2451, 1994
35–44 69.2% (66.7%–71.6%) .85 (.70–1.03) 1.36 (1.04–1.79) 1669, 1972 36.5% (34.4%–38.8%) 1.12 (.95–1.32) 1.73 (1.39–2.15) 2324, 1914
Marital status p < .0001 p < .0001 p = .0003 p = .0001
Married 68.5% (66.1%–70.9%) 1.00 1.00 1521, 2207 33.7% (31.7%–35.7%) 1.00 1.00 2431, 2358
Cohabiting 70.8% (67.0%–74.4%) 1.12 (.90–1.38) 1.01 (.79–1.29) 642, 912 40.4% (37.2%–43.6%) 1.33 (1.14–1.56) 1.33 (1.11–1.59) 984, 972
Previously married
b
83.8% (79.1%–87.6%) 2.37 (1.70–3.30) 2.81 (1.97–4.01) 312, 238 39.2% (35.3%–43.2%) 1.27 (1.05–1.54) 1.50 (1.23–1.84) 677, 398
Single, never married 77.9% (75.6%–80.0%) 1.62 (1.36–1.92) 1.70 (1.29–2.24) 1929, 1918 38.9% (36.4%–41.5%) 1.26 (1.09–1.44) 1.25 (1.04–1.51) 1900, 1399
Any children p < .0001 p = .010 p < .0001 p < .0001
No 77.8% (75.9%–79.5%) 1.00 1.00 2557, 2824 43.4% (41.1%–45.7%) 1.00 1.00 2288, 1978
Yes 67.5% (65.1%–69.7%) .59 (.51–.69) .73 (.59–.93) 1853, 2457 32.7% (31.0%–34.3%) .63 (.56–.71) .66 (.57–.77) 3711, 3154
Social class p < .0001 p < .0001 p < .0001 ns
I/II 80.0% (77.7%–82.1%) 1.00 1.00 1578,1838 44.5% (41.9%–47.2%) 1.00 1.00 1735, 1393
III (non-manual & manual) 71.6% (69.2%–73.8%) .63 (.52–.75) .73 (.59–.91) 1734, 2142 36.4% (34.3%–38.5%) .71 (.62–.82) .97 (.82–1.14) 2399, 2092
IV/V/unemployed 62.6% (58.7%–66.4%) .42 (.34–.52) .54 (.42–.70) 804, 956 30.7% (27.9%–33.6%) .55 (.47–.65) .89 (.73–1.09) 1212, 1057
Education p < .0001 p < .0001 p < .0001 p < .0001
Degree 82.2% (79.5%–84.7%) 1.00 1.00 1065, 1182 49.9% (46.7%–53.0%) 1.00 1.00 1216, 935
A-level 77.1% (74.6%–79.4%) .73 (.58–.91) .81 (.63–1.05) 1392, 1690 40.7% (38.0%–43.5%) .69 (.58–.82) .71 (.59–.85) 1567, 1396
GCSE/O-level 69.0% (66.3%–71.5%) .48 (.39–.60) .58 (.45–.76) 1444, 1811 32.6% (30.5%–34.7%) .49 (.41–.57) .53 (.44–.65) 2351, 2088
Foreign qualification 55.7% (36.9%–73.0%) .27 (.12–.59) .31 (.13–.77) 38, 35 37.3% (24.5%–52.1%) .60 (.32–1.11) .61 (.29–1.28) 59, 45
None 55.3% (50.1%–60.4%) .27 (.20–.35) .37 (.27–.52) 464, 555 23.9% (20.8%–27.3%) .32 (.25–.39) .36 (.27–.48) 790, 651
Ethnicity p < .0001 p < .0001 p = .0078 p = .0240
White 74.7% (73.2%–76.2%) 1.00 1.00 3958, 4844 37.4% (36.0%–38.9%) 1.00 1.00 5389, 4734
Black Caribbean 53.6% (41.1%–65.7%) .39 (.24–.65) .38 (.22–.66) 91, 77 32.6% (24.3%–42.2%) .81 (.53–1.22) .76 (.49–1.19) 140, 71
Black African 42.5% (29.5%–56.7%) .25 (.14–.44) .16 (.09–.31) 73, 52 21.1% (12.6%–33.2%) .45 (.24-.83) .40 (.22–.75) 90, 40
Indian 49.5% (37.5%–61.5%) .33 (.20–.54) .35 (.20–.63) 71, 75 32.5% (21.2%–46.3%) .81 (.45–1.44) .65 (.34–1.26) 91, 86
Pakistani 52.7% (33.2%–71.3%) .38 (.17–.85) .58 (.23–1.44) 29, 43 10.0% (3.2%–27.6%) .19 (.05–.64) .37 (.10–1.37) 36, 34
Other 60.1% (51.3%–68.3%) .51 (.35–.74) .50 (.32–.79) 180, 182 33.1% (26.4%–40.7%) .83 (.60–1.15) .88 (.61–1.26) 241, 156
Religiosity
c
p = .003 ns p = .004 p = .007
No 73.7% (72.2%–75.2%) 1.00 1.00 3961, 4786 37.6% (36.1%–39.1%) 1.00 1.00 5132, 4455
Yes 66.1% (61.0%–70.9%) .69 (.55–.88) .79 (.59–1.05) 442, 488 31.7% (28.3%–35.4%) .77 (.65–.92) .76 (.63–.93) 855, 667
a
Odds ratio (OR) adjusted for all variables in Table 2.
b
Separated, divorced or widowed.
c
Religion and/or beliefs very/fairly important and attend religious services/meetings at least once a month.
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
were significantly more likely to report masturbation than
those in other ethnic groups (crude OR for reporting mas-
turbation by white participants relative to non-white par-
ticipants: 2.49 (95% CI 1.97–3.16, p < .0001) and 1.42
(95% CI 1.13–1.79, p = .003) for men and women, re-
spectively). For both men and women, reported religious
denomination was not significantly associated with report-
ing masturbation after controlling for the other sociodemo-
graphic factors listed in Table 2 (data not shown). However,
those who reported their religion and/or religious beliefs
as “very” or “fairly important” and who attended religious
services/meetings at least once a month were less likely to
report masturbation. After controlling for other sociodemo-
graphic factors, this association was only significant among
women.
Factors relating to first/early sexual experience
Participants who said that they found it difficult to talk to
one or both of their parents about sex when they were grow-
ing up (see footnote to Table 3 for question wording) were
more likely to report masturbation. After adjustment, this
association remained significant only for women. A signifi-
cantly larger proportion of women who reported first inter-
course before age 16 reported masturbation in the last four
weeks (adjusted OR 1.19, Table 3). This association was not
observed for men. Among those participants who reported
ever having had homosexual sex (defined as genital contact
with someone of the same sex), 93.1% of men and 69.1%
of women reported masturbation, in contrast to 71.8% and
35.1%, respectively, among those who did not report such
experience (p < .0001 for both men and women).
Factors relating to current/recent health and sexual behavior
Prevalence of masturbation declined with worse self-
perceived general health but this association remained sig-
nificant only among women after adjustment. There was no
significant association with smoking but masturbation was
more likely to be reported with reporting greater alcohol
consumption (Table 4).
The prevalence of masturbation was associated with fre-
quency of partnered sexual activity in the last four weeks
but the direction of the association was reversed for men and
women (Table 5). For women, prevalence increased from
33.8% among those reporting sex less than four times in the
last four weeks (corresponding to the median number of oc-
casions (Johnson et al., 2001)) to 47.2% among those women
reporting at least 16 occasions. In contrast, the prevalence
of masturbation was most frequent among men reporting
less than four occasions of sex in the last four weeks. These
associations remained significant in multivariate analysis.
