Spatial Adaptations for Plant Foraging: Women Excel and Calories Count

Article (PDF Available)inProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 274(1626):2679-84 · December 2007with32 Reads
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2007.0826 · Source: PubMed
Abstract
We present evidence for an evolved sexually dimorphic adaptation that activates spatial memory and navigation skills in response to fruits, vegetables and other traditionally gatherable sessile food resources. In spite of extensive evidence for a male advantage on a wide variety of navigational tasks, we demonstrate that a simple but ecologically important shift in content can reverse this sex difference. This effect is predicted by and consistent with the theory that a sexual division in ancestral foraging labour selected for gathering-specific spatial mechanisms, some of which are sexually differentiated. The hypothesis that gathering-specific spatial adaptations exist in the human mind is further supported by our finding that spatial memory is preferentially engaged for resources with higher nutritional quality (e.g. caloric density). This result strongly suggests that the underlying mechanisms evolved in part as adaptations for efficient foraging. Together, these results demonstrate that human spatial cognition is content sensitive, domain specific and designed by natural selection to mesh with important regularities of the ancestral world.

Figures

Spatial adaptations for plant foraging: women
excel and calories count
Joshua New
1,
*
, Max M. Krasnow
2
, Danielle Tr uxaw
2
and Steven J. C. Gaulin
3
1
Department of Psychology, Yale University, 2 Hillhouse Avenue, New Haven, CT 06520-8205, USA
2
Department of Psychology, and
3
Department of Anthropology, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106-9660, USA
We present evidence for an evolved sexually dimorphic adaptation that activates spatial memory and
navigation skills in response to fruits, vegetables and other traditionally gatherable sessile food resources.
In spite of extensive evidence for a male advantage on a wide variety of navigational tasks, we demonstrate
that a simple but ecologically important shift in content can reverse this sex difference. This effect is
predicted by and consistent with the theory that a sexual division in ancestral foraging labour selected for
gathering-specific spatial mechanisms, some of which are sexually differentiated. The hypothesis that
gathering-specific spatial adaptations exist in the human mind is further supported by our finding that
spatial memory is preferentially engaged for resources with higher nutritional quality (e.g. caloric density).
This result strongly suggests that the underlying mechanisms evolved in part as adaptations for efficient
foraging. Together, these results demonstrate that human spatial cognition is content sensitive, domain
specific and designed by natural selection to mesh with important regularities of the ancestral world.
Keywords: foraging adaptations; sex differences; optimal foraging theory; spatial cognition; navigation
1. INTRODUCTION
Spatial cognitionin humans is not a unitary faculty (Halpern
2000); rather, it seems to reflect the operation of a number of
functionally distinct (and neurally dissociable) cognitive
specializations, each designed for solving a different adaptive
problem. The selection pressures shaping some of these
specializations would have been similar for ancestral men
and women, producing sexually monomorphic compu-
tational design. In certain cases, however, ancestral men and
women would have faced distinct spatial demands; in these
cases, we should find that the resultant cognitive
mechanisms are sexually dimorphic.
Such sex differences are well documented in the existing
literature on human spatial abilities. Spatial tasks exhibit
some of the largest and most reliable sex differences in
cognitive performance. On many spatial tasks, male
advantage is typical (Linn & Petersen 1985; Voyer et al.
1995) and these findings have often been used to conclude
that men have superior spatial ability (Linn & Petersen
1985). Using an evolution-minded approach, however,
Silverman & Eals (1992) predicted and documented a
specific female advantage. Their foraging adaptation theory
argues that hunting mobile prey and gathering immobile
resources have different computational requirements; to the
extent that the universal sexual division of foraging labour
among described hunter–gatherers (Murdock 1967)
characterized our ancestral past, sexually dimorphic fora-
ging-related cognitive specializations should be observable
in the minds of modern men and women.
According to this foraging adaptation theory, many of
the spatial tasks that presently show a male advantage
engage cognitive mechanisms designed by natural selec-
tion for successful hunting. In hunting, mobile prey are
pursued over erratic and unpredictable courses often
through unfamiliar environments; given the energetic
costs of transport, the spoils should be carried home via
a more direct route, a task that can be accomplished by
vector integration (Gallistel 1990) or from vector compu-
tation within a survey representation of the environment.
Past tests of navigating and wayfinding (Moffat et al. 1998;
Sandstrom et al. 1998; Lawton & Morrin 1999; Silverman
et al. 2000; Malinowski 2001), as well as some laboratory
tasks such as mental rotation (Linn & Petersen 1985;
Voyer et al. 1995), may elicit a male advantage by engaging
spatial mechanisms that evolved for this kind of navigation
while hunting.
