ArticlePDF Available

Subtle Linguistic Cues Affect Children's Motivation

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
Short Report
Subtle Linguistic Cues Affect
Children’s Motivation
Andrei Cimpian, Holly-Marie C. Arce, Ellen M. Markman, and Carol S. Dweck
Stanford University
Are preschoolers’ reactions to setbacks influenced by whether
their successes are rewarded with generic or nongeneric praise?
Previous research has focused on the role of category-referring
generics (e.g., ‘‘Dogs are friendly’’) in shaping children’s
knowledge about natural kinds (see Gelman, 2004). Generic
sentences can, however, refer to individuals as well as catego-
ries. For example, ‘‘John is friendly’’ is generic because it reports
a general regularity—albeit about a single person—rather than
a particular fact or episode (Carlson & Pelletier, 1995). In
contrast, the nongeneric ‘‘John was friendly at the party’’ refers
to a specific past event. Generic sentences about an individual
imply that the particular behavior commented on (e.g., John
smiling warmly) stems from a stable trait (e.g., friendliness) or
skill (see Gelman & Heyman, 1999). Are children sensitive
to this subtle connotation with respect to their own behavior? If
so, then generic praise may lead children to think in trait terms,
such that later mistakes could signal a negative trait or low
ability and therefore undermine motivation (Dweck, 1999,
2006).
Preliminary support for this claim comes from a study by
Kamins and Dweck (1999). Praising the whole person (e.g., ‘‘You
are a good boy/girl’’) after success on a task fostered helpless
responses to subsequent mistakes more than praising the
process through which success was achieved (e.g., ‘‘You found
a good way to do it’’). We suggest that children’s behavior was
in part driven by the fact that the person praise was generic,
connoting a stable trait of the child, while the process praise was
nongeneric, focusing on one specific episode. Would manipu-
lating only the genericness of the praise result in similar patterns
of coping? For example, would children’s motivation be affected
differently by ‘‘You are a good drawer’’ (generic) than by ‘‘You
did a good job drawing’’ (nongeneric)? Note that these sentences
are much more similar than the person-process pairs used by
Kamins and Dweck—so similar, in fact, that adults may not be
aware of their contrasting implications and are thus likely to use
them interchangeably in interactions with children. Demon-
strating that children praised in these two ways react differently
to a challenge would be evidence for the importance of the
generic/nongeneric distinction in shaping young children’s
conceptions of their abilities.
METHOD
Twenty-four 4-year-old children (M54 years 6 months; range 5
4 years 1 month to 5 years 1 month; 12 boys) acted out several
scenarios
1
(see Kamins & Dweck, 1999, Experiment 2). Chil-
dren chose a puppet they could use to act out their part in the
scenarios; the experimenter handled a second puppet, repre-
senting a teacher. In each scenario, the teacher puppet asked
the child puppet to draw a different object. Small pipe cleaners
were used as pretend crayons; no actual drawing or pictures
were involved. The child puppet successfully completed the
requested drawing in the first four scenarios, and the teacher
puppet praised each success generically for half the children
(‘‘You are a good drawer’’) and nongenerically for the other half
(‘‘You did a good job drawing’’). The four success scenarios were
followed by two in which the child puppet made a mistake
(omitting ears on a cat and wheels on a bus) that the teacher
puppet commented on, identically in the two conditions. These
mistake scenarios allowed us to test children’s reactions to
criticism. For debriefing, the mistake scenarios were completed
successfully.
A baseline set of self-evaluation questions was given after the
third success scenario, which involved drawing an apple. The
four questions were as follows:
Question a: ‘‘Do you like the apple that you drew, or do you not
like it?’’ (answered on a 6-point scale from 1, really not like it,
to 6, really like it)
Question b: ‘‘Did what happened in the apple story make you
feel happy or sad?’’ (answered on a 6-point scale from 1, really
sad,to6,really happy)
Address correspondence to Andrei Cimpian, Department of Psy-
chology, Stanford University, Jordan Hall, Bldg. 420, 450 Serra
Mall, Stanford, CA 94305, e-mail: acimpian@psych.stanford.edu.
1
For the full script, see http://psychology.stanford.edu/acimpian/full_script.
pdf.
