ArticlePDF Available

Associations between iris characteristics and personality in adulthood

Authors:
  • Affiliated (see below).

Abstract and Figures

Variable and person-oriented analyses were used to explore the associations between personality and three previously untested general iris characteristics: crypts, pigment dots and contraction furrows. Personality data, as measured by the NEO PI-R and ratings of iris characteristics from 428 undergraduate students were collected. Crypts were significantly associated with five approach-related behaviors, i.e., feelings, tendermindedness, warmth, trust and positive emotions, whereas furrows were associated with impulsiveness. These findings suggest that because Pax6 induces tissue deficiencies in both the iris and the left anterior cingulate cortex, Pax6 may influence the extent people engage in approach-related behaviors. The results from using a person-oriented analysis suggested that people with different iris configurations tend to develop along different personality trajectories. Future longitudinal studies, twin-studies and genetic association studies, may benefit from collecting iris data and testing candidate genes for crypts and furrows.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Associations between iris characteristics and personality in adulthood
Mats Larsson
a,
*, Nancy L. Pedersen
b
,Ha
˚kan Stattin
a
a
Department of Behavioral, Social and Legal Sciences, Center for Developmental Research, O
¨rebro University, 701 82 O
¨rebro, Sweden
b
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics Karolinska Institutet, Box 281, 171 77 Stockholm, Sweden
Received 10 March 2005; accepted 24 January 2007
Available online 1 February 2007
Abstract
Variable and person-oriented analyses were used to explore the associations between personality and three previously untested general iris
characteristics: crypts, pigment dots and contraction furrows. Personality data, as measured by the NEO PI-R and ratings of iris characteristics from
428 undergraduate students were collected. Crypts were significantly associated with five approach-related behaviors, i.e., feelings, tendermind-
edness, warmth, trust and positive emotions, whereas furrows were associated with impulsiveness. These findings suggest that because Pax6
induces tissue deficiencies in both the iris and the left anterior cingulate cortex, Pax6 may influence the extent people engage in approach-related
behaviors. The results from using a person-oriented analysis suggested that people with different iris configurations tend to develop along different
personality trajectories. Future longitudinal studies, twin-studies and genetic association studies, may benefit from collecting iris data and testing
candidate genes for crypts and furrows.
#2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Personality; Iris characteristics/crypts/pigment dots/contraction furrows; Candidate genes/Pax6/Six3/Lmx1b; Anterior cingulate; Hemispheric
asymmetries
1. Introduction
The idea that personality differences are related to iris
characteristics is not new. In 1965, Cattell (1965) observed
differences in cognitive styles between blue and brown eyed
subjects and since then eye color has been found to be related to a
great variety of physiological and behavioral characteristics.
Dark eyed people have on average higher scores on extraversion,
neuroticism (Gentry et al., 1985), ease of emotional arousal
(Markle, 1976) and sociability (Gary and Glover, 1976).
However, there are a number of studies that fail to replicate
the personality findings, typically because the effect tends to fade
after early childhood. For instance, Rubin and Both (1989) found
that blue-eyed children in kindergarten and Grade 2 were
overrepresented in groups of extremely withdrawn youngsters,
whereas no association could be found in Grade 4 or between eye
color and extreme sociability in any grade. Furthermore, in
subjects around 20 years of age, eye color failed to account for
any difference between high versus low scorers on personality
measures (Lester, 1987; Rim, 1983). Consequently, it has been
assumed that the biological mechanisms that may underlie the
association between eye color and social wariness in early
childhood tends to be ‘overcome’’ around age 9 when the
increasing influence of social environmental factors has gained
momentum (Rubin and Both, 1989). To our knowledge, no
research has been reported concerning eye color and its relation
to personality since 1998, when Coplan et al. (1998) found a
significant eye color by gender interaction for social wariness in a
sample of 3.5–5.5-year-old toddlers. This finding raised further
questions regarding the phenotypic penetrance of the suggested
biological mechanism involved. Leading researchers in the field
concluded that eye color cannot be a useful marker for
personality in older ages (Coplan et al., 1998; Rosenberg and
Kagan, 1989; Rubin and Both, 1989).
One of the reasons eye color has been considered to be a
useful biomarker is its high heritability (98%), and the fact that
eye color does not change much over time (Bito et al., 1997).
Other key reasons are that eye color can be linked to biological
mechanisms that provide a somewhat speculative, but still
plausible, biological explanation for the associations found. For
instance, Kagan suggested that melanin production in the eyes
(the pigment that colors the eye brown), may be associated with
norepinephrine and cortisol production, which are biological
www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho
Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +46 19 303415; fax: +46 19 303484.
E-mail address: mats.larsson@bsr.oru.se (M. Larsson).
0301-0511/$ – see front matter #2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.01.007
indices of behavioral inhibition (Kagan et al., 1988; Reznick,
1989).
High levels of norepinephrine and cortisol can thus
potentially inhibit melanin production in the eye and increase
responsiveness of the limbic system, which has been associated
with higher scores on inhibition (Rosenberg and Kagan, 1989).
Placental cortisol level during the initial stages of pregnancy
and the gene POMC, which are likely to play an important role
in individuals’ responses to varying levels of arousal
(Kupfermann, 1991) and to the synthesis of melanin (Pawelek
and Korner, 1982), have also been suggested to contribute to the
associations found.
However, if one carefully examines people’s irises, it is clear
that there are other iris characteristics than eye color that
potentially could be associated with personality, such as Fuchs’
crypts, contraction furrows and pigment dots. To take one
example, if the biological mechanisms associated with the
frequency of Fuchs’ crypts (a measure characterized by
different degrees of hypoplasia of the anterior border layer
and underlying stroma in the iris), influence behavior, then
biological mechanisms other than those suggested to be
associated with eye color can be responsible for the
associations. This study explores the association between iris
characteristics other than eye color and personality. Three
general iris characteristics that to our knowledge not have been
considered previously will be tested.
The iris characteristics of interest in this study are depicted
in Fig. 1: frequency of Fuchs’ crypts in the main stroma leaf
(which from now on will only be called ‘‘crypts’’); frequency of
pigment dots; the distinction and extension of contraction
furrows. These iris characteristics are moderately to highly
heritable, show no sex differences and are stable over time
(Larsson et al., 2003).
Crypts and contraction furrows are measures that are related
to the thickness and density of the iris. Crypt frequency is
characterized by different degrees of hypoplasia in the two top
cell layers in the iris (i.e., anterior border layer and underlying
stroma), whereas contraction furrows are characterized by
different degrees of hypoplasia or density conditions in any of
the five cell layers present in the iris (Oyster, 1999). What is
intriguing about these cell layers is that genes that influence the
growth of precursors to these cell layers during the
embryological development also cause tissue loss in the brain.
For example, the genes Pax6, Six3 and Lmx1b have been
recognized as candidate genes for crypts and contraction
furrows (Hsieh et al., 2002; Pressman et al., 2000; Sale et al.,
2002) and the embryological events that make these genes
plausible candidates for crypts and contraction furrows have
been reviewed (Larsson and Pedersen, 2004). In addition, the
most recent studies available confirm that Pax6 is a likely
candidate gene for tissue differences in the iris. Pax6 dosage
influences the muscle-cell differentiation in the iris (Jensen,
2005), and Davis-Silberman et al. (2005) conclude that (p.
2474) iris stroma defects are likely to be due to cell-non-
autonomous events, where Pax6 regulates the expression of the
cells that are required for the adhesion of the iris stroma. Thus,
it is possible that Pax6, Six3 and Lmx1b, which influence the
growth of the precursors to the cell layers in the iris, could
influence the variability of crypts and contraction furrows
typically present in people’s irises (Hsieh et al., 2002; Pressman
et al., 2000; Sale et al., 2002; Simpson and Price, 2002).
Furthermore, the expression pattern of thesegenes in the brain
supports the notion that these iris characteristics can be
associated with personality. In the first study published that
has demonstrated a major genetic contribution to frontal lobe
dysfunction, family members with a mutation in Pax6 showed
high rates of unusual behavior including disinhibition, impulsive
behavior, impaired social understanding and impaired verbal
inhibition (Heyman et al., 1999). In addition, magnetic resonance
imaging (MRI) in this family (Ellison-Wright et al., 2004)
demonstrated local gray and white matter changes in anterior
cingulate cortex (approximate Brodmann areas 24 and 32), as
well as posterior white matter abnormalities in corpus callosum
extending posteriorly into cingulate cortex, which are brain areas
that have been associated with personality (Davidson, 2001;
Johnson et al., 1999; Posner and Rothbart, 1998). Healthy
university subjects with larger right anterior cingulate gyrus
(Brodmann’s areas 24a, 24b and 25), score higher on harm
avoidance, which is associated with emotional responses that
regulate withdrawal-related behaviors, whereas subjects with
larger left cingulate gyrus, score higher on Novelty seeking,
which is associated with emotional responses that regulate
approach-related behaviors (Pujol et al., 2002). Indeed,
anatomical data revealed that hemispheric asymmetry in the
anterior cingulate gyrus was very common (83% of
cases, n= 100) and that surface measures of the right anterior
Fig. 1. The numbered arrows point toward the iris characteristics of interest. (1) Furchs’ crypts in the main stroma leaf; (2) pigment dots; (3) contraction furrows.
M. Larsson et al. / Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175166
cingulate gyrus accounted for 24% of the variance in harm
avoidance. Overall, these findings are consistent with the
assumed specialization of each hemisphere in the control of
individual differences in withdrawal- and approach-related
behaviors (Davidson, 1992, 2001; Davidson and Irwin, 1999;
Pujol et al., 2002).
Moreover, the fact that the production of dopamine (Kohwi
et al., 2005) and noradrenalin neurons (Jaworski et al., 1997)
has been associated with Pax6 functioning provides another
link to personality (Reif and Lesch, 2003; Rosenberg and
Kagan, 1989). But Pax6 is not the only candidate gene for crypt
frequency and contraction furrows that potentially could
influence personality. Lmx1b is required for the survival of
the dopaminergic neurons in the mesolimbic system (Burbach
et al., 2003). It is also essential for the production of
serotoninergic neurons (Ding et al., 2003), and Six3, which
is a downstream target to Pax6 (Simpson and Price, 2002) and
which controls the division of the brain into two hemispheres
(Wallis and Muenke, 2000). Thus, based on these findings we
assume that tissue differences in the iris, such as crypts and
contraction furrows, could be associated with Pax6, Six3 and
Lmx1b, which in turn could be associated with personality.
