ArticlePDF Available

A Cluster-Randomized Trial of Screening for Language Delay in Toddlers: Effects on School Performance and Language Development at Age 8

American Academy of Pediatrics
Pediatrics
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The goal of this study was to assess the effects of screening and early treatment of preschool children for language delay on language development and school performance at age 8. A cluster-randomized, controlled trial and follow-up study of 55 child health centers in 6 geographic regions were conducted from January 2002 to September 2005. A total of 9419 children who were from the general population and aged 15 months at entry were studied. School type end school progress was known for 5406 (57.4%) children. In the intervention group, a structured screening instrument was conducted twice (at ages 15/18 and 24 months), and usual care was applied in the control group. The screening instrument consisted of a uniform set of questions for the parents and test elements for the child. A positive screen result was followed by multidisciplinary assessments at speech and hearing centers and subsequent early treatment if needed. Percentages of children who attended a special school, repeated a class because of language problems, and scored low on standardized language tests, in intention-to-screen analyses, were measured. At age 8, 2.7% in the intervention group and 3.7% in the control group attended a special school, 6.1% vs 4.9% had repeated a grade, 8.8% vs 9.7% had deficient oral language performance, 4.7% vs 4.7% had deficient reading, and 2.8% vs 4.2% had deficient spelling. Screening toddlers for language delays reduces the number of children who require special education and leads to improved language performance at age 8. Nationwide implementation of the screening might be recommended.
Content may be subject to copyright.
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2006-3145
2007;120;1317Pediatrics
W. Verhoeven and Harry J. de Koning
Heleen M.E. van Agt, Heleen A. van der Stege, Hanneke de Ridder-Sluiter, Ludo T.
Effects on School Performance and Language Development at Age 8
A Cluster-Randomized Trial of Screening for Language Delay in Toddlers:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/120/6/1317.full.html
located on the World Wide Web at:
The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.
Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2007 by the American Academy
published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned,
PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
ARTICLE
A Cluster-Randomized Trial of Screening for
Language Delay in Toddlers: Effects on School
Performance and Language Development at Age 8
Heleen M. E. van Agt, MA
a
, Heleen A. van der Stege, PhD
b
, Hanneke de Ridder-Sluiter, PhD
b
, Ludo T. W. Verhoeven, PhD
c
,
Harry J. de Koning, MD, PhD
a
a
Department of Public Health, Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, Netherlands;
b
Dutch Foundation for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Child, Amsterdam, Netherlands;
c
Pedagogical and Educational Sciences, Faculty of Social Sciences, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands
Financial Disclosure: Drs Van de Stege and de Ridder-Sluiter are employed by the Dutch foundation for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Child, where the VTO language-screening instrument was created; Drs van
Agt, Verhoeven, and de Koning have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
ABSTRACT
OBJECTIVE. The goal of this study was to assess the effects of screening and early
treatment of preschool children for language delay on language development and
school performance at age 8.
METHODS. A cluster-randomized, controlled trial and follow-up study of 55 child
health centers in 6 geographic regions were conducted from January 2002 to
September 2005. A total of 9419 children who were from the general population
and aged 15 months at entry were studied. School type end school progress was
known for 5406 (57.4%) children. In the intervention group, a structured screen-
ing instrument was conducted twice (at ages 15/18 and 24 months), and usual care
was applied in the control group. The screening instrument consisted of a uniform
set of questions for the parents and test elements for the child. A positive screen
result was followed by multidisciplinary assessments at speech and hearing centers
and subsequent early treatment if needed. Percentages of children who attended
a special school, repeated a class because of language problems, and scored low on
standardized language tests, in intention-to-screen analyses, were measured.
RESULTS. At age 8, 2.7% in the intervention group and 3.7% in the control group
attended a special school, 6.1% vs 4.9% had repeated a grade, 8.8% vs 9.7% had
deficient oral language performance, 4.7% vs 4.7% had deficient reading, and
2.8% vs 4.2% had deficient spelling.
CONCLUSIONS. Screening toddlers for language delays reduces the number of children
who require special education and leads to improved language performance at age
8. Nationwide implementation of the screening might be recommended.
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/
peds.2006-3145
doi:10.1542/peds.2006-3145
Key Words
screening, language delay, preschool
children, RCT
Abbreviations
RCT—randomized, controlled trial
VTO—VroegTijdige Onderkenning
Ontwikkelingsstoornissen
RR—relative risk
CI— confidence interval
Accepted for publication Jun 19, 2007
Address correspondence to Heleen M. E. van
Agt, MA, Department of Public Health,
Erasmus MC, PO Box 2040, 3000 CA
Rotterdam, Netherlands. E-mail: h.vanagt@
erasmusmc.nl
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005;
Online, 1098-4275). Copyright © 2007 by the
American Academy of Pediatrics
PEDIATRICS Volume 120, Number 6, December 2007 1317
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
C
HILDREN’S GENERAL DEVELOPMENT is crucial. In
health care, there is much focus on the monitoring
of developmental steps in individual young children.
1
Serious problems in cognitive and/or socioemotional de-
velopment at school age or adolescence often originate
from developmental disorders in childhood, of which
language delays are the most prevalent.
2–5
In a large
Dutch sample from the open population, the prevalence
of language delays was estimated between 2.4% and
5.3% in 3-year-olds.
6
Although up to 60% of language
delays at the age of 2 to 3 years probably resolve spon-
taneously,
5
some indicate severe and long-lasting lan-
guage impairment with detrimental effects at later age.
7
Although effective treatment exists for young children
with several underlying causes of language delay,
8
it is
unclear whether systematic screening of language delay
at an early age is effective.
9
Whether screening leads to
better language performance as compared with usual
practice can be investigated only in a randomized, con-
trolled trial (RCT).
Several studies
10–14
have evaluated test characteristics
of specific screening instruments to detect language de-
lays in preschool children. Only 1 study was set up as an
RCT to evaluate the accuracy of a structured test and a
parent-led method for language screening among 582
3-year-old children
11
; however, none of these studies
evaluated the effects of screening on language perfor-
mance at later age.
In the Netherlands, on average, 85% of all 0- to
4-year-old children and their parents visit child health
centers (for free) at regular times for assessing the child’s
general development by physicians, including language
development. This article describes the effects of a spe-
cific screening instrument in a cluster-RCT among 9419
children on school performance and linguistic skills at
age 8 in the Netherlands. At this age, children in Dutch
schools should normally be in grade 2 of primary school,
having had 1 year of reading education. If children are
not capable of attending a regular primary school be-
cause of learning, behavioral, or health problems, then
special education services are offered. Children with se-
vere language delays run a high risk of being placed in
special schools or having to repeat a grade.