Gender differences were also evident in terms of the rela-
tionship between masturbation and the occurrence of other
sexual activities. For women, reporting vaginal, oral, anal, or
other genital contact not leading to intercourse in the last four
weeks was significantly associated with also reporting mas-
turbation in this time frame. However, no such association
was evident for men. Indeed, men who reported vaginal sex
were significantly less likely to report masturbation (70.5%
vs. 80.8%, adjusted OR .59).
In terms of sexual risk for STI/HIV, after adjusting for
potential confounding factors, there was no significant as-
sociation between reporting masturbation and unsafe sex,
defined here as reporting at least two partners in the last year
and inconsistent condom use in the last four weeks (Johnson
et al., 2001). Increasing partner numbers was associated with
reporting masturbation for women, with prevalence doubling
from 27.8% among women reporting no partners in the last
year to 58.5% of women reporting at least five partners in this
time frame, an association that remained significant after ad-
justment. An association was also observed among men but
there was no evidence of any linear trend and this association
was not significant after adjustment.
The duration of the participant’s most recent partnership
was associated to some extent with masturbation. In uni-
variate analysis, with increasing length of partnership, men
were significantly less likely to report masturbation, while
for women, there was some evidence of increasing likeli-
hood to report masturbation. These associations were not
significant after adjustment.
Men and women reporting at least one sexual function
“problem(s)” lasting at least one month in the last year were
significantly more likely to report masturbation. Consider-
ing specific “problems,” participants reporting lacking inter-
est in sex (men only), anxiety about performance, inability
to experience orgasm, and/or premature orgasm, problems
achieving and/or maintaining an erection (men only, not sig-
nificant after adjustment) and trouble lubricating (women
only) were significantly more likely to report masturbation.
Reporting “persistent problems,” defined as sexual func-
tion “problems” lasting at least six months in the last year
(Mercer et al., 2003), was also associated with masturbation
(data not shown in Table 6), although only for men (adjusted
OR 1.63, 95% CI 1.11–2.40, p = .013).
It is worth noting that among women who did not report
orgasm “problems,” there was a significant increase in re-
porting masturbation with increasing numbers of occasions
of sex in the last four weeks, while no such significant asso-
ciation was observed for women who did report an inability
to experience orgasm. There was no such interaction “effect”
for men.
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
Table 3 Selected factors relating to first/early sexual experience and their association with reporting masturbation in the last 4 weeks by gender
Denominator Denominator
Men (unweighted/ Women (unweighted/
Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted) Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted)
All 73.0% (71.5%–74.4%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 4410, 5281 36.8% (35.4%–38.2%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 5999, 5132
Was difficult to talk about
sex with one or both
parent(s) when growing
up
b
p = .005 ns p = .003 p = .035
No 72.3% (70.7%–73.8%) 1.00 1.00 3871, 4675 35.9% (34.4%–37.4%) 1.00 1.00 4961, 4288
Yes 79.8% (75.0%–84.0%) 1.52 (1.14–2.03) 1.24 (.91–1.70) 364, 420 42.0% (38.3%–45.9%) 1.30 (1.09–1.53) 1.22 (1.01–1.46) 821, 691
Had first sex before age 16
c
ns ns p = .006 p = .042
No 73.5% (71.7%–75.2%) 1.00 1.00 3141, 3739 35.8% (34.3%–37.3%) 1.00 1.00 4749, 4038
Yes 71.8% (68.9%–74.5%) .92 (.78–1.09) 1.04 (.86–1.26) 1269, 1542 40.4% (37.4%–43.5%) 1.22 (1.06–1.40) 1.19 (1.01–1.40) 1250, 1094
Ever had homosexual sex
(with genital contact)
p < .0001 p < .0001 p < .0001 p < .0001
No 71.8% (70.2%–73.3%) 1.00 1.00 4097, 4986 35.1% (33.8%–36.6%) 1.00 1.00 5674, 4885
Yes 93.1% (89.3%–95.6%) 5.28 (3.25–8.58) 4.19 (2.55–6.88) 313, 295 69.1% (62.9%–74.7%) 4.12 (3.10–5.48) 3.32 (2.44–4.52) 325, 247
a
Odds ratio (OR) adjusted for age, marital status, any children, ethnicity (coded white vs. other), social class, ever had homosexual sex, partner numbers in the last year.
b
The corresponding question was worded “Now I’d like to ask you some questions about when you were growing up and learning about sex. When you were about 14, did you find it easy or
difficult to talk to your parent(s) about sexual matters, or didn’t you discuss sexual matters with him/her/them at that age?”
c
Heterosexual/homosexual.
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
Table 4 Selected factors relating to current health and their association with reporting masturbation in the last 4 weeks by gender
Denominator Denominator
Men (unweighted/ Women (unweighted/
Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted) Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted)
All 73.0% (71.5%–74.4%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 4410, 5281 36.8% (35.4%–38.2%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 5999, 5132
Self-perceived general
health
p = .0145 ns p = .0010 p = .0254
Very good 73.6% (71.4%–75.8%) 1.00 1.00 1970, 2375 39.2% (37.2%–41.3%) 1.00 1.00 2588, 2214
Good 73.3% (71.0%–75.5%) .98 (.83–1.16) .97 (.81–1.16) 1862, 2239 35.9% (33.8%–38.0%) .87 (.76–.98) .88 (.76–1.01) 2513, 2198
Fair 72.4% (67.5%–76.8%) .94 (.72–1.22) .94 (.70–1.26) 475, 551 33.5% (29.8%–37.3%) .78 (.64–.94) .85 (.69–1.06) 759, 616
Bad/very bad 56.4% (45.2%–67.1%) .46 (.29–.74) .78 (.43–1.40) 102, 115 24.5% (17.6%–33.2%) .50 (.33–.78) .46 (.25–.83) 139, 103
Smoking status ns ns p = .0074 ns
Never 74.1% (71.9%–76.2%) 1.00 1.00 1989, 2399 34.7% (32.7%–36.7%) 1.00 1.00 2681, 2317
Ex-smoker 74.5% (72.0%–79.1%) 1.09 (.87–1.37) 1.11 (.87–1.40) 678, 831 39.5% (36.2%–42.9%) 1.23 (1.04–1.46) 1.15 (.96–1.39) 988, 861
Light smoker 70.9% (64.5%–74.1%) .85 (.70–1.04) .91 (.73–1.13) 900, 1010 40.0% (37.1%–43.1%) 1.26 (1.08–1.47) 1.19 (1.00–1.41) 1315, 1110
Heavy smoker 70.3% (66.6%–73.7%) .83 (.67–1.01) .90 (.72–1.13) 835, 1031 35.5% (32.2%–38.9%) 1.04 (.87–1.23) 1.02 (.84–1.24) 1015, 844
Alcohol consumption
b
p < .0001 p = .0010 p < .0001 p < .0001
None 56.5% (51.2%–61.6%) 1.00 1.00 449, 516 25.2% (22.5%–28.2%) 1.00 1.00 1164, 963
Not more than
recommended limit
73.9% (72.3%–75.5%) 2.18 (1.73–2.74) 1.45 (1.11–1.88) 3464, 4179 38.6% (37.0%–40.2%) 1.86 (1.58–2.20) 1.54 (1.27–1.86) 4236, 3665
More than recommended
limit
80.9% (76.6%–84.6%) 3.26 (2.33–4.57) 2.00 (1.39–2.89) 491, 780 45.8% (41.2%–50.5%) 2.51 (1.97–3.19) 1.74 (1.32–2.28) 596, 502
a
Odds ratio (OR) adjusted for age, marital status, any children, ethnicity (coded white vs. other), social class, ever had homosexual sex, partner numbers in the last year.
b
Recommended limit is defined as less than 15 units of alcohol per week for women and less than 22 units of alcohol per week for men.