The links between pigment dots and personality are less well
documented, but nevertheless, pigment dots could potentially be
associated with personality, as the development of pigment dots
is in part regulated by neurotransmitters produced by the
autonomic nervous system (Hu, 2000; Hu et al., 2000; Mukuno
and Witmer, 1977), which in turn is influenced by the extent the
individual engages in withdrawal- and approach-related beha-
viors (Davidson and Irwin, 1999). Thus, through the expression
patterns of Pax6, Six3 and Lmx1b in the iris and the brain, crypts,
contraction furrows and pigment dots may be associated with
both brain structures and neurotransmitters that influence
personality. However, there is no evidence available that shows
that these genes influence crypts and contractionfurrows directly.
In addition, other candidate genes for crypts and contraction
furrows have been recognized (Larsson and Pedersen, 2004).
In order to discriminate between the biological pathways
that theoretically could influence the presumed association
between iris characteristics and personality, and to more fully
evaluate a potential role of Pax6, we test three hypotheses. They
draw heavily on the Pax6 expression pattern in the anterior
cingulate cortex as documented by Ellison-Wright et al. (2004),
in conjunction with the findings made by Pujol et al. (2002),
Davidson (1992, 2001) and Davidson and Irwin (1999). These
hypotheses increase in their specificity. If all three hypotheses
are supported, we believe there is a high likelihood that Pax6,
rather than other candidates, is involved. Thus, based on the
findings reviewed above, we expect, first, that crypts more often
than other iris characteristics will be associated with
personality. Crypts are related to greater tissue loss in the
iris than the other two iris characteristics, which in turn may
reflect greater tissue loss in the brain (which makes it likely that
more measures of personality are associated with crypts than
the other iris characteristics). Second, because the tissue
damage that was due to Pax6 mutations in anterior cingulate
cortex primarily affected the left hemisphere (Ellison-Wright
et al., 2004), and this hemisphere primarily relates to approach-
related behaviors (Davidson, 1992, 2001; Davidson and Irwin,
1999), we expect that crypt frequency in most cases will be
related approach-related behaviors. Third, because greater
tissue loss in the iris, may reflect greater tissue loss in the left
anterior cingulate cortex, we expect individuals with an open
crypt structure (those with more tissue loss in the iris) to score
lower on traits that relate to approach-related behaviors than
those with a dense crypt structure. Furthermore, in order to
deepen the analysis, the present study combines variable- and
person-oriented approaches. As has been shown repeatedly
within a holistic interactionistic framework, person-oriented
approaches are able to answer different questions than more
traditional variable-oriented approaches (Bergman and Mag-
nusson, 1997; Magnusson and Stattin, 1998; von Eye and
Bergman, 2003). For example, in variable-oriented analysis the
relationship among variables and their way of functioning in the
totality of an individual is assumed to be the same for all
individuals. However, this prerequisite may not be fulfilled in
the present study since most of the genetic variance for each iris
characteristic is independent of the other iris characteristics
(Larsson and Pedersen, 2004). In other words, the behavioral
meaning of having a specific crypt frequency may depend on
which other iris characteristics are present in any one iris
(Lander and Schork, 1994). Person-oriented analysis takes
these possibilities into consideration and is based on the
perspective that the individual organism can be thoroughly
understood in its totality (Bergman and Magnusson, 1997).
Furthermore, this totality is assumed to result from self-
organization. This means that although there are, theoretically,
an infinite variety of differences with regard to process
characteristics and observed states at a detailed level, at a more
global level there will often be a small number of more
frequently observed patterns (Bergman and Magnusson, 1997;
Bergman et al., 2003). This principle of self-organization has
been demonstrated in many human biological systems and has
been applied to the development of the neural, sensory and
cognitive systems (Post and Weiss, 1997) as well as personality
(Bosma and Kunnen, 2002; Carver and Scheier, 2002).
Consequently, we expect that from the embryological events
that form the adult iris (detailed level), distinct configurations
of iris characteristics will occur in the population studied.
Furthermore, if the iris characteristics that define these
naturally occurring configurations are associated with person-
ality, then we expect that these configurations will define
subgroups of individuals that differ from each other on
personality in a systematic manner. Developmentally, the
individuals in these subgroups may represent different
trajectories with regard to personality, and subgroups with
more tissue loss in the iris are, just as in the variable-oriented
analysis, expected to score lower on approach-related
behaviors. Moreover, because the relationship among iris
variables and their way of functioning in the totality of an
individual is not likely to be the same for all individuals (Lander
and Schork, 1994; Larsson and Pedersen, 2004), the effect size
in the variable-oriented analysis is expected to be lower than in
the person-oriented analyses.
M. Larsson et al. / Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175 167
To sum up, the purpose of this study is to explore the
association between iris characteristics other than eye color and
personality, using both variable- and person-oriented analyses,
as well as highlight genes expressed in the iris that potentially
could be candidate genes for personality. In performing the
variable-oriented analysis, we test three hypotheses, which
together explore the likelihood that the neurodevelopmental
control gene Pax6 influences personality. In performing the
person-oriented analyses, we test the extent to which self-
organizing properties of the developmental system create
configurations of iris characteristics that can identify subgroups
of individuals who differ in their personality. In comparing the
effect size in the variable- and person-oriented analyses, we
explore how efficiently the self-organizing properties of the
developmental system can identify individuals who differ in
their personality.
2. Method
2.1. Participants and recruitment procedure
The participants were all undergraduate student volunteers at O
¨rebro
University, Sweden, recruited from programs in psychology (47%), social
studies (30%), economics (10%), automatic data processing (5%), political
science (4%) and engineering (4%). Most participants were female (80% of 428
participants). The mean age of the sample was 26.6 years (19–52 years,
S.D. = 7.1). The participants were recruited after a regular lecture by the first
author, who was given an opportunity to describe the study. Consenting
participants filled out the NEO PI-R personality questionnaire in a location
where they were not disturbed (Costa and McCrea, 1992). However, psychology
and social studies students responded to a computerized version of NEO PI-R in
a group setting with 15 students. Close-up iris color photography was taken
individually in a lab setting at the university.
There were two waves of data collection. In the first wave of data collection,
all students that volunteered were accepted as participants. In the second wave,
only those individuals with irises that were most similar to scale-step 1 and 3–5
on the crypt frequency scale (see below) were accepted. This was done in order
to arrive at approximately the same number of participants with each level of
crypt frequency.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Personality
Personality was assessed with the Swedish version of the NEO PI-R (Costa
and McCrea, 1992), which is considered to be a reliable and valid assessment of
personality based on the Five-Factor Model of Personality. Eight-item scales
measured six specific facets for each of the five broad factors. The internal
consistencies (Cronbach’s alpha) were high overall, ranging from .69 to .85
(mean = .72) and did not differ substantially from what has been found for other
samples with NEO PI-R measured in Sweden (Costa and McCrae, 1985), and
other countries.
2.2.2. Iris characteristics
Close-up photos, where the diameter of the iris on the 36 mm 24 mm
Fujicolor, Professional 100 ASA slide film was about 22 mm, were taken of
both irises from all subjects. An Olympus OM-4T camera with a double sided
flash device from Lena Medical Photo Design Systems, and an Olympus 50 mm
macro lens with a 1:2 converter was used. The shutter speed was 60 and the
aperture was set to 16. To assist focus adjustment and to standardize the extent
the iris was dilated, a lamp with standard brightness was shown into the
subjects’ irises during focus and photography. To prevent blurring caused by
involuntary movements from the subjects, all participants rested their cheeks on
a stand during the procedure. The iris photos were digitized using Minoltas
35 mm Dimage Scan Dual film scanner. The resolution of the digitized color
pictures was 768 512 pixels (96 pixels/in.). A high contrast color computer
screen (Brand: Eizo; Model: FlexScan F55) with 1024 768/85 Hz resolution
(.28 mm Dot Pitch CRT; fH: 27–70 kHz/fV: 50–120 Hz) and the software
program Photo Shop 7.0 was used during the rating process.
Three scales, one for each iris characteristic of interest, were constructed:
frequency of crypts, pigment dots and extension and distinction of contraction
furrows. Specially trained raters independently reviewed the photographs of the
right iris and judged which scale step to which each photo was most similar. The
raters’ judgments were scored on an ordinal scale. Scale construction, the rating
procedure and the reliability of the procedures are reported in detail in Larsson
et al. (2003). The pictures used for the scales may be seen at: http://www.mol-
vis.org/molvis/v10/a98/ (or obtained directly from the first author).
2.2.3. Analysis
The first set of analyses was variable-oriented in nature and tested whether
our three expectations derived from Pax6 expression pattern in the anterior
cingulate cortex were correct. This was done by computing Pearson correlations
between the three iris characteristics and the personality measures. Due to the
fact that the facet level traits in NEO PI-R have their own biological basis (Jang
et al., 1998) and the necessity to include as specific measures as possible has
been emphasized in studies, such as ours (Heyman et al., 1999), both facet level
traits and the main domains of personality were included in the analysis. The
distributions of the iris characteristics used in these analyses are presented in
Table 1.
In the second step, we performed the person-oriented analyses. They tested
if the iris configurations that typically exist in early adults can identify
subgroups of individuals that differ in their personality in a systematic manner.
First a cluster analysis based on WARD’s method (Peck and Williams, 2002)
was performed. Only the iris characteristics that were found to be significantly
associated with personality in the correlation analysis were included. A four
cluster solution was chosen which explained 77% of the total variance in the
sample. The variance explanation was higher than the minimum criterion of
67%, recommended by Bergman et al. (2003). One-way analyses of variance
were used to investigate if any mean differences in personality existed between
the clusters (Tukey HSD were used for post hoc tests). Both facet level traits and
the main domains of personality were included in the variance analysis.
Finally, to explore how efficiently the self-organizing properties of the
developmental system could pick out subgroups of individuals that differed in
their personalities, we calculated the effect size d (Cohen, 1988) for all
associations that were found to be significant in the variable- and person-
oriented analysis.