12
We hypoth-
esized that the screening would result in a reduction in
the proportion of children who need to attend a special
school, repeat a year in regular school, or have scores in
the lowest percentile of several standardized language
tests. We reported previously
6
that the screening did lead
to more earlier diagnoses and treatments in the first 3
years of life, as compared with a control group.
METHODS
Methods of this cluster-RCT have been published be-
fore.
6
Individual randomization is the ideal design, but
we used a cluster trial design to avoid biased results
(induced by the alternating use of the specific screening
instrument for the intervention children and standard
monitoring for the control subjects by 1 physician).
Child health care physicians were the units for random-
ization, and children were the units for analysis.
Randomization
We asked the child heath care physicians to identify low-
and high-socioeconomic neighborhoods within their re-
gion. Within the identified socioeconomic strata, each
physician was then allocated a number and randomly
classified by rolling dice by the trial’s manager as alter-
nately intervention or control physician. Physicians in
the control group performed the usual monitoring sys-
tem, which is based on physicians’ observation and on
questioning the parents in a limited manner without
clearcut referral criteria.
15
The child health center phy-
sicians in the intervention group were trained to use the
specific screening instrument.
Screening and Diagnosis
The VroegTijdige Onderkenning Ontwikkelingsstoornis-
sen (VTO; early detection of developmental disorders)
Language Screening instrument consisted of questions
about the language production, language reception, and
interaction of children in the age group 12 to 29 months
(Appendixes 1 and 2).
6,16,17
With this instrument, the
child health center physician in the intervention group
interviewed the parents who routinely visited the child
health center with their child, which took 5 minutes.
The complete screening procedure embraced a screening
interview at 15/18 months as well as at 24 months. The
final score was obtained by adding the scores on both
screenings, which ranged between 0 and 7. When chil-
dren had a final score of 2, they were referred to the
general practitioner for additional assessment at a speech
and hearing center to confirm language delay and, if so,
to assess the underlying causes.
6
This was done by a
uniform protocol of multidisciplinary diagnostic proce-
dures in all regions, which included assessment of lan-
guage production, language reception, hearing, cogni-
tive development, and socioemotional development.
6
The cutoff score of the VTO Language Screening instru-
ment was obtained in a pilot study by using the Reynell
language comprehension test as gold standard. A cutoff
score of 2 was found to be the most optimal point,
allocating 80% of the children as having either true-
positive or true-negative results. More details on the
validity of the VTO, which was proved to be satisfactory,
have been published before.
6
Follow-up
Follow-up was aimed at all children in both intervention
and control groups who according to their date of birth
should normally now have been in grade 2 of primary
school, in the school years 2001–2002 and 2002–2003,
respectively. In the Netherlands, there are separate spe-
1318 VAN AGT et al
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
cial schools for children with learning problems, for chil-
dren with a visual disability, for children who are deaf
and hard of hearing and for children with severe speech
difficulties, for children with mental and/or physical dis-
abilities, and for children with behavioral difficulties. In
January of each school year, we informed all primary
schools and special schools in the regions of the study
population about the follow-up project. Then we in-
formed the parents by mail and asked them for their
written consent to obtain data on their child’s linguistic
abilities from the school and teacher (plus name and
address details of the school and teacher). Two months
later, the parents received a questionnaire and, if neces-
sary, a reminder for the informed consent. The parents
were asked a number of detailed questions about the
history of language problems (age and type of problem)
and related treatment. The parent questionnaire con-
tained some questions about background characteristics
(number of older brothers/sisters, foreign language spo-
ken at home, educational level of parents, and whether
the child had a physical or mental disability/illness),
which are known predictors for language develop-
ment.
18,19
Schools received a list with the names of the chil-
dren for which we obtained informed consent from
the parents. At the end of the school year, the teachers
of these children were asked to fill out a questionnaire
and to supply the scores on a set of widely known
specified standardized language assessment tests. In
case these specified tests were not (yet) applied in a
particular school, we included the relevant test mate-
rial in the mail parcel and asked the teacher whether
he or she was willing to administer the test(s) to the
child. We also included a book for the classroom and
a theater voucher as presents for all teachers (inde-
pendent of the response). Reminders were sent to all
parents and teachers who did not respond in the pre-
vious periods.
Participants
In 6 regions in the Netherlands, 4 regions in the south, 1
in the midsouth, and in 1 large city in the west, 55
physicians of child health centers were randomly as-
signed. Inclusion started in May 1995 in the 4 regions in
the south, in March 1996 in the midsouth region, and in
August 1996 in the city in the west. The participating
children were those who were between the age of 15 to
24 months in the given inclusion period and were living
within the area of the intervention physicians’ health
care location and those who were living within the area
of the control physician (n 11 440).
Primary Outcome Measures
The primary outcome measures (at the individual level)
were school performance and linguistic ability at age 8.
School performance embraced school career, defined by
the type of school the child was currently attending, and
functioning at school, assessed by determining whether
the child had repeated a grade. We assessed both oral
and written linguistic abilities of the children by means
of standardized assessment tests and the independent
(blinded toward arms) judgment of teachers.
School Type and Grade
Data on school type and grade were derived from the
parent report on school address details. School type was
determined by linking name and address details of the
school to the Dutch central registry system, in which
each school is allocated a unique number and catego-
rized by school type. Children were assumed to have
repeated a grade when their grade was below grade 2
(group 4 in the Netherlands) of primary school, which
was the expected grade given the age of the included
children.
Standardized Assessment Measures
Teachers were asked to supply the scores of the individ-
ual children with respect to receptive and productive
oral and written language usage, consisting of a vocab-
ulary test
20
; spelling
21
and reading comprehension
22
tests,
which can be administered at group level; and sentence
construction
23
and technical reading
24
tests, which are to
be administered for each child individually. These out-
comes are part of the national pupil monitoring system,
which is widely used by teachers in Dutch schools to
follow the school progress of individual children in pri-
mary school. Each test comprises separate units designed
for specific measuring moments in the school year.
Norm scores consist of 5 levels, which are based on the
scores of these tests on the specified measuring moments
in a national sample (A: 25% highest scores; B: 25% just
above the average score; C: 25% just below the average
score; D: 15% far below the average score; and E: 10%
lowest scores).
Teacher and Parent Questionnaires
Apart from the standardized tests, the teacher and par-
ent questionnaires also included questions with respect
to oral and written linguistic abilities and learning. In
addition, the teacher was asked about the child’s future
development (“Do you think that in the future the child
would develop in a normal way?”).
Secondary Outcome Measures
The secondary outcome measure (at the individual
level) was the frequency of occurrence of (past) treat-
ment to spur the child’s language development, as re-
ported by the parent. Interventions for language diffi-
culties may take many forms because of the broad range
of problems as well as the broad range of underlying
causes.