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
314 Volume 18—Number 4Copyright r2007 Association for Psychological Science
Question c: ‘‘Did everything that happened in the apple story
make you feel like you were good at drawing or not good at
drawing?’’ (forced choice)
Question d: ‘‘Did everything that happened in the apple story
make you feel like you were a good boy/girl or not a good boy/
girl?’’ (forced choice)
A second set of questions was administered after the two mistake
scenarios. In addition to versions of Questions a through d,
tailored to the content of the last mistake scenario, this second
set included the following four persistence-related questions:
Question e (would the child prefer to work again on an un-
successful or successful previous drawing?): ‘‘On another day,
when you had a chance to draw one of these again, would you
want to draw the bus [unsuccessful], want to draw the tree
[successful], or want to draw the cat [unsuccessful]?’’ (forced
choice)
Question f: ‘‘If you had a chance to do something tomorrow,
would you draw or would you do something else?’’ (forced
choice)
Questions g and h (what would the child do after the teacher’s
criticism on each of the two mistake scenarios?): ‘‘Think
about the story where you drew a cat and forgot the ears. What
would you do now?’’ and ‘‘Think about the story where you
drew a bus and forgot the wheels. What would you do now?’’
(open-ended)
For Questions g and h, answers that provided a solution to the
mistakes (e.g., ‘‘Fix it’’) were coded as mastery oriented, whereas
all others (e.g., ‘‘I would walk away’’) were coded as helpless;
intercoder reliability was 100%.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
To compare the two conditions, we derived a composite measure
of helpless/mastery-oriented behavior by standardizing chil-
dren’s responses within each question and averaging across
questions. At baseline, before children experienced any mis-
takes, there was no significant difference between the generic-
and nongeneric-praise conditions. This result suggests that the
two types of praise were equally rewarding. However, on the
postmistake measures, children who received generic praise
exhibited significantly more helpless behavior than children
who received nongeneric praise, t(22) 52.86, p
rep
5.953, d5
1.17. This pattern was true both for children’s self-evaluation
(Questions a–d), t(22) 51.94, p
rep
5.858, d50.79, and for
their persistence (Questions e–h), t(22) 52.28, p
rep
5.903, d5
0.93. Moreover, the pattern for all eight questions went in the
predicted direction (see Fig. 1), p5.008 by a sign test, although
only for two of them—(b) and (f)—did the difference between
conditions reach significance independently.
Generic praise implies there is a stable ability that underlies
performance; subsequent mistakes reflect on this ability and can
therefore be demoralizing. When criticized, children who had
been told they were ‘‘good drawers’’ were more likely than those
who had been told they ‘‘did a good job drawing’’ to denigrate
their skill, feel sad, avoid the unsuccessful drawings and even
drawing in general, and fail to generate strategies to repair their
mistake. When asked what he would do after the teacher’s criti-
cism, one child said, ‘‘Cry. I would do it for both ofthem. Yeah, for
the wheels and the ears.’’ Children who were told they had done
a good job had less extreme emotional reactions and better
strategies for correcting their mistakes. In sum, subtle differences
Fig. 1. Children’s responses to the eight postmistake questions in the generic- and nongeneric-praise conditions.
Error bars represent 11SEM.
Volume 18—Number 4 315
A. Cimpian et al.
in the genericness of language can influence children’s concep-
tion of their abilities and their achievement motivation.
Acknowledgments—We thank Ewart Thomas, Vikram Jaswal,
Joyce Ehrlinger, and Joe Robinson for their helpful comments,
and the teachers, children, and parents at Bing Nursery School.
REFERENCES
Carlson, G.N., & Pelletier, F.J. (1995). The generic book. Chicago:
Chicago University Press.
Dweck, C.S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality,
and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Dweck, C.S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York:
Random House.
Gelman, S.A. (2004). Learning words for kinds: Generic noun phrases
in acquisition. In D.G. Hall & S.R. Waxman (Eds.), Weaving a
lexicon (pp. 445–484). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gelman, S.A., & Heyman, G.D. (1999). Carrot-eaters and creature-
believers: The effects of lexicalization on children’s inferences
about social categories. Psychological Science,10, 489–493.
Kamins, M.L., & Dweck, C.S. (1999). Person versus process praise and
criticism: Implications for contingent self-worth and coping.
Developmental Psychology,35, 835–847.