The spatial problems posed by gathering are quite
different. Gathered resources are stationary and vary in
quality and availability with time—a fig tree may have
nothing valuable now but be laden with fruit in the near
future. To relocate diverse resources as they become valuable
requires a mechanism that registers and stores the locations
of many stationary food resources within a survey map of a
more constrained and well-known environment. A well-
designed adaptive specialization for gathering would
accumulate such information automatically, without special
attention, during daily activities. Silverman & Eals (1992)
argued that, if a sexually dimorphic spatial adaptation for
gathering exists, women should excel at remembering the
locations of items within a complex spatial array, especially
in incidental learning tasks.
Silverman and Eals operationalized these skills as
‘object-location memory’ and devised a number of
pencil-and-paper and desktop tasks to assess it. The
predicted female advantage has sometimes been demon-
strated on these tasks, but the effect is sensitive to details of
task presentation, sometimes appearing robustly and
sometimes disappearing entirely (Eals & Silverman
1994; James & Kimura 1997; Dabbs et al. 1998; see
Postma et al. 2004 for a review). Arguably, some of these
Proc. R. Soc. B (2007) 274, 2679–2684
doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0826
Published online 21 August 2007
* Author for correspondence (joshua.new@yale.edu).
Received 19 June 2007
Accepted 2 August 2007
2679 This journal is q 2007 The Royal Society
effects are theory relevant (e.g. larger female advantage in
incidental than directed learning tasks), but others are
more difficult to interpret (e.g. no female advantage for
difficult-to-name objects), leaving their experimental
demonstration vulnerable to alternative interpretations.
Such fragile effects may be a consequence of using
experimental tasks that only weakly engage spatial
adaptations for plant foraging. Put another way, the
division-of-labour model has not been effectively tested
because the scale (8.5
00
by 11
00
or desktop), content (e.g.
household items or machine parts) and task (did the item
move?) do not match the scale (walking), content
(immobile foods) and task (which way to the mongongo
nut grove?) of real-world plant foraging. Thus, a stronger
female advantage should be observed on a spatial task that
better approximates the ancestral conditions of plant
foraging: specifically, a task that provides both the cues
appropriate to engage the hypothesized mechanism and
the kind of information it is designed to process (i.e. the
content and location of nutritional resource patches). No
prior investigation has tested whether women demonstrate
superior spatial memory for food resource locations at a
real-world scale.
In this study, we employed the scale, spatial complexity
and item diversity of a large farmers’ market to assess
memory for the location of immobile food resources such
as leaves, fruits, nuts, roots and honey. If the Silverman
and Eals’ hypothesis is correct, then
H
1
: Females will remember the locations of such food
resources more accurately than men.
In general, women prefer to navigate by landmarks and
routes, rather than by vectoring (Halpern 2000), and a
methodology that emphasized route- and landmark-based
strategies might well have produced a female advantage.
However, our intention was not to replicate these well-
known sex differences, but to test for a predicted content
effect: females more accurately recalling the location of
immobile foods. Thus, in order to most effectively jeopardize
this hypothesis, we designed our study to favour men’s
established wayfinding strengths. To this end, participants
were tested in a newly learned and directly experienced
environment (Montello et al.1999), were not explicitly
oriented to any landmarks during exposure (Baenninger
1997) and were tested via pointing to the non-visible
resource locations (Hegarty et al. 2006)—a vectoring
measure that advantages the orientation strategy favoured
by men and disadvantages the route strategy typically
favoured by women (Lawton 1994). It is possible that
such a design might obscure any actual female advantage.
However, effective plant foragers would require the ability to
flexibly integrate various navigation strategies as dictated by
their nutritional needs and patterns of resource availability.
Thus, we predict that women will also be able to form survey
representations and estimate vectors to resource locations.
To test for this ability, participants were tested at a central
off-route location via pointing, a measure that minimizes the
possible employment of route- and landmark-based
strategies for responding. A female advantage on such a
task would be strong evidence for a foraging-related
adaptation.
The suite of cognitive skills labelled ‘spatial abilities’
most likely arose from selection pressures in a number of
domains (e.g. searching for nutrients, searching for mates;
Hewlett et al. 1986; Gaulin & FitzGerald 1989) and
subdomains (e.g. searching for food versus searching for
water; Petrinovich & Bolles 1954). Thus, although we
predict a female advantage on resource location memory,
some foraging-related selection pressures may have
impinged similarly on ancestral males and females. For
example, optimal foraging theory (Schoener 1971)
addresses the mechanisms that underlie dietary choice.