5
To assess the number of children treated for
language problems in both intervention and control
PEDIATRICS Volume 120, Number 6, December 2007 1319
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
group, we therefore asked the parents the following
question: “Has there ever been anything done to spur on
your child’s language development?” Children were
identified as treated for a language problem when par-
ents confirmed the question by answering 1ofthe
following: “Yes, treatment by speech and language ther-
apist/ear, nose, and throat specialist/remedial teacher/
physiotherapist/psychologist/social worker.” In addition,
parents were asked to report the age around which their
child had been treated.
Sample Size
Under conditions of usual care, we estimated that
2.5% of children would be having serious language
problems at 8 years (as defined by attending special
education). On the basis of the estimated treatment ef-
fect sizes from Law et al,
5
the observed proportions of
different types of language delays among the children
whose screening was positive at age 2, and the number
of children who as a result of the screening would be
treated for language delay,
6
we estimated that we could
reduce the percentage of serious language delays at age
8 with 20% in the intervention arm, for a significance
level of 5% (1-sided), a power of 80%, and equal allo-
cation. For a trial with randomization of individual chil-
dren to be able to detect such a reduction, a minimum of
2925 children in total would need to be recruited. Be-
cause we used cluster randomization, we required a
larger sample size to compensate for this design effect.
The formula 1 [(m 1) R], where m the number
ot children per cluster, R s
2
b/(s
2
b s
2
w), the intra-
cluster correlation coefficient, is used where s
2
b is the
variance between clusters and s
2
w is the variance within
clusters.
25
On the basis of the additional assumptions of
an estimated intracluster correlation coefficient of 0.005
and an average of 110 eligible and participating children
in each cluster [1 (110 1) 0.005 1.55], we
would need 1.55 2925 4534 children in total, which
is at least 20 clusters in each trial arm.
Analyses
Comparisons were made between intervention and con-
trol groups (intention-to-screen analysis) and between
children who completed the full screening procedure
(screened at age 15/18 months as well as 24 months)
and children who had never been screened by VTO (in
this comparison, we excluded children who were
screened at 1 age only). The primary outcomes were put
to binary variables: regular education/special education,
repeating a grade (yes/no), oral and written linguistic
abilities according to standardized tests (E level/higher
than E level), and normal future development (yes/no)
according to the teacher. We analyzed the primary out-
come variables by multilevel analysis with 2 levels (clus-
ter and child) by using EGRET 2.0.1
26
for logistic regres-
sion with distinguishable binomial random effect. To
adjust for possible regional differences, we subsequently
included region in the model. Although the trial was
designed with a 1-sided hypothesis,
27
we report the re-
sults for 2-sided 5% tests for the primary outcomes as
well to follow statistical convention.
RESULTS
Figure 1 shows the flow of children in clusters through
the trial. In all, 28 child health center physicians were
allocated to the intervention group and 27 physicians to
the control group. Thirty-six physicians were not en-
rolled because of either the very small numbers of chil-
dren in their care or their inability to meet requirements
for participation. During the follow-up period, 15% of
the cohort was not reachable as a result of a change of
address. The parents of a total of 9419 children were
asked for consent, 5424 (57.6%) of whom agreed; the
parents of 5406 children supplied usable information on
school type, and the teachers of 4329 children and the
parents of 4281 children completed detailed question-
naires. The response (written consent) in low-socioeco-
nomic neighborhoods was 53.7% (1447 of 2695),
whereas the response in middle- and high-socioeco-
nomic neighborhoods was 58.9% (3763 of 6388) and
63.9% (214 of 335; P .000), respectively. In low-
socioeconomic neighborhoods, the response in the in-
tervention and control groups was 55.6% and 51.6%
(P .015), in middle-socioeconomic neighborhoods was
59.4% and 58.3% (P .537), and in high-socioeco-
nomic neighborhoods was 71.4% and 56.9% (P .006),
respectively. Baseline and follow-up characteristics of
clusters and children were similar between arms (Table
1). Only between regions were there some differences in
the number of clusters and children.
Before the age of 2 (start of screening), there were no
differences in the cumulative percentages of reported
treatments between the intervention and control groups
(Table 2). Before age 3, 3.5% of the children in the
intervention group and 2.4% in the control group had
been treated to spur language development (P .069).
Before age 5, the percentage of children who were ever
treated was significantly higher in the intervention
group than in the control group: 10.8% vs 8.6% (P
.024). Before the age of 9, 26.5% of the children in the
intervention group and 23.7% in the control group had
been treated to spur language development (P .054).
The intention-to-treat analyses revealed that, in children
who were allocated to the intervention arm, the relative
risk (RR) for special school attendance was 0.71 and the
RR for the lowest level of the spelling test was 0.68,
calculated according to logistic regression with distin-
guishable binomial random effect, which takes cluster
randomization into account (Table 3). After adjustments
for regional differences, the RRs were 0.70 (95% confi-
dence interval [CI]: 0.49 –1.02; P .063, P .032 for
1-sided testing) and 0.66 (95% CI: 0.43–1.01; P .054,
1320 VAN AGT et al
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
P .027 for 1-sided testing), respectively. For the other
outcomes, no significant differences were found. In the
group of children who underwent the complete screen-
ing procedure, the RR for special school attendance was
0.75 (95% CI: 0.62– 0.91; P .003) and the RR for
lowest level of oral language performance was 0.74
(95% CI: 0.62– 0.90; P .002, adjusted for regional
differences; Table 4). For the other outcomes, no signif-
icant differences were found.
DISCUSSION
We have shown that early detection of language delays
in toddlers at child health care centers by means of a
specific screening instrument followed by early treat-
ment can reduce the percentage of children who attend
a special school at 8 years by 30%. At the same time, the
number of children with spelling problems was reduced
by 33%. Screened children seemed to have fewer prob-
lems with oral linguistic skills. The screening led to more
treatments and support in the preschool period.
Parent report on school type and grade proved to be
reliable; only 1% of the school addresses were found to
be incorrect. With the help of the teachers, the children’s
linguistic abilities were measured by standardized tests,
which were validated and proved to be reliable in pre-
vious research.
28
Only a small proportion of teachers
were not familiar with the tests; however, this propor-
tion did not differ between the intervention and control
groups, so this could not have been a potential bias.
Importantly, none of the teachers knew whether the
child belonged to the intervention or control group.