3. Results
3.1. Variable-oriented analysis
The results of the variable-oriented analyses are presented in
Table 2. We found that two of the five domains were
Table 1
Distribution of iris characteristics
Iris characteristic and scale description Number of
observations
Fuchs’ crypt frequency
(1) Only parallel and/or densely packed curly fibres 129
(2) Spots of wavy fibres and 1–3 two shallow crypts 169
(3) Four shallow crypts or more 130
Pigment dots
(1) Absence of, or only minute grains of pigment dots 280
(2) One pigment dots or more 148
Contraction furrows
(1) Extending less than 1/4 circle 106
(2) Extending between 1/4 and 8/10 of a circle 142
(3) More distinct, extending 8/10 of one circle or more 180
M. Larsson et al. / Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175168
significantly associated with crypt frequency: openness to
experience and agreeableness. In contrast, furrows were only
associated with neuroticism, and pigment dots were not
associated with any of the main domains of personality. Not
surprisingly, most of the correlations between the specific
facets of personality and crypt frequency were found for facets
belonging to the two domains with significant associations.
Thus, our first hypothesis – i.e., that most association should
involve crypt frequency – was confirmed. Furthermore, our
second and third hypotheses – i.e., that crypt frequency in most
cases should be related to approach-related behaviors and that
individuals with a dense crypt structure should score higher on
such behaviors – was also confirmed. Most of the significant
associations with crypt pinpointed behaviors that measure
approach-related behaviors and individuals with a dense crypt
structure scored higher on such behaviors (i.e., warmth, positive
emotions, openness to experience, feelings, agreeableness, trust
and tendermindedness).
In addition, the magnitude of the associations with the iris
characteristics was, as expected, quite modest and ranged from
.10 to .15. No associations between eye color and personality
could be found (results not shown).
We are aware that many correlations are reported in Table 2.If
we were to apply a conservative Bonferroni correction, no
significant correlation would remain. Nevertheless, for two of the
three measures of iris characteristics, the number of significant
correlations obtained is significantly above chance. As for the
associations between crypts and the Big Five measures, we
conducted 35 separate analyses. By chance, wewould expect 1.75
analyses with p-values of .05 or lower (35 analyses .05 = 1.75)
and .35 analyses with p-values of .01 or lower (35 analyses
.01 = .35). For crypts, we obtain eight significant correlations at
the .05 level and five at the .01. For furrows, we obtain three
correlations with p-values of .05 or lower and 2 at the p-value of
.01 or lower. Hence, for crypts and furrows, the number of signi-
ficant correlations is well above the chance. For pigment dots, on
the other hand, the findings are not much different from chance
expectations. Moreover, the fact that most of the significant
associations in Table 2 are consistent with our hypotheses
decreases the likelihood that these findings are due to random
effects. The significant results in the person-oriented analysis,
which pinpoint the same behaviors, also support this notion.
In the person-oriented analysis, a four cluster solution was
chosen based on the criteria that each group should differ as
much as possible in their iris morphologically, as well as define
subgroups with a sufficient number of members with a view to
the subsequent analyses of variance. Only the iris measures that
were significantly associated with personality in the variable-
oriented analysis were included in the cluster analysis. The
result of the analysis, which represents the configurations of iris
characteristic that typically tends to appear in early adults, is
presented in Table 3.
3.2. Person-oriented analysis
Two different types of configurations emerged. The
individuals in clusters 1 and 2 had dense crypt structures,
whereas clusters 3 and 4 contained individuals with open crypt
structures. The amount of furrows that existed in each of these
clusters further differentiated between the clusters. Clusters 1
and 4 individuals had few contraction furrows, whereas subjects
in clusters 2 and 3 had many.
The one-way analyses of variance that tested if any mean
differences emerged between these four clusters, produced
results which were in agreement with our expectations, mean
differences in regards to personality emerged (Table 4). Overall,
individuals in the clusters with more tissue loss in the iris scored
lower on approach-related behaviors, compared with those with a
dense iris structure. For example, the individuals in cluster 3
scored significantly lower than the individuals in cluster 1 on all
approach-related behaviors that in the variable-oriented analysis
were found to be significantly associated to crypts. Hence, tissue
loss in the iris was in the person-oriented analysis, just as in the
variable-oriented analyses, associated with lower scores on
approach-related behaviors.
Table 2
Pearson correlation between iris characteristics and the personality measures
Personality measure Iris characteristics
Crypts Pigment dots Furrows
Neuroticism .04 .06 .11
*
Anxiety .02 .01 .06
Angry-hostility .04 .07 .04
Depression .01 .07 .09
Self-consciousness .05 .08 .09
Impulsiveness .06 .02 .15
**
Vulnerability .08 .04 .07
Extraversion .09 .02 .04
Warmth .15
**
.04 .04
Gregariousness .05 .06 .01
Assertiveness .01 .02 .00
Activity .05 .00 .05
Excitement seeking .01 .01 .07
Positive emotions .13
**
.01 .03
Openness to experience .10
*
.01 .06
Fantasy .01 .02 .03
Aesthetics .13
**
.01 .06
Feelings .14
**
.05 .03
Actions .03 .02 .06
Ideas .01 .03 .02
Values .07 .03 .08
Agreeableness .10
*
.06 .00
Trust .11
*
.05 .01
Straightforwardness .04 .10
*
.03
Altruism .06 .01 .03
Compliance .04 .01 .02
Modesty .04 .05 .01
Tendermindedness .13
**
.03 .03
Conscientiousness .07 .05 .09
Competence .02 .07 .09
Order .00 .03 .08
Dutifulness .06 .09 .01
Achievement striving .08 .03 .06
Self-discipline .08 .03 .12
**
Deliberation .04 .03 .04
*
p<.05.
**
p<.01.
M. Larsson et al. / Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175 169
But the results of the analyses of variance also demonstrated
that furrows were associated with mean differences between
clusters that had the same crypt structure. For example, subjects
in clusters 1 and 2, both of which contained individuals with
dense crypt structures, scored differently on the neuroticism
domain as well as the facet scale Impulsiveness in this domain.
The individuals in cluster 1 who had few furrows, scored
significantly lower than individuals in cluster 2 who had many
furrows. Likewise, subjects in clusters 3 and 4, which both
contained individuals with open crypt structures, scored
differently on the Conscientiousness domain as well as the
facet scales achievement-striving and self-discipline in this
domain. The individuals in cluster 3 who had many furrows,
scored significantly lower than individuals in cluster 4 who had
few furrows. Furrows were thereby associated with mean
differences between clusters that had the same crypt structure,
and the direction of these associations was consistent with the
significant association found between furrows and neuroticism,
impulsiveness and self-discipline in the variable-oriented
analyses.
Especially noteworthy was that mean differences emerged
on behaviors that were not significantly associated with crypts
in the variable-oriented analysis. Mean differences between
clusters 1 and 3 were observed for values in the openness to
experience domain, as well as the conscientiousness domain
and the facet scales achievement-striving and self-discipline in
this domain. These traits were not significantly associated with
crypts in the variable-oriented analysis.
The effect sizes for all associations that were significant in
the variable- and person-oriented analyses are presented in
Table 5. Overall, the effect sizes in the person-oriented analysis
were about twice as large as in the variable-oriented analysis.
Table 3
The configurations of iris characteristic that typically exists in early adults
Iris characteristic and scale description Cluster 1
(N= 143)
1
Cluster 2
(N= 155)
2
Cluster 3
(N= 73)
3
Cluster 4
(N= 57)
4
Crypt frequency
(1) Only parallel and/or densely packed curly fibres Yes Yes
(2) Spots of wavy fibres and 1–3 two shallow crypts Yes Yes
(3) Four shallow crypts or more – – Yes Yes
Contraction furrows
(1) Extending less than 1/4 circle Yes Yes
(2) Extending between 1/4 and 8/10 of a circle Yes Yes
(3) More distinct, extending 8/10 of one circle or more Yes Yes
1
Mean
crypts
= 1.61/mean
furrows
= 1.65.
2
Mean
crypts
= 1.52/mean
furrows
= 3.00.
3
Mean
crypts
= 3.00/mean
furrows
= 2.34.
4
Mean
crypts
= 3.00/mean
furrows
= 1.00.
Table 4
Means of the personality measures with standard deviations within parentheses in each of the four clusters, with test for mean differences between the clusters (one-
way ANOVA, post hoc = Tukey HSD)
Personality measure Cluster 1
(N= 143)
Cluster 2
(N= 155)
Cluster 3
(N= 73)
Cluster 4
(N= 57)
d.f. F
Neuroticism .15 (.89) .16 (1.01)
1
.01 (1.08) .11 (1.08) 3; 423 2.63
*
Impulsiveness .14 (.95) .19 (.96)
1
.04 (1.14) .18 (1.11) 3; 423 3.52
*
Extraversion – –
Warmth .13 (.97)
3
.03 (.95) .26 (1.13) .13 (.99) 3; 423 2.90
*
Positive emotions .13 (.92)
3
.05 (1.02) .26 (1.09) .11 (.97) 3; 423 2.86
*
Openness to experience .17 (.99)
3
.00 (.98) .30 (1.07) .06 (.88) 3; 423 3.82
**
Aesthetics .21 (.96)
3
.01 (.97) .35 (1.09) .06 (.90) 3; 423 5.42
**
Feelings .22 (.94)
3
.01 (1.01)
3
.38 (1.00) .15 (.96) 3; 423 6.49
**
Values .12 (.97)
3
.00 (.99) .34 (1.10) .07 (.87) 3; 423 3.67
**
Agreeableness .18 (.97)
3
.04 (1.01) .18 (1.09) .14 (.88) 3; 423 2.92
*
Trust .20 (.93)
3
.03 (.98) .22 (1.17) .09 (.90) 3; 423 3.48
**
Tendermindedness .20 (.93)
3
.07 (1.02) .20 (1.08) .12 (.91) 3; 423 3.57
**
Conscientiousness .11 (.94)
3
.02 (.94) .31 (1.04) .17 (1.14)
3
3; 423 3.53
*
Achievement striving .04 (.93)
3
.04 (.97)
3
.33 (.99) .16 (1.15)
3
3; 423 3.39
*
Self-discipline .20 (.95)
3
.08 (.98) .39 (1.00) .19 (1.02)
3
3; 423 6.84
**
The table show Z-transformed values. Elevated numbers indicates between which clusters a significant mean difference was observed (post hoc = Tukey HSD).
*
p<.05.
**
p<.01.
