ArticlePDF Available

Genetic Variation in a Human Odorant Receptor Alters Odour Perception

Authors:
  • PlusOrtho Prothetik

Abstract and Figures

Human olfactory perception differs enormously between individuals, with large reported perceptual variations in the intensity and pleasantness of a given odour. For instance, androstenone (5alpha-androst-16-en-3-one), an odorous steroid derived from testosterone, is variously perceived by different individuals as offensive ("sweaty, urinous"), pleasant ("sweet, floral") or odourless. Similar variation in odour perception has been observed for several other odours. The mechanistic basis of variation in odour perception between individuals is unknown. We investigated whether genetic variation in human odorant receptor genes accounts in part for variation in odour perception between individuals. Here we show that a human odorant receptor, OR7D4, is selectively activated in vitro by androstenone and the related odorous steroid androstadienone (androsta-4,16-dien-3-one) and does not respond to a panel of 64 other odours and two solvents. A common variant of this receptor (OR7D4 WM) contains two non-synonymous single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), resulting in two amino acid substitutions (R88W, T133M; hence 'RT') that severely impair function in vitro. Human subjects with RT/WM or WM/WM genotypes as a group were less sensitive to androstenone and androstadienone and found both odours less unpleasant than the RT/RT group. Genotypic variation in OR7D4 accounts for a significant proportion of the valence (pleasantness or unpleasantness) and intensity variance in perception of these steroidal odours. Our results demonstrate the first link between the function of a human odorant receptor in vitro and odour perception.
OR7D4 variation affects androstenone and androstadienone quality perception.a, Differences in median odour valence ranking for the same odours and genotypes as in a. b, c, Change in odour valence ranking for the same odours and genotypes as in b, c. Significance in a–c was assessed with a Mann–Whitney U-test with Bonferroni correction. Before correction: asterisk, P < 0.00073; two asterisks, P < 0.00014; three asterisks, P < 1.47  10-5. After correction: asterisk, P < 0.05; two asterisks, P < 0.01; three asterisks, P < 0.001. The whisker plots show the median rank (normalized to the median rank of the RT/RT group), the first and third quartile and the upper and lower limits. d, e, Odour profiling of vanillin by RT/RT (n = 242), RT/WM (n = 96) and WM/WM (n = 10) subjects. Plotted are the differences in descriptor usage by genotype of the 23 descriptors used for vanillin in more than 10% of sessions (d) and the percentage of sessions (with 95% confidence intervals) in which the three descriptors that showed significant differences were used (e). f, g, Odour profiling of androstenone for the same genotypes as in d and e; 21 descriptors were used in more than 10% of all sessions, and two descriptors showed significant differences. Significance was assessed in d–g with a 2 test with Bonferroni correction. Before correction: asterisk, P < 0.0022 (vanillin) and P < 0.0024 (androstenone); two asterisks, P < 0.0004 (vanillin) and P < 0.0005 (androstenone). After correction: asterisk, P < 0.05; two asterisks, P < 0.01.
… 
Content may be subject to copyright.
LETTERS
Genetic variation in a human odorant receptor alters
odour perception
Andreas Keller
1
*, Hanyi Zhuang
2
*, Qiuyi Chi
2
, Leslie B. Vosshall
1
& Hiroaki Matsunami
2,3
Human olfactory perception differs enormously between indivi-
duals, with large reported perceptual variations in the intensity
and pleasantness of a given odour. For instance, androstenone
(5a-androst-16-en-3-one), an odorous steroid derived from tes-
tosterone, is variously perceived by different individuals as offens-
ive (‘‘sweaty, urinous’’), pleasant (‘‘sweet, floral’’) or odourless
1–3
.
Similar variation in odour perception has been observed for several
other odours
4–6
. The mechanistic basis of variation in odour per-
ception between individuals is unknown. We investigated whether
genetic variation in human odorant receptor genes accounts in part
for variation in odour perception between individuals
7,8
. Here we
show that a human odorant receptor, OR7D4, is selectively acti-
vated in vitro by androstenone and the related odorous steroid
androstadienone (androsta-4,16-dien-3-one) and does not respond
to a panel of 64 other odours and two solvents. A common variant of
this receptor (OR7D4 WM) contains two non-synonymous single
nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), resulting in two amino acid
substitutions (R88W, T133M; hence ‘RT’) that severely impair
function in vitro. Human subjects with RT/WM or WM/WM geno-
types as a group were less sensitive to androstenone and androsta-
dienone and found both odours less unpleasant than the RT/RT
group. Genotypic variation in OR7D4 accounts for a significant
proportion of the valence (pleasantness or unpleasantness) and
intensity variance in perception of these steroidal odours. Our
results demonstrate the first link between the function of a human
odorant receptor in vitro and odour perception.
We investigated the hypothesis that polymorphisms in odorant
receptors contribute to variability in human odour perception by
combining a cell-based assay technique to identify active ligands
for odorant receptors
9
with an olfactory psychophysical study of a
diverse population of human subjects
10
. A total of 66 odorants were
used to measure both odorant receptor responses in vitro and
psychophysical responses in human subjects, with a focus on odorous
steroids because the perception of these odours is exceptionally vari-
able
3,11
. We cloned a panel of 335 putative human odorant receptors,
representing more than 85% of the odorant receptors with full open
reading frames, and expressed them in Hana3A cells, an HEK293T-
derived cell line stably expressing accessory factors for odorant re-
ceptor expression
9,12
. We screened for androstenone-mediated
stimulation with a luciferase reporter
9
. Among the receptors tested,
that encoded by OR7D4 showed the strongest responses to andros-
tenone (Fig. 1a). Recent expression analysis shows that OR7D4 is
selectively expressed in human nasal epithelium
13
. Several other
receptors showed smaller responses to androstenone that may be
relevant in vivo (Supplementary Table 1), but these were not inves-
tigated further. The failure of a specific odorant receptor to respond
in this assay must be interpreted with caution because it may reflect a
failure of the odorant receptor to be functional in the assay rather
than a lack of sensitivity to the tested odour.
A search for polymorphisms in OR7D4 in SNP databases and
sequencing the coding region of OR7D4 in 391 subjects identified
13 non-synonymous SNPs in this receptor, with four occurring at
*These authors contributed equally to this work.
1
Laboratory of Neurogeneti cs and Behaviour, The Rockefeller University, 1230 York Avenue, New York, New York 10065, USA.
2
Department of Molecular Genetics and Microbiology,
and
3
Department of Neurobiology, Duke University Medical Centre, Research Drive, Durham, North Carolina 27710, USA.