Special school attendance can be considered to be a valid
91 eligible clusters (child health center
physicians) N = 91
36 clusters not enrolled
(too-small clusters or
inability to participate)
Lost to follow-up
Moved (n = 971 [15.0%])
Refused (n = 190 [2.9%])
Lost to follow-up
Moved (n = 752 [15.2%])
Refused (n = 108 [2.2%])
55 clusters randomized with
children aged 15 mo at entry
Analyzed
Mean (SD) age (y): 8.1 (0.4)
School type (n = 3118)
Class (n = 3084)
Oral language tests (n = 1270)
Reading tests (n = 1844)
Spelling tests (n = 1728)
Invited to participate at age 8 y (n = 5324) Invited to participate at age 8 y (n = 4095)
Analyzed
Mean (SD) age (y): 8.1 (0.4)
School type (n = 2288)
Class (n = 2250)
Oral language tests (n = 925)
Reading tests (n = 1328)
Spelling tests (n = 1225)
Intervention group (28 clusters with
6485 children)
Specific screening at 15/18 and 24
mo (n = 3776)
Specific screening at 15/18 or 24
mo (n = 560)
No specific screening/usual care
(n = 2149) (for logistic reasons, holidays,
etc
)
Control group (27 clusters with 4955
children)
No specific screening/usual care
(n = 4955)
Written consent (n = 3127 [58.8%])
Teacher questionnaires (n = 2491)
Parent questionnaires (n = 2474)
Written consent (n = 2297 [56.1%])
Teacher questionnaires (n = 1838)
Parent questionnaires (n = 1807)
FIGURE 1
Flowchart of child health physicians (clusters) and children
through the trial.
PEDIATRICS Volume 120, Number 6, December 2007 1321
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
measure of school performance, because only children
with severe educational problems attend such schools in
the Netherlands. It should be noted, however, that prob-
lems other than language might be the only reason for
special school entrance (eg, behavioral problems); there-
fore, we took account of possible differences in the re-
ferral policy for special education by adjusting for re-
gional differences.
At 3 years of age, the VTO screening had been found
to result in larger proportions of children with diagnosed
and/or treated language problems.
6
In this study, parents
in the intervention group reported significantly more
TABLE 1 Characteristics of Child Health Care Physicians (Clusters) and Children: Initially Recruited and
Follow-up at Age 8
Characteristic Intervention
Group
Control
Group
Total P
Initially recruited
Clusters
Total No. of clusters 28 27 55
No. of children per cluster, mean (SD) 224 (173) 184 (133) 204 (155)
Region, No. of clusters (children)
South part (south) 4 (1153) 4 (1141) 8 (2294)
South part (mid) 5 (1537) 4 (957) 9 (2494)
South part (southwest) 3 (824) 2 (466) 5 (1290)
South part (southeast) 3 (1166) 4 (1084) 7 (2250)
Midsouth 5 (1409) 8 (1210) 13 (2619)
Large city in west part 8 (396) 5 (97) 13 (493)
Socioeconomic neighborhood, No. of clusters
Low 5 6 11
Middle 21 20 41
High 2 2 4
Children
Total 6485 4755 11440
Male, % 50.1 52.0 50.9 .045
Follow-up age 8
Total 3127 2297 5424
Age, mean (SD), y 8.1 (0.4) 8.1 (0.4) 8.1 (0.4) .862
Male, % 49.9 50.1 50.0 .926
Parental questionnaire
Physical disability/illness, % 6.8 7.8 7.2 .226
Maternal education, % .196
Low 14.6 16.6 15.4
Intermediate 59.9 59.2 59.6
High 25.5 24.3 25.0
Paternal education, % .921
Low 18.7 18.8 18.8
Intermediate 45.5 44.8 45.2
High 35.8 36.3 36.0
Foreign language in family, % 9.5 10.7 10.0 .208
Children in family, n (SD) 2.31 (0.84) 2.26 (0.82) 2.28 (0.83) .053
TABLE 2 Children Being Treated Per Age (Parent Questionnaire)
Age at Which Child Was Treated
to Spur Language
Development, y
a
Intervention (n 2192),
Cumulative n (%)
Control (n 1601),
Cumulative n (%)
Total (N 3793),
Cumulative n (%)
P
2 26 (1.2) 20 (1.2) 46 (1.2) .881
3 76 (3.5) 39 (2.4) 115 (3.0) .069
4 141 (6.4) 85 (5.3) 226 (6.0) .165
5 237 (10.8) 137 (8.6) 374 (9.9) .024
6 331 (15.1) 214 (13.4) 545 (14.4) .134
7 452 (20.6) 296 (18.5) 748 (19.7) .107
8 557 (25.4) 364 (22.7) 921 (24.3) .060
9 581 (26.5) 380 (23.7) 961 (25.3) .054
a
Has there ever been anything done to spur on your child’s language development (by speech language therapist; ear, nose, and throat specialist, remedial teacher, physiotherapist, psychologist,
or social worker)?
1322 VAN AGT et al
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
treatments related to language development in their
child’s preschool period, in particular around the time of
school entrance. Given the time period, parents might be
remembering their child’s being treated for language
problems rather than the occurrence of (past) language
problems; however, these results must be evaluated with
caution, because we did not collect detailed data on
language problems from specialists as we did previously.
6
The parents of 57% of all eligible children participated in
the follow-up study. Given that we addressed an open
population and asked to sign for consent, the response is
moderate but acceptable. The response (proportion of writ-
ten consent) was only slightly lower in low-socioeconomic
neighborhoods than in middle- and high-socioeconomic
neighborhoods, so we think that the sample is still repre-
sentative of the total population. The response of the
schools and teachers was very high (82%). Although small
but significant differences in response between interven-
tion and control groups within low- and high-socioeco-
nomic neighborhoods were found, this could not have
been a potential bias of the results because we did not find
any significant differences in the distribution of educational
level of the parents between the intervention and control
groups. In addition, there were no substantial differences in
loss to follow-up and nonresponse between the study arms
and hence could not have biased the observed differences
in outcomes between the intervention and control groups
either.
The population visiting the child health centers in the
Netherlands is not a selected group: 95% of all parents
visit these centers during the first year of their child’s life.
6
Only children who have severe disabilities diagnosed at
birth and need specialized care during their first year are
seen by pediatricians and rarely visit a child health center.
Most studies
11–14,29,30
on early detection of language
problems concluded that it is possible to identify children
with language problems at an early stage in the preschool
period, sometimes by 2-step screening methods. These
studies focused on the test characteristics of the screening
instrument. Comparisons across studies are difficult be-
cause there is no generally accepted definition of language
problems or gold standard, and methods of assessment
differ.
5
The most valid method would be clinical examina-
tion; however, this is not feasible in population-based stud-
ies. Apart from the sensitivity and specificity of the instru-
ment, it is important for economic reasons to consider the
proportion of children who have positive screening results
and require additional assessment. Previously, we
6
found
that the sensitivity of the VTO instrument, resulting in
2.4% of positive screenings, was between 25% and 52%,
depending on the assumed prevalence of language prob-
lems, which was based on either specialist or parent report.