M. Larsson et al. / Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175170
4. Discussion
The introduction in this study reviews several possible
biological pathways that theoretically could explain an
association between iris characteristics and personality. In
order to more fully evaluate a potential role of the
neurodevelopmental control gene Pax6 we tested three
hypotheses by performing both variable- and person-oriented
analysis. These hypotheses draw heavily on the Pax6
expression pattern in the human iris (Davis-Silberman et al.,
2005; Jensen, 2005; Sale et al., 2002) as well as the anterior
cingulate cortex as documented by Ellison-Wright et al. (2004)
in conjunction with the findings made by Pujol et al. (2002),
Davidson (1992, 2001) and Davidson and Irwin (1999).The
specificity of the hypotheses increased. We believed that if all
predictions were supported the likelihood that Pax6 would be
involved would have to be considered high.
Our first hypothesis, that most of the associations with
personality would be found for crypt frequency, was confirmed.
Furthermore, our second and third hypotheses were also
confirmed; crypts were primarily associated with approach-
related behaviors and individuals with a dense crypt structure
scored higher than those with an open crypt structure on such
behaviors.
Thus, the findings in the variable-oriented analyses show
that crypt frequency is associated with the extent in which
individuals engage in approach-related behavior. Furthermore,
since our hypotheses were confirmed, crypt frequency may
likely be caused by subtle Pax6 differences. In turn, these Pax6
differences may also act as a mediator to approach-related
behavior since Pax6 potentially can induce tissue deficiencies
in the left anterior cingulate cortex.
The results in the person-oriented analysis supported this
notion, as the findings were essentially the same. Clusters with
less tissue loss in the iris were associated with higher scores on
approach-related behaviors and the significant mean differ-
ences between clusters 1 and 3 were systematically pinpointing
such traits (i.e., warmth, positive emotion, feelings, agreeable-
ness, trust and tendermindedness) (see Table 4).
But maybe the most intriguing finding was that smaller
tissue differences than crypts were also of major importance.
This was apparent in three different ways. First, the association
between furrows and the facet scale impulsiveness in the
variable-oriented analyses were of the same magnitude as the
strongest association found for crypts. Second, in the person-
oriented analyses furrows could differentiate between indivi-
duals who scored high and low on the neuroticism and
conscientiousness domain, even when the individuals in these
clusters had the same crypt structure. Third, when the combined
effect of having specific configurations of crypts and furrows
was taken into consideration in the person-oriented analyses,
the effect sizes were on average about twice the magnitude of
what was found in the variable-oriented analyses. Thus, it is
apparent that even smaller tissue differences than crypts were of
importance. Naturally emerging configurations of both crypts
and furrows were, as expected, pinpointing subgroups of
individuals that were especially likely to differ in their
personalities. Indeed, when both crypts and furrows were
Table 5
The effect size for all associations that was significant in the variable- and person-oriented analysis
Personality measure Cohen’s d
Variable-oriented analysis
a
Person-oriented analysis
b
Pearson correlations Clusters
1 and 2
Clusters
1 and 3
Clusters
2 and 3
Clusters
3 and 4
Crypts furrows Crypts furrows
Neuroticism – .22
*
.33
*
–––
Impulsiveness – .30
**
.36
*
–––
Extraversion – – – –
Warmth .30
**
– .37
*
––
Positive emotions .26
**
– .39
*
––
Openness to experience .20
*
– .47
**
––
Aesthetics .26
**
– .55
**
––
Feelings .28
**
– .62
**
.40
*
Values – – .44
**
––
Agreeableness .20
*
– .36
*
––
Trust .22
*
– .39
**
––
Tendermindedness .26
**
– .41
**
––
Conscientiousness – .42
*
– .44
*
Achievement striving .38
*
.38
*
.49
*
Self-discipline – .24
**
– .61
**
– .57
**
a
Cohen’s d=2r/H(1 r
2
), where ris the Pearson correlation.
b
Cohen’s d=M
1
M
2
/s
pooled
, where M
1
is the mean in one of the cluster and M
2
is the mean in the other cluster. s
pooled
is the pooled standard deviation for the two
clusters (spooled ¼p½ðs2
1þs2
2Þ=2).
*
p<.05.
**
p<.01.
M. Larsson et al. / Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175 171
taken into consideration in the person-oriented analyses, mean
differences in all main domains of personality emerged.
Overall, these findings support the notion that people with
different iris configurations tend to develop along different
trajectories in regards to personality. Furthermore, the self-
organizational properties of the developmental system that are
reflected in the iris appear to be quite efficient in pinpointing the
individuals who differ in their personalities. The effect sizes
between the clusters in the person-oriented analyses ranged
from d= .33 to .62, which is much larger than, for example,
women’s tendency to be more emotional than men, d= .05 to
.30 (Brebner, 2003).
When considering which approach-related behaviors were
most salient in the variable-oriented analyses, it was apparent
that a group of five approach-related traits (i.e., feelings,
tendermindedness, warmth, trust and positive emotions) was
driving the significant associations in the main domains. Both
the significant associations with the facet scales in the openness
to experience and agreeableness domain suggest that.
People with dense structures were more receptive to their
inner feelings and tended to sympathize and feel concern for
other people’s needs more than people with open crypt
structures. Furthermore, the most robust results for crypt
frequency suggest that people with dense structures form
warmer and more trustful attachments to others and experience
and express positive emotions, such as joy, happiness, and
excitement more often than those with open crypt structures.
Crypt frequency was thereby predominately associated with
facet scales that tap the emotional space in the extraversion,
openness to experience, and agreeableness domains. Thus,
tissue loss in the two top cell layers in the iris, as assessed by our
crypt measure, is in this general sense most strongly associated
with the emotional component of personality.
However, contraction furrows, which measure the thickness
and density differences in all five-cell layers in the iris, also
appear to be associated with emotional functioning. Furrows
were primarily associated with impulsiveness, and people with
many contraction furrows were less able than people with few
contraction furrows to control their cravings and urges. Thus,
the variable-oriented analyses revealed that the behavioral
correlates of the two iris characteristics differ. Crypt frequency
is most strongly related to the emotional space in the
extraversion, agreeable and openness to experience domains,
whereas the effect of contraction furrows is more specific and
relates directly to impulse control.
It is, therefore, interesting to note that when the principles of
self-organization were applied in the person-oriented analysis
(Bergman and Magnusson, 1997), the configurations of iris
characteristics that naturally emerge pinpointed clusters of
individuals that scored significantly different on the con-
scientiousness domain—a domain that was unrelated to the iris
characteristics in the variable-oriented analysis. At first sight
this may not be surprising since the inability to resist impulses,
or poor self-control, generally also influences the active process
of planning, organizing and carrying out tasks, which are basic
tendencies in the conscientiousness domain (Costa and
McCrea, 1992). However, the fact that the relationship with
the conscientiousness domain was only possible to identify
when both crypts and furrows were taken in consideration,
suggests two things that previously have not been recognized.
First, small differences in the extent to which people engage in
approach-related behaviors – as assessed by crypt frequency –
matters for how poor impulse control influences the con-
scientiousness domain. Second, crypt frequency identifies for
whom poor impulse control most likely also results in a
tendency to score low on the conscientiousness domain. Thus,
given that it has been difficult to replicate associations between
single genes and personality traits influenced by impulsive
behavior (Persson et al., 2000; Reif and Lesch, 2003), and that
single genes generally explain less than 1.5% of the variance for
a personality trait (Comings et al., 2000), our crypt frequency
scale may be helpful to use in future association studies. If not
considered, crypt frequency may mask potential associations
between candidate genes that potentially could influence the
conscientiousness domain, and potentially many other beha-
vioral traits that in part are dependent on the individual’s ability
to exercise self-control (Schmeichel and Baumeister, 2004).
However, if the crypt frequency scale is considered, or even
better, if the genes that determine crypt frequency can be
identified, this may help to find genes that add up in their effect,
which eventually can account for a greater proportion of the
genetic variance that influences personality as well as other
behaviors (Comings et al., 2000; Schmeichel and Baumeister,
2004). Thus, future association studies may be more fruitful,
and their result more applicable on the individual level, if iris
data is considered. The fact that people with different iris
configurations tend to develop along different trajectories, and
that the self-organizational properties of the developmental
system that are reflected in the iris are quite efficient in
pinpointing the individuals who differ more in their person-
alities, also supports this notion.
The behavioral findings in this study suggest that because
Pax6 induces tissue deficiencies in both the iris (Davis-
Silberman et al., 2005) and the left anterior cingulate cortex
(Ellison-Wright et al., 2004), it may influence the extent to
which people engage in approach-related behaviors (Pujol
et al., 2002). Indeed, there is a substantial body of research that
supports this notion. For example, positive emotions experi-
enced by monkeys increased the left-sided activation of their
brains (Kosslyn et al., 2002). People with positive dispositions
show greater relative left frontal EEG asymmetry at baseline
and respond more strongly to positive than to negative stimuli
(Sutton and Davidson, 1997; Wheeler et al., 1993). Ten-month-
old infants who did not cry when they were separated from their
mothers exhibited more left-brain activation (Fox and
Davidson, 1987). The thresholds for experiencing positive
emotions is lower for people who display left-sided activation
patterns (Fox and Carlkins, 1993). In addition, these left-sided
patterns are stable in both early childhood (Fox et al., 1992) and
adulthood (Davidson, 2003). Thus, the association between
frontal brain asymmetries and approach-related behaviors
exhibits enough stability and consistency to be considered an
underlying biological disposition for personality (Buss and
Larsen, 2005). Overall, these findings correspond well with our
M. Larsson et al. / Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175172
finding as well as the fact that the principal distribution and
depth of crypts are present at birth (Oyster, 1999).
Furthermore, our conscientiousness and Impulsiveness
findings are also consistent with previously published results.
The anterior cingulate function at large is to integrate
attentional and affective information that is critical for self-
regulation and adaptability, and it plays a crucial role in
initiation, motivation and goal-directed behaviors (Devinsky
et al., 1995; Thayer and Lane, 2000). The ventral region (BA
24a–c, 25, 32, 33) is primarily involved in assessing the salience
of emotions, hurtful rejections and romantic love, for example,
as well as the regulation of emotional responses involved in
social interactions which assess one’s own and other people’s
emotions and states of minds (Bartels and Zeki, 2000;
Eisenberger and Lieberman, 2004; Simmons et al., 2006).
The dorsal region (BA 24b0–c0,32
0) is primarily involved in
response selection and the processing of cognitively demanding
information. For example, the cognitions that are activated in
reward-related conflicts and the evaluation of different
outcomes are processed in the dorsal anterior cingulate (Bush
et al., 2000). Furthermore, damage to the anterior cingulate is
associated with a host of social affective impairments including
blunted affect, impulsivity, disinhibition and poor social
judgments (Devinsky et al., 1995; Heyman et al., 1999).