Such theories assume that foragers are capable of assessing
the ‘profitability’ of potential food items as a basis for
eating or rejecting them and have been validated on
human foragers ( Winterhalder & Smith 2000). This
assumption justifies a collateral prediction:
H
2
: The locations of more nutritionally valuable resources
will be more accurately remembered than less nutritionally
valuable resources.
Such a bias would support the argument that the
mechanisms underlying resource location memory are
adaptations for foraging.
2. MATERIAL AND METHODS
Data were collected during the spring and summer of 2004 at
six separate meetings of the Saturday morning farmers’
market in Santa Barbara, California. The market comprises
10 orderly rows of vendors laid out in a rectangular 0.6 ha
area. There were typically approximately 90 food stalls.
(a) Participants
Eighty-six adults (41 females; mean age, 35 years) participated
in this experiment. Data from 18 additional participants were
discarded due to participant attrition, food running out at a
target location or experimenter error during the task.
Participants were recruited near an entrance to the
farmers’ market and told that they could earn $10 (or $5
and a UCSB tote bag) by participating in ‘farmers’ market
research’. This cover story—which does not refer to any
spatial task—was used to ensure that the encoding of resource
locations would be strictly incidental. All participants were
given a consent form on which they were asked to indicate
food allergies. Participants were then asked their age and how
frequently they visited this farmers’ market. The answers to
these questions were recorded by the experimenter, who also
made note of each participant’s sex.
(b) Materials and procedures
Participants were led by a circuitous route to each of six food
stalls, where they were given a food item to eat. The precise
food items, stalls and routes were fixed on any given day of
data collection but varied over the 6 days. Within each day,
subjects were assigned equally to the ‘forward’ and ‘reversed’
versions of the fixed route (table 1 for food items and orders).
At each stall, participants were asked a set list of questions
which served in part to promote the cover story and were also
analysed as possible predictors of pointing accuracy:
(i) ‘On a scale of 1–7, how much do you like the taste
of this?’
(ii) ‘On a scale of 1–7, how often do you eat this?’
(iii) ‘On a scale of 1–7, how attractive do you think this
stall is?’
(iv) ‘How many times have you purchased from this stall?’
2680 J. New et al. Foraging adaptations: spatial cognition
Proc. R. Soc. B (2007)
Experimenters recorded participants’ answers.
After visiting all six stalls, participants were taken to a
pointing device in the centre of the market area. The pointing
device was a flat, horizontal, circular board of 30 cm in
diameter mounted on a camera tripod and located such that
all of the visited stalls were obscured from view. The board
was marked radially in 18 increments. A wooden pointer was
fixed at the centre of the circle and could be spun freely.
Participants were asked to aim the pointer at each of the six
food items in one of two predetermined orders that differed
from both presentation orders. The experimenter recorded
the indicated bearing for each food item.
Participants then were asked to assess their general sense of
direction on a 1–7 scale, a measure known to be correlated with
field measures of navigational ability (Kozlowski & Bryant
1977; Sholl 1988; Montello & Pick 1993; Prestopnik &
Roskos-Ewoldson 2000; Sholl et al.2000). Participants were
then offered information about the background and rationale of
the study and were finally taken back to the farmers’ market
entrance to receive payment.
3. RESULTS
(a) Analytical methods
Pointing error, our inverse estimate of accuracy of food
location memory, was measured in degrees as the smallest
difference between each participant’s estimate and the true
bearing to each of the six food locations. Errors, therefore,
ranged from 08 (perfect accuracy) to 1808 (opposite from
correct direction), with 908 indicating chance performance
(Sholl et al.2000). Smaller errors indicate greater accuracy.
To test our hypotheses, we modelled pointing error in a
two-level hierarchical linear model (HLM) with individual
pointing errors as first-level observations and participants as
second-level observations (following the random coeffi-
cients method described in Raudenbush & Bryk 2002).
As first-level predictors, we entered caloric density
(kcal 100 g
K1
, an estimate of nutritional value; USDA,
no date http://www.nal.usda.gov/fnic/foodcomp/search/)
which we log-transformed to correct for a strong negative
skew, as well as the participants four ratings of the pointed-
to foods and stalls ((i)–(iv)). As second-level predictors we
entered participant sex, as well as their number of weekly
visits to the market, their self-rated sense of direction and a
rank variable reflecting differences among weeks in the
difficulty of the test environment. (Stalls and routes differed
from week to week, leading to differences in weekly average
error; including this variable allowed us to separate these
theory-irrelevant differences from the effects of interest.)
