ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract

This article considers the informed consent process in relation to carrying out research with intoxicated participants in 'field' research settings. There is little discussion in the literature of the potential problems that the intoxication of research participants may pose to research. Intoxication is a potential problem for all researchers but is heightened in field research that takes place in settings where participants are likely to be intoxicated, such as licensed venues, in drug consumption rooms, or police custody suites. The risks to research participants that intoxication poses should not be resolved by electing not to do research with intoxicated participants; it is argued that these risks can be managed to some extent, and are offset by the benefits of such research. Moreover, intoxication (and the impairment of cognitive functions relevant to valid informed consent) may not always be identifiable through behavioural or biochemical methods of detection. The search for accurate and field-practical methods for identifying intoxication amongst participants is useful, but not the only strategy for researchers who want to ensure the validity of the consent process. Suggestions are provided for devising research protocols that acknowledge and accept intoxication of research participants and attempt to protect them. One solution is to side-step identification of intoxication per se as a strategic objective in the consent process, and turn instead to established methods for ensuring that information has been understood by potential research participants.
A
vailable online at www.sciencedirect.com
Drug and Alcohol Dependence 93 (2008) 191–196
Commentary
Researching the intoxicated: Informed consent
implications for alcohol and drug research
Judith Aldridge a,, Vikki Charles b
aSchool of Law, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PL, United Kingdom
bNational Addiction Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Kings College London,
4 Windsor Walk, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
Received 3 April 2007; received in revised form 4 September 2007; accepted 5 September 2007
Available online 22 October 2007
Abstract
This article considers the informed consent process in relation to carrying out research with intoxicated participants in ‘field’ research settings.
There is little discussion in the literature of the potential problems that the intoxication of research participants may pose to research. Intoxication is
a potential problem for all researchers but is heightened in field research that takes place in settings where participants are likely to be intoxicated,
such as licensed venues, in drug consumption rooms, or police custody suites. The risks to research participants that intoxication poses should
not be resolved by electing not to do research with intoxicated participants; it is argued that these risks can be managed to some extent, and are
offset by the benefits of such research. Moreover, intoxication (and the impairment of cognitive functions relevant to valid informed consent)
may not always be identifiable through behavioural or biochemical methods of detection. The search for accurate and field-practical methods for
identifying intoxication amongst participants is useful, but not the only strategy for researchers who want to ensure the validity of the consent
process. Suggestions are provided for devising research protocols that acknowledge and accept intoxication of research participants and attempt
to protect them. One solution is to side-step identification of intoxication per se as a strategic objective in the consent process, and turn instead to
established methods for ensuring that information has been understood by potential research participants.
© 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Intoxication; Ethics; Informed consent; Research process; Alcohol; Illegal drugs
1. Introduction
Intoxication by alcohol and other psychoactive illegal/illicit
drugs1may compromise the informed consent process in
research. Intoxication can impair cognition and judgement that
are essential in order that potential research participants can
understand what participation in research involves, and thus
make informed decisions about their participation.
Researchers may be more likely to encounter intoxi-
cated research participants when the populations from which
researchers draw their samples are drug and alcohol users
themselves, and in settings where participants are likely to
Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: Judith.Aldridge@manchester.ac.uk (J. Aldridge),
Vikki.Charles@iop.kcl.ac.uk (V. Charles).
1Under consideration here are the psychoactive substances primarily associ-
ated with intoxication in field-based studies: alcohol, and primarily illegal/illicit
drugs such as cannabis, ecstasy, and heroin.
be intoxicated at the time of obtaining consent: in the homes
of research participants, in ‘the streets’, in drug consumption
rooms, in licensed and other social venues. For example, the
last 15 years have seen a resurgence of venue-based drug and
alcohol research with the study of raves and club drug use, from
the first ‘dance drug’ research carried out and published in the
early 1990s in Britain (Newcombe, 1992) followed by studies
in Britain (Deehan and Saville, 2003; Measham et al., 2001;
Release, 1997; Riley et al., 2001; Sanders, 2005; Silverstone,
2006), the US (Kelly, 2006; Perrone, 2006; Yacoubian et al.,
2003), Australia (Gourley, 2004), and elsewhere in Europe
(Tossmann et al., 2001; van de Wijngaart et al., 1999) employing
this venue-based design. Quantitative in situ research has also
involved data collection with intoxicated participants, in which
consent will, of necessity, have been obtained from intoxicated
participants. Recently for example, Voas et al. (2006) carried out
surveys along with alcohol breath tests and oral fluid drug assays
using their ‘portal’ method outside electronic music dance events
to estimate levels of drug and alcohol consumption.
0376-8716/$ – see front matter © 2007 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.09.001
192 J. Aldridge, V. Charles / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 93 (2008) 191–196
We discuss here research participant intoxication regarding
the informed consent process where research occurs ‘in the
field’, as opposed to in other more controlled settings.
2. How researchers have dealt with intoxicated research
participants
The alcohol and illegal drug research literature provides scant
discussion or guidance in relation to carrying out research with
intoxicated participants. This suggests that many researchers
who encounter intoxication amongst their research participants –
particularly in field research where intoxication may be prevalent
– implicitly accept that research participants may be intoxicated
without questioning and unpacking issues around that intoxica-
tion. Researchers who have acknowledged the intoxication of
their research participants have dealt with it in one of two ways:
(1) excluding intoxicated participants; or (2) excluding those
deemed ‘too intoxicated’ to participate.
The first method is the more conservative: excluding the
intoxicated from (at least) the consent process, and often from
the research as well. A US survey found that even amongst
researchers conducting primarily (non-field-based) clinical out-
come studies, few had a policy for dealing with intoxicated
participants, and of those who did, ‘most indicated they would
attempt to keep the participant at the site until sober or until
transportation was arranged’ (McCrady and Bux, 1999 p. 190).
Others, in discussing research that involves administering the
‘substance of dependence’ to a drug user (College of Problems
of Drug Dependence, 1995; Foddy and Savulescu, 2006), argue
that heroin users are sufficiently free to consent to receive their
drug of addiction, but that consent must occur when the addict is
sober. The underlying assumption here is that the consent pro-
cess – or even the research itself – may be compromised by the
intoxication of research participants.