Thus, since processes of planning, organizing and carrying
out tasks as well as resisting impulses are so central for anterior
cingulate functioning at large, as well as for the conscientious-
ness domain, it is not surprising – given the expression pattern
of Pax6 in the iris and brain – that individuals with different
configurations of crypts and furrows scored differently on this
domain.
These findings in conjunction with our findings indicate that
there are sizeable bodies of research that support the notion that
tissue loss in the iris potentially could be associated with tissue
loss in the brain. However, there are also other biological
pathways that may contribute to the associations found. As
mentioned in Section 1, tissue differences in the iris may be
associated with both brain structures and neurotransmitters that
influence personality. The fact that even small differences in the
monoamine systems during the embryological development of
the brain has the strongest influence on the neural development of
the anterior cingulate cortex, effects that persist into adulthood,
further underscores this possibility (Levitt et al., 1997).
Thus, to elucidate these genetic developmental complexities
additional studies are needed. However, given that the genetic
complexities that underlie the appearances of crypts and
furrows probably are difficult to dissect (Aota et al., 2003;
Davis-Silberman et al., 2005). It may be fruitful to investigate
to what extent crypts and furrows can be useful biomarkers in
their own right. The following highlights three areas were it
may be especially rewarding to further investigate the
usefulness of the iris as a biomarker.
4.1. MRI-studies
MRI-studies can identify the extent crypts and furrows are
associated with structural differences in the anterior cingulate
cortex. Thus, crypts and furrows could potentially be
biomarkers for developmental brain differences in large
samples, where it would be too expensive and time consuming
to collect MRI data of the brain. In contrast to MRI data, it takes
about 3 min per person to take digital photos, which readily can
be rated automatically by an iris scanner if the algorithm
currently used for personal identification (Daugman, 2003)
were to be modified to fit this purpose (Dougman, personal
communication, July, 2, 1999). Such efforts would make it
possible to study many neurodevelopmental questions that
today are impractical to study in large samples. For example, do
genes that influence the dopamine, serotonin and noradrenalin
systems and their assumed behavioral associations to specific
personality traits (Comings et al., 2000) change, when the
structure of the brain shifts?
4.2. Longitudinal studies
Iris data may be able to map out how impulsive and
approach-related behaviors of genetic origin, in interaction
with environmental factors, influence dyadic relationships
between family members. For example, it would be interesting
to find out if scores on child disclosure, parental solicitation,
parental control, parental support and parents’ bad reactions to
disclosure, differ from what is normal, in parental–child dyads
where there is a low concordance between the child and the
parent iris configurations. The fact that the expression pattern
for Pax6 in the brain overlaps with the cortical foundation for
maternal behaviors (mainly BA 24/32 and 47) and hurtful
rejections (BA 24/32), in conjunction with our results with
regard to approach-related and impulsive behaviors, suggests
that the iris can be useful marker for biological mechanisms that
influence the interaction between such child and parent based
behaviors (Eisenberger and Lieberman, 2004; Kerr and Stattin,
2003; Lorberbaum et al., 2002; Neiderhiser et al., 2004).
4.3. Twin-studies
The monozygotic (MZ) intra-pair twin correlation for
crypts, pigment dots and furrows are .66, .58 and .79,
respectively (Larsson and Pedersen, 2004). Thus, given that
it actually is quite common, at least in some MZ pairs, to
observe intra-pair differences that are relatively large,
especially with regard to crypts and pigment dots, and smaller
iris differences than this were associated with personality
differences in our sample, it is quite possible that small MZ
intra-pair iris differences could be associated with non-
systematic non-shared environmental effects for personality,
that is, random embryological events that make MZ twins less
similar. In any event, since there is a quest for more theory and
research in this area (Plomin et al., 2001; Plomin and Daniels,
1987; Turkheimer and Waldron, 2000). It is worth mentioning
that by collecting iris data from MZ twins it may be possible to
learn more about how non-systematic non-shared environ-
mental effects, influence personality as well as other behaviors.
In conclusion, our hypothesis was largely confirmed. Crypt
frequency is likely, via Pax6, to be associated with tissue
M. Larsson et al. / Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175 173
deficiencies in the left anterior cingulate cortex, which in turn
influences how much individuals engage in approach-related
behaviors. Furthermore, the self-organizing property of the
developmental system that is reflected in the iris appears to be
quite efficient in pinpointing individuals who differ in their
personalities. When both crypts and furrows were taken into
consideration in the person-oriented analyses, mean differences
in all main domains of personality emerged. Furthermore, the
effect sizes in the person-oriented analyses were about twice as
large as in the variable-oriented analyses. Overall, these
findings support the notion that people with different iris
configurations tend to develop along different trajectories in
regards to their personalities. Thus, it may be fruitful to
investigate further the usefulness of the iris as a biomarker
within a holistic interactionistic framework, using both
variable- and person-oriented approaches. Future genetic
association studies as well as MIR-, longitudinal- and twin-
studies may benefit from collecting iris data and testing
candidate genes for crypts and contraction furrows.
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by funds from the Department of
Social Sciences at O
¨rebro University in Sweden and The
Swedish Foundation for International Cooperation in Research
and Higher Education. We are grateful to the participants for
their cooperation.
References
Aota, S., Nakajima, N., Sakamoto, R., Watanabe, S., Ibaraki, N., Okazaki, K.,
2003. Pax6 autoregulation mediated by direct interaction of Pax6 protein
with the head surface ectoderm-specific enhancer of the mouse Pax6 gene.
Developmental Biology 257, 1–13.
Bartels, A., Zeki, S., 2000. The neural basis of romantic love. Neuroreport 11,
3829–3834.
Bergman, L.R., Magnusson, D., 1997. A person-oriented approach in research
on developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology 9,
291–319.
Bergman, L.R., Magnusson, D., El Khouri, B.M., 2003. Studying Individual
Development in an Interindividual Context: A Person-Oriented Approach.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ.
Bito, L.Z., Matheny, A., Cruickshanks, K.J., Nondahl, D.M., Carino, O.B.,
1997. Eye color changes past early childhood. The Louisville Twin Study.
Archives of Ophthalmolmology 115, 659–663.
Bosma, H.A., Kunnen, E., 2002. Identity and Emotion: Development Through
Self-Organization. Cambridge University Press, New York.
Brebner, J., 2003. Gender and emotions. Personality and individual differences
34, 387–394.
Burbach, J.P., Smits, S., Smidt, M.P., 2003. Transcription factors in the
development of midbrain dopamine neurons. Annals of The New York
Academy of Sciences 991, 61–68.
Bush, G., Luu, P., Posner, M.I., 2000. Cognitive and emotional influences in
anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 215–222.
Buss, D.M., Larsen, R.J., 2005. Personality Psychology: Domains of Knowl-
edge About Human Nature. McGraw-Hill, New York.
Carver, C.S., Scheier, M.F., 2002. Control processes and self-organization as
complementary principles underlying behavior. Journal of Social Psychol-
ogy Review 6, 304–315.
Cattell, R.B., 1965. The Scientific Analysis of Personality. Aldine, Chicago.
Cohen, J., 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, vol. 2.
Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.
Comings, D.E., Gade-Andavolu, R., Gonzalez, N., Wu, S., Muhleman, D.,
Blake, H., Mann, M.B., Dietz, G., Saucier, G., MacMurray, J.P., 2000. A
multivariate analysis of 59 candidate genes in personality traits: the
temperament and character inventory. Clinical Genetics 58, 375–385.
Coplan, R.J., Coleman, B., Rubin, K.H., 1998. Shyness and little boy blue: iris
pigmentation, gender, and social wariness in preschoolers. Developmental
Psychobiology 32, 37–44.
Costa, P.T., McCrae, R.R., 1985. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-
R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional manual. Psy-
chological Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL. Psykologifo
¨rlaget AB,
Svensk version: NEO PI-R
TM
, Stockholm, Sweden, 2003.
Costa, P.T., McCrea, R.R., 1992. Neo Personality Inventory Revised. Psychologi-
cal Assessment Resources, Inc., 16204 North Florida Avenue, Lutz, Florida.
Daugman, J., 2003. The importance of being random: statistical principles of
iris recognition. Pattern Recognition 36, 279–291.
Davidson, R.J., 1992. Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion.
Brain and Cognition 20, 125–151.
Davidson, R.J., 2001. Toward a biology of personality and emotion. In: Damasio,
A.R. (Ed.), Unity of Knowledge: The Convergence of Natural and Human
Science. New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp. 191–207.
Davidson, R.J., 2003. Affective neuroscience and psychophysiology: toward a
synthesis. Psychophysiology 40, 655–665.
Davidson, R.J., Irwin, W., 1999. The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and
affective style. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3, 11–21.
Davis-Silberman, N., Kalich, T., Oron-Karni, V., Marquardt, T., Zillig, M.,
Tamm, E.R., Ashery-Padan, R., 2005. Genetic dissection of Pax6 dosage
requirements in the developing mouse eye. Human Molecular Genetics 14,
2265–2276.
Devinsky, O., Morrell, M.J., Vogt, B.A., 1995. Contributions of anterior
cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain 118 (Pt 1), 279–306.
Ding, Y.Q., Marklund, U., Yuan, W., Yin, J., Wegman, L., Ericson, J., Deneris,
E., Johnson, R.L., Chen, Z.F., 2003. Lmx1b is essential for the development
of serotonergic neurons. Nature Neuroscience 6, 933–938.
Eisenberger, N.I., Lieberman, M.D., 2004. Why rejection hurts: a common
neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences 8, 294–300.
Ellison-Wright, Z., Heyman, I., Frampton, I., Rubia, K., Chitnis, X., Ellison-
Wright, I., Williams, S.C., Suckling, J., Simmons, A., Bullmore, E., 2004.
Heterozygous PAX6 mutation, adult brain structure and fronto-striato-
thalamic function in a human family. European Journal of Neuroscience
19, 1505–1512.
Fox, J., Gelb, A.W., Enns, J., Murkin, J.M., Farrar, J.K., Manninen, P.H., 1992.
The responsiveness of cerebral blood flow to changes in arterial carbon
dioxide is maintained during propofol-nitrous oxide anesthesia in humans.