(b) H
1
: Are women more accurate than men at
pointing to newly learned food locations?
Yes. Women were, on average, 98 more accurate in their
pointing estimates than men (gZK8.917, t(80)Z2.454,
pZ0.017). This corresponds to a 27% improvement in
performance compared with men.
This female advantage in accuracy was not due to
women having more experience at the farmers’ market
than men. Experience at the market did not predict
performance, either as a zero-order effect or in the HLM
model (table 2, between-subjects effects). The female
Table 1. Weekly items, routes and descriptive statistics. (Food items used over the course of the 6 day experiment are listed
below. Item number indicates the order in which participants were led to each food item. Half of the participants were led around
in the ‘forward’ direction, as indicated by the first number listed. The other half of the participants were led around in the
‘backward’ direction, as indicated by the second number, listed in parentheses.)
day
item number 1 2 3 4 5 6
1 (6) cherries oranges olive oil fennel basil strawberries
2 (5) sugar peas cherimoya peaches blueberries peaches plums
3 (4) honey almonds radish sprts apples tangerines carrots
4 (3) tomatoes basil cauliflower peaches strawberries cucumbers
5 (2) almonds rd lf lettuce cherries zucchini peppers green onion
6 (1) cherimoya avocado zucchini olive oil zucchini tangerines
n(F : M) 9(5 : 4) 15(7 : 8) 17(8 : 9) 17(6 : 11) 6(4 : 2) 22(11 : 11)
raw error (s.d.) 28.978 (13.91) 27.908 (19.36) 30.788 (13.93) 25.178 (12.72) 22.958 (7.58) 48.318 (17.71)
Table 2. Pointing error: hierarchical linear model (with robust standard errors).
effect coefficient s.e. t d.f. p
between-subjects effects
intercept 32.891 1.786 18.414 80 1.68!10
K30
sex K8.917 3.634 K2.454 80 0.017
sense of direction K3.737 1.276 K2.928 80 0.005
week rank 2.699 1.389 1.943 80 0.055
experience K0.952 1.129 K0.843 80 0.402
within-subjects effects
log(kcal) K14.309 3.030 K4.722 478 3.07!10
K6
food liking K0.275 0.794 K0.347 478 0.729
food eat often 1.380 0.790 1.747 478 0.081
stall liking K0.903 1.105 K0.817 478 0.415
stall shop often 0.018 0.175 K0.104 478 0.918
Foraging adaptations: spatial cognition J. New et al. 2681
Proc. R. Soc. B (2007)
advantage is significant even after controlling for experi-
ence at the market.
Did this female advantage arise because our sample was
composed of women who are unusually gifted at navigation?
No. The sense of direction measure has been shown to be a
good index of one’s general ability to navigate and, as in prior
research, there was a male advantage on this measure
(M
m
Z5.37, s.d.
m
Z1.26; M
f
Z4.56, s.d.
f
Z1.51, t(84)Z
2.68, pZ0.009, two-tailed, dZ0.58). Internal evidence
confirms the ecological validity of this self-report measure: it
predicted unique variance in pointing accuracy for both
sexes (gZK3.737, t(80)Z2.928, pZ0.005).
Clearly, a male advantage in general sense of direction
cannot explain a female advantage in pointing accuracy on
our task. Importantly, the female advantage in vectoring
towards food items was independent of this general ability:
it remains after controlling for sense of direction.
Weekly differences in the difficulty of test conditions did
explain unique variance in pointing accuracy (gZ2.699,
t(80)Z1.943, pZ0.055), but the unique contribution of
sex remains even after controlling for this and all other
variables measured.
(c) H
2
: Do people remember the locations of
higher-quality foods more accurately?
Yes. Foods with higher caloric density were pointed to more
accurately by both sexes (gZK14.309, t(478)Z4.722,
pZ3.07!10
K6
). Since small errors indicate greater
accuracy, this relationship manifests as a negative corre-
lation: high caloric density predicts low pointing errors.
This effect was not due to subjects preferring the taste of
high over low caloric density foods. How much subjects liked
the taste of each food did not correlate with pointing
accuracy, even as a zero-level effect. In contrast, the effect of
caloric density on pointing accuracy remained significant
even after controlling for how much subjects liked the taste
of each food, how often they eat each food, how attractive
they found the stall selling the food, and how often they had
purchased food from that stall. Indeed, none of these other
variables made a zero-order or unique contribution to
performance (table 2, within-subjects effects).