Androstenone
O
H
O
Androstadienone
(–)-Menthol
(+)-Menthol
1-Butanol
2-Butanone
2-Decenal
2-Ethylfenchol
2-Methoxy-4-methylphenol
4-Methylvaleric acid
Ambrette
Androstenone
Anise
Banana
Bourgeonal
Butyl acetate
Butyric acid
Hexanoic acid
Cedarwood
Cineole
Cinnamon
Cis-3-hexen-1-ol
Citral
Citronella
Decyl aldehyde
Diacetyl
Diallyl sulphide
Diphenyl ether
Ethyl vanillin
Ethylene brassylate
Eugenol
Eugenol acetate
Eugenol methyl ether
Fenchone
Fir
Galaxolide
Androstadienone
Geranyl acetate
Guaiacol
Heptaldehyde
Heptyl acetate
Hexyl butyrate
Isobornyl acetate
Isobutyraldehyde
Isobutyric acid
Isoeugenol
Isovaleric acid
Jasmine
Lime
Linalool
Methanethiol
Methyl salicylate
Nonyl aldehyde
Nutmeg
Octyl acetate
Octyl aldehyde
Orange
Pentadecalactone
Phenyl acetaldehyde
Pyrazine
(R)-Carvone
(R)-Limonene
Sandalwood
Spearmint
Terpineol
Terpinyl acetate
Undecanal
Vanillin
Paraffin oil
Propylene glycol
None
Odorant
c
1.0
0.5
0
0.1
0
Normalized
response
OR families
1234 5678910111213515256
a
Normalized
response
1.0
0.5
0
OR7D4
30 µM androstenone
Residue
change
refseq
P79L
S84N
R88W
T133M
Allele
frequency
0.786
0.040
0.013
0.157
0.157
b
Figure 1
|
OR7D4 is selectively activated by androstenone and
androstadienone. a, Hana3A cell luciferase assays of 335 unique human
odorant receptors; the concentration of androstenone used was 30 mM.
Numbers and coloured bars indicate different OR families.
b, Allele
frequencies of common variants. refseq, reference sequence.
c, OR7D4 RT
(black columns) and WM (red columns) tested against 66 odours and 2
solvents (30 mM, or 1/30,000). Normalized responses are shown as means
and s.e.m. (n 5 4).
Vol 449
|
27 September 2007
|
doi:10.1038/nature06162
468
Nature
©2007
Publishing
Group
frequencies greater than 1% (Supplementary Table 2). Two non-
synonymous polymorphisms in complete linkage disequilibrium in
this population occurred at the highest frequency and led to two
amino-acid changes (R88W and T133M; Fig. 1b). We refer to the
most common allele of this receptor, or the reference sequence, as RT,
and to the other as WM.
We investigated the ligand specificity of RT and WM receptor
variants in vitro with a panel of 66 odours and 2 solvents. OR7D4
RT responds selectively to androstenone and the closely related
odorous steroid androstadienone but shows no response to any other
stimuli tested (Fig. 1c, top). OR7D4 WM shows no response to any
compound at the concentrations tested here (Fig. 1c, bottom). Dose–
response curves with RT and WM show that the paired SNPs in the
WM variant, which affect amino acids in extracellular loop 2 and
intracellular loop 2 (Fig. 2a), severely impair function (Fig. 2b). We
generated odorant receptors with each of the SNPs and found that
OR7D4 R88W and OR7D4 T133M retained an intermediate level of
function, suggesting that both residues are important for OR7D4
function (Fig. 2b).
We examined two other SNP variants found at frequencies
greater than 1%, which led to amino acid changes P79L and S84N,
respectively (Fig. 1b and 2a). P79L and S84N possess residues R88
and T133 and are referred to by the single variant residue. Analysis of
P79L function in vitro showed severely impaired function at all con-
centrations of either steroidal odour tested (Fig. 2c). In contrast,
OR7D4 S84N showed remarkable sensitivity to both odours in vitro,
exceeding the activity of the common RT variant at every concentra-
tion tested, with a concentration giving half-maximal response
(EC
50
) to androstadienone less than one-fifth of that of the RT vari-
ant (Fig. 2c).The functional differences between these two variants
are not due to the amino-terminal epitope tag (Supplementary Fig.
1). The other non-synonymous substitutions in OR7D4 result in
varying receptor functions (Fig. 2d).
OR7D4 is situated in a cluster of seven intact odorant receptor
genes, but we found that none of the polymorphisms of the six intact
odorant receptors in the OR7D4 gene cluster showed significant
linkage with the OR7D4 SNPs (Supplementary Fig. 2a and Supple-
mentary Table 3). We tested the responses of all the major variants of
odorant receptors in the OR7D4 cluster and found that none showed
responses to androstenone and androstadienone exceeding that of
the impaired WM variant (Supplementary Fig. 2b, c).
The chimpanzee OR7D4 orthologue differs from the human RT
reference sequence at five amino acid residues: at the S84N substi-
tution also found in humans and at four additional non-synonymous
substitutions not found in humans (V26I, G171V, G227R and
K232E). A dose–response analysis of the chimpanzee OR7D4 ortho-
logue in vitro showed robust responses to both steroidal odours,
exceeding the activity of the human S84N variant (Fig. 2c; compare
purple and green curves).
What accounts for the functional differences between OR7D4 var-
iants? We found no obvious difference in subcellular distribution or
expression level in permeabilized Hana3A cells expressing RT, WM,
P79L or S84N (Supplementary Fig. 3). Western blot analysis con-
firms that all are expressed at comparable levels (Fig. 2e), and RT,
WM and P79L have similar low levels of surface staining as measured
by flow cytometry of live cells stained to reveal the N-terminal epi-
tope (Fig. 2f). S84N showed considerably more surface expression
(Fig. 2f), suggesting that the increased function of this variant may
stem from enhanced stability at the cell surface or from enhanced
cell-surface trafficking.
We next asked whether variation in OR7D4 is correlated with
variation in the perception of androstenone and androstadienone
measured in human subjects. Psychophysical data on 391 subjects
performing three different tasks were collected: subjects rated the
perceived intensity and valence of 66 different odours at two con-
centrations (Supplementary Fig. 4); detection thresholds were mea-
sured to androstenone and androstadienone in a subset of subjects,
and to three control odours in all subjects
14,15
(Supplementary Fig. 5);
subjects profiled four odours with 146 semantic labels
10,16
(see
Supplementary Methods).
Psychophysical data on the subjects were subsequently divided
according to genotype and assessed for the influence of OR7D4 geno-
type on perceptual phenotype (Supplementary Table 2). Of the 66
odours and two solvents rated by RT/RT and RT/WM subjects, only
androstenone and androstadienone showed a significant effect of
genotype (Fig. 3a and Supplementary Fig. 6). The steroids were rated
as less intense by the RT/WM group (Fig. 3a); the proportion of
RT/WM subjects rating the high concentration of androstenone as
‘‘extremely weak’’ was fourfold that of RT/RT subjects (Supplemen-
tary Fig. 6). This phenotype was specific for these two compounds, as
the perception of all the other odours was not affected by OR7D4
genotype (Fig. 3a, b). The significant effect of OR7D4 genotype on
steroidal odour intensity perception was replicated in both males and
females (Supplementary Fig. 7) and in the largest racial category,
Caucasians (Supplementary Fig. 8). Although the WM allele strongly
affected androstenone intensity perception even in heterozygous
subjects, the group of WM/WM subjects showed an even stronger
effect on intensity ratings, rating both steroidal odours as less intense
EC
50
(µM)
RT
R88W
T133M
2.9
8.9
10
WM
>30
RT
S84N
Chimp
WM
EC
50
(µM)
0.10
0.52
3.3
>30
P79L
>30
1.0
EC
50
(µM)
Normalized response
[Androstenone] (M)
010
–7
10
–6
10
–5
[Androstadienone] (M)
[Androstenone] (M) [Androstadienone] (M)
010
–7
10
–6
10
–5
0
0.5
1.0
Normalized response
0
0.5
1.0
0
0.5
1.0
0
0.5
RT
R 88W
T133M
12
17
>30
WM
>30
0
10
–7
10
–6
10
–5
10
–8
0
10
–7
10
–6
10
–5
10
–9
10
–8
RT
S84N
Chimp
WM
EC
50
(µM)
0.91
4.2
12
>30
P79L
>30
c
Normalized
response
1.0
0.5
0
RT
WM
D52G
S75C
P79L
S84N
R88W
H131Q
T133M
M136I
C139Y
L162P
A279D
L292M
C139R
0 µM
3 µM
Androstenone
d
b
a
ef
100
10
1
10
2
Counts
80
60
40
20
0
10
0
PE fluorescence
WM
P79L
S84N
RT
No receptor
RT
WM
S84N
P79L
80
60
50
40
120
kDa
Anti-GFP
Anti-Rho
NH
2
COOH
R88W
S84N
P79L
T133M
30 µM
Figure 2
|
Functional characterization of OR7D4 polymorphisms. a, OR7D4
snake plot with amino acid changes indicated.