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
Table 5 Selected factors relating to current/recent sexual behavior and their association with reporting masturbation in the last 4 weeks by gender
Denominator Denominator
Men (unweighted/ Women (unweighted/
Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted) Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted)
All 73.0% (71.5%–74.4%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 4410, 5281 36.8% (35.4%–38.2%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 5999, 5132
Number of occasions of sex, last
4 weeks
b
p < .0001 p < .0001 p < .0001 p = .0003
< 4 77.5% (75.5%–79.5%) 1.00 1.00 2124, 2396 33.8% (31.9%–35.9%) 1.00 1.00 2773, 2210
4–7 68.4% (64.9%–71.7%) .63 (.52–.76) .66 (.52–.82) 881, 1143 36.1% (33.3%–39.0%) 1.10 (.95–1.29) 1.10 (.93–1.31) 1345, 1222
8–11 67.6% (63.3%–71.5%) .60 (.48–.75) .59 (.46–.76) 584, 774 36.4% (33.0%–40.0%) 1.12 (.94–1.34) 1.10 (.90–1.34) 858, 779
12–15 67.3% (61.4%–72.7%) .60 (.45–.79) .55 (.40–.76) 342, 414 43.4% (38.6%–48.4%) 1.50 (1.21–1.87) 1.53 (1.19–1.96) 466, 437
16 + 70.0% (64.4%–75.1%) .68 (.51–.90) .58 (.42–.79) 328, 402 47.2% (41.6%–52.8%) 1.75 (1.37–2.23) 1.67 (1.27–2.19) 383, 349
Vaginal sex, last 4 weeks p < .0001 p < .0001 p = .030 ns
No 80.8% (78.1%–83.2%) 1.00 1.00 1221, 1247 34.2% (31.4%–37.0%) 1.00 1.00 1415, 1053
Yes 70.5% (68.8%–72.2%) .57 (.47–.68) .59 (.46–.76) 3119, 3959 37.8% (36.2%–39.3%) 1.17 (1.01–1.35) 1.14 (.94–1.38) 4486, 4006
Oral sex, last 4 weeks
b
ns ns p < .0001 p < .0001
No 71.5% (69.1%–73.8%) 1.00 1.00 1882, 2115 27.4% (25.6%–29.2%) 1.00 1.00 2808, 2269
Yes 74.0% (72.0%–75.8%) 1.13 (.97–1.32) 1.04 (.87–1.24) 2528, 3166 44.3% (42.4%–46.2%) 2.11 (1.87–2.38) 1.92 (1.67–2.21) 3191, 2863
Anal sex, last 4 weeks
b
ns ns p < .0001 p < .0001
No 72.7% (71.2%–74.2%) 1.00 1.00 4162, 5006 36.0% (34.6%–37.4%) 1.00 1.00 5651, 4849
Yes 77.4% (70.9%–83.3%) 1.31 (.91–1.89) 1.17 (.75–1.82) 248, 275 57.9% (50.9%–64.5%) 2.44 (1.84–3.26) 2.33 (1.68–3.23) 261, 222
Other genital contact, last 4 weeks
b
ns ns p < .0001 p < .0001
No 71.6% (69.3%–73.8%) 1.00 1.00 1965, 2222 28.9% (27.1%–30.7%) 1.00 1.00 2865, 2281
Yes 74.0% (72.0%–75.8%) 1.13 (.97–1.31) 1.11 (.94–1.33) 2443, 3057 43.1% (41.2%–45.0%) 1.87 (1.66–2.10) 1.77 (1.55–2.03) 3134, 2851
“Unsafe sex,” last 4 weeks
b,c
p < .0001 ns p < .0001 ns
No 70.0% (68.1%–71.8%) 1.00 1.00 2765, 3556 36.7% (35.1%–38.3%) 1.00 1.00 4168, 3780
Yes 79.2% (75.2%–82.7%) 1.63 (1.28–2.08) 1.40 (.93–2.09) 592, 653 49.9% (45.1%–54.7%) 1.72 (1.40–2.11) .91 (.59–1.41) 554, 429
Numbers of partners, last year
b
p = .0001 ns p < .0001 p < .0001
0 75.2% (69.3%–80.2%) 1.00 1.00 359, 331 27.8% (23.7%–32.3%) 1.00 1.00 485, 327
1 71.0% (69.1%–72.8%) .81 (.60–1.10) .89 (.61–1.29) 2723, 3537 35.5% (33.9%–37.1%) 1.43 (1.14–1.79) 1.55 (1.19–2.03) 4453, 3971
2 78.3% (73.8%–82.3%) 1.20 (.81–1.75) 1.05 (.68–1.63) 488, 544 46.6% (41.8%–51.5%) 2.27 (1.70–3.03) 1.99 (1.46–2.71) 544, 412
3–4 76.4% (71.1%–81.0%) 1.07 (.72–1.59) .99 (.63–1.56) 405, 426 54.2% (46.7%–61.5%) 3.07 (2.11–4.47) 3.21 (2.14–4.81) 245, 195
5+ 82.7% (77.5%–86.9%) 1.58 (1.02–2.47) 1.28 (.79–2.07) 305, 298 58.5% (48.4%–67.9%) 3.66 (2.30–5.82) 3.03 (1.81–5.06) 127, 106
Duration of most recent partnership
b
p = .0003 ns p < .0001 ns
Most recent occasion was also
first occasion
76.9% (73.2%–80.3%) 1.00 1.00 755, 817 33.9% (29.7%–38.5%) 1.00 1.00 589, 490
Less than 6 months 79.2% (74.2%–83.4%) 1.14 (.81–1.61) .81 (.55–1.18) 364, 358 45.7% (40.1%–51.4%) 1.64 (1.22–2.20) 1.31 (.93–1.84) 406, 313
At least 6 months but less than
12 months
75.0% (68.3%–80.6%) .90 (.61–1.31) .66 (.42–1.02) 257, 286 42.3% (36.1%–48.8%) 1.43 (1.02–1.99) 1.36 (.93–1.99) 320, 256
At least 1 year but less than
3 years
76.0% (71.8%–79.8%) .95 (.71–1.28) .77 (.55–1.09) 582, 646 44.2% (40.2%–48.3%) 1.54 (1.20–1.99) 1.52 (1.13–2.04) 808, 675
At least 3 years but less than
5 years
73.3% (67.9%-78.2%) .82 (.59-1.15) .80 (.54-1.18) 353, 456 42.7% (38.1%-47.4%) 1.45 (1.10–1.91) 1.38 (1.01–1.90) 529, 442
At least 5 years but less than
10 years
74.5% (70.1%–78.4%) .87 (.65–1.17) .96 (.65–1.40) 504, 652 36.9% (33.6%–40.3%) 1.14 (.87–1.45) 1.13 (.84–1.52) 897, 775
At least 10 years 66.3% (62.4%–70.0%) .59 (.45–.77) .76 (.51–1.12) 628, 891 33.9% (31.2%–36.8%) 1.00 (.79–1.26) 1.13 (.84–1.52) 1262, 1161
a
Odds ratio (OR) adjusted for age, marital status, any children, ethnicity (coded white vs. other), social class, ever had homosexual sex, partner numbers in the last year.
b
Heterosexual/homosexual.
c
Defined as reporting two or more heterosexual and/or homosexual partners in the last year and inconsistent condom use in the last 4 weeks.