The greater accuracy in locating high-calorie food
items could conceivably have been driven by some other
property confounded with caloric density in our sample of
foods. For example, several of the highest calorie items
might also be considered non-standard for other reasons
(people rarely drink olive oil; olive oil and honey are
liquids whereas the other items are countable objects,
etc.). To account for this general class of alternative
hypotheses, we replicated the HLM analysis after omitting
data for the four highest calorie items (olive oil, almonds,
honey and avocados). An inspection of figure 1 indicates
that the negative correlation between caloric density and
pointing accuracy is actually stronger below 2 log(kcal)
than above, and despite the loss in power due to a
restricted sample, the calorie/accuracy relationship is still
significant when these items are removed.
A different and simpler approach to analysing these data
is to use food items as the unit of analysis, averaging across all
participants’ pointing errors to a given item. This allows one
to test for a relationship between average pointing accuracy
elicited by each food and predictor variables. In this case,
log-transformed caloric density of the food item as well as the
four average ratings of the foods and stalls were regressed on
the average pointing errors. As in the HLM analysis, foods
with higher caloric densities were pointed to more accurately
(bZK0.519, t(25)Z2.849, p!0.01; figure 1). This effect
was constant across women and men in both the HLM and
item-wise analyses (z’s!0.451, n.s.). As in the HLM,
participants did not point more accurately to foods that were
more liked or eaten more often, nor to stalls that were liked
more or shopped at more often (b’sOK0.180, t’s(25)!
0.914, n.s.).
Thus, in accordance with the prediction that the
farmers’ market context would activate adaptations that
evolved for foraging, foods higher in nutritional quality
were pointed to more accurately. Importantly, nutritional
quality was the only variable to independently predict
cucumber
lettuce
bell pepper
carrot
c
b
a
d
g
f
e
h
i
j
zucchini
cherimoya
apple
blueberry
cherry
orange
fennel
avocado
honey
almond
olive oil
b = –0.519**
90
80
70
60
50
40
pointing error (deg.)
30
20
10
0
(error bars indicate ± 1 s.e.)
1.0 1.5 2.0
log (kcal)
2.5 3.0
a – tomato
b – cauliflower
c – green onion
d – plum
e – basil
f – peaches
g – sugar peas
h – strawberry
i – radish sprouts
j – tangerine
Figure 1. Item-wise plot of mean error (Gs.e.) by (log) caloric density.
2682 J. New et al. Foraging adaptations: spatial cognition
Proc. R. Soc. B (2007)
variance in pointing accuracy; indeed, liking, familiarity,
experience and other variables were not significantly
correlated with pointing accuracy.
4. DISCUSSION
Silverman & Eals (1992) argue that the female advantage
on pencil-and-paper and desktop measures of object-
location memory reflects a selective pressure on ancestral
women for plant-foraging efficiency. But their measures
did not involve foods, tested spatial memory on a very
small scale, and included no measure of vectoring; as a
result, a female advantage on their measures is open to
many alternative interpretations. For this reason, we
deemed it important to examine whether a female
advantage could be demonstrated on a task that closely
resembles foraging for plant foods. From this theory, we
predicted that women should remember the locations
where they have previously encountered immobile
resources (e.g. plants, honey) more accurately than do
men. This is a counter-intuitive hypothesis. Accurate
performance on our pointing task requires vectoring
relativetoasurveyrepresentationofresource
locations—the type of spatial representation more often
(Lawton 1994) and more proficiently (Saucier et al. 2002)
employed by men. Although prior research suggests that
men are frequently better at pointing to the locations of
landmarks and other non-food objects under such
circumstances, we have shown that women are better
than men at pointing to spatial locations that contain
nutritional resources. That navigational tasks requiring
vector integration (dead reckoning) show a male advan-
tage when the ‘landmarks’ are not food makes the present
finding of a female advantage all the more compelling and
offers less ambiguous novel support for the idea that
ancestral sex differences in foraging behaviour may have
shaped sex-specific cognitive adaptations.
Given that females are often the primary shopper for
household goods ( Fram & Axelrod 1990), for example,
constituting 73% of the food shopping respondents in a
1992 consumer research study (IMRA 1992), it is
reasonable to question whether the general shopping
environment or context, rather than the food items per se,
provided the cues that enhanced female performance.
However, past research indicates that females are no better
than men at learning generic item locations in real-world
shopping locations (Kirasic 2000), nor pointing to the
locations of unseen vendors, even when they were more
familiar with the shopping centre (Dogu & Erkip 2000).
More recently, males were generally better in learning the
spatial layout of a simulated shopping centre ( Tlauka et al.
