Content uploaded by Jamie Ward
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Jamie Ward
Content may be subject to copyright.
Mirror-touch synesthesia is
linked with empathy
Michael J Banissy & Jamie Ward
Watching another person being touched activates a similar
neural circuit to actual touch and, for some people with ‘mirror-
touch’ synesthesia, can produce a felt tactile sensation on their
own body. In this study, we provide evidence for the existence
of this type of synesthesia and show that it correlates with
heightened empathic ability. This is consistent with the notion
that we empathize with others through a process of simulation.
Recent research indicates that people’s ability to empathize with others
relies on shared affective neural systems in which common brain areas
are activated during both experience and passive observation. More-
over, building on the discovery of mirror neurons in the monkey
brain
1
, functional imaging has suggested the existence of mirror
systems in humans not only for actions, but also for sensations and
emotions
2–6
. For example, watching another human being touched
(relative to an object being touched) activates the primary and
secondary somatosensory cortex along with premotor and superior
temporal regions. These systems may be crucial for empathy because
they enable the observer to simulate another’s experience by activating
the same brain areas that are active when the observer experiences the
sameemotionorstate
7
. Consistent with this, a recent study provides
evidence that increased activations in the auditory mirror system are
correlated with improved perspective-taking abilities
8
. Moreover, this
correlation not only included premotor areas, but also extended to
somatosensory cortices, indicating that individuals may start to mirror
the tactile consequences of heard actions
8
. Furthermore, there is a
growing body of evidence suggesting that individuals with autistic
spectrum disorder (ASD) have impaired activity in the action mirror
system
9,10
, which may lead to the deficits in imitation and empathy
observed in ASD
11
.
One recent study reported a single case of vision-touch or mirror-
touch synesthesia in which the observation of touch on other humans
results in tactile sensations on her own body. Functional magnetic
resonance imaging showed that these conscious tactile experiences are
associated with hyperactivity in the same mirror-touch network that is
evoked by observed touch in nonsynesthete controls in which no overt
tactile experience is elicited
3
. As such, mirror-touch synesthesia offers a
unique opportunity to explore the role that the tactile mirror system
has in empathy.
We developed a new behavioral protocol to provide evidence for the
authenticity of this form of synesthesia (see Supplementary Methods
online). If synesthetic touch uses the same neural circuit as actual touch
and is phenomenologically similar to actual touch, then participants
should have difficulty in discriminating between actual and synesthetic
touch. We designed two experiments, with participants being required
to report the location of actual touch (left, right, both, none) applied to
the facial cheeks in one, and the location of actual touch applied to the
back of the hands (left or right hand) in the other. During the task they
also observed another person being touched, but were asked to ignore
this. All participants gave informed written consent to the experiments.
For synesthetes, but not for controls, the observed touch elicited a
tactile sensation, whose location was either in the same spatial location
as the actual touch (congruent condition) or in a different spatial
location (incongruent condition). For example, in an incongruent trial
they might receive an actual touch on the left cheek (and are thus
required to give the response ‘left’), but a synesthetic touch on the right
cheek. Reporting in the incongruent condition was expected to be
slower and more error prone. In particular, we were interested in errors
in which the participant treated the synesthetic touch as if it were an
actual touch (that is, giving the response ‘both’ in the example above): a
mirror-touch error (Fig. 1).
For some synesthetes an observed touch on the left cheek triggered a
synesthetic sensation on their left cheek (anatomical correspondence),
but for others the synesthetic sensation was felt on the right cheek (as if
they had been looking in a mirror, a specular correspondence). As such,
congruency was determined according to each synesthete’s self report
(see Supplementary Methods and Supplementary Table 1).
On both the faces and hands experiment, synesthetes (n ¼ 10)
produced a higher percentage of mirror-touch errors than did controls
(n ¼ 20). This pattern of errors implies that synesthetic touch feels like
real touch. Synesthetes were significantly faster at identifying a site
© 2007 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience
Incongruent trail
Visual
display
Participant
Correct response = left
Mirror-touch error = both or right
Correct response = right
Other error = left, both or none
Other error = none
“Left”
“Right”
Synesthetic
touch
Actual
touch
Actual
touch
Synesthetic
touch
Possible
responses
Congruent trail
Figure 1 Participants were required to report the site on which they were
actually touched (that is, left cheek, right cheek, both cheeks or no touch)
while ignoring observed touch (and the synesthetic touch induced by it).
Hands and faces were presented in two separate blocks. In the hands
experiment, the perspective from which touch was observed was manipulated
so that touch was shown from either one’s own or from another’s perspective.
Received 30 January; accepted 18 May; published online 17 June 2007; doi:10.1038/nn1926
Department of Psychology, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London, WC1H OAP, UK. Correspondence should be addressed to M.J.B. (m.banissy@ucl.ac.uk).
NATURE NEUROSCIENCE AD VANCE ON LINE PUBLICA TION 1
BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS
touched on their face or hands when actual touch was congruent with
their synesthesia compared with incongruent trials (Fig. 2). This
pattern was not found when participants observed touch to objects
(see Supplementary Results online).
The empathic ability of mirror-touch synesthetes (n ¼ 10) was
compared with those of nonsynesthetic control participants
(n ¼ 20) and of controls (n ¼ 25) who reported other types of
synesthesia (minimally, grapheme-color) but not mirror-touch
synesthesia. The empathy quotient (EQ) has three main subscales:
(i) cognitive empathy, (ii) emotional reactivity and (iii) social
skills
12,13
. Mirror-touch synesthetes showed significantly higher
scores on the emotional reactivity subscale of the EQ relative to
controls (Table 1), but not on the other subscales. It has been
suggested that the experiencing aspects of affective empathy may
particularly depend on shared interpersonal representations
14
.This
supports the notion that empathy is multifaceted and that the
tactile mirror system may modulate some, but not all, aspects of
this ability.