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
Table 6 Sexual function “problems” experienced for at least one month in the last year and their association with reporting masturbation in the last 4 weeks by gender
Denominator Denominator
Men (unweighted/ Women (unweighted/
Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted) Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted)
All 73.0% (71.5%–74.4%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 4410, 5281 36.8% (35.4%–38.2%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 5999, 5132
At least one sexual function
“problem(s)”
b
p < .0001 p < .0001 p = .001 p = .004
No 69.1% (67.1%–71.0%) 1.00 1.00 2483, 3105 35.2% (33.2%–37.3%) 1.00 1.00 2890, 2462
Yes 80.2% (77.7%–82.5%) 1.81 (1.52–2.16) 1.65 (1.37–1.99) 1436, 1699 40.0% (38.0%–42.0%) 1.22 (1.09–1.38) 1.21 (1.06–1.38) 3106, 2668
Lack of interest in sex
b
p < .0001 p < .0001 ns ns
No 71.5% (69.7%–73.2%) 1.00 1.00 3205, 3959 37.7% (35.9%–39.6%) 1.00 1.00 3190, 2771
Yes 80.4% (76.9%–83.5%) 1.64 (1.31–2.05) 1.62 (1.28–2.05) 711, 841 37.8% (35.6%–40.1%) 1.01 (.89–1.14) 1.03 (.90–1.17) 2170, 1906
Anxiety about
performance
b
p < .0001 p = .001 p < .0001 p = .018
No 71.7% (70.1%–73.3%) 1.00 1.00 3520, 4352 37.0% (35.5%–38.5%) 1.00 1.00 5005, 4364
Yes 85.8% (81.3%–89.3%) 2.37 (1.70–3.32) 1.77 (1.25–2.53) 396, 448 47.8% (41.9%–53.7%) 1.56 (1.22–1.99) 1.38 (1.06–1.81) 355, 312
Unable to experience
orgasm
b
p < .0001 p = .025 p < .0001 p < .0001
No 72.3% (70.7%–73.9%) 1.00 1.00 3670, 4539 36.1% (34.6%–37.6%) 1.00 1.00 4565, 4007
Yes 85.5% (80.0%–89.8%) 2.26 (1.52–3.38) 1.61 (1.06–2.44) 246, 223 47.6% (43.8%–51.6%) 1.61 (1.36–1.91) 1.47 (1.22–1.76) 795, 669
Premature orgasm
b
p = .009 p = .006 ns ns
No 72.3% (70.6%–73.9%) 1.00 1.00 3470, 4234 37.6% (36.2%–39.1%) 1.00 1.00 5292, 4617
Yes 78.8% (74.3%–82.7%) 1.43 (1.09–1.86) 1.50 (1.13–2.00) 446, 566 45.8% (32.7%–59.6%) 1.40 (.80–2.45) 1.24 (.70–2.20) 68, 60
Painful intercourse
b
ns ns ns ns
No 72.9% (71.4%–74.4%) 1.00 1.00 3839, 4711 37.4% (35.9%–38.9%) 1.00 1.00 4754, 4120
Yes 79.0% (67.5%–87.2%) 1.40 (0.77–2.54) 1.15 (0.59–2.23) 77, 88 40.4% (36.1%–44.9%) 1.14 (.93–1.38) 1.02 (.83–1.26) 606, 556
Unable to achieve or
maintain erection
b
p = .002 ns na na na na
No 72.4% (70.8%–74.0%) 1.00 1.00 3648, 4510
Yes 83.1% (76.9%–87.9%) 1.88 (1.26–2.80) 1.49 (0.98–2.28) 268, 290
Trouble lubricating
b
na na na na p < .0001 p < .0001
No 36.3% (34.8%–37.8%) 1.00 1.00 4861, 4243
Yes 52.1% (47.1%–57.0%) 1.91 (1.55–2.35) 1.61 (1.29–2.02) 499, 433
a
Odds ratio (OR) adjusted for age, marital status, any children, ethnicity (coded white vs. other), social class, ever had homosexual sex, partner numbers in the last year.
b
Among those reporting at least one heterosexual and/or homosexual partner in the last year.
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
Factors relating to current/recent sexual attitudes
A larger proportion of participants who were considered as
having relatively liberal attitudes about sex and sexual rela-
tions (see Table 7 for definition) reported masturbation (AOR
of 1.66 for both men and women). Participants who said that
they found it difficult to talk to their sexual partner(s) about
sex were significantly less likely to report masturbation than
those who did not report this difficulty. Regarding satisfac-
tion with their current level of sexual activity, among those
who reported sex in the last year, men and women who said
that the amount of sex they were having was “about right”
or that they would like sex “less often” were significantly
less likely to report masturbation, even after adjustment than
those who would like sex “much” or “a bit” more often. In
terms of enjoyment of sex, men who said that they “always
enjoy it” were significantly less likely to report masturbation
than men who said “I enjoy it most of the time,” “I don’t
often enjoy it” or “I never enjoy it.” No such association was
observed for women.
Discussion
In this large, nationally representative sample of adults in
Britain, masturbation was relatively common but the gender
difference in prevalence was striking, with 28.8% of women,
but only 5.4% of men reporting that they had never mastur-
bated. Although reported rates of masturbation in different
studies vary widely, our findings with regard to factors asso-
ciated with masturbation were broadly consonant with those
from other studies.
Reporting masturbation was most common among those
aged 25-34 years, the better educated, and those who re-
ported a “white” ethnicity. These findings were consis-
tent with previous national surveys in the U.S. and France
(Laumann et al., 1994; Spira et al., 1994). The positive as-
sociations between reporting masturbation and any previous
homosexual experiences, as well as between reporting mas-
turbation and an earlier age at first sex (among women only)
confirmed findings reported on a Finnish sample (Kontula
& Haavio-Mannila, 2002). The significant increase in re-
ported masturbation among both men and women as level
of education and social class increased supports associations
reported in other national surveys. These associations have
been attributed to greater access to public debate, to infor-
mation, and to sex education, which may help reduce fears
and guilt (Kontula & Haavio-Mannila, 2002), and increased
willingness to report masturbation.
The inverse association among women between report-
ing masturbation and stronger religious beliefs mirrors find-
ings from the French national survey (Spira et al., 1994).
Kinsey et al. (1953) also noted that “devoutness” rather than
religious affiliation was related to a reduced likelihood of
masturbation, particularly for women.
There were some limitations to our study. We asked only
one question–about the last occasion of masturbation–in the
Natsal 2000 survey. We have no information on the frequency
of masturbation or on attitudes towards masturbation. Fur-
ther, despite the use of a computer assisted technique of
interviewing found to be more reliable for reporting sensi-
tive behaviors than face-to-face data collection techniques
or pen and paper methods (Johnson et al., 2001; Johnson,
Wadsworth, Wellings, Bradshaw, & Field, 1992; Johnson,
Wadsworth, Wellings, & Field, 1994), sensitive sexual be-
haviors, such as masturbation, may still be under-reported in
sexual health surveys (Copas et al., 2002). Despite the rel-
atively high reported prevalence of masturbation in Natsal
2000, it is a solitary sex act that remains somewhat stigma-
tized, is not discussed as openly as other sexual activities, and
is associated with guilt and myths (Cardamakis et al., 1993;
Coleman, 2002; Kitamura, 1990). It is, therefore, possible
that social acceptability bias led to some under-reporting in
Natsal 2000. Female under-reporting was clearly identified
in the 1993 French sexual behavior survey (B
´
ejin, 1996),
which may explain some of the gender gap observed in the
Natsal 2000 data, but is unlikely to account for such a large
and consistent difference.
The differential reporting between men and women was
entirely consistent with findings from large-scale national
surveys carried out in other countries (Kontula & Haavio-
Mannila, 2002; Laumann et al., 1994; Oliver & Hyde, 1993;
Spira et al., 1994). While gender differences in other sexual
behaviors have diminished, with respect to masturbation they
remain substantial (Hyde, 2005). Various explanations have
been put forward for this, including differences in anatomy
and in sexual development (B
´
ejin, 1996). Furthermore, boys
may be more socialized to masturbate by their peers than girls
(Kontula & Haavio-Mannila, 2002; Laumann et al., 1994).
Some have argued that differential rates of masturbation may
reflect a gender difference in sex drive and sexual fantasizing,
with women having a lower sex drive than men (Baumeister,
Catanese, & Vohs, 2001), and men more likely to report
sexual fantasies (Leitenberg & Henning, 1995). Men are
also more likely to report becoming aroused by their fantasies
(Ellis & Symons, 1990) and to fantasize during masturbation
(Leitenberg & Henning, 1995).