The second method for dealing with intoxication involves
‘screening out’ the more extremely intoxicated from being
selected for research, and is less conservative because only intox-
ication beyond a particular level is deemed problematic. Deehan
and Saville assessed visible signs of intoxication in their research
with club-goers in six venues in England on a scale from 0 ‘no
intoxication’ to 5 ‘extremely intoxicated’. Only those 90% of
potential interviewees considered ‘able to take part in the inter-
view comfortably’ were approached (scoring from 0 to 2 on
the scale) (2003). Those researching drug use by detainees in
Australia (Mouzos et al., 2006), and Britain via NEW-ADAM
(Bennett, 2000) have also excluded from their research those
deemed ‘too intoxicated’ to interview. The British NEW-ADAM
detainee study assessed ‘fitness for interview’ in part on the
basis of drug and/or alcohol intoxication, and those deemed unfit
included 29% for alcohol and 2% for other drugs, on the basis of
an initial impressionistic subjective assessment by the custody
sergeant. However, as Bennett points out, this did not guarantee
that interviewees were not intoxicated at the time of interview, as
it was observed that some of them found it hard to keep awake (a
feature of substantial use of depressant drugs such as alcohol or
heroin) (Bennett, personal communication). This method may
have been useful for ‘skimming off’ the extremely intoxicated,
but clearly was not able to identify all research participants who
were intoxicated in ways that interfered with data collection.
Both approaches address and problematise research par-
ticipant intoxication. Albeit in different ways, each approach
involves assessing intoxication in order to identify and exclude
intoxicated potential participants. We turn now to problems in
assessing intoxication.
3. Assessing intoxication
Intoxication involves being under the influence of the effects
of one or more psychoactive substances that may alter emotional
state, perception, judgement and performance. However, intox-
ication is not a straightforwardly identifiable state that occurs
in an easily measurable way when a psychoactive substance is
ingested, regardless of place, expectation, and individual dif-
ferences. Although biochemical markers for the presence of a
substance have sometimes been demonstrated to correlate with
the subjective effects of intoxication (e.g., Cone and Huestis,
1993), biochemical markers cannot accurately gauge intoxica-
tion itself. In relation to alcohol, for example, blood alcohol
concentration (BAC) is not a reliable indicator of intoxication
since the same BAC may have different effects in different peo-
ple, and different effects in the same person across different
circumstances including physical condition, personality, mood
and situation (Krober, 1998). Regarding the substances that field
researchers typically encounter – alcohol and illicit drugs –
biochemical detection is not suited to identifying intoxication
because it is geared to identifying the presence of substances
or their metabolites in the body. As such, biochemical detec-
tion does not take into account tolerance, rate of substance
metabolism, and other individual and context differences.
Some have turned to behavioural approaches to identifying
impairment thought to result from intoxication (e.g., Burns and
Moskowitz, 1977) because of (1) the inability of biochemical
detection to serve as a measure of intoxication; and (2) the
need for field tests that do not involve expensive, bulky equip-
ment or specialised technical expertise. However, success with
the use of behavioural approaches to detecting impairment due
to intoxication with illicit drugs is mixed, having been used
successfully with some substances but not others. For exam-
ple, Papafotiou et al. (2005) found that impaired performance
on field tests assessing driving impairment was positively cor-
related with THC dose; however, field test results were not
correlated with low levels of amphetamine intoxication (Silber
et al., 2005). Recent evidence by Perham et al. (2007) suggests
that subjective ratings of the physical manifestations of drunken-
ness (staggering gait, slurred speech, glazed eyes) of city centre
drinkers by trained observers corresponded with blood alcohol
concentrations. However, Brick and Carpenter (2001) found that
police officers, watching a video-recorded interview, were able
to correctly identify that alcohol-intoxicated subjects had been
drinking only at relatively high levels of blood alcohol concen-
tration (BAC 0.15–0.16%). Thus, intoxication may be difficult
to gauge even for police officers who are likely to be both trained
in doing so, and who encounter alcohol intoxication frequently.
Overall, these results suggest that observed behaviour is often
J. Aldridge, V. Charles / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 93 (2008) 191–196 193
not a very accurate predictor of substance level, suggesting that
it might not always be a useful tool for researchers concerned
about ability to give informed consent. Moreover, it is likely
that intoxication levels that affect cognitive functions, thus in
turn affecting the consent process, may not always be associ-
ated with observable impairment. Again therefore, behavioural
approaches to identifying impairment will have only limited util-
ity for researchers concerned about the ability of participants to
give informed consent.
4. The impact of intoxication on informed consent
Because of the effect of intoxication by some psychoac-
tive substances on cognition and judgement, it is likely that
intoxication could impact a participant’s capacity to understand
explanations about research and on the capacity to make deci-
sions about participation, by for example, impairing judgements
about the merits or drawbacks of participation. It is likely that
the impact of intoxication by some psychoactive drugs on feel-
ings or emotional state (Parker et al., 1998) (e.g., happy, sad,
scared, vulnerable and empathetic) may also colour perceptions
and judgements in such a way as to influence decision making.
There is research to suggest that alcohol intoxication affects sug-
gestibility (Santtila et al., 1999), risk-taking (Lane et al., 2004)
and moral reasoning (Fincham and Barling, 1979), all variables
that could play a role in informed consent. Intoxication may
therefore increase the chances of uninformed consent, reluctant
consent, or consent that is later regretted.
There has been no systematic research on the impact of intox-
ication on the informed consent process. However, there is some
evidence that intoxication may increase willingness to consent
to research, and thus influence decision making. In venue-based
research on dance drug use by Measham and colleagues, field-
work staff regularly reported their perceptions that intoxication
affected willingness to consent to interview. Indeed, respondents
agreeing to an interview reported having already consumed more
alcohol (8.3 units) than those who refused (6.6 units), a statisti-
cally significant difference, suggesting that alcohol intoxication
may increase willingness to participate (Measham et al., 2001,
p. 89).
5. Ethical principles to guide research with intoxicated
participants
Ethical decision making in most medical, health-related and
social research draws on principles which are relevant to con-
ducting research with intoxicated participants: (1) autonomy:
participants should be free to participate; (2) non-malificience:
participants should be protected from harm; (3) beneficience: the
benefits of research should outweigh the risks; (4) justice: people
should be treated equally (Beauchamp and Childress, 2001).
Intoxication could be seen to compromise the autonomy
of would-be research participants to consent, thus potentially
leading to harm. On the other hand, the benefits of increased
understanding generated about alcohol and other drug users in
natural settings where intoxication is prevalent could be seen
to outweigh the risks. This kind of research has a long and
distinguished history, much of it influenced by the ‘Chicago
School’ style of urban ethnography: Dai’s Opium Addiction in
Chicago (1937, 1970);Lindesmith’s Opiate Addiction (1947,
1968); and Preble and Casey’s Taking Care of Business (1969).
Although research with some groups (e.g., the very young or
old, those with physical or mental disabilities) can present spe-
cial challenges in order for researchers to achieve their inclusion
in research, it has been argued that to exclude them from research
conflicts with the principle of justice through discrimination
(Schuklenk, 2000) or through an unwillingness of researchers
to speak directly to respondent groups that present challenges
(Rodgers, 1999). Similar thinking can be applied to research
with the intoxicated.