(RECEIVED 7/13/06; REVISION ACCEPTED 9/1/06;
FINAL MATERIALS RECEIVED 9/6/06)
316 Volume 18—Number 4
Subtle Linguistic Cues Affect Motivation
... Regarding research on parents and mindset specifically, several studies have examined how parenting practices impact the development of students' mindsets Cimpian et al., 2007;Henderlong Corpus & Lepper, 2007;Gunderson et al., 2018;Mueller & Dweck, 1998;Pomerantz & Kempner, 2013;Song et al., 2022). Researchers have also found that parents' mindsets can impact their parenting practices, as well as students' motivation, affect, and achievement (Haimovitz & Dweck, 2016;Justice et al., 2020;Moorman & Pomerantz, 2010;Muenks et al., 2015;Pomerantz & Dong, 2006;Rowe & Leech, 2019). ...
... Overall, our findings support the idea that parents and siblings should communicate a growth mindset to students in order to create a growth mindset family context. Extant research suggests that one possible way to communicate a growth mindset is to use process-focused praise including feedback on strategies, effort, and challenge-seeking (e.g., highlighting how much progress the child has made in terms of learning in math), rather than person-focused praise which focuses on children's talent or outcomefocused praise which centers on the final product Cimpian et al., 2007;Dweck, 2008). Further, recent work with college STEM students has found that they are more likely to perceive their instructor to have a growth mindset when their instructor communicates positive messages about student progress and success, provides opportunities for practice and feedback, responds to poor performance with ways to improve, and places value on student learning and development (Kroeper et al., 2022). ...
Article
Mindsets are defined as people’s beliefs about the nature of intelligence, and previous research has found effects of students’ mindsets on their academic outcomes. In the present study, we bring together two recent lines of mindset research: research that has demonstrated that the mindset contexts that surround students matter above and beyond students’ own mindsets; and research that has demonstrated the importance of parents’ mindsets on students’ academic outcomes. Specifically, we explored associations among the family mindset context—operationalized as undergraduate students’ perceptions of their parents’ and older siblings’ mindsets beliefs about math ability—and their motivation, behavior, and affect in math. We found that students’ (N = 358) perceptions of their parents’ and older siblings’ fixed math mindsets were negatively associated with their motivation, engagement, and help-seeking behaviors in math. These findings underscore the importance of family mindset contexts to students’ math motivation and engagement, especially the role of older siblings, which is a particularly novel contribution.
... Many studies have shown that verbal praise would support children's resilience and persistence performance, and criticize would diminish children's interests of continuing. Such praise should encourage more positive behaviors, including better academic performance, later in life [29,30,31,32]. In both experimental and real-world settings, researchers found that children display higher levels of motivation and more persistence on work tasks when receiving praise for their efforts [25,29,32]. ...
... Such praise should encourage more positive behaviors, including better academic performance, later in life [29,30,31,32]. In both experimental and real-world settings, researchers found that children display higher levels of motivation and more persistence on work tasks when receiving praise for their efforts [25,29,32]. Kamins & Dweck (1999) found that children would show more helpless responses when they received personal criticism or praise rather than process criticism or praise [33]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Resilience can affect and influence individuals’ executive functioning, social behavior, and academic learning ability. Many researchers have examined on the effect of parenting style on children’s resilience. However, less is known about how specific parental language influences children’s resilience. To close the important knowledge gap, this study aims to test whether the positiveness of long-term parental language input is significantly related to high level of behavioral persistence among children beyond infancy. Data will be collected from children at three different age groups (18-month-old, 4-year-old, and 8-year-old) to critically evaluate how parents’ positive (activity-engagement) language style and negative (activity-avoidant) language style will influence children’s level of persistence and resilience at different developmental stages. It is hypothesized that, across age groups, children who receive more positive activity-engagement language input from parent’s exhibit will show higher level of resilience.
... A teacher's praise influences the learning environment in a positive way, thus contributing substantially to the classroom climate longitudinally (Ingemarson et al., 2020). In addition, studies have shown that a teacher's praise can enhance students' learning and prosocial behavior, as well as contribute to a positive learning environment and reduce stress and school burnout (Cimpian et al., 2007;Gable et al., 2009;Hoferichter & Raufelder, 2022;Zentall & Morris, 2012). Overall, research studies until now have shown that the relationship aspect is more prominent than the interaction aspect (Schlesier, 2020). ...