Some studies
31
found higher sensitivity measures at the
expense of high referral rates. One RCT reported that the
sensitivity of a structured test and a parent-led method
was, respectively 66% and 56%
11
; however, the applica-
TABLE 3 Primary Outcome Measures at Age 8: Intervention and Control Groups (Intention-to-Screen Analysis)
Intervention Group,
N (n %)
Control Group,
N (n %)
Total, N (n %) RR 95% CI P Intracluster
Correlation
2-Sided
Testing
1-Sided
Testing
In special school 3118 (83 2.7) 2288 (85 3.7) 5406 (168 3.1) 0.71
a
0.48–1.04 .076 .038 0.0031
0.70
b
0.49–1.02 .063 .032 0.0028
Repeating a grade 3084 (443 14.4) 2250 (318 14.1) 5334 (761 14.3) 0.99
a
0.81–1.21 .905 .453 0.0000
0.99
b
0.81–1.22 .935 .468 0.0000
Repeating a grade because of language problems in 2401 (146 6.1) 1721 (84 4.9) 4122 (230 5.6) 1.26
a
0.89–1.80 .196 .098 0.0070
regular primary school (parent questionnaire) 1.28
b
0.89–1.84 .189 .095 0.0072
Below 10th percentile of oral language tests 1270 (112 8.8) 925 (90 9.7) 2195 (202 9.2) 0.88
a
0.63–1.23 .464 .232 0.0043
0.89
b
0.64–1.24 .248 .124 0.0000
Below 10th percentile of reading tests in grade 2 1844 (86 4.7) 1328 (62 4.7) 3172 (148 4.7) 1.00
a
0.72–1.40 .994 .497 0.0000
1.00
b
0.71–1.40 .944 .497 0.0000
Below 10th percentile of spelling tests in grade 2 1728 (48 2.8) 1225 (52 4.2) 2953 (100 3.4) 0.68
a
0.41–1.13 .138 .069 0.0064
0.66
b
0.43–1.01 .054 .027 0.0000
Do you think that in the future the child would 1769 (201 11.4) 1311 (175 13.3) 3080 (376 12.2) 0.83
a
0.67–1.03 .096 .048 0.0000
develop in a normal way (teacher “no”) 0.83
b
0.67–1.03 .094 .047 0.0000
a
Calculated according to logistic regression with distinguishable binomial random effect, which takes cluster randomization into account.
b
Calculated according to logistic regression with distinguishable binomial random effect, which takes cluster randomization into account, adjusted for region.
PEDIATRICS Volume 120, Number 6, December 2007 1323
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
bility of these results to the general population is question-
able because the sample came from a deprived area. More-
over, according to Laing et al,
11
the low uptake and high
attrition rate had probably biased the results toward over-
estimating the performance of the screening test. Laing et
al did not recommend formal screening on language prob-
lems, because they considered the sensitivity of the struc-
tured test not to be substantially higher than that of the
parent-led method; however, we think that the sensitivity
of the VTO screening instrument pertains to a realistic and
acceptable figure, given the low referral rate and the young
age of the children.
6
Most important, this is the first study
to evaluate the effects of an early language screening pro-
gram in an RCT, to assess whether the specific screening
procedure leads to extra diagnosed and/or treated language
problems and, most important, to extra “health” benefits at
later age as compared with usual practice.
9
After all, the
results of an RCT allow inferences about causal relation-
ships between the screening and the effects.
CONCLUSIONS
We have shown that an early language screening pro-
gram including a protocol of multidisciplinary diagnostic
procedures can reduce special school entrance and lin-
guistic problems. Nationwide implementation of the in-
tervention as part of routine monitoring of children’s
general development can be recommended.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study was funded by the Health Care Insurance
Board (College voor zorgverzekeringen).
We are grateful to the schools, teachers, parents,
and children who participated in this study; Anna
Brandt, Sandra Laurier, Toon Juttmann, Marianne
Quak, and Dick Slof for assistance with the data col-
lection and computer input of the data; Jan Alberts for
designing the procedures for data management and
data processing; and Carin Reep-van den Bergh, MA,
and Paul van der Maas, MD, PhD, for general and
methodologic support.
APPENDIX 1 Items of the VTO Language Screening Instrument
17
Age, mo Items
15 1. Word production
Animals, people, toys, food/drink
2. Language comprehension
Going out, eating, where is the ball, put the
doll to bed, get a spoon, let the doll drink
3. Understanding each other
Can clearly express her/his need for food/drink,
can clearly express her/his need for help
4. Playing (parent-child interaction)
How often playing together, what is favorite
play, can clearly express that she/he wants to
play, playing alone
18 1. Word production
Animals, people, toys, food/drink
2. Playing (parent-child interaction)
How often playing together, what is favorite
play, can clearly express that she/he wants to
play, playing alone
3. Language comprehension
Getting 3 objects: sock, spoon, small block
24 1. Word production
Animals, people, toys, food/drink
2. Playing (parent-child interaction)
How often playing together, what is favorite
play, playing alone
3. Language comprehension
Body parts: eyes, mouth, belly, feet, hair, hand
TABLE 4 Primary Outcome Measures at Age 8: Screened With Specific Instrument (Completed Screens) and Never Screened With Specific
Instrument
Screened Not Screened Total RR 95% CI P Intracluster
Correlation
2-Sided
Testing
1-Sided
Testing
In special school 1980 (41 2.1) 3142 (114 3.6) 5122 (115 3.0) 0.75
a
0.62–0.92 .005 .003 0.0044
0.75
b
0.62–0.91 .003 .002 0.0024
Repeating a grade 1961 (265 13.5) 3092 (447 14.5) 5053 (712 14.1) 0.95
a
0.86–1.04 .256 .129 0.0000
0.95
b
0.86–1.04 .282 .141 0.0000
Repeating a grade because of language 1585 (92 5.8) 2469 (142 5.8) 4054 (234 5.8) 0.98
a
0.84–1.15 .821 .411 0.0051
problems (in regular primary school) 0.98
b
0.84–1.15 .830 .415 0.0056
Below 10th percentile of oral language 817 (55 6.7) 1271 (137 10.8) 2088 (192 9.2) 0.74
a
0.62–0.90 .002 .001 0.0083
tests 0.74
b
0.62–0.90 .002 .001 0.0070
Below 10th percentile of reading tests 1188 (55 4.6) 1829 (88 4.8) 3017 (143 4.7) 0.98
a
0.82–1.16 .819 .410 0.0000
in grade 2 0.98
b
0.82–1.16 .791 .396 0.0000
Below 10th percentile of spelling tests 1127 (30 2.7) 1685 (65 3.9) 2812 (95 3.4) 0.87
a
0.68–1.12 .287 .144 0.0099
in grade 2 0.84
b
0.66–1.08 .175 .088 0.0032
Do you think that in the future the child 1124 (221 12.3) 1793 (134 11.9) 2917 (355 12.2) 0.98
a
0.88–1.10 .745 .373 0.0000
would develop in a normal way
(answer teacher “no”)
0.98
b
0.88–1.10 .770 .385 0.0000
a
Calculated according to logistic regression with distinguishable binomial random effect, which takes cluster randomization into account.
b
Calculated according to logistic regression with distinguishable binomial random effect, which takes cluster randomization into account, adjusted for region.