Some researchers have addressed consent issues for carry-
ing out research with such ‘vulnerable’ groups where cognition
and judgement may be impaired or deficient in such a way that
normal procedures for obtaining informed consent are inap-
propriate or insufficient. The analogy with intoxication is not
perfect. An intoxicated person, qua their intoxication, is not
inevitably ‘vulnerable’; however, it is their status as potentially
vulnerable that concerns us here. Moreover, insights into the
informed consent process particularly from those researching
people with mental disabilities will be limited to some extent,
since the incapacities addressed for that population group will
be relatively enduring, whereas the relevant incapacities for
intoxicated people will be relatively transitory. Recommenda-
tions that flow from these and other insights are considered
below.
6. Recommendations
Although some researchers have taken a conservative stance
on the problem of intoxication to the informed consent pro-
cess by determining to exclude intoxicated participants from
aspects of the research process (i.e., considering the risks to the
participant too great), we contend here that the problem that
intoxication poses should not be resolved by electing not to do
research with intoxicated participants: (1) it is not possible com-
pletely to avoid the intoxicated, given the prevalence of the use
of and intoxication by alcohol and illicit drugs; (2) intoxication
is just one of a number of ‘altered states’ in which individu-
als find themselves, that include other common states such as
stress and heightened emotions. Attempting to secure the par-
ticipation of research participants who are in a ‘pure’ state (free
from intoxication or other factors that may interfere with their
cognitive processes in a way that impacts on providing con-
sent) is likely to prove impossible; (3) both biochemical and
behavioural methods for identifying intoxication (with a view
to excluding the intoxicated) are problematic. We should rather
seek to devise research protocols that acknowledge intoxication
and protect research participants, thus off-setting risk in the form
of potential harm to participants.
6.1. Ensuring understanding
One suggestion to enable people of varying capacities to
consent to participate in research is to ensure that information
194 J. Aldridge, V. Charles / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 93 (2008) 191–196
provided to them is comprehensible and appropriate, and then
check that the information has actually been understood. Sudore
et al. (2006) have demonstrated a method of ‘teaching then test-
ing’ potential respondents on information about their study, with
a view to excluding from the consent process those who, after
repeated teaching/testing, are unable to demonstrate adequate
understanding. Regarding intoxicated participants for whom
cognitive functioning may be diminished or altered, researchers
need to have appropriate expectations regarding a participant’s
ability to take in, process and act on the information they have
been given. Researchers may therefore consider building extra
time into research protocols to check that understanding has
occurred. For field research (which often takes place in busy
or chaotic environments), a verbal variant of this approach
could be used. The advantage of this approach is that it side-
steps the problem of assessing intoxication per se, and instead
jumps directly to attempting to ensure that information about
the research has been understood in spite of intoxication. As has
been argued, in relation to carrying out research with people with
mental disabilities (Fisher, 2003), that enhancing aspects of the
consent setting that reduce consent vulnerability (i.e., provid-
ing comprehensible information and ensuring comprehension)
may be preferable to conceiving consent competence as a ‘state’
within an individual; this may be a particularly important insight
as regards intoxication, which is mostly a relatively transitory
and changeable state.
6.2. Extending the timeframe for consent and consent
withdrawal
Lawton (2001), commenting on her ethnographic research
in a hospice, found that changes in the condition of patients
meant that they were not always able to state whether they
still wanted to take part in her research. The similarity between
intoxicated participants and Lawton’s dying patients lies in the
fact of their conditions being changeable in ways that may
be important regarding ongoing consent to participate. It is
likely that levels of intoxication will vary over fairly short
spaces of time (certainly hours, but even over shorter periods
than that). Given the connection between intoxication and emo-
tions/feelings and various cognitive processes, the willingness
of participants to continue may, during the course of partic-
ipation, change. If consent to participate in research should
be treated as a process rather than as a one-off event, as has
been argued (e.g., Ramcharan and Cutcliffe, 2001), it is sensible
for fieldworkers to be alert – and remain alert – to changing
signs of unwillingness to participate and to remind research
participants where appropriate that they may terminate their
participation at any time. Rodgers (1999) argues that there are
signs that participants use aside from verbal dissent to signify a
wish to opt out of participation that has already begun. A par-
ticipant who becomes distracted/disinterested/agitated/irritated
during an interview could be reluctant to continue. Being alert
to these signs, checking that participants are happy to continue,
and re-emphasising their right to withdraw at any time, should
be built into research protocols and fieldworker/interviewer
training.
Research participants may have regrets about their participa-
tion in the minutes, days or weeks following their participation.
A standard de-brief should allow participants the space to recon-
sider their participation before fieldworker and participant have
broken contact. The fieldworker can provide contact details that
participants can easily keep or take away with them in a small
and portable ‘credit card’ size. This allows the possibility for
a former participant to initiate contact so that consent can be
retroactively withdrawn, and effectively extends the time frame
over which the process of providing consent can occur into peri-
ods in which former subjects are sober. The idea of retroactive
withdrawal of consent is controversial, not least because it raises
the rarely articulated issue of ownership of data; in other words,
what are the limits of the ‘rights’ of a former research partic-
ipant to control the data that resulted from their participation
(see Wiles et al., 2006)? There are complexities in relation to
retroactive withdrawal of consent that researchers will need
to anticipate in their research protocols: what time limit for
withdrawal is suitable, and how data already collected will be
handled.
When researchers know that candidates for participation in
research are under the influence of alcohol or illegal drugs, they
may ask those who have already indicated a willingness to partic-
ipate whether they believe that the effects of their consumption
may have influenced their willingness. This question could pro-
vide potential respondents with the specific opportunity to reflect
on their intoxication in relation to their willingness to participate
in the research.
6.3. Training fieldwork staff for intoxication awareness
Research should be carried out by staff trained to be sensitive
to signs of intoxication outside of the more expected and com-
mon alcohol intoxication (loss of co-ordination, staggering gait,
drowsiness, slurred speech and glazed eyes). Signs of intoxi-
cation associated with other illicit drugs may also be assessed
(paranoia, anxiety, eye-rolling, pupil dilation/constriction, head
movements or jerks). It may be neither feasible nor desirable
that research staff should be clinically trained, but they should
have knowledge about the typical effects of the substances they
are likely to encounter during fieldwork, and be trained to be
aware of these, remembering to remind participants of their right
to withdraw, particularly when observable signs of intoxication
appear to change.
6.4. Excluding the obviously intoxicated at the outset
In spite of the limitations of visual/behavioural approaches
to identifying intoxication, the use of an initial visual assess-
ment of intoxication in order to screen out the most obviously
and extremely intoxicated research participants may neverthe-
less be good practice for researchers (e.g, Bennett, 2000; Deehan
and Saville, 2003). Although some very intoxicated participants
may ‘slip through the net’ (Bennett, personal communication),
excluding at least the most obviously intoxicated is a feasible
and achievable objective.