Article
This paper describes the development and validation of an instrument to assess how students deal with emotionally challenging classroom situations (the DECCS Questionnaire). The questionnaire is based on a vignette with one learning and one performance situation in a classroom, and is intended for students in grades 4 to 7. On a sample of N = 639 students (Mage = 10.6 years; SD = 1.25, 52.4% girls) in northwest Germany, factor analytic procedures for the subdimensions of students dealing with emotionally challenging classroom situations show that two factors can be distinguished each for students’ appraisals, for students’ adaptive behavior, for students’ maladaptive behavior, and for students’ desired teacher behavior. The questionnaire and subscales demonstrate good reliability and validity values. Thus, the DECCS questionnaire shows good psychometric properties and is well suited for determining how students deal with emotionally challenging classroom situations. Open Access Link: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/02724316231162307
... Additionally, it is crucial for students to understand that their abilities can be cultivated through effort and to recognize the different concepts and behaviors associated with a growth mindset and a fixed mindset (Rissanen et al., 2019). Research has suggested that language, words, and praise used in interventions can significantly influence a student's mindset (Brock & Hundley, 2018;Cimpian et al., 2007). In particular, words of praise should focus on process rather than ability (e.g. ...
Article
To date, few online game-based learning studies have focused on developing children’s growth creativity mindset (growth CM). This study, therefore, aimed to develop a game-based learning system to help children develop their growth CM. Additionally, we investigated the relationship between growth CM, hope belief, and creativity self-efficacy after game-based learning. Participants consisted of 132 fourth and fifth graders. With a pretest-posttest control group design, the experimental group completed a four-week intervention, whereas the control group received regular computer classes. Four types of creativity mindsets were identified: growth-internal, growth-external, fixed-internal, and fixed-external. The findings suggest that it is easier to overcome fixed CM than to enhance growth CM through game-based learning. Moreover, growth CM contributes to the development of hope belief and creativity self-efficacy, whereas fixed CM undermines hope belief and creativity self-efficacy in a game-based environment. Finally, hope belief is a mediator between growth CM and creativity self-efficacy. The findings of this study have implications for the design of a game-based learning system used to enhance growth CM, hope belief, and creativity self-efficacy.
... In addition to growth mindset interventions aimed at high-schoolers, both experimental and correlational studies document that adults' praise emphasizing the role of malleable effort in success (rather than fixed aspects of a person's ability) increases preschool and elementary-school children's persistence on, enjoyment of, and choice of challenging tasks (e.g., Cimpian et al., 2007;Gunderson et al., 2013;Mueller & Dweck, 1998; but see Li & Bates, 2019). Furthermore, parents' messages about failure may be even more important than messages about success in shaping children's beliefs about the role of effort and fixed ability. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this review, we propose that fiber arts – a wide array of practices that use string, yarn, and fabric to create functional and fine art textiles – present a novel avenue to both explore basic science questions about spatial skills and to design interventions that help children learn spatial skills. First, we outline how fiber arts are applicable to existing theoretical frameworks that aim to organize our understanding of spatial skills and highlight how fiber arts may be particularly relevant for understanding critically understudied non-rigid spatial skills. Next, we review the environmental factors that influence spatial skill development. In the third section of the paper, we review the literature on gender differences in spatial skill performance, as well as intervention approaches that have been taken to close gender gaps. Fourth, we outline how motivational features of fiber arts, specifically the roles of individual choice in goal-setting, and growth-mindset-consistent messages in fiber arts contexts, could contribute to spatial learning. Finally, we suggest several avenues for future research, including leveraging fiber arts materials and techniques to investigate non-rigid mental transformation skills, and designing gender-inclusive fiber-arts-based spatial skills interventions that maintain the motivationally relevant features of fiber arts practices and contexts.
Chapter
Self-Determination Theory (SDT) is a broad theory of psychological growth and wellness that has revolutionized how we think about human motivation and the driving forces behind personality development. SDT focuses on people’s basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness and how social environments that support these needs foster more volition, vitality, and full functioning. SDT has supplied the basis for new and more effective practices in parenting, education, business, sport, healthcare, and other areas of life, fostering higher-quality motivation, engagement, and satisfaction. Drawing on over four decades of evidence-based research and application, The Oxford Handbook of Self-Determination Theory delivers a truly integrative volume by the top researchers and experts within the field of SDT. Edited by SDT co-founder Richard M. Ryan, this Handbook not only provides the theory’s historical and scientific underpinnings but also draws together the latest research and insights, covering topics from the social and biological underpinnings of motivation and wellness to practical applications in all aspects of life. This volume will be an invaluable resource for both researchers and practitioners, as well as any student of human nature, with practical research and guidance.