2005). This research suggests that females’ greater
experience with typical shopping layouts themselves, or
other aspects of the shopping context, did not account for
their more accurate performance in the present study.
Against the background of prior research on spatial
learning in shopping environments, the present results
suggest that it was the food items that provided the cues
which preferentially engaged female location memory.
The finding that nutritional quality enhances spatial
memory further supports the idea that our task is engaging a
foraging-related spatial adaptation. Interestingly, spatial
memory performance was not explicable in terms of
consciously mediated and articulated preferences for the
food items or their presentation. From the perspective of
current theory in behavioural ecology, the registration of
potential foods’ relative nutritional values is a central
requirement for optimally gathering foods from dispersed
and varying locations (Schoener 1971). Thus, the more
accurate localization of high-calorie food items (and not
those explicitly preferred) appears to reflect the priorities of a
psychological mechanism which was adapted for the
efficient exploitation of plant foods during the 99.7% of
human evolution when our ancestors were foragers. This
result strongly indicates a cognitive system with fine-tuned
dimensions of valuation (e.g. caloric density) built into its
architecture and should encourage the further development
of models of cognition that incorporate ecologically valid
valuation as a computational element (To o b y et al. 2005).
We gratefully thank Leda Cosmides, Tamsin German and the
members of the CEP and SCRAM groups at UC Santa Barbara
for their feedback on earlier drafts of this manuscript and
Nancy Collins for invaluable statistical advice. Additionally, we
thank the vendors and staff of the Santa Barbara Certified
Farmers Market Association for their assistance and patience
during our study. Finally, we thank our team of research
assistants who pitched in so many hours collecting data with us.
REFERENCES
Baenninger, M. 1997 Sex-related differences in the develop-
ment of spatial ability: the efficacy of training. Paper
presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in
Child Development, Washington, DC.
Dabbs, J. M., Chang, E.-L., Strong, R. A. & Milun, R. 1998
Spatial ability, navigation strategy, and geographic knowl-
edge among men and women. Evol. Human Behav. 19,
89–98. (doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(97)00107-4)
Dogu, U. & Erkip, F. 2000 Spatial factors affecting
wayfinding and orientation: a case study in a shopping
mall. Environ. Behav. 32, 731–755. (doi:10.1177/0013
9160021972775)
Eals, M. & Silverman, I. 1994 The hunter–gatherer theory of
spatial sex differences: proximate factors mediating the
female advantage in recall of object arrays. Ethol. Sociobiol.
15, 95–105. (doi:10.1016/0162-3095(94)90020-5)
Fram, E. H. & Axelrod, J. 1990 The distressed shopper. Am.
Demograph. 12, 44–45.
Gallistel, C. R. 1990 Learning, development, and conceptual
change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gaulin, S. J. C. & FitzGerald, R. W. 1989 Sexual selection for
spatial-learning ability. Anim. Behav. 37, 322–331.
(doi:10.1016/0003-3472(89)90121-8)
Halpern, D. F. 2000 Sex differences in cognitive abilities, 3rd
edn. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hegarty, M., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., Ishikawa,
T. & Lovelace, K. 2006 Spatial abilities at different scales:
individual differences in aptitude-test performance and
spatial-layout learning. Intelligence 34, 151–176. (doi:10.
1016/j.intell.2005.09.005)
Hewlett, B. S., van de Koppel, J. M. H. & Cavalli-Sforza,
L. L. 1986 Exploration and mating ranges of the Aka
Pygmies of the Central African Republic. In African
Pygmies (ed. L. L. Cavalli-Sforza), pp. 65–79. New York,
NY: Academic Press.
International Mass Retail Association (IMRA) 1993 The
1992 consumer research study: stop, look and buy.where and
why. Washington, DC: IMRA.
James, T. W. & Kimura, D. 1997 Sex differences in
remembering the locations of objects in an array: location-
shifts versus location-exchanges. Evol. Human Behav. 18,
155–163. (doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(97)00004-4)
Foraging adaptations: spatial cognition J. New et al. 2683
Proc. R. Soc. B (2007)
Kirasic, K. C. 2000 Age differences in adults’ spatial abilities,
learning environment layout, and wayfinding behavior.
Spat. Cognit. Comput. 2, 117–134. (doi:10.1023/A:101144
5624332)
Kozlowski, L. T. & Bryant, K. J. 1977 Sense of direction,
spatial orientation, and cognitive maps. J. Exp. Psychol. 3,
590–598.
Lawton, C. A. 1994 Gender differences in way-finding
strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety.