b, c, Dose–response curves and
EC
50
values of OR7D4 RT, WM, R88W and T133M (b) and of OR7D4 RT,
WM, P79L and S84N and chimpanzee OR7D4 (
c) to androstenone (left) and
androstadienone (right).
d, Activity of 13 SNP variants compared with that of
RT and WM variants. In
bd, normalized responses are shown as means
and s.e.m. (n 5 4–6).
e, Western blot analysis of whole-cell lysates from
HEK293T cells transfected with OR7D4 RT, WM, P79L or S84N and co-
transfected with green fluorescent protein (GFP).
f, Flow cytometry analysis
of cell-surface OR7D4 RT, WM, P79L and S84N expression as measured by
intensity of phycoerythrin (PE) signal among GFP-positive cells.
NATURE
|
Vol 449
|
27 September 2007 LETTERS
469
Nature
©2007
Publishing
Group
than the group of RT/RT subjects (Fig. 3b). The few subjects among
our population carrying the less frequent P79L and S84N variants
were examined, and RT/P79L subjects as a group showed a trend to
perceive both androstenone and androstadienone as less intense than
the group of RT/RT subjects (Fig. 3c). Conversely, RT/S84N subjects
as a group showed a trend to rate both odours as more intense
(Fig. 3c). However, these differences measured in subjects with the
rare alleles were not statistically significant.
Detection thresholds of 121 subjects were determined for both
steroidal odours (Fig. 3d, e). RT/WM subjects as a group had a higher
detection threshold—and were therefore less sensitive—to both
compounds than the group of RT/RT subjects (Fig. 3d) but had
normal thresholds to three control odours (data not shown). The
threshold for androstenone in the RT/WM group was 11-fold, and
that for androstadienone 16-fold, of that in the RT/ RT group. In
addition, 46% of RT/WM subjects but only 28% of RT/RT subjects
could not detect the highest concentration of androstenone we pro-
vided (P , 0.05; x
2
test). Detection thresholds were also obtained
from the few RT/P79L, WM/WM, WM/P79L and RT/S84N subjects
(Fig. 3e and Supplementary Fig. 9). The proportion of RT/P79L
subjects unable to detect the highest concentration of androstenone
and androstadienone provided was more than double that of RT/RT
subjects (P , 0.05; x
2
test) (Fig. 3e). It is unclear why the loss of one
functional allele has such a profound effect on the median detection
threshold. The same trend was found with the WM/WM and WM/
P79L groups (Supplementary Fig. 9), whereas the RT/S84N group
was more sensitive to both steroids, with lower detection thresholds
than the group of RT/RT controls. However, these differences were
not statistically significant (Fig. 3e).
We next examined whether variation in OR7D4 affects the percep-
tion of androstenone and androstadienone odour quality. The RT/
WM group rated both steroidal odours as less unpleasant than the
RT/RT group (Fig. 4a, b and Supplementary Fig. 10), such that the
proportion of RT/WM subjects rating the high concentration of
androstadienone as ‘‘extremely unpleasant’’ was less than half of that
of RT/RT subjects (Supplementary Fig. 10). None of the other 64
odours or the solvents showed a statistically significant difference
between the genotypes (Fig. 4a), and the effect was statistically sig-
nificant for both steroidal odours in both males and females (Sup-
plementary Fig. 7). The group of subjects carrying the impaired RT/
P79L variant showed a trend to rate both androstenone and andros-
tadienone as less unpleasant than RT/RT controls (Fig. 4c), whereas
the opposite was found in the RT/S84N group carrying a more sens-
itive variant of OR7D4 (Fig. 4c); however these differences were not
statistically significant.
We asked subjects to assess androstenone odour quality by profil-
ing this odour with a standard set of 146 semantic descriptors (see
Supplementary Methods)
10,16
. All descriptors used by more than 10%
of the subjects were analysed and descriptor usage in individuals with
different genotypes was compared. Of the 74 descriptors used for
androstenone, pentadecalactone, vanillin and the solvent propylene
glycol, only five differed significantly by genotype (see Supplemen-
tary Methods). OR7D4 RT/WM subjects were more likely to rate
vanillin as smelling ‘‘honey’’, ‘‘sweet’’ and ‘‘vanilla’’ (Fig. 4d, e),
Terpinyl acetate
Vanillin
Cinnamon
Galaxolide
Diacetyl
1-Butanol
Anise
Butyl acetate
Citronella
Hexyl butyrate
Isobornyl acetate
Orange
Undecanal
4-Methylvaleric acid
Geranyl acetate
2-Methoxy-4-methylphenol
Banana
Butyric acid
Cineole
Citral
Diphenyl ether
Eugenol methyl ether
Guaiacol
Heptyl acetate
Jasmine
Pentadecalactone
(R)-Carvone
2-Ethylfenchol
Eugenol
(R)-Limonene
(–)-Menthol
2-Decenal
Ethyl vanillin
Eugenol acetate
Isobutyraldehyde
Isobutyric acid
Isoeugenol
Linalool
Methyl salicylate
Octyl acetate
Paraffin oil
Propylene glycol
Spearmint
Terpineol
Cis-3-hexen-1-ol
Ethylene brassylate
Fenchone
Fir
(+)-Menthol
Bourgeonal
Heptaldehyde
Isovaleric acid
Phenyl acetaldehyde
Sandalwood
2-Butanone
Decyl aldehyde
Hexanoic acid
Diallyl sulphide
Nonyl aldehyde
Nutmeg
Octyl aldehyde
Pyrazine
Methanethiol
Lime
Ambrette
Cedarwood
Androstadienone
Androstenone
RT/P79L (n = 29)
RT/S84N (n = 7)
b
a
*
*
*
Odorant
More intense in RT/WM
Less intense in RT/WM
Difference in
odour intensity ran
k
*
*
*
c
d
RT/RT (n = 242)
RT/WM (n = 96)
WM/WM (n = 10)
***
*
***
*
Androstenone
Androstadienone
Propylene glycol
0
20
40
60
–20
RT/RT (n = 242)
*
Relative change in odour intensity rank
Androstenone
Androstadienone
Propylene glycol
Pentadecalactone
Vanillin
Pentadecalactone
Vanillin
0
20
40
60
–20
Less
intense
More
intense
Less
intense
More
intense
20
20
10
10
0
0
0
2
4
6
8
0
2
4
6
8
5
10
15
20
25
RT/RT
median
RT/WM
median
*
0
5
10
15
20
25
RT/WM
median
RT/RT
median
*
e
Androstenone detection
threshold
Androstadienone detection
threshold
RT/
S
84
N
me
dian
RT/
P
79L
me
dia
n
No. of subjects
R
T/P
79
L
m
ed
ia
n
RT/S
8
4
N
m
ed
ia
n
H
ig
her conce
ntr
ation
Higher
conce
ntration
H
igher concent
ration
Higher concen
tratio
n
Figure 3
|
OR7D4 variation affects androstenone and androstadienone
intensity perception. a
, Differences in median odour intensity ranking of 66
odours and 2 solvents between OR7D4 RT/WM and RT/RT groups. Data for
two different odour concentrations were pooled.
b, Change in odour
intensity ranking relative to solvent of four odours for the RT/RT, RT/WM
and WM/WM groups (
b) and for the RT/RT, RT/P79L and RT/S84N groups
(
c). The whisker plots show the median rank (normalized to the median rank
of the RT/RT group), the first and third quartile and the upper and lower
limits. Significance was assessed in
ac with a Mann–Whitney U-test with a
Bonferroni correction. Before correction: asterisk, P , 0.00073; two
asterisks, P , 0.00014; three asterisks, P , 1.47 3 10
25
. After correction:
asterisk, P , 0.05; two asterisks, P , 0.01; three asterisks, P , 0.001.
d, e, Detection thresholds measured in RT/RT (n 5 47) and RT/WM subjects
(n 5 49) (
d) and in RT/P79L (n 5 12) and RT/S84N (n 5 3) subjects
(
e) plotted as the number of subjects detecting the odour at a given binary
dilution (x-axis concentrations are binned from left to right: the first bar
represents binary dilution 6, the subsequent 15 bars represent binary
dilutions 7–21, and the last bar represents dilutions 22–27). Significance was
assessed with a Mann–Whitney U-test (asterisk, P , 0.05).
LETTERS NATURE
|
Vol 449
|
27 September 2007
470
Nature
©2007
Publishing
Group
42% less likely to consider androstenone ‘‘sickening’’ and 129% more
likely to rate it as smelling like ‘‘vanilla’’ than RT/RT subjects (Fig. 4f,
g). There was no effect of OR7D4 genotype on odour profiling of
pentadecalactone or propylene glycol (data not shown).
Last, we performed a non-parametric regression analysis
17
to
estimate the fraction of the phenotypic variance in steroid odour
perception attributable to OR7D4. We found that OR7D4 genotype
in our population explained 19% and 39% of the variance in the
valence and intensity ratings of the steroid odours, respectively
(P , 0.0001; see Supplementary Methods).
We identify OR7D4 as a significant heritable factor influencing
androstenone and androstadienone perception, thus providing the
first reported link between genetic polymorphisms in an odorant
receptor gene and altered perception of the ligands that activate this
receptor. As predicted by the theory of combinatorial coding
18
,we
find that polymorphisms in the OR7D4 protein-coding sequence
alone do not fully account for specific anosmia to androstenone
and androstadienone. We think it likely that additional human odor-
ant receptors sensitive to androstenone and androstadienone remain
to be discovered.
Previous work indicated that sensitivity to androstenone
19,20
, and
to several other odours
21
, is modulated by non-genetic effects such as
central processing
22
or peripheral sensitization
23
that might obscure
any underlying genetic influences on androstenone perception.
About half of the subjects initially unable to detect androstenone
were able to smell this compound after being exposed to androste-
none daily for six weeks
19
. Although it has been assumed that olfact-
ory induction is a phenomenon that occurs only in individuals with
specific anosmia to androstenone, more recent studies have found
that induction of enhanced olfactory sensitivity seems to be a general
phenomenon affecting several odorants
21
. The role of OR7D4 in this
sensitization can now be tested in subjects chronically exposed to
androstenone.
In this study we investigated only the olfactory percept reported
when odorous steroids were sniffed, but olfactory exposure to
androstenone and androstadienone has also been shown to induce
several physiological responses in both men and women
24,25
. The
identification of an odorant receptor gene that is strongly correlated
with the perception of these odours will permit future analysis of
olfactory-induced autonomic responses in humans.
METHODS SUMMARY
Heterologous expression of human odorant receptors. A total of 423 human
odorant receptors, including 335 predicted functional receptors, were cloned.
The chimpanzee OR7D4 orthologue was cloned from chimpanzee genomic
DNA (Coriell Cell Repositories). Odorant receptors containing the first 20
amino acids of human rhodopsin
26
in pCI (Promega) were expressed in the
Hana3A cell line together with a short form of mRTP1 called RTP1S (M37 to
*
*
*
*
*
*
RT/P79L (n = 29)
RT/S84N (n = 7)
c
b
a
Odorant
Less pleasant in RT/WM
More pleasant in RT/WM
0
10
20
10
20
Androstadienone
Androstenone
(–)-Menthol
Banana
Fir
Jasmine
Methyl salicylate
Eugenol methyl ether
Cedarwood
Eugenol
Isoeugenol
Linalool
(+)-Menthol
Anise
Cis-3-hexen-1-ol
Pyrazine
Fenchone
Isobutyraldehyde
Isovaleric acid
Pentadecalactone
Terpineol
Vanillin
Citral
1-Butanol
2-Methoxy-4-methylphenol
Butyric acid
Cinnamon
Diacetyl
Diphenyl ether
Ethyl vanillin
Galaxolide
Isobornyl acetate
Isobutyric acid
Octyl aldehyde
Orange
Propylene glycol
Spearmint
2-Ethylfenchol
2-Butanone
4-Methylvaleric acid
Ethylene brassylate
Lime
(R)-Carvone
Ambrette
Cineole
Decyl aldehyde
Diallyl sulphide
Eugenol acetate
Guaiacol
Hexyl butyrate
Paraffin oil
(R)-Limonene
Sandalwood
Geranyl acetate
2-Decenal
Heptyl acetate
Methanethiol
Nutmeg
Phenyl acetaldehyde
Terpinyl acetate
Butyl acetate
Hexanoic acid
Citronella
Nonyl aldehyde
Octyl acetate
Bourgeonal
Heptaldehyde
Undecanal
Difference in
odour valence rank
e
f
*
*
Used less by RT/WM
Descriptors of androstenone odour
0
10
10
*
Used more by RT/WM
Sickening
Heavy
Putrid foul
Bitter
Rancid
Urine
Ammonia
Sweaty
Cleaning fluid
Dirty linen
Sharp pungent
Chemical
Musky
Musty mouldy
Aromatic
Stale
Light
Fragrant
Rotten fruit
Sweet
Vanilla
d
Used more by RT/WM
Used less by RT/WM
Descriptors of vanillin odour
0
10
10
Difference in descriptor usage (%)
*
*
*
Light
Cologne
Fruity other
Buttery fresh
Cool
Aromatic
Fragrant
Cinnamon
Floral
Perfumery
Incense
Caramel
Coconut
Heavy
Musky
Chocolate
Maple syrup
Almond
Molasses
Honey
Sweet
Warm
Vanilla
RT/RT (n = 242)
RT/WM (n = 96)
WM/WM (n = 10)
0
20
40
60
***
Androstenone
***
Androstadienone
Propylene glycol
Pentadecalactone
Vanillin
–20
More
unpleasant
Less
unpleasant
More
unpleasant
Less
unpleasant
RT/RT (n = 242)
0
20
40
60
Relative change in odour valence rank
Androstenone
Androstadienone
Propylene glycol
Pentadecalactone
Vanillin
–20
‘Vanilla’‘Sweet’
Odour profiled:
vanillin
0
50
100
‘Honey’
*
*
*
RT/RT (n = 242)
RT/WM (n = 96)
WM/WM (n = 10)
g
Descriptor usage (%)
‘Sickening’
‘Vanilla’
Odour profiled:
androstenone
0
50
100
**
*
RT/RT (n = 242)
RT/WM (n = 96)
WM/WM (n = 10)
Figure 4
|
OR7D4 variation affects androstenone and androstadienone
quality perception. a, Differences in median odour valence ranking for the
same odours and genotypes as in Fig. 3a.
b, c, Change in odour valence
ranking for the same odours and genotypes as in Fig. 3b, c. Significance in
ac was assessed with a Mann–Whitney U-test with Bonferroni correction.
Before correction: asterisk, P , 0.00073; two asterisks, P , 0.00014; three
asterisks, P , 1.47 3 10
25
. After correction: asterisk, P , 0.05; two asterisks,
P , 0.01; three asterisks, P , 0.001. The whisker plots show the median rank
(normalized to the median rank of the RT/RT group), the first and third
quartile and the upper and lower limits.
d, e, Odour profiling of vanillin by
RT/RT (n 5 242), RT/WM (n 5 96) and WM/WM (n 5 10) subjects. Plotted
are the differences in descriptor usage by genotype of the 23 descriptors used
for vanillin in more than 10% of sessions (
d) and the percentage of sessions
(with 95% confidence intervals) in which the three descriptors that showed
significant differences were used (
e). f, g, Odour profiling of androstenone
for the same genotypes as in
d and e; 21 descriptors were used in more than
10% of all sessions, and two descriptors showed significant differences.
Significance was assessed in
dg with a x
2
test with Bonferroni correction.
Before correction: asterisk, P , 0.0022 (vanillin) and P , 0.0024
(androstenone); two asterisks, P , 0.0004 (vanillin) and P , 0.0005
(androstenone). After correction: asterisk, P , 0.05; two asterisks, P , 0.01.
NATURE
|
Vol 449
|
27 September 2007 LETTERS
471
Nature
©2007
Publishing
Group
the carboxy-terminal end), which enhances functional expression of the odorant
receptors
12
. For immunocytochemistry, cells were fixed, permeabilized and
incubated with monoclonal anti-rhodopsin antibody (4D2; ref. 27), followed
by Cy3-conjugated donkey anti-mouse IgG (Jackson Immunologicals). For
fluorescence-activated cell sorting analysis, this antibody was conjugated with
phycoerythrin.
Human odorant receptor genotyping and sequencing. Venous blood was col-
lected from all subjects, and genomic DNA was prepared with the Qiagen
PAXgene blood DNA kit. Polymorphisms in OR7D4 were assayed by sequencing
and allele-specific polymerase chain reaction. Polymorphisms in the other odor-
ant receptors in the same odorant receptor gene cluster as OR7D4 were assayed
by sequencing only.
Human olfactory psychophysics. All procedures involving human subjects were
approved by the Rockefeller University Institutional Review Board. All subjects
completed two replicates of the test separated by at least 4 days. Odours were
presented in bar-coded amber vials to ensure that subjects were blind to the
identity of all odours
28
. The intensity and valence of 66 odours at two concen-
trations (‘high’ and ‘low’) and two solvents was rated on a seven-point scale.
Thresholds were calculated by using the single-staircase method with seven
reversals
14,15
. Threshold tests included both steroids as binary dilutions from
1:64 (binary dilution 6) to 1:134,217,728 (binary dilution 27). Odour profiling
used a previously established method
16
.
Full Methods and any associated references are available in the online version of
the paper at www.nature.com/nature.
Received 20 June; accepted 8 August 2007.
Published online 16 September 2007.
1. Wysocki, C. J. & Beauchamp, G. K. Ability to smell androstenone is genetically
determined. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 81, 4899
4902 (1984).
2. Gower, D. B., Nixon, A. & Mallet, A. I. in Perfumery (eds Van Toller, S. & Dodd,
G. H.) 47
75 (Chapman & Hall, London, 1998).
3. Bremner, E. A., Mainland, J. D., Khan, R. M. & Sobel, N. The prevalence of
androstenone anosmia. Chem. Senses 28, 423
432 (2003).
4. Guillot, M. Physiologie des sensations—anosmies partielles et odeurs
fondamentales. C.R. Hebd. Acad. Sci. 226, 1307
1309 (1948).
5. Gilbert, A. N. & Kemp, S. E. Odor perception phenotypes: multiple, specific
hyperosmias to musks. Chem. Senses 21, 411
416 (1996).
6. Whissell-Buechy, D. & Amoore, J. E. Odour-blindness to musk: simple recessive
inheritance. Nature 242, 271
273 (1973).
7. Menashe, I., Man, O., Lancet, D. & Gilad, Y. Different noses for different people.
Nature Genet. 34, 143
144 (2003).
8. Gilad, Y. & Lancet, D. Population differences in the human functional olfactory
repertoire. Mol. Biol. Evol. 20, 307
314 (2003).
9. Saito, H., Kubota, M., Roberts, R. W., Chi, Q. & Matsunami, H. RTP family
members induce functional expression of mammalian odorant receptors. Cell 119,
679
691 (2004).
10. Keller, A. & Vosshall, L. B. Human olfactory psychophysics. Curr. Biol. 14,
R875
R878 (2004).
11. Stevens, D. A. & O’Connell, R. J. Enhanced sensitivity to androstenone following
regular exposure to pemenone. Chem. Senses 20, 413
419 (1995).
12. Zhuang, H. & Matsunami, H. Synergism of accessory factors in functional
expression of mammalian odorant receptors. J. Biol. Chem. 282, 15284
15293
(2007).
13. Zhang, X. et al. Characterizing the expression of the human olfactory receptor
gene family using a novel DNA microarray. Genome Biol. 8, R86 (2007).
14. Doty, R. L., McKeown, D. A., Lee, W. W. & Shaman, P. A study of the test
retest
reliability of ten olfactory tests. Chem. Senses 20, 645
656 (1995).
15. Doty, R. L. & Laing, D. G. in Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation (ed. Doty, R. L.)
203
228 (Marcel Dekker, New York, 2003).
16. Dravnieks, A. Odor quality: semantically generated multidimensional profiles are
stable. Science 218, 799
801 (1982).
17. Cleveland, W. S. Robust locally weighted regression and smoothing scatterplots.
J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 74, 829
836 (1979).
18. Malnic, B., Hirono, J., Sato, T. & Buck, L. B. Combinatorial receptor codes for odors.
Cell 96, 713
723 (1999).
19. Wysocki, C. J., Dorries, K. M. & Beauchamp, G. K. Ability to perceive androstenone
can be acquired by ostensibly anosmic people. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 86,
7976
7978 (1989).
20. Dorries, K. M., Schmidt, H. J., Beauchamp, G. K. & Wysocki, C. J. Changes in
sensitivity to the odor of androstenone during adolescence. Dev. Psychobiol. 22,
423
435 (1989).
21. Dalton, P., Doolittle, N. & Breslin, P. A. Gender-specific induction of enhanced
sensitivity to odors. Nature Neurosci. 5, 199
200 (2002).
22. Mainland, J. D. et al. One nostril knows what the other learns. Nature 419, 802
(2002).
23. Wang, L., Chen, L. & Jacob, T. Evidence for peripheral plasticity in human odour
response. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 554, 236
244 (2004).
24. Jacob, S., Kinnunen, L. H., Metz, J., Cooper, M. & McClintock, M. K. Sustained
human chemosignal unconsciously alters brain function. Neuroreport 12,
2391
2394 (2001).
25. Wyart, C. et al. Smelling a single component of male sweat alters levels of cortisol
in women. J. Neurosci. 27, 1261
1265 (2007).
26. Krautwurst, D., Yau, K. W. & Reed, R. R. Identification of ligands for olfactory
receptors by functional expression of a receptor library. Cell 95, 917
926 (1998).
27. Laird, D. W. & Molday, R. S. Evidence against the role of rhodopsin in rod
outer segment binding to RPE cells. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 29, 419
428
(1988).
28. Keller, A. & Vosshall, L. B. A psychophysical test of the vibration theory of
olfaction. Nature Neurosci. 7, 337
338 (2004).
Supplementary Information is linked to the online version of the paper at
www.nature.com/nature.
Acknowledgements L.B.V. and A.K. thank E. Gotschlich, B. Coller, A. N. Gilbert,
I. Gomez, P. Hempstead and C. Vancil; H.M. and H.Z. thank H. Amrein, M. Cook,
M. Kubota, D. Marchuk, R. Molday, D. Tracey and R. Valdivia. This research was
supported in part by an NIH Clinical and Translational Science Award to
Rockefeller University and by grants to L.B.V. from the Irma T. Hirschl Trust, to
H.M. from the NIH, to H.Z. from an NIH National Research Servic e Award, and to
A.K. from a Marco S. Stoffel Fellowship.
Author Contributions H.Z. and H.M. screened for androstenone receptors,
identified polymorphisms, performed functional expression of receptor variants,
and genotyped the human subjects with assistance from Q.C. A.K. and L.B.V.
devised the human olfactory psychophysics study, for which A.K. supervised data
collection and analysis.
Author Information The sequences of the human OR7D4 variants are deposited in
Genbank under accession numbers EU049291
EU049294. Reprints and
permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints. The authors
declare competing financial interests: details accompany the paper on
www.nature.com/nature. Correspondence and requests for materials should be
addressed to H.M. (hiroaki.matsunami@duke.edu) and L.B.V.
(leslie@mail.rockefeller.edu).
LETTERS NATURE
|
Vol 449
|
27 September 2007
472
Nature
©2007
Publishing
Group
METHODS
Heterologous expression of human odorant receptors. For untagged odorant
receptor experiments, OR7D4 RT and S84N variants without the Rho tag were
cloned into pCI. Luciferase assays were performed as described
9
. Only a single
variant of each receptor was used in the functional screen, and it is known that
many human odorant receptor genes are highly polymorphic
7
. Because of this
and because we do not know whether each odorant receptor is capable of func-
tional expression in the cell line used, we cannot exclude the possibility that
additional human odorant receptors respond to androstenone as strongly as
OR7D4. For immunocytochemistry, cells were fixed, permeabilized and incu-
bated with monoclonal anti-rhodopsin antibody, 4D2 (ref. 27), followed by Cy3-
conjugated donkey anti-mouse IgG (Jackson Immunologicals). Western blot
analysis was performed in accordance with the Mini-Protean 2 Cell (Bio-Rad)
protocol. Enhanced chemiluminescence (ECL; Amersham) was used for detect-
ing proteins on membranes. After the initial exposure, the membrane was incu-
bated with stripping buffer (25 mM glycine-HCl pH 2, 1% SDS, 25 mM glycine,
0.036 M HCl, 1% SDS) and incubated with rabbit anti-GFP (Invitrogen). See
Supplementary Methods for detailed information.
Odours. All odours were supplied by Sigma-Aldrich, with these exceptions:
androstadienone (gift from Human Pheromone Sciences, Inc.), banana (Bell
Flavors and Fragrances), bourgeonal (Biomol), galaxolide (gift from Inter-
national Flavors and Fragrances) and (R)-carvone (Research Chemical Ltd).
The same batch and lot of each odour was used for both cell-based analysis
and human olfactory psychophysics. Detailed information on odours, odour
concentrations and perceived odour quality is provided in Supplementary
Tables 4–6.
Genotyping and sequencing of human odorant receptors. For sequencing,
human genomic DNAs were amplified, purified and sequenced with a 3100 or
3730 Genetic Analyser (ABI Biosystems) or by GeneWiz. Detailed methods are
given in Supplementary Methods.
Human olfactory psychophysics. All human subjects gave informed consent to
participate in this study and were tested in a well-ventilated room of the
Rockefeller University Hospital Outpatient Unit. Normal human subjects were
pre-screened to exclude pregnant women and those with medical conditions
causing general impairment of the sense of smell. Of the 412 subjects who
completed the study, 21 were excluded because of general anosmia (see
Supplementary Methods). The remaining 391 subjects (210 female, 181 male;
median age 34 years, age range 19–75 years) were included in the evaluation.
Detailed methods are given in Supplementary Information. Our smell tests were
purposely conducted under conditions that would not be expected to induce
odour sensitivity in our subjects. A given subject sniffed androstenone in only
two sessions, rather than the dozens of sessions spread over six to eight weeks
required for sensitization in previous studies
19
. A comparative analysis of
androstenone responses in the first and second visits does not suggest that sub-
jects became more sensitive through these brief, non-chronic exposures (data
not shown).
doi:10.1038/nature06162
Nature
©2007
Publishing
Group
... Previous studies have shown that non-synonymous SNPs within the OR genes may have a significant impact on olfactory perception [36,37]. We thus searched the variation database and found a total of 42 non-synonymous SNPs in the CDS region of pig OR7D4 (Table S11). ...
... We thus searched the variation database and found a total of 42 non-synonymous SNPs in the CDS region of pig OR7D4 (Table S11). Given P79 and A202 are part of the ligand-binding cavity of human OR7D4, which affects the response of androstenone [38], M105 and G108 are members of the 22 amino acid residues that make up the ligand-binding cavity of all ORs, as proposed by Man et al. [39] (2004), and M133 was proven to strongly affect the androstenone perception in humans [36], so we further evaluated the effect of these mutation on responding to androstenone. As presented in Figure 5, only M133V significantly reduced the response of OR7D4 to androstenone, while no significant difference was found after the mutation of the rest of the loci. ...
Article
Full-text available
Olfactory receptors (ORs) are members of the transmembrane G protein-coupled receptor superfamily, playing a crucial role in odor recognition, which further mediates crucial biological processes in mammals. In sows, androstenone can trigger sexual behaviors through olfaction, but the underlying mechanism remains to be explored. To efficiently and accurately screen pig olfactory receptors responding to androstenone and the key structure determinant, we adapted the high-throughput RNA-seq strategy to screen the altered genes upon androstenone treatment in the olfactory epithelium of pigs, yielding 1397 downregulated genes. Of which, 15 OR genes and 49 OR-like genes were candidate androstenone-responsive genes, and 5 ORs (OR2D2, OR8D1, OR8D2, OR10Z1 and OR7D4) were proven as responsible for androstenone-mediated olfaction in vitro. Among the five ORs, pig OR7D4 has the highest level of androstenone response. To further find the structural determinant, we performed ligand-binding cavity analysis on pig OR7D4 with androstenone, predicted seven potential structural sites and further confirmed that F178 and T203 are the key sites for recognizing androstenone. Nevertheless, the natural non-synonymous mutation M133V (rs696400829) of pig OR7D4 was proven to significantly impair the respondence to androstenone. This is the first time the ORs responding to androstenone in pigs and the key structural determinant of pig OR7D4 were identified, which highlights the significance of investigating the role of OR7D4 in pig reproduction performance in the future.
... These steroids have been described as possessing pheromone-like activities, although their classification as pheromones remains contested 75 . Functional polymorphisms in human OR7D4, caused by amino acid substitutions, result in reduced sensitivity to these steroids 76 . The frequency of these polymorphisms varies worldwide, showing substantial deviations from neutrality 77 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Analysis of genome-scale evolution has been difficult in large, endangered animals because opportunities to collect high-quality genetic samples are limited. There is a need for novel field-friendly, cost-effective genetic techniques. This study conducted an exome-wide analysis of a total of 42 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) across six African regions, providing insights into population discrimination techniques. Wild chimpanzee DNA was extracted noninvasively from collected fecal samples using the lysis-buffer storage method. To target genome-scale regions of host DNA, exome-capture sequencing was performed using cost-effective baits originally designed for humans (closely related to chimpanzees). Multivariate analysis effectively discriminated differences in local populations, aiding in the identification of samples’ geographical origins. Exome-wide heterozygosity was negatively correlated significantly with genome-wide nonsynonymous–synonymous substitution ratios, suggesting that mutation loads exist at the local population level. Exome sequences revealed functional diversity and protein-coding gene divergence. Segregating pseudogenes were comprehensively annotated, with many being population-specific and others shared among populations. Focusing on multicopy chemosensory receptor genes, the segregating pseudogenes OR7D4 (an olfactory receptor) and TAS2R42 (a bitter taste receptor) were shared among western and eastern chimpanzees. Overall, our analytical framework offers ecological insights into chimpanzees and may be applicable to other organisms.
... People have genetic differences in their ability to smell. These genetic differences can cause people to perceive certain odors more or less strongly [6]. ...
Article
Odor, one of our senses, is an important sense of life. In some cases, smell can be vitally important. A smell can also help in the correct recognition of a substance. Odor is composed of gas molecules. In this study, 8 simple semiconductor gas sensors were used to detect the odor of various substances. A gas sensor fusion setup was created with gas sensors and a candidate data set was created by collecting sensor data with the help of Arduino Mega embedded system. With the help of this data set, the odor of 7 different cleaning agents and similar substances was detected with the help of Decision Trees (DT). The results obtained from the decision tree (DT) classifier using the data set obtained from the fusion setup (95.44%) are close to the state-of-the-art results. As a result of the study, the feasibility of an embedded odor detection device has been demonstrated.
... Each receptor binds to a set of chemicals, enabling one to recognize specific odorants or tastants. Genetic differences in many of these receptors are known to play a role in how we perceive tastes and smells [11,12,13,14]. ...
Preprint
The leaves of the Coriandrum sativum plant, known as cilantro or coriander, are widely used in many cuisines around the world. However, far from being a benign culinary herb, cilantro can be polarizing---many people love it while others claim that it tastes or smells foul, often like soap or dirt. This soapy or pungent aroma is largely attributed to several aldehydes present in cilantro. Cilantro preference is suspected to have a genetic component, yet to date nothing is known about specific mechanisms. Here we present the results of a genome-wide association study among 14,604 participants of European ancestry who reported whether cilantro tasted soapy, with replication in a distinct set of 11,851 participants who declared whether they liked cilantro. We find a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) significantly associated with soapy-taste detection that is confirmed in the cilantro preference group. This SNP, rs72921001, (p=6.4e-9, odds ratio 0.81 per A allele) lies within a cluster of olfactory receptor genes on chromosome 11. Among these olfactory receptor genes is OR6A2, which has a high binding specificity for several of the aldehydes that give cilantro its characteristic odor. We also estimate the heritability of cilantro soapy-taste detection in our cohort, showing that the heritability tagged by common SNPs is low, about 0.087. These results confirm that there is a genetic component to cilantro taste perception and suggest that cilantro dislike may stem from genetic variants in olfactory receptors. We propose that OR6A2 may be the olfactory receptor that contributes to the detection of a soapy smell from cilantro in European populations.
... As it is commonly accepted, on the basis of the scores obtained during olfactory tests, individuals can be classified as normosmic (normal olfactory function), hyposmic (reduced olfactory function) or functionally anosmic (general or specific inability to perceive odors) [3,19,[42][43][44][45]. The reasons of this variability are multiple and can be of an environmental/behavioral nature (e.g., habitual smokers, sedentary lifestyle, polluted habitats), genetic (expression and functionality of OBPs, ORs, Kv1.3 channels, etc.) and physiological (age and sex) [40,42,[46][47][48][49][50][51][52][53][54][55][56][57][58][59][60][61][62][63]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
One of the still debated topics regarding the olfactory function concerns the presence or absence of sex-related differences of individuals. In this study, we checked for a relationship between the olfactory function of females and males and their ability to perceive single molecules, and how this can influence the intensity with which the complex odor formed by a set of single molecules is perceived. First, females and males were classified as normosmic or hyposmic based on the TDI olfactory score obtained using the Sniffin' Sticks test. Subsequently, the headspace of roasted coffee beans as a complex olfactory stimulus was broken down into single molecules by means of a chromatographic column; these were simultaneously conveyed to a mass spectrometer (for their subsequent classification) and to the human nose which acts as a chemical sensor by means of an olfactometer port. The results obtained with this gas chromatography-olfactometry approach show both qualitative and quantitative differences between females and males, with females performing better than males. In addition, the odor intensity reported by females when sniffing pen #10 containing coffee aroma is significantly higher than that reported by males. In conclusion, these data highlight that the human ability to perceive both single compounds and complex odors is strongly conditioned, not only by olfactory function of individuals, but also by their sex.
... As it is commonly accepted, on the basis of the scores obtained during olfactory tests, individuals can be classified as normosmic (normal olfactory function), hyposmic (reduced olfactory function) or functionally anosmic (general or specific inability to perceive odors) [3,19,[42][43][44][45]. The reasons for this variability are multiple and can be of an environmental/behavioral nature (e.g., habitual smokers, sedentary lifestyle, polluted habitats), genetic (expression and functionality of OBPs, ORs, Kv1.3 channels, etc.) and physiological (age and sex) [40,42,[46][47][48][49][50][51][52][53][54][55][56][57][58][59][60][61][62][63]. ...
Article
Full-text available
One of the still-debated topics regarding the olfactory function concerns the presence or absence of sex-related differences in individuals. In this study, we checked for a relationship between the olfactory function of females and males and their ability to perceive single molecules, and researched how this can influence the intensity with which the complex odor formed by a set of single molecules is perceived. First, females and males were classified as normosmic or hyposmic based on the TDI olfactory score obtained using the Sniffin’ Sticks test. Subsequently, the headspace of roasted coffee beans, as a complex olfactory stimulus, was broken down into single molecules by means of a chromatographic column; these were simultaneously conveyed to a mass spectrometer (for their subsequent classification) and to the human nose, which acts as a chemical sensor by means of an olfactometer port. The results obtained with this gas chromatography–olfactometry approach show both qualitative and quantitative differences between females and males, with females performing better than males. In addition, the odor intensity reported by females when sniffing pen #10, containing coffee aroma, is significantly higher than that reported by males. In conclusion, these data highlight that the human ability to perceive both single compounds and complex odors is strongly conditioned, not only by the olfactory function of individuals, but also by their sex.