During the developmental qualitative study for the first
Natsal survey, Spencer et al. (1988) found that people as-
sociated masturbation with males, particularly adolescent
boys, and that it was viewed as a substitute for intercourse
with a partner. Interviewers suspected these views led to
some under-reporting of masturbation. The data presented
here do not support the idea that masturbation was a substi-
tute for partnered sex, at least for women. On the contrary,
among women, reporting masturbation was associated with
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
Table 7 Selected factors relating to current/recent sexual attitudes and their association with reporting masturbation in the last 4 weeks, by gender
Denominator Denominator
Men (unweighted/ Women (unweighted/
Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted) Prevalence% (95% CI) Crude OR Adjusted OR
a
weighted)
All 73.0% (71.5%–74.4%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 4410, 5281 36.8% (35.4%–38.2%) (95% CI) (95% CI) 5999, 5132
Liberal attitudes towards
sex/sexual relations
b
p < .0001 p = .001 p < .0001 p < .0001
No 71.3% (69.6%–72.8%) 1.00 1.00 3802, 4601 35.3% (33.9%–36.7%) 1.00 1.00 5432, 4679
Yes 84.6% (81.0%–87.6%) 2.21 (1.70–2.87) 1.66 (1.25–2.22) 608, 680 52.2% (47.5%–56.9%) 2.00 (1.64–2.44) 1.66 (1.33–2.06) 567, 453
Find it difficult to talk about
sexwithpartner(s)
c
p = .002 ns p < .0001 p < .0001
No 76.2% (73.8%–78.4%) 1.00 1.00 1656, 1885 45.5% (42.3%–48.9%) 1.00 1.00 1141, 941
Yes 71.2% (69.3%–73.0%) .77 (.66–.91) .84 (.70–1.00) 2754, 3396 34.8% (33.4%–36.3%) .64 (.55–.74) .72 (.61–.85) 4858, 4191
Satisfaction with amount of
sex
d
p < .0001 p < .0001 p < .0001 p < .0001
Would like sex “much/a
bit more often”
81.9% (80.0%–83.7%) 1.00 1.00 2035, 2452 45.5% (43.2%–47.9%) 1.00 1.00 2207, 1833
About right” 64.2% (61.8%–66.6%) .40 (.34–.47) .40 (.33–.48) 1831, 2296 33.1% (31.2%–35.0%) .59 (.52–.67) .63 (.55–.72) 2986, 2701
Would like sex “less
often”
45.9% (30.9%–61.7%) .19 (.10–.36) .21 (.10–.45) 50, 50 28.2% (21.0%–36.6%) .47 (.32–.70) .58 (.37–.91) 165, 142
Enjoyment of sex
d
p < .0001 p = .013 ns ns
“When I have sex these
days I always enjoy it”
70.2% (68.1%–72.3%) 1.00 1.00 2228, 2821 38.0% (35.7%–40.4%) 1.00 1.00 2007, 1753
“When I have sex these
days I enjoy it most of
the time/I don’t often
enjoy it/I never enjoy it”
76.8% (74.4%–78.9%) 1.40 (1.19–1.64) 1.25 (1.05–1.48) 1623, 1913 37.5% (35.6%–39.4%) .98 (.86–1.11) 1.00 (.87–1.14) 3159, 2784
a
Odds ratio (OR) adjusted for age, marital status, any children, ethnicity (coded white vs. other), social class, ever had homosexual sex, partner numbers in the last year.
b
Defined as reporting that pre-marital sex, one-night stands, sexual relations between two adult men, and sexual relations between two adult women are all “not wrong at all.”
c
The corresponding question was worded “Some people who have sex together find it easy to talk openly about it, others find it difficult to talk openly about it for example to tell each other
what they like and dislike in sex. What about you, how easy or difficult would it be for you?” Those who reported: “Easy with a husband, wife or regular partner, but difficult with a new partner,”
“Easy with a new partner, but difficult with a husband, wife or regular partner,” or “Difficult with any partner” were coded as Yes for this dichotomous variable.
d
Among those reporting at least one heterosexual and/or homosexual partner in the last year.
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
reporting more frequent sexual activity as well as more sex-
ual partners. Furthermore, women reporting a wide range
of sexual practices and/or reporting being comfortable dis-
cussing sex with their partners were also more likely to report
masturbating in the last four weeks. In contrast, among men,
those reporting vaginal sex in the last four weeks were less
likely to report masturbation than those who reported no
vaginal sex. Also, for men only, the likelihood of mastur-
bation decreased with increasing frequency of partnered sex
and increased among those who reported less enjoyment in
sex with a current partner.
As a broad generalization, it is difficult to avoid the con-
clusion that masturbation for many predominantly hetero-
sexual men may represent a substitute for vaginal sex, while
for women the practice appears to be part of the wider reper-
toire of sexual fulfilment, supplementing, rather than com-
pensating for, partnered sex among women. Some women
may be more sexually adventurous and more interested in
sex, and this phenomenon may be reappearing in the context
of masturbation. It is not clear what factors might be driv-
ing these gender differences. Baumeister’s (2000) hypothesis
that women have greater “erotic plasticity” may be relevant
here. If women’s sex drive is more malleable than men’s, it
may be more dependent on their current sexual situation and
level of stimulation. Thus, when a woman is more sexually
active, her sex drive (including her desire for masturbation)
may be higher as a result. Although other studies have de-
scribed associations between masturbation and other sexual
activities, there have been few reports of gender differences
in this respect. Laumann et al. (1994), for example,found that
both women and men with higher levels of “autoeroticism”
(a composite variable comprised of masturbation, sexual fan-
tasy, and use of erotica materials) were more likely to engage
in oral and/or anal sex and to have multiple partners.
Kinsey’s view of masturbation as one of several “outlets”
resulting in orgasm may prove more relevant for women than
men. Women who are more sexualand comfortable with their
sexuality may masturbate more because they are more likely
to climax during masturbation than during intercourse. A
study that asked men and women in steady relationships to
describe their last intercourse and last masturbation found
that, although more men and women reported reaching or-
gasm through masturbation than through intercourse, the dif-
ference was small among men but substantial among women
(Dekker & Schmidt, 2002). Although the Natsal 2000 study
did not ask participants about frequency or context of or-
gasm, those reporting inability to experience orgasm were
more likely to report masturbation, but we did not find any
significant gender differences in this respect.
This study provided an initial exploration of the factors
associated with reporting masturbation in a large national
probability sample of the British population, which we hope
will encourage further investigation into a common sexual
act that provides pleasure without the risks that place sexual
health on the public health agenda. Proponents of masturba-
tion as a means of enhancing sexual health have called for
research on masturbation that goes beyond asking whether,
and how often, people masturbate, and to what extent guilt is
experienced (Coleman, 2002). Understanding gendered pat-
terns with regard to the possible function sexual practices
may serve is important in the context of attempts to harness
such sexual activities to public health and preventive goals.
Acknowledgments We would like to thank the study participants
and the team of interviewers, operations, and computing staff from
the National Centre for Social Research. The 2000 National Survey of
Sexual Attitudes and Lifestyles (Natsal 2000) was supported by a grant
from the Medical Research Council with funds from the Department of
Health, the Scottish Executive and the National Assembly for Wales.
The views expressed in this paper are the authors and do not necessarily
reflect the views of the funding bodies.
References
Barbey, M. A. (1991). Switzerland’s videotext computer sex education
programme. Planned Parenthood in Europe, 20, 22–23.
Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The
female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychological
Bulletin, 126, 347–374.
Baumeister, R. F., Catanese, K. R., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Is there a
gender difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, con-
ceptual distinctions, and a review of relevant evidence. Personality
and Social Psychology Review, 5, 242–273.
B
´
ejin, A. (1996). Female masturbation in France: Estimation and anal-
ysis of an under-reported practice. In M. Bozon & H. Leridon
(Eds), Sexuality and the social sciences: A French survey on sex-
ual behaviour (pp. 253–264). Dartmouth: Aldershot.