J. Aldridge, V. Charles / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 93 (2008) 191–196 195
7. Further research
Although this contribution addressed the implications for the
informed consent process of research participant intoxication
where research occurs ‘in the field’, as opposed to in other more
controlled settings, the implications apply also to some extent
to research carried out in more controlled (for example clinical)
settings, and also where research is conducted in the general pop-
ulation, such as in household surveys. The prevalence of drug
and alcohol use is well documented; the prevalence of intoxi-
cation in the general population with the range of psychoactive
substances remains undocumented. For example, it may be use-
ful to for household survey researchers to know how often people
are likely to be intoxicated in their homes during data collection.
Indeed, intoxication as a phenomenon remains under-
analysed within social research approaches to drugs and alcohol
research. In order to address this, social researchers need to
develop methods of conceptualising, operationalising and mea-
suring, and explaining and theorising intoxication for a range
of substances. Of particular importance should be assessing the
effect of intoxication on validity and accuracy of self-reports.
More systematic research is required to establish the effects of
intoxication by different drugs, or combinations of drugs, on the
consent process across diverse research contexts.
Conflict of interest
None.
Acknowledgements
Many thanks to Trevor Bennett, Karen Clarke, Phil Edwards,
Angela Melville, Toby Seddon and anonymous reviewers for
their comments and suggestions.
Work in the preparation of the manuscript was supported by
internal funds from the institutions of each author.
Contributors: Vikki Charles conducted some of the informed
consent literature search and summarised this literature. Judith
Aldridge drafted the manuscript. Both authors have contributed
to and approved the final manuscript.
References
Beauchamp, T.L., Childress, J.F., 2001. Principles of Biomedical Ethics. Oxford
University Press, Oxford.
Bennett, T., 2000. Drugs and Crime: The Results of the Second Developmental
Stage of the NEW-ADAM Programme. Home Office Research Study 205,
London. http://www.homeoffice.gov.uk/rds/pdfs/hors205.pdf.
Brick, J., Carpenter, J.A., 2001. The identification of alcohol intoxication by
police. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 25, 850–855.
Burns, M., Moskowitz, H., 1977. Psychophysical Tests for DWI Arrest. US
Department of Transportation, National Highway Traffic Safety Adminis-
tration, Final Report DOT-HS-5-01242.
College of Problems of Drug Dependence, 1995. Human subject issues in drug
abuse research. Drug Alcohol Depend. 37, 167–175.
Cone, E.J., Huestis, M.A., 1993. Relating blood concentrations of tetrahydro-
cannabinol and metabolites to pharmacologic effects and time of marijuana
usage. Ther. Drug Monit. 15, 527–532.
Dai, B., 1937/1970. Opium Addiction in Chicago. Patterson Smith, Montclair,
NJ.
Deehan, A., Saville, E., 2003. Calculating the risk: recreational drug use among
clubbers in the South East of England. Home Office Online Report 43/03,
London.
Fincham, F., Barling, J., 1979. Effects of alcohol on moral functioning in male
social drinkers. J. Genet. Psychol. 134, 79–88.
Fisher, C.B., 2003. Goodness-of-fit ethic for informed consent to research involv-
ing adults with mental retardation and developmental disabilities. Ment.
Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. 9, 27–31.
Foddy, B., Savulescu, J., 2006. Addiction and autonomy: can addicted peo-
ple consent to the prescription of their drug of addiction? Bioethics 20,
1–15.
Gourley, M., 2004. A subcultural study of recreational ecstasy use. J. Sociol. 40,
59–73.
Kelly, B.C., 2006. Concepts of risk in the lives of ecstasy-using youth. In:
Sanders, B. (Ed.), Drugs, Clubs and Young People. Ashgate, Aldershot,
pp. 50–65.
Krober, H.-L., 1998. Psychiatric criteria of legal responsibility after the con-
sumption of alcohol: the German situation. Eur. Addict. Res. 4, 107–112.
Lane, S.D., Cherek, D.R., Pietras, C.J., Tcheremissine, O.V., 2004. Alco-
hol effects on human risk taking. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 172, 68–
77.
Lawton, J., 2001. Gaining and maintaining informed consent: ethical concerns
raised in a study of dying patients. Qual. Health Res. 11, 693–705.
Lindesmith, A.R., 1947/1968. Addiction and Opiates. Adline Publishing,
Chicago.
McCrady, B.S., Bux, D.A., 1999. Ethical issues in informed consent with sub-
stance abusers. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 67, 186–193.
Measham, F., Aldridge, J., Parker, H., 2001. Dancing on Drugs: Risk, Health
and Hedonism in the British Club Scene. Free Association Books, London.
Mouzos, J., Smith, L., Hind, N., 2006. Drug Use Monitoring in Australia: 2005
Annual Report on Drug Use Among Police Detainees. Australian Institute
of Criminology, Research and Public Policy Series No. 70, Canberra.
Newcombe, R., 1992. A researcher reports from the rave. Druglink, Jan-
uary/February, 14–16.
Papafotiou, K., Carter, J.D., Stough, C., 2005. An evaluation of the sensitiv-
ity of the Standardised Field Sobriety Tests (SFSTs) to detect impairment
due to marijuana intoxication. Psychopharmacology (Berl) V180, 107–
114.
Parker, H., Aldridge, J., Measham, F., 1998. Illegal Leisure. The Normalisation
of Adolescent Recreational Drug Use. Routledge, London.
Perham, N., Moore, S.C., Shepherd, J., Cusens, B., 2007. Identifying drunken-
ness in the night-time economy. Addiction 102, 377–380.
Perrone, D., 2006. New York City club kids: a contextual understanding of club
drug use. In: Sanders, B. (Ed.), Drugs, Clubs and Young People. Ashgate,
Aldershot, pp. 26–49.
Preble, E., Casey, J.J., 1969. Taking care of business—the heroin user’s life on
the street. Int. J. Addict. 4, 1–24.
Ramcharan, P., Cutcliffe, J.R., 2001. Judging the ethics of qualitative research:
considering the ‘ethics as process’ model. Health Soc. Care Community 9,
358–366.
Release, 1997. Release Drugs and Dance Survey: An Insight into the Culture.
Release, London.
Riley, S.C.E., James, C., Gregory, D., Dingle, H., Cadger, M., 2001. Patterns of
recreational drug use at dance events in Edinburgh, Scotland. Addiction 96,
1035–1047.
Rodgers, J., 1999. Trying to get it right: undertaking research involving people
with learning difficulties. Disabil. Soc. 14, 421–433.