Article
Behavioral science interventions have the potential to address longstanding policy problems, but their effects are typically heterogeneous across contexts (e.g., teachers, schools, and geographic regions). This contextual heterogeneity is poorly understood, however, which reduces the field’s impact and its understanding of mechanisms. Here, we present an efficient way to interrogate heterogeneity and address these gaps in knowledge. This method a) presents scenarios that vividly represent different moderating contexts, b) measures a short-term behavioral outcome (e.g., an academic choice) that is known to relate to typical intervention outcomes (e.g., academic achievement), and c) assesses the causal effect of the moderating context on the link between the psychological variable typically targeted by interventions and this short-term outcome. We illustrated the utility of this approach across four experiments (total n = 3,235) that directly tested contextual moderators of the links between growth mindset, which is the belief that ability can be developed, and students’ academic choices. The present results showed that teachers’ growth mindset-supportive messages and the structural opportunities they provide moderated the link between students’ mindsets and their choices (studies 1 to 3). This pattern was replicated in a nationally representative sample of adolescents and did not vary across demographic subgroups (study 2), nor was this pattern the result of several possible confounds (studies 3 to 4). Discussion centers on how this method of interrogating contextual heterogeneity can be applied to other behavioral science interventions and broaden their impact in other policy domains.
Chapter
Implicit theories of intelligence, or intelligence mindsets, refer to individuals’ basic beliefs regarding whether intelligence is a fixed trait or a malleable ability. Recently, these mindsets have been analyzed at the organizational level to characterize an environment’s endorsement of these beliefs. Environments embracing a fixed mindset believe postulated inherent brilliance rather than continuous dedication is the key to success. The present chapter begins by providing a selective review of implicit theories of intelligence at both the individual and the organizational levels, demonstrating the acquisition of these beliefs and their profound effects in shaping adults' and children's behaviors. Next, this chapter introduces another class of lay beliefs about intelligence, that is, stereotypes about intelligence based on gender or race. Finally, the chapter summarizes evidence showing how a fixed organizational mindset exerts detrimental impacts on members of negatively stereotyped groups, casting light on the deep causes of group-based inequality in the current society.
Article
This article examines how language affects children's inferences about novel social categories. We hypothesized that lexicalization (using a noun label to refer to someone who possesses a certain property) would influence children's inferences about other people. Specifically, we hypothesized that when a property is lexicalized, it is thought to be more stable over time and over contexts. One hundred fifteen children (5- and 7-year-olds) learned about a characteristic of a hypothetical person (e.g., “Rose eats a lot of carrots”). Half the children were told a noun label for each character (e.g., “She is a carrot-eater”), whereas half heard a verbal predicate (e.g., “She eats carrots whenever she can”). The children judged characteristics as significantly more stable over time and over contexts when the characteristics were referred to by a noun than when they were referred to by a verbal predicate. Lexicalization (in the form of a noun) provides important information to children regarding the stability of personal characteristics.
Article
In an attempt to address the theoretical gap between linguistics and philosophy, a group of semanticists, calling itself the Generic Group, has worked to develop a common view of genericity. Their research has resulted in this book, which consists of a substantive introduction and eleven original articles on important aspects of the interpretation of generic expressions. The introduction provides a clear overview of the issues and synthesizes the major analytical approaches to them. Taken together, the papers that follow reflect the current state of the art in the semantics of generics, and afford insight into various generic phenomena.
Article
Conventional wisdom suggests that praising a child as a whole or praising his or her traits is beneficial. Two studies tested the hypothesis that both criticism and praise that conveyed person or trait judgments could send a message of contingent worth and undermine subsequent coping. In Study 1, 67 children (ages 5-6 years) role-played tasks involving a setback and received 1 of 3 forms of criticism after each task: person, outcome, or process criticism. In Study 2, 64 children role-played successful tasks and received either person, outcome, or process praise. In both studies, self-assessments, affect, and persistence were measured on a subsequent task involving a setback. Results indicated that children displayed significantly more "helpless" responses (including self-blame) on all dependent measures after person criticism or praise than after process criticism or praise. Thus person feedback, even when positive, can create vulnerability and a sense of contingent self-worth.