Bullough, V. L. (2002). Masturbation: A historical overview. Journal
of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 14, 17–33.
Cardamakis, E., Vinakos, G., Lambou, T., & Papathanasiou, Z. (1993).
Comments by the “Information by Phone” department of the Sex
Medical Institute on the telephone calls related to sexuality and
contraception. European Journal of Obstetrics, Gynecology, and
Reproductive Biology, 52, 125–129.
Coleman, E. (2002). Masturbation as a means of achieving sexual
health. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 14, 5–16.
Copas, A. J., Wellings, K., Erens, B., Mercer, C. H., McManus, S.,
Fenton, K. A., et al. (2002). The accuracy of reported sensitive
sexual behaviour in Britain: Exploring the extent of change 1990-
2000. Sexually Transmitted Infections, 78, 26–30.
Darby, R. (2003). The masturbation taboo and the rise of routine male
circumcision: A review of the historiography. Journal of Social
History, 36, 737–757.
Dekker, A., & Schmidt G. (2002). Patterns of masturbatory behaviour:
Changes between the sixties and the nineties. Journal of Psychol-
ogy and Human Sexuality, 14, 35–48.
Editorial. (1994). The politics of masturbation. Lancet, 344, 1714-
1715.
Ellis, B. J., & Symons, D. (1990). Sex differences in sexual fantasy:
An evolutionary psychological approach. Journal of Sex Research,
27, 527–56.
Erens, B., McManus, S., Field, J., Korovessis, C., Johnson, A. M.,
Fenton, K. A., et al. (2001). National survey of sexual attitudes
and lifestyles II: Technical report. London: National Centre for
Social Research.
Springer
Arch Sex Behav
Fenton, K. A., Mercer, C. H., McManus, S., Erens, B., Wellings, K.,
Macdowall, W., et al. (2005). Ethnic variations in sexual behaviour
in Great Britain and risk of sexually transmitted infections: A
probability survey. Lancet, 365, 1246–1255.
Heiman, D. R., & LoPiccolo, J. (1988). Becoming orgasmic: A sexual
and personal growth program for women.NewYork:Simon&
Schuster.
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psy-
chologist, 60, 581–592.
International Planned Parenthood Federation (IPPF), Office for East,
South East Asia and Oceania Region (ESEAOR). (1996).
Teenagers’ concerns about sex. People and Development Chal-
lenges, 3, 11–12.
Johnson, A. M., Copas, A. J., Erens, B., Mandalia, S., Fenton, K.,
Korovessis, C., et al. (2001). Effect of computer-assisted self-
interviews on reporting of sexual HIV risk behaviours in a general
population sample: A methodological experiment. AIDS, 15, 111–
115.
Johnson, A. M., Mercer, C. H., Erens, B., Copas, A. J., McManus,
S., Wellings, K., et al. (2001). Sexual behaviour in Britain: Part-
nerships, practices, and HIV risk behaviours. Lancet, 358, 1835–
1842.
Johnson, A. M., Wadsworth, J., Wellings, K., Bradshaw, S., & Field,
J. (1992). Sexual lifestyles and HIV risk. Nature, 360, 410–412.
Johnson, A. M., Wadsworth, J., Wellings, K., & Field, J. (1994). Sexual
attitudes and lifestyles. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Press.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior
in the human male. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & Gebhard, P. H. (1953).
Sexual behavior in the human female. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Kitamura, K. (1990). Communicating with adolescents. Telephone
counselling and adolescent health clinic services of the Japan Fam-
ily Planning Association. Integration 25, 40–41.
Kontula, O., & Haavio-Mannila, E. (2002). Masturbation in a genera-
tional perspective. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality,
14, 49–83.
Kuriansky, J. (1996). Sexuality advice on the radio: An overview in the
United States and around the world. SIECUS Report, 24, 6–9.
Laqueur, T. W. (2003). Solitary sex: A cultural history of masturbation.
New York: Zone Books.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994).
The social organizationof sexuality: Sexual practices in the United
States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Leitenberg, H., & Henning, K. (1995). Sexual fantasy. Psychological
Bulletin, 117, 469–496.
Lynn P., & Clarke P. (2002). Separating refusal bias and non-contact
bias: Evidence from UK national surveys. The Statistician, 51,
319–333.
Mercer, C. H., Fenton, K. A., Johnson, A. M., Copas, A. J., Wellings,
K., Macdowall, W., et al. (2003). Sexual function problems and
help seeking behaviour in Britain: National probability sample
survey. British Medical Journal, 327, 426–427.
Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29–51.
Pinkerton, S. D., Bogart, L. M., Cecil, H., & Abramson, P. R. (2002).
Factors associated with masturbation in a collegiate sample. Jour-
nal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 14, 103–121.
Robinson, B. E., Bockting, W. O., & Harrell, T. (2002). Masturbation
and sexual health: An exploratory study of low income African
American women. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality,
14, 85–101.
Royal College of Physicians, Psychiatrists and General Practitioners.
(1995). Alcohol and the heart in perspective: Sensible limits reaf-
firmed. London: Royal College of Physicians, Psychiatrists and
General Practitioners.
Spencer, L. A., Faulkner, A., & Keegan, J. (1988). Talking about sex.
London: Social and Community Planning Research.
Spira, A., Bajos, N., & ACSF Group. (1994). Sexual behaviour and
AIDS. Aldershot: Avebury.
Wellings, K., Nanchahal, K., Macdowall, W., McManus, S., Erens, B.,
Mercer, C. H., et al. (2001). Sexual behaviour in Britain: Early
heterosexual experience. Lancet, 358, 1843–1850.
Zamboni, B. D., & Crawford, I. (2002). Using masturbation in sex
therapy: Relationships between masturbation, sexual desire, and
sexual fantasy. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 14,
123–141.
Springer
... Masturbation is prevalent in countries around the world including Australia, China, Croatia, Denmark, Finland, Germany, the UK, and the USA (Baćak & Štulhofer, 2011;Burri & Carvalheira, 2019;Das et al., 2009;Gerressu et al., 2008;Haavio-Mannila et al., 2003;Hald, 2006;Herbenick et al., 2010;Lindau et al., 2018;Richters et al., 2014). More men than women report lifetime masturbation and recent masturbation; also, men report more frequent masturbation as compared to women (Gerressu et al., 2008;Herbenick et al., 2010;Oliver & Hyde, 1993;Petersen & Hyde, 2011). ...
... Masturbation is prevalent in countries around the world including Australia, China, Croatia, Denmark, Finland, Germany, the UK, and the USA (Baćak & Štulhofer, 2011;Burri & Carvalheira, 2019;Das et al., 2009;Gerressu et al., 2008;Haavio-Mannila et al., 2003;Hald, 2006;Herbenick et al., 2010;Lindau et al., 2018;Richters et al., 2014). More men than women report lifetime masturbation and recent masturbation; also, men report more frequent masturbation as compared to women (Gerressu et al., 2008;Herbenick et al., 2010;Oliver & Hyde, 1993;Petersen & Hyde, 2011). In the 2009 National Survey of Sexual Health Behavior (NSSHB), a U.S. nationally representative survey of 5865 men and women ages 14-94, 28% of men ages 70 + , 43% of men ages 14-15 and 60-69, and more than half of men ages 16-59 reported having engaged in solo masturbation in the prior month (Herbenick et al., 2010). ...