Anesthesiology 77, 453–456.
Fox, N.A., Carlkins, S.D., 1993. Multiple-measure approaches to the study of
infant emotion. In: Lewis, M., Haviland, J.M. (Eds.), Handbook of Emo-
tions. Guilford, New York, pp. 167–185.
Fox, N.A., Davidson, R.J., 1987. Electroencephalogram asymmetry in response
to the approach of a stranger and maternal separation in 10-month-old
infants. Developmental Psychology 23, 233–240.
Gary, A.L., Glover, J., 1976. Eye-Coulor, Sex and Children’s Behavior. Nelson-
Hall, Chicago.
Gentry, T.A., Polzine, K.M., Wakefield, J.A., 1985. Human genetic markers
associated with variation in intellectual abilities and personality. Personality
and individual differences 6, 111–113.
Heyman, I., Frampton, I., van Heyningen, V., Hanson, I., Teague, P., Taylor, A.,
Simonoff, E., 1999. Psychiatric disorder and cognitive function in a family
with an inherited novel mutation of the developmental control gene PAX6.
Psychiatric Genetics 9, 85–90.
Hsieh, Y.W., Zhang, X.M., Lin, E., Oliver, G., Yang, X.J., 2002. The homeobox
gene Six3 is a potential regulator of anterior segment formation in the chick
eye. Developmental Biology 248, 265–280.
Hu, D.N., 2000. Regulation of growth and melanogenesis of uveal melanocytes.
Pigment Cell Research 13, 81–86.
Hu, D.N., Woodward, D.F., McCormick, S.A., 2000. Influence of autonomic
neurotransmitters on human uveal melanocytes in vitro. Experimental Eye
Research 71, 217–224.
M. Larsson et al. / Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175174
Jang, K.L., McCrae, R.R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., Livesley, W.J., 1998.
Heritability of facet-level traits in a cross-cultural twin sample: support for a
hierarchical model of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology 74, 1556–1565.
Jaworski, C., Sperbeck, S., Graham, C., Wistow, G., 1997. Alternative splicing
of Pax6 in bovine eye and evolutionary conservation of intron sequences.
Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications 240, 196–202.
Jensen, A.M., 2005. Potential roles for BMP and Pax genes in the development
of iris smooth muscle. Developmental Dynamics 232, 385–392.
Johnson, D.L., Wiebe, J.S., Gold, S.M., Andreasen, N.C., Hichwa, R.D.,
Watkins, G.L., Boles Ponto, L.L., 1999. Cerebral blood flow and person-
ality: a positron emission tomography study. American Journal of Psychia-
try 156, 252–257.
Kagan, J., Reznick, J.S., Snidman, N., 1988. Biological bases of childhood
shyness. Science 240, 167–171.
Kerr, M., Stattin, H., 2003. Parenting of adolescents: action or reaction? In:
Crouter, A.C., Booth, A. (Eds.), Children’s Influence on Family Dynamics:
The Neglected Side of Family Relationships. Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-
ates, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 121–151.
Kohwi, M., Osumi, N., Rubenstein, J.L., Alvarez-Buylla, A., 2005. Pax6
is required for making specific subpopulations of granule and periglo-
merular neurons in the olfactory bulb. Journal of Neuroscience 25,
6997–7003.
Kosslyn, S.M., Cacioppo, J.T., Davidson, R.J., Hugdahl, K., Lovallo, W.R.,
Spiegel, D., Rose, R., 2002. Bridging psychology and biology: the analysis
of individuals in groups. American Psychologist 57, 341–351.
Kupfermann, I., 1991. Hypothalamus, limbis system and cerebral cortex:
homeostasis and arousal. In: Kandel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H., Jessell, T.M.
(Eds.), Principles of Neural Science. Appleton & Lange, East Norwalk, CT,
pp. 732–760.
Lander, E.S., Schork, N.J., 1994. Genetic dissection of complex traits. Science
265, 2037–2048.
Larsson, M., Pedersen, N.L., 2004. Genetic correlations among texture char-
acteristics in the human iris. Molecular Vision 10, 821–831.
Larsson, M., Pedersen, N.L., Stattin, H., 2003. Importance of genetic effects for
characteristics of the human iris. Twin Research 6, 192–200.
Lester, D., 1987. Eye color, ectomorphy and autonomic nervous system
balance. Personality and individual differences 8, 767–768.
Levitt, P., Harvey, J.A., Friedman, E., Simansky, K., Murphy, E.H., 1997. New
evidence for neurotransmitter influences on brain development. Trends in
Neuroscience 20, 269–274.
Lorberbaum, J.P., Newman, J.D., Horwitz, A.R., Dubno, J.R., Lydiard, R.B.,
Hamner, M.B., Bohning, D.E., George, M.S., 2002. A potential role for
thalamocingulate circuitry in human maternal behavior. Biological Psy-
chiatry 51, 431–445.
Magnusson, D., Stattin, H., 1998. Person-context interaction theories. In:
Lerner, R.M., Damon, W. (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology: vol. 1.
Theoretical Models of Human Development. John Wiley & Sons Inc., New
York, pp. 685–759.
Markle, A., 1976. Eye color and responsiveness to arousing stimuli. Perceptual
and Motor Skills 43, 127–133.
Mukuno, K., Witmer, R., 1977. Innervation of melanocytes in human iris. An
electron microscopic study. Albrecht Von Graefes Archiv fur Klinische und
Experimentelle Ophthalmologie 203, 1–8.
Neiderhiser, J.M., Reiss, D., Pedersen, N.L., Lichtenstein, P., Spotts, E.L.,
Hansson, K., Cederblad, M., Ellhammer, O., 2004. Genetic and environ-
mental influences on mothering of adolescents: a comparison of two
samples. Developmental Psychology 40, 335–351.
Oyster, C.W., 1999. The Human Eye Structure and Function. Sunderland, MA.
Sinauer Associates, Boston.
Pawelek, J.M., Korner, A.M., 1982. The biosynthesis of mammalian melanin.
American Science 70, 136–145.
Peck, R., Williams, L.A., 2002. SPSS Manual/Roger Peck. Addison Wesley
Publishing Company, New York.
Persson, M.L., Wasserman, D., Geijer, T., Frisch, A., Rockah, R., Michaelovsky,
E., Apter, A., Weizman, A., Joensson, E.G., Bergman, H., 2000. Dopamine
D4 receptor gene polymorphism and personality traits in healthy volunteers.
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 250, 203–206.
Plomin, R., Asbury, K., Dunn, J., 2001. Why are children in the same family so
different? Nonshared environment a decade later. Canadian Journal of
Psychiatry 46, 225–233.
Plomin, R., Daniels, D., 1987. Why are children in the same family so different
from one another? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10, 1–16.
Posner, M.I., Rothbart, M.K., 1998. Attention, self-regulation and conscious-
ness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B:
Biological Sciences 353, 1915–1927.
Post, R.M., Weiss, S.R., 1997. Emergent properties of neural systems: howfocal
molecular neurobiological alterations can affect behavior. Development and
Psychopathology 9, 907–929.
Pressman, C.L., Chen, H., Johnson, R.L., 2000. LMX1B, a LIM homeodomain
class transcription factor, is necessary for normal development of multiple
tissues in the anterior segment of the murine eye. Genesis 26, 15–25.
Pujol, J., Lopez, A., Deus, J., Cardoner, N., Vallejo, J., Capdevila, A., Paus, T.,
2002. Anatomical variability of the anterior cingulate gyrus and basic
dimensions of human personality. Neuroimage 15, 847–855.
Reif, A., Lesch, K.P., 2003. Toward a molecular architecture of personality.
Behavioural Brain Research 139, 1–20.
Reznick, J.S., 1989. Perspectives on Behavioral Inhibition. University of
Chicago Press, Chicago.
Rim, Y., 1983. Eye-colour, ethnic origin or family size? Personality and
individual differences 4, 101–102.
Rosenberg, A.A., Kagan, J., 1989. Physical and physiological correlates of
behavioral inhibition. Developmental Psychobiology 22, 753–770.
Rubin, K.H., Both, L., 1989. Iris pigmentation and sociability in childhood: a re-
examination. Developmental Psychobiology 22, 717–725.
Sale, M.M., Craig, J.E., Charlesworth, J.C., FitzGerald, L.M., Hanson, I.M.,
Dickinson, J.L., Matthews, S.J., Heyningen Vv, V., Fingert, J.H., Mackey,
D.A., 2002. Broad phenotypic variability in a single pedigree with a novel
1410delC mutation in the PST domain of the PAX6 gene. Human Mutation
20, 322–327.
Schmeichel, B.J., Baumeister, R.F., 2004. Self-regulatory strength. In: Vohs,
K.D., Baumeister, R.F. (Eds.), Handbook of Self Regulation: Research,
Theory, and Applications. Guilford Press, New York, pp. 84–98.
Simmons, A., Stein, M.B., Matthews, S.C., Feinstein, J.S., Paulus, M.P., 2006.
Affective ambiguity for a group recruits ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
Neuroimage 29, 655–661.
Simpson, T.I., Price, D.J., 2002. Pax6; a pleiotropic player in development.
BioEssays 24, 1041–1051.
Sutton, S.K., Davidson, R.J., 1997. Prefrontal brain asymmetry: a biological
substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychological
Science 8, 204–210.
Thayer, J.F., Lane, R.D., 2000. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion
regulation and dysregulation. Journal of Affective Disorders 61, 201–216.
Turkheimer, E., Waldron, M., 2000. Nonshared environment: a theoretical,
methodological, and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin 126,
78–108.
von Eye, A., Bergman, L.R., 2003. Research strategies in developmental
psychopathology: dimensional identity and the person-oriented approach.
Development and Psychopathology 15, 553–580.
Wallis, D., Muenke, M., 2000. Mutations in holoprosencephaly. Human Muta-
tion 16, 99–108.
Wheeler, R.E., Davidson, R.J., Tomarken, A.J., 1993. Frontal brain asymmetry
and emotional reactivity: a biological substrate of affective style. Psycho-
physiology 30, 82–89.