Sanders, B., 2005. In the club: ecstasy use and supply in a London nightclub.
Sociology 39, 241–258.
Santtila, P., Ekholm, M., Niemi, P., 1999. The effects of alcohol on interrogative
suggestibility: the role of state-anxiety and mood states as mediating factors.
Leg. Criminol. Psychol. 4, 1–13.
Schuklenk, U., 2000. Protecting the vulnerable: testing times for clinical research
ethics. Soc. Sci. Med. 51, 969–977.
Silber, B.Y., Papafotiou, K., Croft, R.J., Stough, C.K.K., 2005. An evalua-
tion of the sensitivity of the standardised field sobriety tests to detect
196 J. Aldridge, V. Charles / Drug and Alcohol Dependence 93 (2008) 191–196
the presence of amphetamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 182, 153–
159.
Silverstone, D., 2006. Pub space, rave space and urban space: three different
night-time economies. In: Sanders, B. (Ed.), Drugs, Clubs and Young People.
Ashgate, Aldershot, pp. 141–151.
Sudore, R.L., Landefeld, C.S., Williams, B.A., Barnes, D.E., Lindquist, K.,
Schillinger, D., 2006. Use of a modified informed consent process among
vulnerable patients: a descriptive study. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 21, 867–
873.
Tossmann, P., Boldt, S., Tensil, M.-D., 2001. The use of drugs within the techno
party scene in European Metropolitan Cities. Eur. Addict. Res. 7, 2–23.
van de Wijngaart, G.F., Braam, R., de Bruin, D., Fris, M., Maalste, N.J.M.,
Verbraeck, H.T., 1999. Ecstasy use at large-scale dance events in the Nether-
lands. J. Drug Issue 29, 679–702.
Voas, R.B., Furr-Holden, D., Lauer, E., Bright, K., Johnson, M.B., Miller, B.,
2006. Portal surveys of time-out drinking locations: a tool for studying binge
drinking and AOD use. Eval. Rev. 30, 44–65.
Wiles, R., Charles, V., Crow, G., Heath, S., 2006. Researching researchers:
lessons for research ethics. Qual. Res. 6, 283–299.
Yacoubian, G.S., Boyle, C., Harding, C.A., Loftus, E.A., 2003. It’s a rave new
world: estimating the prevalence and perceived harm of ecstasy and other
drug use among club rave attendees. J. Drug Educ. 33, 187–196.
... In particular, the settlement had a high population of young psychoactive substance users. It was apparent that some participants could be influenced by one or more psychoactive substances, which would alter their emotional state, perception, judgement and performance [61]. We therefore trained research assistants to be sensitive to behavioural signs of intoxication such as loss of co-ordination, staggering gait, drowsiness, slurred speech and glazed eyes for alcohol users, and paranoia, anxiety, eye-rolling, pupil dilation/constriction, head movements or jerks for other substances [61]. ...
... It was apparent that some participants could be influenced by one or more psychoactive substances, which would alter their emotional state, perception, judgement and performance [61]. We therefore trained research assistants to be sensitive to behavioural signs of intoxication such as loss of co-ordination, staggering gait, drowsiness, slurred speech and glazed eyes for alcohol users, and paranoia, anxiety, eye-rolling, pupil dilation/constriction, head movements or jerks for other substances [61]. Research assistants were also cautioned to remind the participants of their right to withdraw, particularly when observable signs of intoxication appeared to change [61]. ...
... We therefore trained research assistants to be sensitive to behavioural signs of intoxication such as loss of co-ordination, staggering gait, drowsiness, slurred speech and glazed eyes for alcohol users, and paranoia, anxiety, eye-rolling, pupil dilation/constriction, head movements or jerks for other substances [61]. Research assistants were also cautioned to remind the participants of their right to withdraw, particularly when observable signs of intoxication appeared to change [61]. This enabled the data collection team to obtain data from participants who were not intoxicated thereby improving the quality of the data. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Young psychoactive substance users exhibit high-risk behaviours such as unprotected sexual intercourse, and sharing needles and syringes, which increases their risk of Hepatitis B infection. However, there is limited evidence of screening, and vaccination status of this subgroup. The aim of this study was to establish the predictors of screening and completion of the hepatitis B vaccination schedule. Methods A cross-sectional study using respondent driven sampling was used to enrol respondents from twelve out of fifty-seven informal settlements in Kampala city. Data were collected using an electronic structured questionnaire uploaded on the KoboCollect mobile application, and analysed using Stata version 14. A “modified” Poisson regression analysis was done to determine the predictors of screening while logistic regression was used to determine the predictors of completion of the Hepatitis B vaccination schedule. Results About 13.3% (102/768) and 2.7% (21/768) of the respondents had ever screened for Hepatitis B, and completed the Hepatitis B vaccination schedule respectively. Being female (aPR 1.61, 95% CI: 1.11–2.33), earning a monthly income >USD 136 (aPR 1.78, 95% CI: 1.11–2.86); completion of the Hepatitis B vaccination schedule (aPR 1.85, 95% CI: 1.26–2.70); lack of awareness about the recommended Hepatitis B vaccine dose (aPR 0.43, 95% CI: 0.27–0.68); and the belief that the Hepatitis B vaccine is effective in preventing Hepatitis B infection (aPRR 3.67, 95% CI: 2.34–5.73) were associated with “ever screening” for Hepatitis B. Knowledge of the recommended Hepatitis B vaccine dose (aOR 0.06, 95% CI: 0.01–0.35); “ever screening” for hepatitis B (aOR 9.68, 95% CI: 2.17–43.16) and the belief that the hepatitis B vaccine is effective in preventing Hepatitis B infection (aOR 11.8, 95% CI: 1.13–110.14) were associated with completion of the hepatitis B vaccination schedule. Conclusions Our findings indicate a low prevalence of Hepatitis B screening and completion of the Hepatitis B vaccination schedule among young psychoactive substance users in informal settings. It is evident that lack of awareness about Hepatitis B is associated with the low screening and vaccination rates. We recommend creation of awareness of Hepatitis B among young people in urban informal settlements.
... Recruitment occurred within the first 12 h of arrival at the facility after clearance by the intake medical team. Medical clearance, consistent with the standards and recommendations of Aldridge and Charles (2008), required that at the time of recruitment potential participants' blood alcohol content was under the legal limit of 0.08 as tested by an on-site breathalyzer. Recruitment was done by research assistants who solicited interest from patients in potentially participating in a study examining the effects of treatment on withdrawal symptoms during alcohol detoxification. ...
... Even though these preliminary results are promising, it is important to recognize the measures were self-report. While these measures appeared appropriate to capturing withdrawal symptoms and ACT constructs, there is a vulnerability to potential biases in reporting when relying on participants' perceptions and awareness (Aldridge & Charles, 2008;Sobell & Sobell, 1990). For instance, maybe receiving the ACT protocol oriented participants to desirable patterns of responding. ...