1324 VAN AGT et al
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
REFERENCES
1. First LR, Palfrey JS. The infant or young child with develop-
mental delay. N Engl J Med. 1994;330:478 483
2. Felsenfeld S, Broen PA, McGue M. A 28-year follow-up of
adults with a history of moderate phonological disorder: edu-
cational and occupational results. J Speech Hear Res. 1994;37:
1341–1353
3. Stothard SE, Snowling MJ, Bishop DV, Chipchase BB, Kaplan
CA. Language-impaired preschoolers: a follow-up into adoles-
cence. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1998;41:407– 418
4. Johnson CJ, Beitchman JH, Young A, et al. Fourteen-year
follow-up of children with and without speech/language
impairments: speech/language stability and outcomes. J Speech
Lang Hear Res. 1999;42:744 –760
5. Law J, Boyle J, Harris F, Harkness A, Nye C. Screening for
speech and language delay: a systematic review of the litera-
ture. Health Technol Assess. 1998;2:1–184
6. de Koning HJ, de Ridder-Sluiter JG, van Agt HM, et al. A
cluster-randomised trial of screening for language disorders in
toddlers. J Med Screen. 2004;11:109 –116
7. Miniscalco C, Nygren G, Hagberg B, Kadesjo B, Gillberg C.
Neuropsychiatric and neurodevelopmental outcome of chil-
dren at age 6 and 7 years who screened positive for language
problems at 30 months. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:
361–366
8. Law J, Garrett Z, Nye C. The efficacy of treatment for children
with developmental speech and language delay/disorder: a
meta-analysis. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2004;47:924 –943
9. Nelson HD, Nygren P, Walker M, Panoscha R. Screening for
speech and language delay in preschool children: systematic
evidence review for the US Preventive Services Task Force.
Pediatrics. 2006;117(2). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/
content/full/117/2/e298
10. Klee T, Carson DK, Gavin WJ, Hall L, Kent A, Reece S. Con-
current and predictive validity of an early language screening
program. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1998;41:627– 641
11. Laing GJ, Law J, Levin A, Logan S. Evaluation of a structured
test and a parent led method for screening for speech and
language problems: prospective population based study. BMJ.
2002;325:1152
12. Westerlund M, Sundelin C. Can severe language disability be
identified in three-year-olds? Evaluation of a routine screening
procedure. Acta Paediatr. 2000;89:94 –100
13. Mattsson CM, Marild S, Pehrsson NG. Evaluation of a lan-
guage-screening programme for 2.5-year-olds at child health
centres in Sweden. Acta Paediatr. 2001;90:339 –344
14. McGinty C. An investigation into aspects of the Mayo early
language screening test. Child Care Health Dev. 2000;26:
111–128
15. Brouwers-de Jong EA, Burgmeijer RJ, Laurent de Angulo MS,
eds. Monitoring development at the Child Health Center: Manual for
the Revised Van Wiechen Examination [in Dutch]. Assen,
Germany: Van Gorcum; 1996
16. de Ridder-Sluiter JG. Early detection of communicative disor-
ders. XXV International Congress of Psychology; 19 –24 July 1992;
Brussels, Belgium: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
17. de Ridder-Sluiter JG. Early Detection of Developmental Delays in
Communication Abilities [in Dutch]. Leiden, Netherlands: Rijks-
universiteit Leiden; 1990
18. Bishop DVM. The biological basis of specific language impair-
ment. In: Fletcher P, Hall D, eds. Specific Speech and Language
Disorders in Children: Correlates, Characteristics and Outcomes. Lon-
don, United Kingdom: Whurr Publishers; 1991:2–17
19. Stanton-Chapman TL, Chapman DA, Bainbridge NL, Scott KG.
Identification of early risk factors for language impairment. Res
Dev Disabil. 2002;23:390405
20. Verhoeven LTW. Vocabulary Test, Manual [in Dutch]. Arnhem,
Netherlands: Cito; 1996
21. Verhoeven LTW. Test of Progress in Spelling Skills, Manual [in
Dutch]. Arnhem, Netherlands: Cito; 1997
22. Verhoeven LTW. Reading Comprehension, Manual [in Dutch].
Arnhem, Netherlands: Cito, 1997
23. Verhoeven LTW, Vermeer A. Language Test All Children, Assess-
ment of Oral Language Skills Dutch for Children in Group 1– 4 [in
Dutch]. Arnhem, Netherlands: Citogroep, 2001
24. Verhoeven LTW. Three-Minutes Test (Technical Reading) [in
Dutch]. Arnhem, Netherlands: Cito; 1997
25. Donner A. Some aspects of the design and analysis of cluster
randomization trials. Appl Stat. 1998;47:95–113
26. Diez Roux AV. A glossary for multilevel analysis. J Epidemiol
Community Health. 2005;56:588–594
27. Bland JM, Altman DG. One and two sided tests of significance.
BMJ. 1994;309:248
28. Droop M, Verhoeven L. Language proficiency and reading
ability in first- and second-language learners. Read Res Q. 2003;
38:78 –103
29. Klee T, Pearce K, Carson DK. Improving the positive predictive
value of screening for developmental language disorder. J
Speech Lang Hear Res. 2000;43:821– 833
30. Stott CM, Merricks MJ, Bolton PF, Goodyer IM. Screening for
speech and language disorders: the reliability, validity and
accuracy of the General Language Screen. Int J Lang Commun
Disord. 2002;37:133–151
31. Westerlund M. Relationship between a global rating of speech
ability at the age of 3 yrs and a phonological screening 1 yr
later: a prospective field study. Scand J Caring Sci. 2001;15(3):
222–227
APPENDIX 2 Question 1 of the Screening Instrument at 24 Months
of Age
17
Let us start with the little sounds and words of (name of the child)
Thinking about the past period, can you tell me:
a. How does (name of the child) call people in his/her proximity?
b. What does (name of the child) say if (name of the child) wants
something to eat or drink?
c. What does (name of the child) say if (name of the child) wants
to play with toys?
Possible answer categories
Sentence
Word or name
Calls by sound or indicates; for instance, br car
Daddy, mama
Pointing out with sound
a
Pointing out without sound
a
Not indicating anything
b
a
If the parent’s answer is “pointing out,” then ask, “Does (name of the child) make any sounds
while pointing?”
b
Does (name of the child) make little sounds? If yes, then what are these sounds like?
PEDIATRICS Volume 120, Number 6, December 2007 1325
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2006-3145
2007;120;1317Pediatrics
W. Verhoeven and Harry J. de Koning
Heleen M.E. van Agt, Heleen A. van der Stege, Hanneke de Ridder-Sluiter, Ludo T.
Effects on School Performance and Language Development at Age 8
A Cluster-Randomized Trial of Screening for Language Delay in Toddlers:
Services
Updated Information &
tml
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/120/6/1317.full.h
including high resolution figures, can be found at:
References
tml#ref-list-1
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/120/6/1317.full.h
at:
This article cites 21 articles, 7 of which can be accessed free
Citations
tml#related-urls
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/120/6/1317.full.h
This article has been cited by 9 HighWire-hosted articles:
Subspecialty Collections
ctice
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/office_pra
Office Practice
the following collection(s):
This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in
Permissions & Licensing
ml
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xht
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
Reprints
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml
Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2007 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published,
PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly
by guest on June 5, 2013pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from
... Of the 19 studies with moderate or strong ratings, only four were at the universal tier (Christakis et al., 2007;Goldfeld et al., 2011Goldfeld et al., , 2012McGillion et al., 2017;van Agt et al., 2007). McGillion et al. (2017) and Goldfeld et al. (2011Goldfeld et al. ( , 2012 found little evidence of a treatment effect at follow-up (see Table 19.2). ...
... Christakis et al. (2007), which involved the distribution of building blocks to parents, found positive effects of the intervention on child language acquisition for children from low-and middle-income households but not high-income households. An RCT examining the effects of screening for language difficulties found small effects, with a reduction in incidence of low language abilities in the intervention group (6.7 per cent) compared to the control group (10.8 per cent) at age 8 years (van Agt et al., 2007). Of the two studies reporting null effects, McGillion et al. (2017) reported a positive effect for parent-reported vocabulary in the low SES families at 4 and 7 months post-intervention, but effects were not sustained 12 months later. ...
... The study by Glogowska et al. (2000Glogowska et al. ( , 2002 found some evidence for the effectiveness of speech and language therapy, with an improvement in auditory comprehension in favour of the intervention group, measured using a standardised direct assessment of language. The study by van Agt et al. (2007) showed those children in the control group performed worse on oral language tests at age 8 compared to the intervention group. ...
Chapter
The majority of children acquire language effortlessly but approximately 10% of all children find it difficult especially in the early or preschool years with consequences for many aspects of their subsequent development and experience: literacy, social skills, educational qualifications, mental health and employment. With contributions from an international team of researchers, this book is the first to draw together a series of new analyses of data related to children's language development, primarily from large-scale nationally representative population studies, and to bring a public health perspective to the field. The book begins with a section on factors influencing the patterns of language development. A second section explores continuity and change in language development over time. The third explores the impact on individuals with developmental language disorders (DLD), the effectiveness of available interventions, and broader issues about the need for equity in the delivery of services to those with DLD.
... *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 and ***p < 0.001 compared with Mainstream Group.specialised services.[20][21][22] Previous research has focused on developing reference values for development14 and identifying children at risk of developmental delay.[20][21][22] ...
... services.[20][21][22] Previous research has focused on developing reference values for development14 and identifying children at risk of developmental delay.[20][21][22] Validation studies have also been performed on developmental delay in referred children, at-risk populations or diagnosed children.[15][16][17]23 ...
Article
Aim Responding to developmental delay promptly is important, as it helps children to reach their full potential. This study investigated how developmental milestones predicted primary school children with special educational needs and disabilities (SEND) at an early stage. Methods We obtained data about 36 milestones between 12 and 45 months using the Dutch Development Instrument. Development, primary school classification and background characteristics were collected from the Dutch Preventive Child Healthcare system in Utrecht from 2008 to 2016. We investigated SEND classifications and the primary schools that the children attended at 4–12 years of age. The findings include area under the curve (AUC) data. Results Data on 30 579 children in mainstream schools and 1055 children with SEND were available. Different milestones predicted SEND classifications. Fourteen milestones and parental education predicted attendance at special needs schools with smaller classes (AUC 0.913). Nine milestones, sex, migration background and parental education predicted attendance at schools for severe communication problems (AUC 0.963). Ten milestones and parental education predicted attendance at schools for severe learning difficulties (AUC 0.995). Milestones did not accurately predict attendance at schools for severe behavioural or psychiatric problems. Conclusion Milestones at 12–45 months predicted most SEND classifications at primary school age, except severe behavioural or psychiatric problems.
... Entende-se que a escola age como um ponto importante na rede de acesso dos usuários ao atendimento fonoaudiológico pois, mesmo quando não é a Contextualizando esses dados e os resultados obtidos nesse estudo, é possível inferir que boa parte das crianças encaminhadas ao fonoaudiólogo residente entre os 4 e 6 anos de idade já poderia ter recebido intervenção em fase pré-escolar. A detecção de alterações na aquisição da linguagem entre os dois e três anos reduz em 30% a necessidade de a criança realizar outros acompanhamentos terapêuticos quando atingir os oito anos de idade 26 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose to analyze the profile and access of patients who need speech-language pathology care in a Primary Health Unit (UBS). Methods a descriptive analytical study, including data obtained from a spreadsheet with information provided by patients from May 2013 to February 2020. The following variables were analyzed: gender, age at referral, speech-language pathology complaint, year of referral, year of the first visit and service, and professional responsible for the health referral, speech therapy conduct after first visit, and outcome after therapy. Categorical variables were analyzed by means of frequency and numeric variables by measures of central tendency and dispersion. For inferential statistical analysis, the Mann-Whitney test was used, considering 5% as the significance level. Results in total, 171 individuals were referred, most were males, aged 4 to 6 years, with language complaints; 73.66% were referred by professionals from the UBS, mainly by pediatricians. After the speech-language pathology assessment, 27.49% were referred to the specialty. Among the 78 individuals who started speech-language therapy at the UBS, 24.36% were discharged and 32.05% were dismissed, due to absences. A statistically significant correlation was observed when analyzing data of waiting time and non-attendance at the first speech therapy visit. Conclusion as it addresses a large part of the demands in Primary Care, speech-language pathology helps to increase solubility at this level of the health system. Lack of compliance is considered a barrier in service organization and in promoting access to other patients. Keywords: Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences; Primary Health Care; Treatment Adherence and Compliance; Health Service Accessibility
... In terms of treatment of speech disorders, the primary expressive language disorders tend to respond better to interventions than the receptive forms. It is postulated that early intervention in language delays can affect long-term outcome since it can reduce the number of children needing special education as well as improving language performance in these children about age 8 years [7,17,18]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: In the literature, there appears to be a lot of data on speech delay in children but most emanated from the western world, but little from sub-Sahara Africa especially Nigeria. This study therefore is to determine the prevalence and the risk factors of speech and language delay among children up to or less than 3years seen in our environment. Methodology: A descriptive cross sectional study carried out in the Paediatric outpatient clinic of the University of Port Harcourt teaching hospital within the period of June 2020 to September 2020. The Language Evaluation Scale Trivandrum (LEST 0-3) and Trivandrum Development Screening Chart (TDSC 0-3) are the tools used both to determine the speech and language delays and the developmental milestone in these children aged 0-3 years. Any child 3 years and below attending the clinic was included in the study; however children with apparent syndromes are excluded as well as children whose parents decline to give consent. IBM SPSS Statistics version 25 was used for data analysis. Descriptive statistics was employed in the analysis. Results: The study comprised of 157 subjects with ages ranging from 2 months to 36 months. There is a significant relationship between hearing impairment and language/speech delay (p=0.002). There was a significant relationship between the various identified delays and a history of jaundice in the neonatal period. A prevalence of 15.3% for language/speech delay was obtained. Conclusion: Language/speech delay is prevalent in our environment. Perinatal risk factors are significantly associated with these delays.