M. Larsson et al. / Biological Psychology 75 (2007) 165–175 175
... User-oriented examinations were applied to find the relationships between personality and three common iris characteristics: pigment dots, crypts ,and contraction furrows [294]. Dark-eyed individuals typically have higher scores for neuroticism and extraversion [295], sociability [296], and ease of emotional arousal [297]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Affective, emotional, and physiological states (AFFECT) detection and recognition by capturing human signals is a fast-growing area, which has been applied across numerous domains. The research aim is to review publications on how techniques that use brain and biometric sensors can be used for AFFECT recognition, consolidate the findings, provide a rationale for the current methods, compare the effectiveness of existing methods, and quantify how likely they are to address the issues/challenges in the field. In efforts to achieve the key goals of Society 5.0, Industry 5.0, and human-centered design better, the recognition of emotional, affective, and physiological states is progressively becoming an important matter and offers tremendous growth of knowledge and progress in these and other related fields. In this research, a review of AFFECT recognition brain and biometric sensors, methods, and applications was performed, based on Plutchik’s wheel of emotions. Due to the immense variety of existing sensors and sensing systems, this study aimed to provide an analysis of the available sensors that can be used to define human AFFECT, and to classify them based on the type of sensing area and their efficiency in real implementations. Based on statistical and multiple criteria analysis across 169 nations, our outcomes introduce a connection between a nation’s success, its number of Web of Science articles published, and its frequency of citation on AFFECT recognition. The principal conclusions present how this research contributes to the big picture in the field under analysis and explore forthcoming study trends.
... Studii anterioare au demonstrat existenţa unei legături dintre structura irisului şi personalitatea (Larsson, 2007). Structura irisului fiind influenţată de genele exprimate în acesta, studiul de faţă şi-a propus să verifice prin comparaţie şi corelaţie dacă putem găsi legături şi diferenţe în ceea ce priveşte tiparul irisului şi personalitatea, împărţind pe baza Modelului Rayid tiparele de iris ale participanţiilor în două, tiparul emoţional (Flower) şi tiparul analitic (Jewel). ...
Thesis
Studiul de față își propune să investigheze legătura dintre caracteristicile irisului şi trăsături ale personalităţii, atât ca stiluri de gândire, preferinţe în procesarea informaţiei, cât şi ca moduri de adaptare la stres. Știm că personalitatea este influențată și genetic, iar în anii trecuţi s-a demonstrat şi faptul că în iris sunt exprimate aproape 2700 de gene (Wistow, 2001) iar mai apoi faptul că aspectul irisului uman este influenţat de genele exprimate în acesta (Larsson et al., 2011). De asemenea s-a studiat şi ereditatea mai multor caracteristici din aspectul unui iris precum Criptele lui Fruch, punctele de pigmenţi şi brazdele de contracții rezultând într-un nivel mediu spre ridicat de ereditate a acestora (Larsson et al., 2003). Având aceste informaţii irisul începe în ultimii ani să fie studiat şi văzut ca o oportunitate mai bună pentru studiul genelor specifice în relaţie cu anumite trăsături ale personalităţii precum deschiderea, conştiinciozitatea, neuroticismul, şi agreabilitatea ca dimensiuni de personalitate din modelul Big Five (Larsson, 2007) ceea ce prin urmare reprezintă şi o oportunitate a cunoaşterii comportamentului uman dintr-o perspectivă nouă, cu posibilitatea ca tiparul irisului să fie un predictor mai eficient pentru trăsăturile personalităţii genetic influenţate decât alte metode de determinare a genelor care influenţează personalitatea (Larsson, 2007). Studiul irisului nu este atât de nou totuşi, existând din 1965 un studiu realizat de Cattel care a identificat diferenţe ale stilurilor cognitive între persoanele cu ochi căprui şi cei cu ochi albaştri. Culoarea ochiilor ca trăsătură a irisului a fost studiată de mai mulţi cercetători în care s-au găsit asocieri între ochii închişi la culoare şi niveluri mai ridicate la neuroticism şi extraversiune (Gentry et al., 1985) precum şi la sociabilitate (Gary and Glover, 1976). Un plus pe care-l poate aduce această cercetare este faptul că poate pune în mâinile oamenilor o unealtă nouă prin care să descopere omul cu care interacţionează. Caracteristicile irisului studiate în această cercetare fiind vizibile ochiului liber, şi putând fi uşor înţelese şi identificate de oricine, având potenţialul de a ne spune multe despre cum funcţionează acel om. Este un mod care nu poate fi ascuns ca reacţiile sau expresiile nonverbale care pot fi controlate pentru a masca vreun adevăr. De asemenea sunt şi o metodă mai rapidă decât testele de personalitate pentru a identifica tendinţe sau trăsături ale personalităţii unui om.
... 40 Thus far, few studies have been carried out to examine the relationship between iris characteristics and mental and behavioral symptoms in schizophrenic patients. However, in these patients, psychotic symptoms, emotional responses, and behavioral traits are associated with neurotransmitters such as dopamine and 5-hydroxytryptamine. 41 Meanwhile, considering the correlation between the iris and brain structure, we therefore speculated that iris crypts, pigment spots, and iris wrinkles may be constitute an external manifestation of abnormalities in neurotransmitters and/or in the neural pathways of the brain. In other words, in schizophrenic patients, a higher number of iris crypts indicates a greater tendency towards aggressive behavior. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Recently, researchers have conducted many studies on the potential contribution of the retina and other eye structures on schizophrenia. This study aimed to evaluate differences in iris characteristics between patients with schizophrenia and healthy individuals so as to find more easily accessible and easily measurable biomarkers with a view to improving clinical assessments and furthering our understanding of the disease. Methods: Overall, 80 patients with schizophrenia and 52 healthy individuals were included in the case group and the control group, respectively. Iris images were collected from all subjects to compare differences in the structure and color of the iris. The Positive and Negative Symptom Scale (PANSS) and the Modified Overt Aggression Scale (MOAS) were used to evaluate the clinical symptoms and characteristics of 45 first-episode untreated schizophrenics, and analyzed correlations between iris characteristics and schizophrenia symptoms. Results: There were significant differences in iris crypts (P<0.05) and pigment spots (P<0.01) between the case and control group, but no significant difference was found in iris wrinkles (P<0.05). The logistic regression analysis demonstrated that the total iris crypts [odds ratio (OR) 1.166, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.022-1.330] and total iris pigment spots (OR 1.815, 95% CI 1.186-2.775) increased the risk of suffering from schizophrenia. Furthermore, it was demonstrated that the number of iris crypts was positively associated with the MOAS score (r=0.474, P<0.01). Moreover, the number of the iris pigment spots (r=0.395, P<0.01) and wrinkles (r=0.309, P<0.05) were positively correlated with the subjects' negative symptom scores, respectively. Conclusion: Iris crypts and pigment spots were identified as potential biomarkers for detecting schizophrenia. In patients with first-episode untreated schizophrenia, iris characteristics may help psychiatrists to identify the illness and its severity, and to detect characteristic clinical symptoms.
... [22][23][24][25][26] Some studies aimed to learn about the behavior and personality disorders of a given individual through iris analysis and a profile analysis performed on them. [27][28][29] In reference 30, and signs within the iris which occur in people who smoke or who are frequently exposed to polluted air, have been identified. In reference 31, an iris recognition system was proposed for diabetes. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this study, the relationship between individuals' cholesterol levels and the width of the sodium ring was investigated. Firstly, iris images were taken of 100 individuals with total cholesterol levels of 200 mg/dL and above. The average grey scale value (AGSV) for each iris image with a sodium ring was calculated. Secondly, the relationship between the AGSV and cholesterol levels was investigated. Finally, a new mathematical model was proposed that described the relationship between AGSV and total cholesterol with ~89% accuracy. The relationship between AGSV and high-density lipoprotein (HDL), low-density lipoprotein (LDL), and triglyceride levels was calculated with an accuracy of 80.5%, 85.1%, and 79.9%, respectively. In addition, AGSV was determined with 90% accuracy using total cholesterol, HDL, LDL, and triglycerides using multiple linear regression. Although multiple linear regression gives better AGSV, the results have shown that multilayer perceptron (MLP) mapping method gives better accuracy up to 6.62%.
... It is related to the thickness and density of the iris and is determined by the hypoplasia or density of some or all of the five cell membranes in the iris. Larsson conducted a study investigating a correlation between personality traits and iris 22 . His finding suggests the nerve rings were associated with neurotic behaviours such as impulsiveness. ...
Article
Objective: The aim of this case study was to describe a case of iris diagnosis of primary hyperhidrosis and the use of Korean medicine.Methods: A patient with symptoms of hyperhidrosis was diagnosed as having Taeeumin after assessment using the four basic Korean diagnostic methods. Iris diagnosis was used for further examination. The images obtained showed a remarkably defined collarette and increased nerve rings, which suggested an overactive sympathetic nerve system. Under the diagnosis of Taeeum, a Korean herbal medicine was prescribed with additional herbs to help alleviate the hyperactivity of the sympathetic nervous system.Results: The patient had been receiving treatment for hyperhidrosis for >30 years, with various medical attempts to relieve her symptoms, which were ineffective. She showed signs of improvement from day 4 into the treatment, and 80% of her symptoms were improved after completing a 6-week treatment course.Conclusion: The herbal medicine prescribed to the patient proved effective for reducing her chronic symptoms that had been unresponsive to previous medical treatments.
... These elements of human iris architecture are potential markers of our genetic makeup. Although their presence was tested in relation to some personality traits [34], other aspects of their signaling potential appear to be scarcely investigated. Visual access to pupils and variations in iris color should therefore be considered in future investigations of the relationship between external eye morphology and sociocognitive functions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Comparative examinations of external eye morphology in primates initially focused on communicative functions of the eye. Subsequent work has failed to find consistent associations between specific eye morphologies and communicative functions. In this article, we review the field of primate external eye morphology and inspect publicly available and unpublished photographs. We identify and describe five commonly occurring traits that have not received attention so far. We cross-examined the clinical and psychological literature to propose potential adaptive functions. These potential adaptive functions include communicative functions, but also photoregulatory functions and photoprotective functions.