Article
Alcohol detoxification inevitably involves physical and emotional discomfort in the form of withdrawal symptoms. Clinical management typically involves pharmacologic treatment with benzodiazepines. The current pilot study examined the incremental efficacy of adding elements from Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) to standard of care medication management (i.e., treatment as usual [TAU]) at a residential rehabilitation facility. The ACT protocol included two 30-45 minutes sessions, based on the ACT matrix, and brief (5-10 minute) daily skills coaching. Forty-five adults (Mage = 42.4 years, 47% female, 84% white) with alcohol use disorder and experiencing severe withdrawal symptoms were randomized to either ACT+TAU (n=22) or TAU (n=23) and spent an average of 4 days in detoxification. At exit the ACT+TAU group reported greater positive changes in daily psychological flexibility (F = 4.62, p = .04), overall psychological inflexibility (F = 7.97, p = .01 and F = 2.92, p < .10), but not overall flexibility (F = .21, p = .65), connection with chosen values (t = 2.01, p = .05), and withdrawal symptoms (F = 4.02, p = .05 and F = 3.42, p = .07). The number of ACT coaching sessions correlated significantly with change in daily psychological flexibility (r = .44, p < .05). Change on one measure of psychological inflexibility mediated change on one measure of withdrawal (ab = -.32 [SE = .15, 95% CI: -.67 to - .08]) and change in daily psychological flexibility mediated patient satisfaction (ab = 1.05 [SE = .74, 95% CI: .05 to 2.87]). Results suggest a potential benefit of adding ACT elements to medication management in acute alcohol detoxification. Replication and extension is necessary to warrant stronger conclusions.
... We excluded non-residents (e.g., visiting relatives or friends), and those who were not mentally sound or intoxicated during the survey. We trained research assistants to be sensitive to behavioural signs of intoxication such as loss of co-ordination, staggering gait, drowsiness, slurred speech and glazed eyes for alcohol users, and paranoia, anxiety, eye-rolling, pupil dilation/constriction, head movements or jerks for other substances [50]. Research assistants were cautioned to remind the participants of their right to withdraw, particularly when observable signs of intoxication appeared to change [50]. ...
... We trained research assistants to be sensitive to behavioural signs of intoxication such as loss of co-ordination, staggering gait, drowsiness, slurred speech and glazed eyes for alcohol users, and paranoia, anxiety, eye-rolling, pupil dilation/constriction, head movements or jerks for other substances [50]. Research assistants were cautioned to remind the participants of their right to withdraw, particularly when observable signs of intoxication appeared to change [50]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Psychoactive substance use is a public health challenge among young people in informal settlements. Though rarely examined, psychoactive substance use is linked to sexual expectancies and inhibitions, and consequently high-risk sexual behaviours. This study examined the association between sexual expectancies and inhibitions, and high-risk sexual behaviours among young psychoactive substance users (PSUs) in informal settlements in Kampala, Uganda. Methods This cross-sectional study recruited 744 young PSUs from informal settlements in Kampala. Respondent driven sampling was used to recruit respondents. A ‘modified’ Poisson regression model was used for inferential statistics. Data were analysed using the Stata 14 software. Results Of the 744 study participants, 45.6% believed that psychoactive substance use improves sexual performance; 43.3% believed that psychoactive substances make sex more pleasurable, and 53.3% believed that psychoactive substances give courage or confidence to approach a partner for sex. The belief that psychoactive substance use improves sexual performance (PR 1.14, 95% CI: 1.01–1.30), increases the likelihood of engaging in sex (PR 1.20, 95% CI: 1.04–1.40) or gives courage or confidence to approach a sexual partner (PR 1.21, 95% CI: 1.05–1.39) were associated with having sex while under the influence of psychoactive substances. The belief that a psychoactive substance user under the influence of psychoactive substances is more likely to engage in sex (PR 1.48, 95% CI: 1.15–1.90), and likely to find it difficult to refuse sex (PR 1.28, 95% CI: 1.06–1.55) were positively associated with engaging in multiple sexual partnerships. The belief that one easily forgets to use a condom when under the influence of psychoactive substances was positively associated with inconsistent condom use (PR 1.26, 95% CI: 1.09–1.45). Conclusion Psychoactive substance use expectancies associated with high-risk sexual behaviours included the belief that psychoactive substances improve sexual performance and improve confidence in approaching a sexual partner. Psychoactive substance use inhibitions associated with high-risk sexual behaviours included an increased likelihood of engaging in sexual intercourse, difficulties in refusing to engage in sexual intercourse, and forgetting to use condoms while intoxicated. Interventions targeting a reduction in high-risk sexual behaviour should integrate the impact of psychoactive substance use on sexual behaviour.
... Intoxication can impair cognition, judgement, and the capacity to understand research and make decisions regarding participation. Henceforth, we identified impairments thought to result from intoxication, such as the physical manifestations of intoxication (staggering gait, slurred speech, glazed eyes), as reported in previous studies [55]. Thereafter, the RAs administered a digitalized structured questionnaire to the eligible users of psychoactive substances. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The use of psychoactive substances such as alcohol, heroin and marijuana is associated with negative health outcomes such as sexual violence and unintended pregnancies, and risky sexual behaviours. Although there is evidence linking psychoactive substance use and risky sexual behaviours such as inconsistent condom use and multiple sexual relationships, there is limited data on sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among young people. This study aimed to investigate the prevalence and predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among young people in informal settlements in Kampala, Uganda. Methods A cross-sectional study was conducted among 744 sexually active young psychoactive substance users in informal settlements in Kampala, Uganda. Data were collected through face-to-face interviews using a digitalized structured questionnaire, preloaded on the Kobocollect mobile application. The questionnaire captured data on the socio-demographic characteristics of the respondents, history of psychoactive substance use, and sexual behaviours. Data were analysed using STATA Version 14.0. A modified Poisson regression model was used to determine the predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances.. Adjusted prevalence ratios at a p-value value ≤ 0.05 with a 95% confidence interval were considered. Results About 61.0% (454/744) of the respondents had had sex under the influence of psychoactive substances in the last 30 days. The predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances were being female (PR 1.18, 95% CI: 1.04–1.34), being 20–24 years of age (PR: 1.22, 95% CI: 1.04–1.44), being married (PR 1.15, 95% CI: 1.01–1.31) or divorced/separated (PR 1.43, 95% CI: 1.26–1.61), not living with biological parents or guardians (PR 1.22, 95% CI: 0.99–1.50), earning 71 USD and below (PR 0.86, 95% CI: 0.79–1.03) and using alcohol (PR 1.43, 95% CI: 1.25–1.69), marijuana (PR 1.16, 95% CI: 1.02–1.31) and khat (PR 1.25, 95% CI: 1.10–1.42) in the last 30 days. Conclusion The study found that a high proportion of sexually active young people in informal settlements in Kampala, Uganda had engaged in sex under the influence of psychoactive substances in the past 30 days. The study also identified several factors associated with sex under the influence of psychoactive substances, including being female, being aged 20–24 years, being married or divorced or separated, not living with biological parents or guardians, and using alcohol, marijuana, or khat in the past 30 days. Our findings suggest the need for targeted sexual and reproductive health programs that incorporate risk-reduction interventions aimed at reducing sex under the influence of psychoactive substances, especially among females and those who do not live with their parents.