Given the neural mechanisms thought to be involved in mirror-
touch synesthesia, the differences in empathic ability reported here
appear consistent with the hypothesis that we understand and
empathize with others by a process of simulation
7
.
Note: Supplementary information is available on the Nature Neuroscience website.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
M.J.B. is supported by an Economic and Social Research Council studentship.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
M.J.B. conducted the experiments. J.W. devised the concept. The authors
contributed equally in all other respects.
COMPETING INTERESTS STATEMENT
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Published online at http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience
Reprints and permissions information is available online at http://npg.nature.com/
reprintsandpermissions
1. Rizzolatti, G. et al. Brain 119, 593–609 (1996).
2. Buccino, G. et al. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 400–404 (2001).
3. Blakemore, S.J. et al. Brain 128, 1571–1583 (2005).
4. Keysers, C. et al. Neuron 42, 335–346 (2004).
5. Singer, T. et al. Science 303, 1157–1162 (2004).
6. Wicker, B. et al. Neuron 40, 655–664 (2003).
7. Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 493–501 (1998).
8. Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L. & Keysers, C. Curr. Bio. 16, 1824–1829 (2006).
9. Dapretto, M. et al. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 28–30 (2005).
10. Oberman, L.M. et al. Brain Res. Cog. Brain Res. 24, 190–198 (2005).
11. Oberman, L.M. & Ramachandran, V.S. Psych. Bulletin 133, 310–327 (2007).
12. Baron-Cohen, S. et al. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 358, 361–374 (2003).
13. Lawrence, E.J. et al. Psychol. Med. 34, 911–924 (2004).
14. Lawrence, E.J. et al. Neuroimage 29, 1173–1184 (2006).
© 2007 Nature Publishing Group http://www.nature.com/natureneuroscience
Figure 2 Behavioral correlates of mirror-touch
synesthesia. (a,b) Faces experiment. Reaction-
time performance was compared using 2
(congruency) 2 (group) mixed ANOVA.
Participants performed faster overall on congruent
than on incongruent trials (F ¼ 10.69,
P ¼ 0.003). A significant interaction was
observed (F ¼ 10.37, P ¼ 0.003), and this was
a result of synesthetes performing slower on
incongruent trials (t ¼ –2.69, P ¼ 0.028). Mirror-
touch synesthetes had a higher percentage of
mirror-touch errors compared with controls
(t ¼ 2.54, P ¼ 0.032), but showed no differences
in other error types. (c–f) Hands experiment. A 2
(congruency) 2 (group) 2 (perspective) mixed
ANOVA conducted on reaction times revealed a
significant congruency group interaction
(F ¼ 18.93, P o 0.001), which was a result of
synesthetes performing slower on incongruent
trials (t ¼ –3.08, P ¼ 0.022). Analysis of errors
revealed a significant main effect of error type
(F ¼ 9.91, P ¼ 0.004) and of group (F ¼ 12.42, P ¼ 0.002), with participants producing more mirror-touch errors than other errors and synesthetes being
more error prone overall. Notably, there was a significant group error type interaction (F ¼ 10.02, P ¼ 0.004) showing that synesthetes produced particularly
more mirror-touch errors than controls on trials shown from either perspective (own perspective: F ¼ 10.91, P ¼ 0.003, c and d; another’s perspective:
F ¼ 11.62, P ¼ 0.002, e and f). For details of other effects see Supplementary Results. Error bars are ± s.e.m.
850
a
b
c
d
e
f
800
750
700
650
600
Reaction time (ms)
550
500
450
400
350
300
Reaction time (ms)
100
150
200
250
300
350
400
450
500
Reaction time (ms)
100
150
200
250
300
350
400
450
500
Error rate (%)
0
8
4
12
16
20
24
28
Error rate (%)
0
8
4
12
16
20
24
28
Error rate (%)
0
8
4
12
16
20
24
28
Mirror touch
(n = 9)
Controls
(n = 20)
Congruent
Incongruent
Congruent
Incongruent
Mirror-touch error
Other error
Congruent
Incongruent
Mirror-touch error
Other error
Mirror-touch error
Other error
Mirror touch
(n = 10)
Controls
(n = 20)
Mirror touch
(n = 7)
Controls
(n = 18)
Mirror touch
(n = 9)
Controls
(n = 18)
Mirror touch
(n = 7)
Controls
(n = 18)
Mirror touch
(n = 9)
Controls
(n = 18)
Table 1 EQ scores
Group EQ score
Cognitive
empathy
Emotional
reactivity Social skills
Mirror-touch
(n ¼ 10)
51.30 ± 3.20 19.30 ± 1.27 17.20 ± 1.41 9.70 ± 1.04
Controls
(n ¼ 45)
46.20 ± 1.82 15.51 ± 0.86 13.56 ± 0.74 8.47 ± 0.42
Significance N.S. N.S. P ¼ 0.036 N.S.
Results from nonsynesthetic controls (n ¼ 20) and synesthetes lacking mirror touch
(n ¼ 25) did not differ and were combined. N.S. ¼ not significant. Data shown as
mean ± s.e.m.
2 ADVANCE ONLINE PUBLICATION NATURE NEUROSCIENCE
BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS