Content uploaded by Michael G Marmot
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Michael G Marmot
Content may be subject to copyright.
ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION
Negative Aspects of Close Relationships
and Heart Disease
Roberto De Vogli, PhD, MPH; Tarani Chandola, DPhil; Michael Gideon Marmot, PhD, FRCP
Background: The aims of the study were to (1) ana-
lyze the association between negative aspects of close re-
lationships and increased risk for coronary heart dis-
ease and (2) examine whether the association is stronger
among women and people of lower social position.
Methods: Prospective cohort study of 9011 British civil
servants (6114 men and 2897 women). Negative as-
pects of close relationships and other social support mea-
sures (confiding/emotional and practical) were assessed
with the Close Persons Questionnaire during phase 2
(1989-1990) or phase 1 (1985-1988). Associations be-
tween negative aspects of close relationships and inci-
dent coronary events were determined during an aver-
age follow-up period of 12.2 years. Covariates included
sociodemographic characteristics (age, sex, marital sta-
tus, and employment grade), biological factors (obesity,
hypertension, diabetes mellitus, and cholesterol level),
psychosocial factors (negative affectivity, depression, and
work stress), and health behaviors (smoking, alcohol in-
take, exercise, and fruit and vegetable consumption).
Results: After adjustment for sociodemographic char-
acteristics, biological factors, and other dimensions of so-
cial support, individuals who experienced negative as-
pects of close relationships had a higher risk of incident
coronary events (hazard ratio, 1.34; 95% confidence in-
terval, 1.10-1.63). The association was attenuated but re-
mained statistically significant after additional adjust-
ment for negative affectivity and depression (hazard ratio,
1.25; 95% confidence interval, 1.02-1.55). Although
women and men in a lower employment grade were more
likely to be exposed to negative aspects of close relation-
ships, sex and social position had no statistically signifi-
cant interaction effects. Confiding/emotional and prac-
tical support were not associated with incident coronary
events.
Conclusion: Adverse close relationships may increase
the risk of heart disease.
Arch Intern Med. 2007;167(18):1951-1957
A
N EXTENSIVE BODY OF RE-
search shows that social re-
lations are associated with
better health
1,2
and re-
duced risks of cardiovas-
cular disease.
3-6
However, contradictory
findings on the health benefits of struc-
tural support
6
and the limited protective
effect of marital status against cardiovas-
cular disease among women
7,8
have stimu-
lated further scientific inquiry into the
quality of social relationships.
Emerging theoretical concepts and em-
pirical evidence indicate that negative as-
pects of close relationships, although oc-
curring less often than positive exchanges,
have more potent effects in determining
daily mood and well-being.
9-12
A longitu-
dinal study of older adults, which evalu-
ated comparable measures of positive and
negative social exchanges, showed that
negative exchanges were associated with
negative affect both cross-sectionally and
during a period of 3 months. Positive ex-
changes, however, exhibited only a cross-
sectional association with positive affect
but no association with negative affect
cross-sectionally or longitudinally.
13
Other
studies showed that negative interper-
sonal interactions are associated with
poorer mental health, physical function-
ing,
14
and sickness absence.
15
So far, little research has examined the
association between negative aspects of
close relationships and coronary heart dis-
ease (CHD). Poor marital quality was an
important prognostic factor for myocar-
dial infarction (MI)
16
and congestive heart
failure.
17
A recent cohort study analyzing
marital quality and the occurrence of the
metabolic syndrome
18
(a cluster of risk fac-
tors for cardiovascular disease
19
and dia-
betes mellitus
20
) found that maritally dis-
satisfied women were 3 times more likely
to have the metabolic syndrome than were
maritally satisfied women.
21
Evidence suggests that negative as-
pects of close relationships are more likely
to be seen in women and people in lower
social positions. Women are more likely
Author Affiliations:
International Institute for
Society and Health, Department
of Epidemiology and Public
Health, University College
London, London, England.
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 167 (NO. 18), OCT 8, 2007 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1951
©2007 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at University of Montana, on April 9, 2009 www.archinternmed.comDownloaded from
to be sensitive and invest more time and energy in so-
cial relationships than are men.
22
Women report higher
psychological distress and negative social interactions,
despite having more close relationships and giving and
receiving more support than men.
23,24
People of lower social position are generally more likely
to be exposed to stressful socioeconomic circumstances
25
that can negatively influence interpersonal relationships.
A recent study showed that financial strain was associ-
ated with negative interactions with relatives and depres-
sive symptoms.
26
Another investigation showed that older
adults living in deteriorated neighborhoods were more
likely to encounter negative exchanges with family mem-
bers and friends.
27
A prospective investigation among the
elderly persons showed that cumulative exposure to nega-
tive interactions was associated with self-reported heart
disease over a 6-year period, but only among the partici-
pants who had less than a high school education.
28
The aims of the present study were to (1) analyze the
association between negative aspects of close relation-
ships and increased risk for CHD and (2) examine whether
the association is stronger among women and people of
lower social position.
METHODS
STUDY POPULATION
The target population of the Whitehall II study, a prospective
cohort study, is all nonindustrial civil servants aged 35 to 55
years who worked in the London offices of 20 civil service de-
partments at baseline (1985-1988). Full details of the meth-
ods of the final cohort, which consisted of 10 308 subjects (3414
women), are reported elsewhere.
29
The present study in-
cluded 9011 respondents (6114 men and 2897 women) with
available information on negative aspects of close relation-
ships in phase 2 (1989-1990) or phase 1 (1985-1988) and no
history of CHD events in phase 2. The association between nega-
tive aspects of close relationships and incident CHD events was
determined over an average follow-up period of 12.2 years.
NEGATIVE ASPECTS OF CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS
AND SOCIAL SUPPORT
In phase 2, 7907 participants completed the Close Persons Ques-
tionnaire (available from the authors on request), which in-
cludes 15 items about negative aspects of close relationships, con-
fiding/emotional support, and practical support received in the
past 12 months. In phase 1, 7669 participants completed the ques-
tionnaire (only 74.39% of respondents were asked to complete
it). The 3 subscales were derived from the items using factor analy-
sis.
30
Although the questionnaire assesses social relationships rela-
tive to a maximum of 4 close relationships, our analyses fo-
cused on the first close relationship only, for which the reliability
was the highest.
30
Negative aspects of close relationships, a 4-item
scale, refer to adverse exchanges and conflict within a relation-
ship nominated by the respondents as their closest one. Sources
of negative aspects of close relationships were divided into 2 cat-
egories: partner or not partner. Confiding/emotional support is
a 7-item scale measuring wanting to confide, confiding, sharing
interests, boosting self-esteem, and reciprocity relative to the first
close relationship. Practical support, also referred to as instru-
mental support, is a 3-item scale that measures major and mi-
nor practical help or support received from the closest person.
30
Each item of the 3 social support measures has been evaluated
on a Likert scale from 1 to 4, with higher scores indicating more
negative aspects for the negative aspect items or more positive
support for scales of confiding/emotional and practical support.
The Likert-scaled responses for the items of each social support
scale were summed. For those with missing scores at phase 2,
we used information from phase 1 when available. The final scores
were grouped into tertiles representing different levels of expo-
sure to negative aspects of close relationships, confiding/
emotional, and practical support.
The Close Persons Questionnaire has been validated in terms
of reliability and validity. Cronbach ␣ was 0.63 for negative as-
pects of close relationships, 0.85 for confiding/emotional sup-
port, and 0.82 for practical support. A retest reliability study
of 4-week intervals showed moderately high agreement for nega-
tive close relationships (r=0.72) and practical support (r=0.71),
and high agreement for confiding/emotional support (r=0.88).
To evaluate validity, the questionnaire was sent to the person
closest to each of the last 60 interviewees who nominated a close
relationship. Negative aspects of close relationships showed the
best agreement between the report of respondent and the first
close relationship (r= 0.65 for female spouse and r=0.40 for male
spouse).
30
INCIDENT CORONARY EVENTS
The main outcome variable was a measure of incident coro-
nary events between phase 2 (1989-1990) and the end of phase
7 (2003-2004), including fatal MI, nonfatal MI, or angina from
clinical records (electrocardiograms [ECGs] and hospital or gen-
eral practitioner records) and excluding those events that were
self-reported. To assess fatal MI, participants’ records were
flagged for mortality at the National Health Service Central Reg-
istry (mortality data were corroborated by other sources in-
cluding the death certificate). Deaths resulting from MI were
given codes 410-414 from the International Classification of Dis-
eases, 9th Revision.
15
Potential new cases of nonfatal MI were
ascertained with questionnaire items on chest pain
16
and phy-
sician’s diagnosis of heart attack. Details of physician diag-
noses and investigation results were sought from medical rec-
ords for all potential cases of MI. Resting ECGs were performed
(Siemens Mingorec, Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Ger-
many) and assigned Minnesota codes.
17
Based on all available
data (from questionnaire, study ECGs, hospital acute ECGs,
and cardiac enzymes), nonfatal MI was defined using MONICA
(Multinational Monitoring of Trends and Determinants in Car-
diovascular Disease) criteria.
18
Classification of MI was car-
ried out blind to other study data independently by 2 trained
coders, with adjudication by a third in the (rare) event of dis-
agreement. Angina was initially assessed through participants’
reports of symptoms
19
and then corroborated by medical rec-
ords or abnormalities on a resting ECG, exercise ECG, or coro-
nary angiogram. The outcome was composed of clinically veri-
fied incident coronary events only.
31
Actual dates of events were
used from National Health Service records.
COVARIATES
Covariates at baseline included sociodemographic character-
istics such as age, sex, marital status (married or cohabiting;
never married; and separated, widowed, or divorced), and em-
ployment grade (administrative, executive, and clerical), as well
as biological, psychosocial, and behavioral factors. Biological
factors were measured in phase 1 and included hypertension
(use of antihypertensive medication or systolic/diastolic blood
pressure ⬎ 140/90 mm Hg vs others), serum total cholesterol
concentration (measured in millimoles per liter), obesity (body
mass index [calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height
in meters squared] ⬎ 30), and self-reported diabetes mellitus.
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 167 (NO. 18), OCT 8, 2007 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1952
©2007 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at University of Montana, on April 9, 2009 www.archinternmed.comDownloaded from
Psychosocial factors included depression, negative affectiv-
ity, and work stress, and were measured in phase 2 or phase 1.
Negative affectivity was measured by the negative affect sub-
scale of the Affect Balance Scale.
32
Participants were divided into
3 tertiles of negative affectivity (lowest, middle, and highest).
Depression was assessed by the depression subscale of the Gen-
eral Health Questionnaire.
33
The scores on the General Health
Questionnaire depression subscale ranged from 0 to 12, with
those scoring 4 or higher being classified as “depressed.” Mea-
sures of work stress (high demands and low job control) were
assessed by an adapted version of the job content instru-
ment.
34
For each participant, we calculated the means of job
demands and job control scores and assigned them in the low-
est, middle, or highest tertile. For those participants with miss-
ing items in phase 2 in any of the psychosocial factors, we used
information from phase 1.
Health behaviors were measured in phase 2 and included
smoking (smoker or nonsmoker), alcohol intake (none, mod-
erate [1-21 units/wk for men and 1-14 units/wk for women],
and heavy drinking [⬎21 units/wk for men and ⬎14 for wom-
en]), exercise (vigorous or ⬎1.5 h/wk of vigorous activity, mod-
erate or ⬎1.5 h/wk of moderate activity, and none/mild or ⬍1.5
h/wk of moderate or vigorous activity), and fruit and veg-
etable consumption (daily vs not daily). Missing data in phase
2 were replaced by equivalent behavioral data from phase 1.
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
The
2
test for trend, the test for heterogeneity, and the analy-
sis of variance were used to assess the linearity or differences
across categories of negative aspects of close relationships by
sex. Survival analyses using Cox proportional hazards models
were used to analyze the association between negative close re-
lationships and incident coronary events during follow-up af-
ter adjustment for other covariates. The proportional hazards
assumption was checked by examining the interaction term be-
tween each of the covariates and time. None of these interac-
tion term regression coefficients were significantly different from
zero. In addition, multicollinearity assumptions were exam-
ined through a correlation matrix of regression coefficients. In
model 1, the association between adverse close relationships
and heart disease was adjusted for sociodemographic charac-
teristics, biological factors, and source of support. Additional
adjustments for negative affectivity and depression were made
in model 2. In model 3, measures of work stress (job demands
and job control) were added to the model. Model 4 addition-
ally adjusted for health behaviors (smoking, exercise, alcohol
intake, and fruit and vegetable consumption).
The role of sex, social position, and other potential effect modi-
fiers (source of support, marital status, confiding/emotional sup-
port, and practical support) were tested by analyzing the statis-
tical significance of age-adjusted interaction terms. All P values
(2-tailed) below .05 were considered to be statistically signifi-
cant. The relation between negative close relationships and coro-
nary events was analyzed using samples less than the total sample
size because fully adjusted models included only participants with
no missing data on all covariates. Participants with a history of
coronary events in phase 2 were excluded from the analyses. All
analyses were performed using the software package SPSS, ver-
sion 11.0 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois).
RESULTS
Table 1 shows characteristics of the participants by dif-
ferent levels of exposure to negative close relationships and
by sex. Negative close relationships were more likely to
be experienced by younger individuals, women (data not
shown), and men in the lower employment grade, and were
less likely to be reported by people who were never mar-
ried. Exposure to negative close exchanges was also asso-
ciated with negative affectivity, depression, work stress,
low confiding/emotional support, and partner as a source
of support. Men experiencing adverse close relationships
were less likely to consume fruit and vegetables daily. There
were no statistically significant differences in terms of prac-
tical support, smoking, exercise, and alcohol intake.
Of 8499 participants free of CHD at baseline and with
no missing values on all covariates, 589 reported a coro-
nary event.
Table 2 displays the hazard ratios of inci-
dent CHD by reports of negative aspects of close rela-
tionships and other measures of social support (confiding/
emotional and practical). After adjustment for age, sex,
marital status, employment grade, obesity, hyperten-
sion, diabetes mellitus, cholesterol level, social support
dimension, and source of support (model 1), a dose-
response association was seen between greater reports
of stressful close relationships and incident CHD. Indi-
viduals who experienced a high level of negative aspects
of close relationships were 1.34 times (95% confidence
interval, 1.10-1.63) more likely to experience an inci-
dent CHD event compared with those with a low level
of negative close relationships. When additionally ad-
justing for negative affectivity and depression (model 2),
the relationship was attenuated (HR, 1.25; 95% confi-
dence interval, 1.02-1.55) but remained statistically sig-
nificant. The effect size of the relationship remained un-
changed when measures of work stress such as job
demands and job control (model 3) were included. Ad-
ditional adjustment for health-related behaviors (exer-
cise, smoking, alcohol intake, and fruit and vegetable con-
sumption) (model 4) had little effect on the relationship.
Confiding/emotional and practical support were not as-
sociated with incident coronary events.
Table 3 examines the association between negative
aspects of close relationships and incident coronary events
by sex and employment grade. No statistically signifi-
cant interaction effects were found for either. We also
analyzed additional interaction terms (marital status,
source of support, confiding/emotional support, practi-
cal support, negative affectivity, depression, and work
stress). Interaction effects were observed in some cases:
the association between negative close relationships and
incident coronary events was higher among individuals
reporting their partner as a first close relationship, those
with higher confidential/emotional and practical sup-
port, and those who were widowed, divorced, or sepa-
rated. However, none of the interaction terms reached
statistical significance (P⬎.10 for all).
COMMENT
Results of this study indicate that negative interactions
in close relationships increase the risk of incident CHD.
The effect is independent of sociodemographic charac-
teristics (age, sex, marital status, and employment grade),
biological factors (obesity, hypertension, diabetes melli-
tus, and cholesterol level), psychosocial factors (nega-
tive affectivity, depression, and work stress), and health-
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 167 (NO. 18), OCT 8, 2007 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1953
©2007 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at University of Montana, on April 9, 2009 www.archinternmed.comDownloaded from
related behaviors (smoking, alcohol intake, exercise, and
fruit and vegetable consumption). In contrast to our origi-
nal hypotheses, interaction effects by sex and social po-
sition were not statistically significant. Although our find-
ings show that negative interactions in close relationships
are more likely to occur to women and participants in
lower employment grade, they produce similar effects on
heart disease regardless of sex and social position. When
we considered other potential effect modifiers, interac-
tion effects were observed for marital status, source of
support, and confiding/emotional and practical sup-
port, but they failed to reach statistical significance.
Table 1. Characteristics of Participants by Levels of Negative Aspects of Close Relationships by Sex
Characteristic
Negative Aspects of Close Relationships
Men, % Women, %
No.
Tertile
P for
Trend No.
Tertile
P for
Trend
Lowest
(n = 2096)
Middle
(n = 2031)
Highest
(n = 1987)
Lowest
(n = 1036)
Middle
(n = 909)
Highest
(n = 952)
Mean age, y 6114 44.4 43.6 43.7 ⬍ .001
a
2897 45.9 44.6 44.7 ⬍.001
a
Employment grade ⬍.001 .45
Administrative 2398 36.1 34.2 29.7 341 30.5 37.0 32.6
Professional 3227 33.5 33.3 33.2 1175 35.2 33.4 31.4
Clerical 489 30.9 27.8 41.3 1381 37.5 28.3 34.2
Marital status ⬍.001
b
.18
b
Married/cohabiting 4987 33.7 33.1 33.2 1771 34.8 30.7 34.5
Never married 798 40.4 33.5 26.2 626 38.8 33.7 27.5
Divorced/widowed/separated 315 28.9 34.6 36.5 485 34.8 30.5 34.6
Confiding/emotional support ⬍.001 ⬍.001
Lowest tertile 2306 30.2 31.7 38.0 938 29.9 32.1 38.1
Middle tertile 1935 29.4 36.0 34.6 1006 33.5 32.2 34.3
Highest tertile 2306 44.4 32.2 23.4 946 44.2 29.8 26.0
Practical support .99 .97
Lowest tertile 2083 35.6 33.4 31.0 1276 36.0 32.4 31.7
Middle tertile 1605 31.3 32.3 36.4 743 32.7 32.6 34.7
Highest tertile 2414 35.3 33.7 31.0 862 38.3 28.5 33.2
Source of support .001 .01
Not partner 4800 36.7 35.3 28.0 1332 36.9 33.3 29.8
Partner 1307 33.6 32.6 33.7 1562 34.7 29.8 35.5
Negative affectivity ⬍.001 ⬍.001
Lowest tertile 2124 49.5 33.2 17.2 1000 50.0 31.9 18.1
Middle tertile 2040 32.5 35.8 31.7 836 38.9 32.4 28.7
Highest tertile 1929 19.6 30.5 49.9 1029 19.7 30.4 49.9
Depression ⬍.001 ⬍.001
Not depressed 4646 37.4 33.6 29.0 2044 39.7 31.7 28.6
Depressed 1462 24.4 31.9 43.6 850 26.4 30.5 43.2
Job demands ⬍.001 ⬍.001
Lowest tertile 1203 36.6 32.7 30.8 877 39.2 30.8 30.0
Middle tertile 2708 35.8 33.4 30.8 1218 36.2 31.6 32.2
Highest tertile 2201 31.2 33.3 35.5 796 31.2 31.8 37.1
Job control ⬍.001 .03
Lowest tertile 1277 31.3 30.2 38.4 1377 34.4 29.9 35.7
Middle tertile 2096 32.9 34.1 33.1 865 37.3 32.8 29.8
Highest tertile 2731 36.7 34.0 29.3 641 36.3 33.1 30.6
Smoking .08 .29
No 5245 34.5 33.8 31.6 2290 36.2 31.4 32.4
Yes 780 32.3 30.3 37.7 590 34.1 31.5 34.4
Exercise .98 .39
Mild 1994 34.6 31.6 33.8 1349 33.1 33.7 33.2
Moderate 2625 34.5 34.5 31.0 1198 38.6 29.3 32.1
Vigorous 1399 33.7 33.0 33.4 261 33.7 30.7 35.6
Alcohol consumption .26
b
.52
b
Moderate 4165 34.9 33.3 31.8 1775 36.5 31.3 32.1
Abstinent 834 32.6 30.8 36.6 889 35.3 29.9 34.8
Heavy 1111 33.1 34.8 32.0 250 32.0 37.2 30.8
Fruit and vegetables ⬍.001 .21
Daily 3433 36.0 35.0 29.0 1826 36.2 31.9 31.9
Not daily 2668 32.1 31.0 36.9 1069 35.0 30.4 34.6
a
Calculated using F test for linearity.
b
Calculated using the test for heterogeneity.
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 167 (NO. 18), OCT 8, 2007 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1954
©2007 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at University of Montana, on April 9, 2009 www.archinternmed.comDownloaded from
Our findings expand and corroborate previous re-
search on the effect of negative aspects of marital quality
in influencing health
35
by showing that negative interac-
tions in close relationships are determinants of coronary
events. This was not the case for confiding/emotional sup-
port and practical support. The possibility that negative close
relationships are more powerful predictors of health than
other aspects of social support is consistent with previous
research findings indicating that individuals tend to men-
tally replay negative encounters more than they replay posi-
tive ones.
36
It is possible that negative aspects of close re-
lationships are more important for the health of individuals
because of the power of negative close relationships to ac-
tivate stronger emotions (worrying and anxiety) and the
consequent physiological effects. In contrast, other more
positive forms of support may not affect the physiology of
individuals in a measurable or clinically relevant way.
37
Pathways linking negative close interactions and heart
disease may include behaviors, emotions, and biologi-
cal reactions. Marital distress has been found to be as-
sociated with poor diet, lack of exercise,
8
substance abuse
such as problem drinking in men,
38
and nonadherence
to medical regimens.
8
However, our results showed that
the association between negative close relationships and
heart disease remained virtually unchanged after con-
trolling for health behaviors. When one considers emo-
tional factors and their biological translation into the body,
research shows that negative marital interactions are as-
sociated with depression, often in combination with re-
duced self-esteem
39
and/or higher levels of anger.
40
These
emotional reactions have been found to influence CHD
41
through the cumulative “wear and tear” on organs and
tissues caused by alterations of autonomic functions, neu-
roendocrine changes, disturbances in coagulation, and
inflammatory and immune responses.
42
Our findings par-
tially support the hypothesis that negative emotions may
mediate the relationship between adverse negative rela-
tionships and heart disease. When we adjusted for emo-
tional factors such as negative affectivity and depres-
sion, the relationship was attenuated.
This study has a number of shortcomings. Our mea-
sure of self-reported negative interactions in close rela-
tionships may not reflect an objective evaluation of this
factor or may be influenced by personality traits or spe-
Table 2. Hazard Ratios (and 95% Confidence Intervals) of Incident Coronary Events
Model 1
a
Model 2
b
Model 3
c
Model 4
d
Cases/No.
Negative aspects of close relationships
Lowest tertile 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 186/2957
Middle tertile 1.19 (0.97-1.46) 1.16 (0.94-1.42) 1.16 (0.94-1.42) 1.15 (0.94-1.42) 189/2785
Highest tertile 1.34 (1.10-1.63) 1.25 (1.02-1.55) 1.25 (1.01-1.54) 1.23 (1.00-1.52) 214/2757
Negative affectivity
Lowest tertile 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 206/3044
Middle tertile 1.03 (0.84-1.26) 1.01 (0.82-1.24) 1.02 (0.83-1.25) 188/2774
Highest tertile 1.15 (0.93-1.42) 1.10 (0.88-1.37) 1.09 (0.88-1.36) 207/2864
Depression
Not depressed 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 436/6462
Depressed 1.16 (0.95-1.40) 1.15 (0.95-1.40) 1.14 (0.94-1.39) 165/2220
Confiding/emotional support
Lowest tertile 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 201/2968
Middle tertile 1.05 (0.86-1.28) 1.07 (0.88-1.31) 1.06 (0.87-1.29) 1.06 (0.87-1.30) 186/2723
Highest tertile 1.05 (0.86-1.28) 1.09 (0.89-1.33) 1.08 (0.89-1.33) 1.09 (0.89-1.33) 202/2808
Negative affectivity
Lowest tertile 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 200/2970
Middle tertile 1.08 (0.88-1.32) 1.05 (0.86-1.29) 1.06 (0.87-1.30) 186/2721
Highest tertile 1.24 (1.01-1.53) 1.19 (0.96-1.46) 1.17 (0.95-1.45) 202/2810
Depression
Not depressed 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 431/6338
Depressed 1.18 (0.97-1.43) 1.17 (0.97-1.42) 1.16 (0.96-1.41) 158/2161
Practical support
Lowest tertile 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 209/3210
Middle tertile 1.17 (0.95-1.44) 1.17 (0.95-1.44) 1.17 (0.95-1.44) 1.18 (0.96-1.46) 172/2201
Highest tertile 1.01 (0.83-1.24) 1.02 (0.84-1.25) 1.01 (0.83-1.24) 1.03 (0.84-1.26) 210/3100
Negative affectivity
Lowest tertile 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 202/2957
Middle tertile 1.05 (0.86-1.29) 1.04 (0.85-1.27) 1.04 (0.85-1.28) 185/2719
Highest tertile 1.22 (1.00-1.50) 1.17 (0.95-1.45) 1.16 (0.94-1.44) 204/2817
Depression
Not depressed 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 1 [Reference] 430/6350
Depressed 1.20 (0.99-1.46) 1.20 (0.99-1.45) 1.19 (0.98-1.44) 161/2161
a
Model 1 adjusts for age, sex, employment grade, marital status, obesity, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, cholesterol level, social support dimension, and
source of support.
b
Model 2 adjusts for model 1, negative affectivity, and depression.
c
Model 3 adjusts for model 2, job demands, and job control.
d
Model 4 adjusts for model 3, smoking, exercise, alcohol intake, and fruit and vegetable consumption.
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 167 (NO. 18), OCT 8, 2007 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1955
©2007 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at University of Montana, on April 9, 2009 www.archinternmed.comDownloaded from
cific characteristics of respondents.
30
Another limita-
tion of our exposure measure is that it is based on the
characteristics of the first close relationship only, of which
64.2% were spouses. This prevented us from investigat-
ing the health effects of negative social interactions in a
more extended network. For the main predictor we used
phase 1 information in cases in which phase 2 informa-
tion was not available. This is a potential limitation be-
cause data may not be missing at random. Compared with
participants with available information on adverse close
relationships in both phases, those with missing infor-
mation in phase 1 but available measures in phase 2 were
slightly younger (44.1 years vs 45.0), more likely to be
in a higher employment grade (P⬍ .001), but similar in
terms of sex, biological factors, and incident coronary
events. This suggests that the missing respondents for the
phase 1 measure of negative aspects of close relation-
ships were largely similar to the analytical sample with
the exception of fewer respondents in the high employ-
ment grade. The different periods in which covariates
were measured could be sources of bias because mea-
sures in phase 1 could conceivably have changed by
phase 2. Time variability, together with the use of crude
dichotomous and 3-category measures, may be respon-
sible for the failure of some interaction terms to reach
statistical significance because the strength of all asso-
ciations could have been reduced. However, dichoto-
mous and 3-category measures may be quite stable over
time; when repeating the analysis with phase 2 or phase
1 data only, adjusted for biological factors in phase 1
and demographic, behavioral, and psychosocial factors
in the respective phases, the associations of negative as-
pects of close relationships with incident coronary
events were very similar. Finally, the generalization of
results may also be questioned because British civil ser-
vants may not adequately represent the general popula-
tion (industrial workers were not included). However,
most of the working population is now employed in of-
fice jobs, and the exclusion of the upper and lower ends
of the social hierarchy is likely to have produced an
underestimation of the health effects of negative close
relationships.
In conclusion, a person’s heart condition seems to be
influenced by negative intimate relationships. In this pro-
spective cohort study, we showed that negative aspects
of close relationships, not confiding/emotional support
and practical support, are associated with CHD.
Accepted for Publication: April 29, 2007.
Correspondence: Roberto De Vogli, PhD, MPH, Depart-
ment of Epidemiology and Public Health, University Col-
lege London, 1-19 Torrington Pl, London WC1E 6BT,
England (r.devogli@ucl.ac.uk).
Author Contributions: Study concept and design: De Vogli,
Chandola, and Marmot. Acquisition of data: De Vogli,
Chandola, and Marmot. Analysis and interpretation of data:
De Vogli, Chandola, and Marmot. Drafting of the manu-
script: De Vogli and Chandola. Critical revision of the
manuscript for important intellectual content: Chandola and
Marmot. Statistical analysis: De Vogli and Chandola.
Obtained funding: Chandola. Study supervision: Marmot.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: The Whitehall II Study was sup-
ported by grants from the Medical Research Council, Brit-
ish Heart Foundation, Health and Safety Executive, and
Department of Health; grant HL36310 from the Na-
tional Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, grant AG13196
from the National Institute on Aging, and grant HS06516
from the Agency for Health Care Policy Research, all part
of the National Institutes of Health; and a grant from the
John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Re-
search Networks on Successful Midlife Development and
Socioeconomic Status and Health.
Additional Contributions: We are grateful for the con-
tributions of all members of the Whitehall II Study team
and extend our gratitude to all participating civil servants
in the Whitehall II Study; the participating civil service de-
partments, their welfare, personnel, and establishment offi-
cers; the Occupational Health and Safety Agency, Lon-
don; and the Council of Civil Service Unions, London.
Table 3. Interaction Effects Among Negative Aspects
of Close Relationships, Sex, and Social Position
and Additional Interaction Terms
Characteristic
Negative Aspects of Close Relationships
by Tertile, Age-Adjusted HR (95% CI)
Lowest Middle Highest
Sex
Men 1 [Reference] 1.28 (1.02-1.61) 1.30 (1.04-1.63)
Women 1 [Reference] 0.99 (0.66-1.48) 1.31 (0.91-1.89)
P for interaction
.39
a
Employment grade
High 1 [Reference] 1.39 (1.00-1.91) 1.09 (0.77-1.55)
Middle 1 [Reference] 1.13 (0.83-1.52) 1.48 (1.12-1.96)
Low 1 [Reference] 1.08 (0.69-1.69) 1.25 (0.83-1.88)
P for interaction
.96
b
Close relationship
Close relationship
or partner
1 [Reference] 1.26 (0.89-1.80) 1.04 (0.71-1.53)
Close relationship
is partner
1 [Reference] 1.17 (0.93-1.49) 1.39 (1.11-1.74)
P for interaction
.23
a
Practical support
Low 1 [Reference] 1.31 (0.95-1.80) 1.19 (0.86-1.66)
Medium 1 [Reference] 1.36 (0.94-1.96) 1.15 (0.79-1.67)
High 1 [Reference] 1.00 (0.71-1.41) 1.53 (1.12-2.09)
P for interaction
.11
b
Confiding/emotional
support
Low 1 [Reference] 1.06 (0.75-1.50) 1.18 (0.85-1.63)
Medium 1 [Reference] 1.27 (0.89-1.81) 1.34 (0.95-1.90)
High 1 [Reference] 1.32 (0.95-1.83) 1.44 (1.03-2.02)
P for interaction
.89
b
Marital status
Married/cohabiting 1 [Reference] 1.20 (0.96-1.50) 1.32 (1.06-1.64)
Never married 1 [Reference] 1.20 (0.72-2.01) 1.09 (0.62-1.91)
Widowed/divorced/
separated
1 [Reference] 1.36 (0.68-2.74) 1.43 (0.73-2.79)
P for interaction
.68
b
Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; HR, hazard ratio.
a
Interaction test between negative social support and covariate on 2 df.
b
Interaction test between negative social support and covariate on 4 df.
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 167 (NO. 18), OCT 8, 2007 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1956
©2007 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at University of Montana, on April 9, 2009 www.archinternmed.comDownloaded from
REFERENCES
1. Berkman LF, Syme SL. Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: a nine-
year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. Am J Epidemiol. 1979;109
(2):186-204.
2. House JS, Landis KR, Umberson D. Social relationships and health. Science. 1988;
241(4865):540-545.
3. Kaplan GA, Salonen JT, Cohen RD, Brand RJ, Syme SL, Puska P. Social con-
nections and mortality from all causes and from cardiovascular disease: pro-
spective evidence from eastern Finland. Am J Epidemiol. 1988;128(2):370-
380.
4. Kawachi I, Colditz GA, Ascherio A, et al. A prospective study of social networks
in relation to total mortality and cardiovascular disease in men in the USA. J Epi-
demiol Community Health. 1996;50(3):245-251.
5. Rosengren A, Wilhelmsen L, Orth-Gome´r K. Coronary disease in relation to so-
cial support and social class in Swedish men: a 15 year follow-up in the study of
men born in 1933. Eur Heart J. 2004;25(1):56-63.
6. Lett HS, Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, Strauman TJ, Robins C, Sherwood A.
Social support and coronary heart disease: epidemiologic evidence and impli-
cations for treatment. Psychosom Med. 2005;67(6):869-878.
7. Johnson NJ, Backlund E, Sorlie PD, Loveless CA. Marital status and mortality:
the national longitudinal mortality study. Ann Epidemiol. 2000;10(4):224-238.
8. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Newton TL. Marriage and health: his and hers. Psychol Bull.
2001;127(4):472-503.
9. Newsom JT, Rook KS, Nishishiba M, Sorkin DH, Mahan TL. Understanding the
relative importance of positive and negative social exchanges: examining spe-
cific domains and appraisals. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2005;60(6):
P304-P312.
10. Finch JF, Okun MA, Pool GJ, Ruehlman LS. A comparison of the influence of
conflictual and supportive social interactions on psychological distress. J Pers.
1999;67(4):581-621.
11. Rook K. Investigating the positive and negative sides of personal relationships:
through a glass darkly? In: Spitzberg BH, Cupach WR, eds. The Dark Side of
Close Relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence A Erlbaum Associates; 1998:369-
393.
12. Rook KS. Emotional health and positive versus negative social exchanges: a daily
diary analysis. Appl Developmental Sci. 2001;5(2):86-97.
13. Newsom JT, Nishishiba M, Morgan DL, Rook KS. The relative importance of three
domains of positive and negative social exchanges: a longitudinal model with
comparable measures. Psychol Aging. 2003;18(4):746-754.
14. Stansfeld SA, Bosma H, Hemingway H, Marmot MG. Psychosocial work char-
acteristics and social support as predictors of SF-36 health functioning: the White-
hall II study. Psychosom Med. 1998;60(3):247-255.
15. Rael EG, Stansfeld SA, Shipley M, Head J, Feeney A, Marmot M. Sickness ab-
sence in the Whitehall II study, London: the role of social support and material
problems. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1995;49(5):474-481.
16. Orth-Gome´r K, Wamala SP, Horsten M, Schench-Gustafsson K, Schneiderman
N, Mittleman MA. Marital stress worsens prognosis in women with coronary heart
disease: the Stockholm Female Coronary Risk Study. JAMA. 2000;284(23):
3008-3014.
17. Coyne JC, Rohrbaugh MJ, Shoham V, Sonnega JS, Nicklas JM, Cranford JA.
Prognostic importance of marital quality for survival of congestive heart failure.
Am J Cardiol. 2001;88(5):526-529.
18. Reaven GM. Syndrome X: is one enough? Am Heart J. 1994;127(5):1439-1442.
19. Lakka HM, Laaksonen DE, Lakka TA, et al. The metabolic syndrome and total
and cardiovascular disease mortality in middle-aged men. JAMA. 2002;288
(21):2709-2716.
20. Hanson RL, Imperatore G, Bennett PH, Knowler WC. Components of the “meta-
bolic syndrome” and incidence of type 2 diabetes. Diabetes. 2002;51(10):3120-
3127.
21. Troxel WM, Matthews KA, Gallo LC, Huller LH. Marital quality and occurrence of
the metabolic syndrome in women. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165(9):1022-1027.
22. Notarius CI, Benson PR, Sloane D, Vanzetti N, Hornyak L. Exploring the inter-
face between perception and behavior: an analysis of marital interaction in dis-
tressed and nondistressed couples. Behav Assess. 1989;11:39-64.
23. Fuhrer R, Stansfeld SA, Chemali J, Shipley MJ. Gender, social relations and men-
tal health: prospective findings from an occupational cohort (Whitehall II study).
Soc Sci Med. 1999;48(1):77-87.
24. Pagel MD, Erdly WW, Becker J. Social networks: we get by with (and in spite of )
a little help from our friends. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;53(4):793-804.
25. Marmot M. Status Syndrome: How Your Social Standing Directly Affects Your
Health and Life Expectancy. London, England: Bloomsbury; 2004.
26. Lincoln KD, Chatters LM, Taylor RJ. Social support, traumatic events, and de-
pressive symptoms among African Americans. J Marriage Fam. 2005;67(3):
754-766.
27. Krause N. Neighborhood deterioration, social skills, and social relationships in
later life. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2006;62(3):185-207.
28. Krause N. Negative interaction and heart disease in later life: exploring varia-
tions by socioeconomic status. J Aging Health. 2005;17(1):28-55.
29. Marmot MG, Smith GD, Stansfeld S, et al. Health inequalities among civil ser-
vants: the Whitehall II study. Lancet. 1991;337(8754):1387-1393.
30. Stansfeld S, Marmot M. Deriving a survey measure of social support: the reli-
ability and validity of the Close Persons Questionnaire. Soc Sci Med. 1992;
35(8):1027-1035.
31. Hemingway H, Shipley M, Britton A, Page M, Macfarlane P, Marmot M. Progno-
sis of angina with and without a diagnosis: 11 year follow up in the Whitehall II
prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2003;327(7420):895. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7420.895.
32. Bradburn N. The Structure of Psychological Well-being. Chicago, IL: Aldine; 1969.
33. Goldberg D. The Detection of Psychiatric Illness by Questionnaire. London, En-
gland: Oxford University Press; 1972.
34. Karasek R, Theorell T. Healthy Work: Stress, Productivity and the Reconstruc-
tion of Working Life. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1990.
35. Bookwala J. The role of marital quality in physical health during the mature years.
J Aging Health. 2005;17(1):85-104.
36. Taylor SE. Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-
minimization hypothesis. Psychol Bull. 1991;110(1):67-85.
37. Umberson D, Williams K, Powers DA, Liu H, Needham B. You make me sick:
marital quality and health over the life course. J Health Soc Behav. 2006;47
(1):1-16.
38. Horwitz AV, White HR. Becoming married, depression, and alcohol problems among
young adults. J Health Soc Behav. 1991;32(3):221-237.
39. Druley JA, Townsend AL. Self-esteem as a mediator between spousal support
and depressive symptoms: a comparison of healthy individuals and individuals
coping with arthritis. Health Psychol. 1998;17(3):255-261.
40. Johnson E. The Deadly Emotions: The Role of Anger, Hostility and Aggression
in Health and Emotional Well-being. New York, NY: Praeger; 1990.
41. Hemingway H, Marmot M. Evidence based cardiology: psychosocial factors in
the aetiology and prognosis of coronary heart disease—systematic review of pro-
spective cohort studies. BMJ. 1999;318(7196):1460-1467.
42. McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N Engl J Med.
1998;338(3):171-179.
(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 167 (NO. 18), OCT 8, 2007 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1957
©2007 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
at University of Montana, on April 9, 2009 www.archinternmed.comDownloaded from