... Although solo masturbation is a valid sexual behavior in its own right, researchers frequently consider it in relation to partnered sex, proposing compensatory and complementary models of masturbation. The compensatory model suggests an inverse relationship between solo masturbation and partnered sex, proposing that a major reason people masturbate is to make up for a lack of partnered sex (Gerressu et al., 2008;Regnerus et al., 2017). In a sense, the compensatory model suggests that solo masturbation "competes" with partnered sex; that is, a person may only have so much desire, built up sexual tension, or capacity for sex. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite well-documented individual, relational, and health benefits, masturbation has been stigmatized and is understudied compared to partnered sex. In a US nationally representative survey of adults, we aimed to: (1) assess the prevalence and frequency of participants’ prior-year masturbation, (2) describe reasons people give for not masturbating, (3) describe reasons people give for masturbating, and (4) examine the association between masturbation frequency and actual/desired partnered sex frequency in the prior year. Significantly more men than women reported lifetime masturbation, past month masturbation, and greater masturbation frequency. The most frequently endorsed reasons for masturbating related to pleasure, feeling “horny,” stress relief, and relaxation. The most frequently endorsed reasons for not masturbating were lack of interest, being in a committed relationship, conflict with morals or values, or being against one’s religion. Among women, those who desired partnered sex much more often and a little more often were 3.89 times (95% CI: 2.98, 5.08) and 2.07 times (95% CI: 1.63, 2.62), respectively, more likely to report higher frequencies of past-year masturbation than those who desired no change in their partnered sex frequency. Among men, those who desired partnered sex much more often and a little more often were 4.40 times (95% CI: 3.41, 5.68) and 2.37 times (95% CI: 1.84, 3.06), respectively, more likely to report higher frequencies of past-year masturbation activity than those who reported that they desired no change in their current partnered sex frequency. Findings provide contemporary U.S. population-level data on patterns of adult masturbation.
... Previous studies have highlighted the importance of considering covariates that could play a significant role in the subjective orgasm experience, such as age, education level, and having a partner or not [5,17,21,22], as well as variables more specific to the context of masturbation, such as age at first masturbation, frequency of masturbation, and religious frequency [19,[23][24][25]. The results found in the previous literature require us to further explore the role of solitary sexual desire and its relevance to other dimensions of sexual functioning. ...
Article
Full-text available
The tridimensional sexual desire proposal (i.e., dyadic to partner, dyadic to attractive other and solitary) has been empirically supported. However, solitary sexual desire and its relationship to other dimensions of sexual functioning has received less attention. Hence, we examined the capacity of solitary sexual desire to explain the subjective orgasm experience (Study 1) and sexual arousal (Study 2) in the context of solitary masturbation. Study 1, composed of 2406 heterosexual adults (M age = 39.72, SD = 11.81), assessed for solitary sexual desire, dyadic sexual desire, and the intensity of the subjective orgasm experience obtained through solitary masturbation, along with other associated parameters. Study 2, consisting of 41 heterosexual young people (M age = 22.49, SD = 3.17), evaluated the genital response (penile circumference/vaginal pulse amplitude) and subjective arousal to sexually explicit films related to solitary masturbation. In both men and women, solitary sexual desire accounted for a significant percentage of the subjective orgasm experience obtained through solitary masturbation. In addition, in women, the propensity for sexual arousal was explained by solitary sexual desire. It is concluded that solitary sexual desire -as opposed to dyadic- is important to explain sexual arousal and orgasm in the solitary masturbation context. These results highlight the importance of addressing sexual desire in the solitary context, given its implications with other dimensions of sexual functioning.
... Hence, this could imply that women with high masturbation activity may feel closer to their partner. According to , this gendered masturbation pattern support gender-specific models-a notion supported by other studies (Carvalheira & Leal, 2013;Fischer et al., 2021;Gerressu et al., 2008;Regnerus et al., 2017), and an indication that the analysis of IOS should be conducted separate for men and women. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explores the connection between relationship duration and feelings of closeness in Norwegian men and women, and the association with sexual satisfaction and activity. A sample of 4,160 Norwegians aged 18–89 years was enrolled from a randomly selected web panel of 11,685 Norwegians. This study focused on participants who were married or cohabiting (1,432 men, 1,207 women). Closeness was the highest for men and women who had been with their partner for 0−6 years. However, among those who had been with their partner for 31 years or longer, men felt closer to their partners than women. Irrespective of relationship duration, the most important factor for both men and women’s perceived closeness with their partner was general sexual satisfaction. Among men who had lived with their partner for 7−20 years and 31 years or longer, having been monogamous in life was significantly associated with “inclusion of others in the self” (IOS). Further, closeness was associated with higher intercourse frequency, lower masturbation frequency, and satisfaction with genital appearance in men who had been with their partners for 31 years or more. Intercourse frequency was significantly associated with IOS in women who had been with their partner for 0−6 years. Furthermore, in women who had been with their partner for 31 years or more, satisfaction with their own weight was important for IOS. In conclusion, men and women reported similar degrees and patterns of IOS up to the point where they had been in their relationship for more than 30 years. Thereafter, women reported feeling less close to their partners, while men’s feelings of closeness increased. This may be related to physiological, psychological, and social changes in the lives of aging men and women.
Article
Objective: To analyze aspects of sexual life and fertility desire among 46, XY DSD people, including those who changed their gender. Methods: It is a cross-sectional study including 127 adults (> 16 years of age) with 46, XY DSD (83 females; 44 males) from a Single Brazilian Tertiary-Care Medical Center. Results: Sexual fantasies and masturbation were more frequent in 46, XY DSD males, whereas orgasm and sexual life satisfaction were similar in both genders. More 46, XY DSD men than women had a long-term romantic relationship. 46, XY DSD women with prenatal androgen exposure reported more fear of being romantically rejected. External genitalia appearance at birth did not impact the sexuality of 46, XY DSD women after surgical genital treatment had been completed. Overall, the sexual life was similar between 46, XY men assigned as males and those who changed to the male gender. Regarding sexual orientation, most self-reported as heterosexual (91% and 92% of women and men, respectively). The desire for fertility had a similar prevalence in both genders, but more women than men considered infertility a barrier to a long-term romantic relationship. Twelve individuals (7 males) had children; 10 out of 12 have adopted children. Conclusion: Fertility desire was shared among 46, XY DSD people, regardless of gender. Prenatal androgen exposure reduced the desire for motherhood in 46, XY women. 46, XY DSD people who changed from female to male gender presented similar sexual parameters as those assigned as males. Among females, virilized genitalia at birth did not affect sexuality once the surgical treatment is completed.
Article
Full-text available
To investigate the impact and factors of home quarantine life on women’s sexual lives and behaviors in different areas of China and analyze the prevalence of female sexual dysfunction (FSD) during the COVID-19 pandemic. We surveyed adult women who had a regular sexual life (including regular masturbation) and had been isolated at home for at least one month during the COVID-19 outbreak using online questionnaires. This survey recovered 678 complete questionnaires after screening. According to the findings, the overall score of the Female Sexual Function Inventory (FSFI) during the pandemic was 21.98 ± 6.38, the frequency of FSD was 61.9%, and the frequencies of FSD in Shanghai, Nanjing, and Ningxia were 60.6%, 75.2%, and 52.2%, respectively. The frequency of FSFI scores and other specific items (Desire, Arousal, Lubrication, Orgasm, Satisfaction, and Pain) varied significantly across the three regions (P < 0.05). The overall frequency of FSD in the masturbation population was 34.4%, which was lower than the frequency of FSD in women having paired sexual intercourse (60.1%) (p < 0.05). Further analysis revealed that the occurrence of FSD during the pandemic was related to different age stages, menopause, mode of delivery, level of anxiety and depression, and sexual lifestyles. The COVID-19 pandemic has had a great impact on people’s spiritual and sexual lives, which are caused by multiple different variables related to both the individual and the environment. We should emphasize the importance of sexual health in epidemics, and having a harmonious and stable sex life will help us survive the boring life of isolation.