Sex Roles 30, 765–779. (doi:10.1007/BF01544230)
Lawton, C. & Morrin, K. 1999 Gender differences in
pointing accuracy in computer-simulated 3D mazes. Sex
Roles 40, 73–92. (doi:10.1023/A:1018830401088)
Linn, M. C. & Petersen, A. C. 1985 Emergence and
characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a
meta-analysis. Child Dev. 56, 1479–1498. (doi:10.2307/
1130467)
Malinowski, J. C. 2001 Mental rotation and real-world
wayfinding. Percept. Motor Skills 92, 19–30. (doi:10.2466/
PMS.92.1.19-30)
Moffat, S., Hampson, E. & Hatzipantelis, M. 1998
Navigation in a “virtual” maze: sex differences and
correlation with psychometric measures of spatial ability
in humans. Evol. Human Behav. 19, 73–87. (doi:10.1016/
S1090-5138(97)00104-9)
Montello, D. R. & Pick, H. L. 1993 Integrating knowledge of
vertically aligned large-scale spaces. Environ. Behav. 25,
457–484. (doi:10.1177/0013916593253002)
Montello, D. R., Lovelace, K. L., Golledge, R. G. & Self,
C. M. 1999 Sex-related differences and similarities in
geographic and environmental spatial abilities. Ann. Assoc.
Am. Geogr. 89, 515–534. (doi:10.1111/0004-5608.00160)
Murdock, G. P. 1967 Ethnog raphic atlas. Pittsburgh, PA:
University of Pittsburgh Press.
Petrinovich, L. & Bolles, R. 1954 Deprivation states and
behavioral attributes. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 47,
450–453. (doi:10.1037/h0054731)
Postma, A., Jage, G., Kessels, R. P. C., Koppeschaar, H. P. F.
& van Honk, J. 2004 Sex differences for selective forms of
spatial memory. Brain Cognit. 54, 24–34. (doi:10.1016/
S0278-2626(03)00238-0)
Prestopnik, J. L. & Roskos-Ewoldson, B. 2000 The relations
among wayfinding strategy use, sense of direction, sex,
familiarity, and wayfinding ability. J. Environ. Psychol. 20,
177–191. (doi:10.1006/jevp.1999.0160)
Raudenbush, S. W. & Bryk, A. S. 2002 Hierarchical linear
models: applications and data analysis methods, 2nd edn.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Sandstrom, N., Kaufman, J. & Huettel, S. 1998 Males and
females use different distal cues in a virtual environment
navigation task. Cognit. Brain Res. 6, 351–360. (doi:10.
1016/S0926-6410(98)00002-0)
Saucier, D. M., Green, S. M., Leason, J., MacFadden, A.,
Bell, S. & Elias, L. J. 2002 Are sex differences in navigation
caused by sexually dimorphic strategies or by differences
in the ability to use the strategies. Behav. Neurosci. 116,
403–410. (doi:10.1037/0735-7044.116.3.403)
Schoener, T. W. 1971 Theory of feeding strategies. Annu.
Rev. Ecol. Syst. 2, 369–404. (doi:10.1146/annurev.es.02.
110171.002101)
Sholl, M. J. 1988 The relationship between sense of direction
and mental geographic updating. Intelligence 12, 299–314.
(doi:10.1016/0160-2896(88)90028-1)
Sholl, M. J., Acacio, J. C., Makar, R. O. & Leon, C. 2000 The
relation of sex and sense of direction to spatial orientation
in an unfamiliar environment. J. Environ. Psychol. 20,
17–28. (doi:10.1006/jevp.1999.0146)
Silverman, I. & Eals, M. 1992 Sex differences in spatial
abilities: evolutionary theory and data. In The adapted
mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture
(eds J. Barkow, L. Cosmides & J. Tooby), pp. 533–549.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Silverman, I., Choi, J., Mackewn, A., Fisher, M., Moro, J. &
Olshansky, E. 2000 Evolved mechanisms underlying
wayfinding: further studies on the hunter–gatherer theory
of spatial sex differences. Evol. Human Behav. 21,
201–213. (doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00036-2)
Tlauka, M., Brolese, A., Pomeroy, D. & Hobbs, W. 2005
Gender differences in spatial knowledge acquired through
simulated exploration of a virtual shopping center.
J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 111–118. (doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.