... Intoxication can impair cognition, judgement and capacity to understand research and make decisions about participation. Henceforth, we identi ed impairment thought to result from intoxication, such as the physical manifestations of intoxication (staggering gait, slurred speech, glazed eyes), as reported in previous studies (44). Thereafter, research assistants administered a digitalized structured questionnaire to the eligible psychoactive substance users. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Psychoactive substance use has an impact on risky sexual behaviour. Globaly, over 275 million people use psychoactive substances, however evidence on the predictors of sex under the influence of these substatnces is scanty. We investigated the prevalence and predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among young people aged 18–24 years. Methods A cross-sectional study was conducted among 744 sexually active young psychoactive substance users living in informal settlements in Kampala, Uganda. Data were collected through face-to-face interviews using a digitalized structured questionnaire preloaded on Kobocollect. The questionnaire captured data on the socio-demographic characteristics of the respondents, history of psychoactive substance use, and sexual behaviours. Data were analysed using STATA Version 14.0. A modified Poisson regression model was used to determine the factors associated with the outcome variable. Adjusted prevalence ratios at a p-value value ≤ 0.05 with a 95% confidence interval were considered. Results About 61.0% (454/744) of the respondents had sex under the influence of psychoactive substances in the last 30 days. The predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances were being female (PR 1.18, 95% CI: 1.04–1.34), being 20–24 years of age (PR: 1.22, 95% CI: 1.04–1.44), being married (PR 1.15, 95% CI: 1.01–1.31) or divorced/separated (PR 1.43, 95% CI: 1.26–1.61), not living with biological parents or guardians (PR 1.22, 95% CI: 0.99–1.50), earning 71 USD and below (PR 0.86, 95% CI: 0.79–1.03) and using alcohol (PR 1.43, 95% CI: 1.25–1.69), marijuana (PR 1.16, 95% CI: 1.02–1.31) and khat (PR 1.25, 95% CI: 1.10–1.42) in the last 30 days. Conclusion This study revealed a high prevalence of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among young people in informal settlements in Kampala, Uganda. The predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances were being female, being 20–24 years of age, being married or divorced/separated, not living with biological parents or guardians, and using alcohol, marijuana and khat in the last 30 days. There is a need for sexual and reproductive health programs to incorporate risk reduction interventions that aim to reduce sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among the said categories in informal settlements.
... Intoxication can impair cognition, judgement and capacity to understand research and make decisions about participation. Henceforth, we identi ed impairment thought to result from intoxication, such as the physical manifestations of intoxication (staggering gait, slurred speech, glazed eyes), as reported in previous studies (44). Thereafter, research assistants administered a digitalized structured questionnaire to the eligible psychoactive substance users. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Psychoactive substance use has an impact on risky sexual behaviour. Globaly, over 275 million people use psychoactive substances, however evidence on the predictors of sex under the influence of these substatnces is scanty. We investigated the prevalence and predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among young people aged 18-24 years. Methods A cross-sectional study was conducted among 744 sexually active young psychoactive substance users living in informal settlements in Kampala, Uganda. Data were collected through face-to-face interviews using a digitalized structured questionnaire preloaded on Kobocollect. The questionnaire captured data on the socio-demographic characteristics of the respondents, history of psychoactive substance use, and sexual behaviours. Data were analysed using STATA Version 14.0. A modified Poisson regression model was used to determine the factors associated with the outcome variable. Adjusted prevalence ratios at a p-value value ≤ 0.05 with a 95% confidence interval were considered. Results About 61.0% (454/744) of the respondents had sex under the influence of psychoactive substances in the last 30 days. The predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances were being female (PR 1.18, 95% CI: 1.04-1.34), being 20-24 years of age (PR: 1.22, 95% CI: 1.04-1.44), being married (PR 1.15, 95% CI: 1.01-1.31) or divorced/separated (PR 1.43, 95% CI: 1.26-1.61), not living with biological parents or guardians (PR 1.22, 95% CI: 0.99-1.50), earning 71 USD and below (PR 0.86, 95% CI: 0.79-1.03) and using alcohol (PR 1.43, 95% CI: 1.25-1.69), marijuana (PR 1.16, 95% CI: 1.02-1.31) and khat (PR 1.25, 95% CI: 1.10-1.42) in the last 30 days. Conclusion This study revealed a high prevalence of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among young people in informal settlements in Kampala, Uganda. The predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances were being female, being 20-24 years of age, being married or divorced/separated, not living with biological parents or guardians, and using alcohol, marijuana and khat in the last 30 days. There is a need for sexual and reproductive health programs to incorporate risk reduction interventions that aim to reduce sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among the said categories in informal settlements.
... Intoxication can impair cognition, judgement and capacity to understand research and make decisions about participation. Henceforth, we identi ed impairment thought to result from intoxication, such as the physical manifestations of intoxication (staggering gait, slurred speech, glazed eyes), as reported in previous studies (44). Thereafter, research assistants administered a digitalized structured questionnaire to the eligible psychoactive substance users. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Psychoactive substance use has an impact on risky sexual behaviour. Globaly, over 275 million people use psychoactive substances, however evidence on the predictors of sex under the influence of these substatnces is scanty. We investigated the prevalence and predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among young people aged 18-24 years. Methods A cross-sectional study was conducted among 744 sexually active young psychoactive substance users living in informal settlements in Kampala, Uganda. Data were collected through face-to-face interviews using a digitalized structured questionnaire preloaded on Kobocollect. The questionnaire captured data on the socio-demographic characteristics of the respondents, history of psychoactive substance use, and sexual behaviours. Data were analysed using STATA Version 14.0. A modified Poisson regression model was used to determine the factors associated with the outcome variable. Adjusted prevalence ratios at a p-value value ≤ 0.05 with a 95% confidence interval were considered. Results About 61.0% (454/744) of the respondents had sex under the influence of psychoactive substances in the last 30 days. The predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances were being female (PR 1.18, 95% CI: 1.04-1.34), being 20-24 years of age (PR: 1.22, 95% CI: 1.04-1.44), being married (PR 1.15, 95% CI: 1.01-1.31) or divorced/separated (PR 1.43, 95% CI: 1.26-1.61), not living with biological parents or guardians (PR 1.22, 95% CI: 0.99-1.50), earning 71 USD and below (PR 0.86, 95% CI: 0.79-1.03) and using alcohol (PR 1.43, 95% CI: 1.25-1.69), marijuana (PR 1.16, 95% CI: 1.02-1.31) and khat (PR 1.25, 95% CI: 1.10-1.42) in the last 30 days. Conclusion This study revealed a high prevalence of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among young people in informal settlements in Kampala, Uganda. The predictors of sex under the influence of psychoactive substances were being female, being 20-24 years of age, being married or divorced/separated, not living with biological parents or guardians, and using alcohol, marijuana and khat in the last 30 days. There is a need for sexual and reproductive health programs to incorporate risk reduction interventions that aim to reduce sex under the influence of psychoactive substances among the said categories in informal settlements.