Article
Many vertebrate animals engage in masturbation and it is also prevalent in primates. Given the gregarious nature of this order, this is perhaps surprising, since, by definition, it occurs to the exclusion of others. Our research maps the masturbatory landscape of the primate order, highlighting the distribution and diverse forms self-stimulation of the genitalia takes: from an infant vervet monkey grasping his own penis in his mouth, to female chimpanzees using water spigots to stimulate their clitorises. We also examine the causation of this behavior. While autosexual behavior can be a substitute for allosexual interactions, many acts of masturbation seem to serve functions, which fall broadly under two categories: avoidance of pathogen transmission and reduction of mate competition. In terms of implications for human public health, the finding that masturbation is ubiquitous throughout the primate order, practiced by wild-living members of both sexes and all age-groups is a strong counter-argument to voices who condemn human masturbation as "unnatural."
Article
The global coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic has significantly altered the lives of college students across the United States. Following the outbreak of COVID-19 in the spring of 2020, college campuses were shuttered, classes moved to remote instruction, and university activities, celebrations, and events were canceled. Cast against a backdrop of uncertainty about the future, studies have documented that the pandemic has significantly increased anxiety among college students as they adjust to a “new normal.” Drawing from general strain theory, we examine the influence of specific COVID-19-related strains on a variety of changes in student behavior including binge-watching streaming services, splurging on online shipping, sexting, “hooking up” with random people, and masturbating. Results using structural equation models on data from 1,287 students at a Midwestern university show that specific sources of strain directly are related to binge-watching, online shopping, hooking up with random people, and masturbating, while anxiety was directly related to increased binge-watching, online shopping, and sexting. Anxiety mediated the pathways between some sources of strain and binge-watching and splurging on online shopping. Overall, findings highlight that the global pandemic not only induces anxiety and interrupts academic life but also carries far-reaching consequences for a wide range of behaviors.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of Review Sexual fantasy has been the subject of scientific scrutiny for nearly a century. This review outlines how, despite that scrutiny, methodological and definitional limitations make it difficult to speak with authority to the clinical, relational, and behavioral implications of sexual fantasy. Recent Findings Estimates of the frequency and content of sexual fantasy for women are limited by volunteer and social desirability biases and thus difficult to interpret. For the same reasons, the role that fantasy plays in individual and partnered sexual satisfaction and function is challenging to assess. The most reliable and recent evidence suggests that the effects of sexual fantasy on satisfaction and function are, overall, neutral to positive. Summary Given the complexity and limitations in research investigating sexual fantasy in women, we conclude with a call for new approaches in this field and humility in interpreting the existing evidence.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Sexual health includes the state of physical, emotional, mental, and social well-being related to sexuality. Masturbation is an important sexual activity with many potential benefits which has gained considerable interest in sexuality research in the past twenty years; however, this research is the first of its kind within the Aotearoa/New Zealand context. In this in-depth investigation, we examined frequencies of, reasons for, and activities during masturbation as well as the relationship between masturbation and other factors. Methods Participants were 698 New Zealand women at least 18 years of age participating in a 42-item anonymous online survey collecting comprehensive information about sexual practices and related factors. Results The results indicated that female masturbation has high prevalence in the New Zealand population. Conclusion The pattern of results enabled us to identify the positive effects of masturbation, masturbation practices commonly used by New Zealand women and the differences between New Zealand women who masturbate frequently and less frequently.
Article
Sexual behaviour is a major determinant of sexual and reproductive health. We did a National Survey of Sexual Attitudes and Lifestyles (Natsal 2000) in 1999-2001 to provide population estimates of behaviour patterns and to compare them with estimates from 1990-91 (Natsal 1990). Methods We did a probability sample survey of men and women aged 16-44 years who were resident in Britain, using computer-assisted interviews. Results were compared with data from respondents in Natsal 1990. Findings We interviewed 11 161 respondents (4762 men, 6399 women). Patterns of heterosexual and homosexual partnership varied substantially by age, residence in Greater London, and marital status. In the past 5 years, mean numbers of heterosexual partners were 3.8 (SD 8.2) for men, and 2.4 (SD 4.6) for women; 2.6% (95% CI 2.2-3.1) of both men and women reported homosexual partnerships; and 4.3% (95% CI 3.7-5.0) of men reported paying for sex. In the past year, mean number of new partners varied from 2.04 (SD 8.4) for single men aged 25- 34 years to 0.05 (SD 0.3) for married women aged 35-44 years. Prevalence of many reported behaviours had risen compared with data from Natsal 1990. Benefits of greater condom use were offset by increases in reported partners. Changes between surveys were generally greater for women than men and for respondents outside London. Interpretation Our study provides updated estimates of sexual behaviour patterns. The increased reporting of risky sexual behaviours is consistent with changing cohabitation patterns and rising incidence of sexually transmitted infections. Observed differences between Natsal 1990 and Natsal 2000 are likely to result from a combination of true change and greater willingness to report sensitive behaviours in Natsal 2000 due to Improved survey methodology and more tolerant social attitudes.
Article
Masturbation may be useful in treating hypoactive sexual desire disorder. To examine this possibility, the current study examined the relationships between masturbation, sexual desire, sexual fantasy, and dyadic sexual activity. The relationships between masturbation and other constructs were also examined: overall sexual dysfunction, relationship satisfaction, life stress, and sexual attitudes. Mediational analyses suggest several causal pathways between masturbatory desire, masturbatory frequency, sexual fantasy, sexual desire, and sexual activity. Developing a model of masturbation may help illustrate its role in sexual functioning and highlight its potential role in sex therapy.
Article
In this study we applied research examining the hypothesized benefits of masturbation in dealing with sexual problems to the urgent health crisis posed by the HIV/AIDS pandemic. This is the first study to test the hypothesized relationship between masturbation and HIV risk as predicted by the Sexual Health Model, a sex-positive approach to sexual health developed in response to the need for a more explicit focus on sexuality and relationships in HIV prevention. This is also the first study to examine the relationship between several masturbation variables (i.e., masturbation guilt, lifetime masturbation, and current masturbation) and HIV-related sexual behaviors and attitudes in a sample of African American women (N =239). Data was collected using face-to-face structured interviews as part of the Women's Initiative for Sexual Health (WISH), a randomized, controlled trial of an HIV prevention intervention based on the Sexual Health Model, targeting low income, adult African American women. Contrary to expectations, results showed that participants who reported masturbating were more likely to report having multiple partners, being in a nonmonogamous relationship and engaging in high-risk sexual behaviors. There was no significant relationship between level of masturbation guilt and HIV risk nor between masturbation and consistent condom use or attitudes toward condoms. This study adds to the growing empirical support for associations between sexual health variables and safer sex and argues for a more explicit focus on sexuality in HIV prevention.
Article
In 1966, 1981 and 1996 the Department of Sex Research at Hamburg University, Germany, carried out three surveys into the sexual behavior of university students. Taken as a longitudinal study they provide information on the social history of sexuality over the past three decades for well-educated young adults (20 to 30 years old) in Germany. The samples consist of 8,641 men and women. In this paper data from the studies is analyzed under two aspects:1. Shifts in masturbatory behavior 1966–1981–1996. Men and especially women begin to masturbate considerably earlier than used to be in the eighties, not to mention in the sixties. As a consequence, most young women nowadays have already experienced masturbation when having their first heterosexual intercoursethus following a pattern of sexual socialization that traditionally was typical for boys. In addition, in 1996, more students of both sexes had masturbated in the year preceding the investigation. The more relevant point is, however, that young adults (according to active incidence during the last 12 months) now masturbate almost irrespective of whether they have intercourse often or rarely, whether they are singles or live in a steady relationship or whether or not they are satisfied with their current relationship. So masturbation peacefully coexists with sex between partners and a loving relationship more often than it did in 1981 and 1966.2. Masturbation in steady relationships, 1996. A detailed analysis of masturbation frequencies during the last four weeks also shows small differences between students who live in a sexually satisfying relationship and those without relationship. Only a minority feels the need to justify masturbation on the grounds that they lack sex with their partner. Three-quarters expressly state that masturbation is a form of sex in its own right and, therefore, does not interfere with partner sex. Furthermore, there is a tendency that students living in steady relationships experience masturbation slightly more positively than those living alone.