2004.12.002)
Tooby, J., Cosmides, L. & Barrett, H. C. 2005 Resolving the
debate on innate ideas: learnability constraints and the
evolved interpenetration of motivational and conceptual
functions. In The innate mind: structure and content (eds
P. Carruthers, S. Laurence & S. Stich), pp. 305–337.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Voyer, D., Voyer, S. & Bryden, M. P. 1995 Magnitude of sex
differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and
consideration of critical variables. Psychol. Bull. 117,
250–270. (doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.250)
Winterhalder, B. & Smith, E. A. 2000 Analyzing adaptive
strategies: human behavioral ecology at 25. Evol. Anthropol.
9,5172.(doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(2000)9:2!51::
AID-EVAN1O3.0.CO;2-7)
2684 J. New et al. Foraging adaptations: spatial cognition
Proc. R. Soc. B (2007)
    • "For example, hunger may increase the accessibility of memories specifying locations in which food was successfully obtained in the past. Indirect support for this hypothesis comes from fMRI evidence that hunger preferentially enhances recognition memory for food over nonfood (Morris & Dolan, 2001), and from evidence that people in a field setting show preferential recall for the locations of calorie-dense food items over those of calorie-sparse ones (New, Krasnow, Truxaw, & Gaulin, 2007; Krasnow et al., 2011 ). Additional evidence that hunger might impact memory comes from research showing superior word sequence recall for words directly following food-related words, but only among hungry individuals (see Lerner, Singer, & Triandis, 1958). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: An evolutionary psychological perspective suggests that emotions can be understood as superordinate mechanisms whose job is to coordinate various psychological and physiological programs in the service of solving an adaptive problem. This paper suggests that it may also be fruitful to approach hunger from this coordinating mechanism perspective. In this paper, I put forward an evolutionary task analysis of hunger, generating novel a priori hypotheses about the coordinating effects of hunger on psychological processes such as perception, attention, categorization, and memory. This approach appears empirically fruitful in that it yields a bounty of testable new hypotheses.
    Full-text · Article · Jun 2016
    • "We found no significant sex differences in our data. Some previous studies have reported differences between the spatial search abilities of men and women, as predicted from an ancestral sexual division of labor, in object location tasks [56] and spatial navigation575859, while others have not (for visual search in [60]; object location memory in [59]). Whether the spatial search strategies and hiding-place recall abilities called upon in our study evoke the same foraging adaptations that others have investigated must be determined before our lack of sex differences can be compared to the results found by others. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: Resources are often distributed in clumps or patches in space, unless an agent is trying to protect them from discovery and theft using a dispersed distribution. We uncover human expectations of such spatial resource patterns in collaborative and competitive settings via a sequential multi-person game in which participants hid resources for the next participant to seek. When collaborating, resources were mostly hidden in clumpy distributions, but when competing, resources were hidden in more dispersed (random or hyperdispersed) patterns to increase the searching difficulty for the other player. More dispersed resource distributions came at the cost of higher overall hiding (as well as searching) times, decreased payoffs, and an increased difficulty when the hider had to recall earlier hiding locations at the end of the experiment. Participants' search strategies were also affected by their underlying expectations, using a win-stay lose-shift strategy appropriate for clumpy resources when searching for collaboratively-hidden items, but moving equally far after finding or not finding an item in competitive settings, as appropriate for dispersed resources. Thus participants showed expectations for clumpy versus dispersed spatial resources that matched the distributions commonly found in collaborative versus competitive foraging settings.
    Full-text · Article · Jul 2015
    • "This finding was not related to food preferences or taste-driven desire for those foods, as this had been evaluated prior to the spatial memory exam and subsequently controlled. More likely, the accuracy reflects the ancestral foraging experiences where survival necessitated the securing of nutritionally dense plant foods [84]. Other researchers have found that human preference for the shininess of contemporary glossy (vs. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: Famed microbiologist René J. Dubos (1901¿1982) was an early pioneer in the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) construct. In the 1960s, he conducted groundbreaking experimental research concerning the ways in which early-life experience with nutrition, microbiota, stress, and other environmental variables could influence later-life health outcomes. He also wrote extensively on potential health consequences of a progressive loss of contact with natural environments (now referred to as green or blue space), arguing that Paleolithic experiences have created needs, particularly in the mental realm, that might not be met in the context of rapid global urbanization. He posited that humans would certainly adapt to modern urban landscapes and high technology, but there might be a toll to be paid in the form of higher psychological distress (symptoms of anxiety and depression) and diminished quality of life. In particular, there might be an erosion of humanness, exemplified by declines in altruism/empathy. Here in the first of a two-part review, we examine contemporary research related to natural environments and question to what extent Dubos might have been correct in some of his 50-year-old assertions.
    Full-text · Article · Jan 2015
Show more