... Research assistants were trained to implement the recommendations by Aldridge and Charles (2008) that have been extensively used in previous on-premise studies that acknowledge intoxication in the informed consent process. One researcher conducted the survey while the other prepared to conduct the breathalyzer test. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective The purpose of this on‐premise study was to determine if mixing alcohol with caffeinated mixers had an impact on objective and subjective intoxication. Methods The study was conducted across eight drinking occasions in the City of Bristol, UK. Participants (N = 1041) were recruited outside popular night‐time entertainment venues and interviewed regarding their alcohol consumption for that particular evening, including whether or not they had consumed caffeinated beverages with alcohol. Subjective intoxication was rated on an 11‐point scale and objective intoxication determined with a breath alcohol test. Depending on their consumption on the night of the interview, participants also reported whether they consumed alcohol mixed with caffeinated mixers or alcohol‐only on other consumption occasions. Results Between‐subjects analyses found that alcohol–caffeine consumers consumed more alcohol and had higher objective and subjective intoxication than those who consumed alcohol‐only. These results remained significant regardless of whether or not they mixed alcohol with caffeinated mixers or consumed alcohol‐only on the night of the interview. Within‐subject analyses revealed that alcohol–caffeine consumers drank the same or less alcohol on alcohol–caffeine occasions compared to alcohol‐only occasions. Conclusions These findings provide support that alcohol–caffeine use does not increase overall alcohol consumption, and may be one manifestation of a high risk‐taking personality.
... Some scholars have suggested pragmatic ways of dealing with this problem, such as extending the timeframe for giving informed consent and withdrawal from participation, building extra time and consideration into ensuring the participant understands what they are agreeing to, and immediately excluding individuals who are clearly too intoxicated (Aldridge and Charles 2008;Edelman 2018). However, if we view drug use as a wild self-care practice, participant 'intoxication' during interviews takes on different meanings. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article outlines the experiences of a postgraduate student conducting research on drug use and ‘wild self-care’ in the format of a dialogue with their supervisor. There is a wealth of literature on the ethics of drug use research, the unique issues postgraduate students contend with during their tenure, researcher emotions in the field, and how self-care can be included in the research process, but there is surprisingly little literature discussing the intersection of these issues. Furthermore, it is established good practice for qualitative researchers to engage in reflexive thinking and writing as part of their data analysis process, yet this does not appear to be commonly applied in qualitative drug research. The structure of a dialogue between the postgraduate (Simon Clay) and their supervisor (Gareth Treharne) is used to critically analyse issues of participant and researcher vulnerability when conducting field work, intoxication, and the potential benefits of researchers being more reflexive and open about their personal relationship to substance use. The role of ‘wildness’ and ‘wild self-care’ are used to explore the study of drug use and the research process in general through a series of questions for researchers to consider in practice.
Article
This article contributes to the growing literature on researcher reflexivity by broaching the often-ignored issue of religious positionalities within political science, as well as speaking to the methodological implications of researching religion more broadly. We present and compare two autoethnographic case studies of research on politico-religious conflict in Vietnam and Lebanon, exploring how a researcher’s religiosity presents unique fieldwork challenges, opportunities and insights. We then discuss the ambivalence faced by religious researchers within the highly secularised academic environment, thus blurring the artificial dichotomy between ‘the field’ and the academy. Our reflections centre around three findings: (1) the importance of taking an intersectional approach which neither essentialises nor ignores religious aspects of positionality, whilst also being sensitive to spatial and temporal shifts in how they interact with a researcher’s gender, ethnicity, class and other identifiers; (2) the opportunities and perils of a researcher’s apparent religious common ground with participants (or lack thereof) in building rapport and negotiating a degree of insider status; and (3) the similarities and differences between suspicions of religious partialism during fieldwork and within academia.
Article
Full-text available
There is widespread debate about ethical practice in social research with most social researchers arguing that situational relativist approaches are appropriate for resolving the ethical issues that emerge. In this article, we draw on research conducted on an ESRC-funded study of informed consent in social research to explore the ethical issues that are raised when conducting research with one’s peers. The study involved conducting focus groups and telephone interviews with academic and non-academic researchers. The ethical issues emerging from the study related to consent, data ownership and the management of confidentiality and anonymity. Participants’ responses to these issues and the ways that we managed them are discussed. We conclude by exploring the implications of this study for research more generally and argue that the increased regulation of research needs to enable researchers to attend reflexively to the social context in which consent takes place.
Article
Full-text available
A researcher reports on ethnographic research into the rave scene and ecstasy use, conducted between 1988 and 1992
Article
Every weekend in the UK millions of young people attend nightclubs and many of them will use ‘dance’ drugs such as ecstasy. Drawing from ethnographic data generated from working as a club security guard and in-depth interview material with other security guards and a club manager, this article describes and analyses the use and supply of ecstasy within a large London nightclub. The analyses are centred on the normalized character of ecstasy within this club, the efforts of several security guards to control the supply of ecstasy and how this setting was home to a lively drug culture complete with a prosperous and somewhat protected drug economy.
Article
This paper uses criteria defined by Zarb to describe research undertaken with people with learning difficulties, in the context of an emerging emancipatory paradigm. First, the paper addresses the question: Who controlled the research and what it was about? It considers consultation with people with learning difficulties, the influence of public service organisations and the ethical committee. Secondly, the paper evaluates how far disabled people were involved in the research process, and discusses issues relating to the inclusion of people with learning difficulties as respondents, obtaining informed consent and the involvement of carers in interviews. Finally, the paper discusses the questions: What opportunities existed for disabled people to criticise the research and influence its future direction? What happened to the products of the research? The role of a pilot study, opportunities to provide feedback and dissemination strategies are described.