... In ancient Greece, philosophers believed that the balance of humors (bodily fluids) determined a person's temperament. Later, distinct "personality theories" related personality and character to different variables -date of birth (expressed as a zodiac sign), skull shape (Gall, 1835), color of your iris (Larsson, Pedersen & Stattin, 2007), body structure (Kretschmer, 1921(Kretschmer, , 1925Sheldon, 1942), facial features, blood type (Rogers & Glendon, 2003;Sharifi, Ahmadian, Jalali, 2015;Cramer & Imaike, 2002) or your birth order (Paulhus, Trapnell, Chen, 1999;Jefferson, Herbst, & McCrae,1998). Recent advances in medicine, such as molecular genetics, biochemistry, endocrinology and brain imaging have enabled us to uncover these fascinating biological sources of behavior and personality. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
The present study examined correlates of temperamental dimensions of personality and long-term romantic and passionate love. We explored personality and temperament using the four factor neurohormonal model of personality designed by Helen Fisher (using Fisher Temperament Inventory, FTI; Brown, Aceveno, Fisher, 2013) and its potential association with Lee’s theory (1977) of Love styles (assessed by Love Attitudes Scale, LAS; Hendrick & Hendrick, 1986), Passionate love (measured by Passionate Love Scale, PLS; Hatfield, Sprecher, l986) and selected variables in long term heterosexual romantic relationships. Our research followed correlational, quantitative, non-experimental design. The research sample consisted of 147 participants (66.7 per cent were women, median age was 38 years and median for relationship length was 102 months). Associations between Personality and Love Styles were most evident in following relationships: between Negotiator personality type and Mania (rs = 0.42), Builder and Pragma (rs = 0.348) and between Explorer and Ludus (rs = 0.235). Regression analysis revealed that Negotiator and Builder personality types are both moderate predictors of above-mentioned Love styles. Explorer showed to be a weak predictor of Ludus. Relationship between Love Style and Passionate Love (PLS) was strongest for Eros (rs = 0.511, r2= 0.261), Agape (rs = 0.472, r2= 0.223) and Mania (rs = 0.328, r2= 0.108). Ludus correlated negatively with PLS (rs = - 0.316, r2= 0.10). Storge and Pragma showed to be the least passionate from Lee’s Love styles (Storge: rs = - 0.085; Pragma: rs = 0.024). Links between Personality and Passionate Love were rather weak, the strongest relationship was found between PLS and Negotiator scale (rs = 0. 263) and Builder (rs = 0. 238). Director was negatively related to PLS (rs = - 0.148). Interesting links were discovered between Passionate Love and Selected Variables. Highest correlations were found between PLS and Sexual Attraction (rs = 0. 577), Enjoying Time Together (rs = 0. 538), Relationship Happiness (rs = 0. 524,) and Physical Attractiveness of the Partner (rs = 0. 519). Moderate correlations were found between PLS and Admiration (rs = 0.482), Relationship Satisfaction (rs = 0.39), Touch (rs = 0.386), Low Boredom (rs = 0.379,), Inspiration (rs = 0.375,), Partner’s Scent (rs = 0.35), Authenticity (rs = 0.348,), Sexual Satisfaction (rs = 0.333) and Laughing together (rs = 0.324). As expected Length of Relationship correlated negatively with PLS (rs = - 0.175). We also discovered that 47.8 per cent of variance of the PLS may explained by independent variables: Pleasure, Sexual Attraction, Jealousy, Relationship Happiness and Relationship Satisfaction (p= 0.034). Multiple regression analysis revealed that Relationship Happiness, Relationship Satisfaction and Pleasure are the strongest predictors for Passionate love, coefficients showed medium correlations.
... Based on data from 428 study participants, Larsson et al. [53] also suggest that some personality traits, including tendermindedness, warmth, trust, and impulsiveness, are genetically linked to certain iris characteristics, offering -besides gaze behavior -another potential ocular biomarker to analyze people's personalities. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Technologies to measure gaze direction and pupil reactivity have become efficient, cheap, and compact and are finding increasing use in many fields, including gaming, marketing, driver safety, military, and healthcare. Besides offering numerous useful applications, the rapidly expanding technology raises serious privacy concerns. Through the lens of advanced data analytics, gaze patterns can reveal much more information than a user wishes and expects to give away. Drawing from a broad range of scientific disciplines, this paper provides a structured overview of personal data that can be inferred from recorded eye activities. Our analysis of the literature shows that eye tracking data may implicitly contain information about a user's biometric identity, gender, age, ethnicity, body weight, personality traits, drug consumption habits, emotional state, skills and abilities, fears, interests, and sexual preferences. Certain eye tracking measures may even reveal specific cognitive processes and can be used to diagnose various physical and mental health conditions. By portraying the richness and sensitivity of gaze data, this paper provides an important basis for consumer education, privacy impact assessments, and further research into the societal implications of eye tracking.
... Other studies linked damaged patterns with practitioners of working-class occupations, such as plumbers, carpenters, and laborers, who wash their hands frequently (Woodward et al. 2001). Other bodily features, such as iris characteristics, have been found to be related to a variety of behavioral and personality traits (see Larsson et al. 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper reviews the social scientific literature on biometric surveillance, with particular attention to its potential harms. It maps the harms caused by biometric surveillance, traces their theoretical origins, and brings these harms together in one integrative framework to elucidate their cumulative power. Demonstrating these harms with examples from the United States, the European Union, and Israel, I propose that biometric surveillance be addressed, evaluated and reframed as a new form of control rather than simply another means of inspection. I conclude by delineating three features of biometric technologies—complexity, objectivity, and agency—that demonstrate their social power and draw attention to the importance of studying biometric surveillance.
Article
Recently, there has been a convergence in lesion and neuroimaging data in the identification of circuits underlying positive and negative emotion in the human brain. Emphasis is placed on the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the amygdala as two key components of this circuitry. Emotion guides action and organizes behavior towards salient goals. To accomplish this, it is essential that the organism have a means of representing affect in the absence of immediate elicitors. It is proposed that the PFC plays a crucial role in affective working memory. The ventromedial sector of the PFC is most directly involved in the representation of elementary positive and negative emotional states while the dorsolateral PFC may be involved in the representation of the goal states towards which these elementary positive and negative states are directed. The amygdala has been consistently identified as playing a crucial role in both the perception of emotional cues and the production of emotional responses, with some evidence suggesting that it is particularly involved with fear-related negative affect. Individual differences in amygdala activation are implicated in dispositional affective styles and increased reactivity to negative incentives. The ventral striatum, anterior cingulate and insular cortex also provide unique contributions to emotional processing.
Article
Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is a part of the brain's limbic system. Classically, this region has been related to affect, on the basis of lesion studies in humans and in animals. In the late 1980s, neuroimaging research indicated that ACC was active in many studies of cognition. The findings from EEG studies of a focal area of negativity in scalp electrodes following an error response led to the idea that ACC might be the brain's error detection and correction device. In this article, these various findings are reviewed in relation to the idea that ACC is a part of a circuit involved in a form of attention that serves to regulate both cognitive and emotional processing. Neuroimaging studies showing that separate areas of ACC are involved in cognition and emotion are discussed and related to results showing that the error negativity is influenced by affect and motivation. In addition, the development of the emotional and cognitive roles of ACC are discussed, and how the success of this regulation in controlling responses might be correlated with cingulate size. Finally, some theories are considered about how the different subdivisions of ACC might interact with other cortical structures as a part of the circuits involved in the regulation of mental and emotional activity.
Article
Objective: To determine whether eye color changes after 6 years of age. Design: Longitudinal data on eye color were obtained from the Louisville Twin Study, Louisville, Ky. Twins (n=1513 [individuals]) were assessed at least once and most twins (n=1386) were examined on 2 or more occasions. Parents of twins were also examined at the study inception, 128 of whom were assessed again from February 1989 to October 1993. Main Outcome Measure: Eye color was assessed at each examination by matching the iridial coloration of the subject to 1 of 15 painted glass eye anterior segments, similar to those in artificial eyes, mounted on a circular disk. The spectrum ranged from light blue (1) to dark brown (15). Results: Among whites (n= 1359), the eye color of 3.8% to 8.6% of the sample twins became 2 U or more darker or 2 U or more lighter during 3- to 9-year intervals between 6 years of age and adulthood (>18 years, <24 years). Among identical (monozygotic) twin pairs, there was a high degree of concordance in eye color (r=0.98 [P<.001]), while in fraternal (dizygotic) twin pairs, the concordance was less pronounced (r=0.49) and decreased with age (r=0.07). Among the sample of the mothers of twins, 9% had irides that lightened by 2 U or more during the follow-up period. Conclusion: Most individuals achieve stable eye color by 6 years of age. However, a subpopulation of 10% to 15% of white subjects have changes in eye color through-out adolescence and adulthood in the eye color range that can be expected to reflect changes in iridial melanin content or distribution. These data also suggest that such changes in eye color, or the propensity to such changes, may be genetically determined.
Article
Holoprosencephaly (HPE) is the most common developmental defect of the forebrain and midface in humans. In holoprosencephaly the cerebral hemispheres of the brain fail to separate into distinct left and right hemispheres. This malformation is due to the improper specification and formation of the forebrain during early development. When one considers the great number and kinds of genetic interactions that must occur to properly pattern the developing forebrain, it is not surprising that HPE is extremely heterogeneous. In addition to teratogenic agents, several genes are implicated as the cause of HPE. At least 12 different loci have been associated with HPE and now several distinct human genes for holoprosencephaly have been identified. These genes include Sonic Hedgehog (SHH), ZIC2, SIX3, and TG-interacting factor (TGIF). Here we present an overview of the presently known genes causing human holoprosencephaly. We discuss their functional role in development of the forebrain and summarize the mutations and polymorphisms that have been identified within them. Hum Mutat 16:99–108, 2000. Published 2000 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Article
Background: Little is known about the regional brain basis of human maternal behavior. To understand this better, we have been examining brain activity in mothers listening to infant cries. Methods: We measured functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging brain activity in healthy, breastfeeding first-time mothers with young infants while they listened to infant cries, white noise control sounds, and a rest condition. Based on the thalamocingulate theory of maternal behavior and pilot work, we hypothesized that the cingulate, medial thalamus, medial prefrontal cortex, and right orbitofrontal cortex would display more activity with infant cries than with white noise (comparison 1) and would uniquely activate with the cries, meaning that these regions would display activity with cry minus rest but not with white noise minus rest (comparison 2). Results: In hypothesized regions, the group displayed more activity in the medial thalamus, medial prefrontal and right orbitofrontal cortices with both comparisons. The anterior and posterior cingulate cortex displayed more activity only with comparison 1. In non-hypothesized brain regions, several other structures thought important in rodent maternal behavior displayed activity with both comparisons including the midbrain, hypothalamus, dorsal and ventral striatum, and vicinity of the lateral septal region. Conclusions: Our results partially support our hypotheses and are generally consistent with neuroanatomical studies of rodent maternal behavior.