Article
Olfactory perception begins when odorous substances interact with specialized receptors located on the surface of dedicated sensory neurons. The recognition of smells depends on a complex mechanism involving a combination of interactions between an odorant and a set of odorant receptors (ORs), where molecules are recognized according to a combinatorial activation code of ORs. Although these interactions have been studied for decades, the rules governing this ligand recognition remain poorly understood, and the complete combinatorial code is only known for a handful of odorants. We have carefully analyzed experimental results regarding the interactions between ORs and molecules to provide a status report on the deorphanization of ORs, i.e., the identification of the first agonist for a given sequence. This meticulous analysis highlights the influence of experimental methodology (cell line or readout) on molecule-receptor association results and shows that 83% of the results are conserved regardless of experimental conditions. The distribution of another key parameter, EC50, indicates that most OR ligands activities are in the micromolar range and that impurities could lead to erroneous conclusions. Focusing on the human ORs, our study shows that 88% of the documented sequences still need to be deorphanized. Finally, we also estimate the size of the ORs’ recognition range, or broadness, as the number of odorants activating a given OR. By analogously estimating molecular broadness and combining the two estimates we propose a basic framework that can serve as a comparison point for future machine learning algorithms predicting OR-molecule activity.
Article
Full-text available
Some adult humans cannot detect the odor of androstenone (5 alpha-androst-16-en-3-one), a volatile steroid. To test for the presence of genetic variance associated with this trait, adult twins were tested for their ability to smell androstenone and another odorant, pyridine, that is readily perceived by most adults. Ascending concentration, two-sample (odor versus blank) forced choice tests were used to assess sensitivity to these odorants. Intraclass correlations for identical and fraternal twin detection thresholds to pyridine were small and not significantly different. However, intraclass correlations for thresholds to androstenone were significantly different, with the correlation for identical twins being greater than that for the fraternal twins. These data indicate a genetic component of variation in sensitivity to this odor. Investigations that use genetic variation could offer a new tool for studies of olfactory transduction mechanisms.
Article
THREE types of odour-blindness or specific anosmia have been studied, but genetical evidence so far obtained is tentative or inconsistent1. The rare anosmia to the n-butylmercaptan of skunk2, and more commonly the scent of freesia flowers3, may be inherited as autosomal recessive traits. Anosmia to hydrogen cyanide4-6 is complex and its inheritance has not been confirmed7.
Article
The visual information on a scatterplot can be greatly enhanced, with little additional cost, by computing and plotting smoothed points. Robust locally weighted regression is a method for smoothing a scatterplot, (x i , y i ), i = 1, …, n, in which the fitted value at z k is the value of a polynomial fit to the data using weighted least squares, where the weight for (x i , y i ) is large if x i is close to x k and small if it is not. A robust fitting procedure is used that guards against deviant points distorting the smoothed points. Visual, computational, and statistical issues of robust locally weighted regression are discussed. Several examples, including data on lead intoxication, are used to illustrate the methodology.
Article
While it has been reported that most, if not all, very young children are able to detect the odor of 5 alpha-androst-16-en-3-one (androstenone), approximately 40-50% of human adults cannot detect its odor. The present study focused on changes in sensitivity to androstenone during adolescence, which may account for this discrepancy. Sensitivity to androstenone was determined in 247 subjects aged 6 to 50. There was a significant increase in the number of males anosmic to androstenone between 9-14 and 15-20 years of age, and a significant increase in threshold with age among males able to detect the odor. We infer that a smaller percentage of females than males becomes anosmic to the odor of androstenone during development, and those able to detect it apparently show a decrease in threshold with age. No age-related changes were observed in tests of pyridine or d,l-beta-phenylethylmethylethylcarbinol (PEMEC).
Article
Nearly half the adult human population does not perceive an odor when sniffing androstenone (5 alpha-androst-16-en-3-one), a volatile steroid found in human perspiration, boar saliva, some pork products (e.g., bacon), truffles, and celery. This variation in ability to perceive androstenone has a significant heritable component, suggesting that androstenone insensitivity is in part determined genetically. We now report that the ability to perceive androstenone was induced in 10 of 20 initially insensitive subjects who were systematically exposed to androstenone. Since olfactory neurons of the olfactory epithelium undergo periodic replacement from differentiating basal cells, and assuming the induction of sensitivity to be peripheral, we propose that a portion of the apparently anosmic human population does in fact possess olfactory neurons with specific receptors for androstenone. Such neurons may undergo clonal expansion, or selection of lineages with more receptors or receptors of higher affinity, in response to androstenone stimulation, much in the manner of lymphocytes responding to antigenic stimulation, thus raising odor stimulation to the level of conscious perception. As a guide to further study of the genetics and mechanism of variation of androstenone perception, we provisionally envisage three categories of human subjects, the truly anosmic, the inducible, and those subjects who either are constitutionally sensitive or have already experienced incidental induction.
Article
The possible role of rhodopsin in the binding and phagocytosis of rod outer segments (ROS) by cultured bovine retinal pigment epithelial (RPE) cells was studied using both quantitative phagocytosis assays and electron microscopy. In inhibition studies an immunoaffinity purified 2-39 N-terminal rhodopsin glycopeptide, a synthetic 1-16 peptide analogue of rhodopsin and purified, unsealed ROS disc membranes were found to be ineffective in inhibiting the binding of 125I-labeled ROS to RPE cells. A two-fold excess of unlabeled intact ROS, however, inhibited 125I-labeled ROS binding to RPE cells by over 40%. In another series of experiments, rhodopsin on the surface of fixed ROS was densely labeled with gold-dextran particles conjugated to an N-terminal-specific (rho 4D2) rhodopsin monoclonal antibody or its F(ab')2 fragment in an effort to block binding and phagocytosis by RPE cells. As visualized by both transmission and scanning electron microscopy using secondary and backscatter electron imaging, these antibody-gold-dextran-labeled ROS were effectively phagocytized by RPE cells. These results provide compelling evidence that rhodopsin in the ROS plasma membrane does not function as the ligand for recognition by RPE cells.
Article
Three types of odour blindness or specific anosmia have been studied, but genetical evidence so far obtained is tentative. Specific anosmia to the musk pentadecalactone (Thibetolide) is found in about 7% of tested subjects. Results on 109 Caucasian families are presented in whom all known inherent and environmental interferences with olfactory sensitivity have been excluded. Insensitivity to musk occurred in thirty six families; males and females were equally affected. The offspring from twenty marriages between smellers were analyzed by the a priori method and there was almost exact correspondence between the expected and observed results. Among the fifteen marriages between smeller and nonsmeller, thirteen produced only smeller children, probably because of small family size. Two marriages produced a total of three children, one smeller and two nonsmellers. A marriage between nonsmellers produced two children, both non smellers. The data acquired with this test protocol, though not conclusively ruling out polygenic inheritance, strongly suggest that the inability to smell pentadecalactone is inherited as a simple recessive autosomal character. The high incidence of the anosmia suggests a genetic polymorphism, but might result from relaxation of natural selection on nonsmellers. human olfactory sense.