... By contextualizing these data and the results obtained in this study, most children referred to the resident speech therapist at 4 to 6 years of age could have received an intervention in the preschool phase. The detection of alterations in language acquisition between 2 and 3 years old reduces by 30% the need for other therapies when the child reaches 8 years of age 26 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose to analyze the profile and access of patients who need speech-language pathology care in a Primary Health Unit (UBS). Methods a descriptive analytical study, including data obtained from a spreadsheet with information provided by patients from May 2013 to February 2020. The following variables were analyzed: gender, age at referral, speech-language pathology complaint, year of referral, year of the first visit and service, and professional responsible for the health referral, speech therapy conduct after first visit, and outcome after therapy. Categorical variables were analyzed by means of frequency and numeric variables by measures of central tendency and dispersion. For inferential statistical analysis, the Mann-Whitney test was used, considering 5% as the significance level. Results in total, 171 individuals were referred, most were males, aged 4 to 6 years, with language complaints; 73.66% were referred by professionals from the UBS, mainly by pediatricians. After the speech-language pathology assessment, 27.49% were referred to the specialty. Among the 78 individuals who started speech-language therapy at the UBS, 24.36% were discharged and 32.05% were dismissed, due to absences. A statistically significant correlation was observed when analyzing data of waiting time and non-attendance at the first speech therapy visit. Conclusion as it addresses a large part of the demands in Primary Care, speech-language pathology helps to increase solubility at this level of the health system. Lack of compliance is considered a barrier in service organization and in promoting access to other patients. Keywords: Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences; Primary Health Care; Treatment Adherence and Compliance; Health Service Accessibility
... The brain is the foundation of the language function, which not only stores a large amount of information (e.g., memory and intelligence) but also decodes and codes language. Language is a form of cognitive expression that reflects the thinking and psychological processes of the human brain (1). Developmental language disorder (DLD) in children refers to the inability of children in the language development period to understand and express language with language symbols due to various reasons compared to normal children in the expected period. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: It is reported that the incidence of language development disorder in children at the age of 2 is as high as 17.0%. Timely discovery of the high-risk factors of language development disorder in children and early intervention can greatly reduce the incidence of language development disorder and shorten the course and condition of the patients with language development disorder. Therefore, in order to facilitate prompt diagnosis and early interventions for children with language development disorder (DLD) and improve their language ability, this study explored the influence of perinatal factors on the language development of children in Ningxia and identified the unfavorable and favorable factors that influenced language development. Methods: Children diagnosed in the General Hospital of Ningxia Medical University during 2018-2021 who met the screening criteria for DLD and practical pediatric diagnostic criteria for DLD were enrolled in this study. Perinatal factors (gestational age, weight, sex, delivery mode, maternal age, presence of intrauterine infection, asphyxia) were retrospectively analyzed. The perinatal factors affecting language development were assessed using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Results: Among 1,500 children aged 0-3, 240 cases (16.00%) had language delay. Of these, 122 were male and 118 were female. There were 115 cases of comprehension and expression disorder, 30 cases of articulation disorder, and 90 cases of mixed manifestation. And there were 194 cases with definite intrauterine and perinatal high-risk factors or neonatal diseases, accounting for 80.83% of the total number of children with language delay. Conclusions: In Ningxia, factors in the neonatal period are the main cause of DLD, followed by fetal and maternal factors. Ischemic encephalopathy is the most common factor.
Article
Istraživanja dovode u vezu niži stupanj obrazovanja majke s povećanim rizikom za razvoj jezično-govornih teškoća (Campbell i sur., 2003; Stanton-Chapman i sur., 2002). Prvi je cilj ovog istraživanja - ispitati postoje li razlike u stupnju obrazovanja roditelja djece uključene u Dječji vrtić Poliklinike SUVAG u odnosu na roditelje djece uključene u redovni sustav predškolskog odgoja i obrazovanja. Uzorak sudionika sastoji se od dvije skupine, 207 djece s prebivalištem u Gradu Zagrebu uključene u kompleksnu rehabilitaciju u Dječjem vrtiću Poliklinike SUVAG u posljednje dvije godine, te jednak broj djece uključene u 5 vrtića Grada Zagreba, izjednačenih po dobi. Drugi je cilj ovog istraživanja - ispitati u kojoj se kronološkoj dobi djeca s poremećajima komunikacije, jezika i govora uključuju u dijagnostičko-rehabilitacijski postupak u Poliklinici SUVAG s obzirom na prethodnu uključenost u redovni sustav predškolskog odgoja i obrazovanja. Svrha je ovog istraživanja - pridonijeti spoznajama o povezanosti stupnja obrazovanja roditelja djece s poremećajima komunikacije, jezika i govora, te vremenu uključivanja u dijagnostičkorehabilitacijski postupak s obzirom na prethodnu uključenost u redovni sustav predškolskog odgoja i obrazovanja. Rezultati istraživanja pokazuju da postoji statistički značajna razlika u stupnju obrazovanja roditelja djece uključene u Dječji vrtić Poliklinike SUVAG u odnosu na roditelje djece uključene u redovni sustav predškolskog odgoja i obrazovanja. Djeca koja su prethodno bila uključena u sustav predškolskog odgoja i obrazovanja, kasnije se uključuju u dijagnostičko-rehabilitacijski postupak u Poliklinici SUVAG u odnosu na djecu koja prethodno nisu pohađala redovni sustav predškolskog odgoja i obrazovanja.
Chapter
The majority of children acquire language effortlessly but approximately 10% of all children find it difficult especially in the early or preschool years with consequences for many aspects of their subsequent development and experience: literacy, social skills, educational qualifications, mental health and employment. With contributions from an international team of researchers, this book is the first to draw together a series of new analyses of data related to children's language development, primarily from large-scale nationally representative population studies, and to bring a public health perspective to the field. The book begins with a section on factors influencing the patterns of language development. A second section explores continuity and change in language development over time. The third explores the impact on individuals with developmental language disorders (DLD), the effectiveness of available interventions, and broader issues about the need for equity in the delivery of services to those with DLD.
Article
Full-text available
Background and objective Low language ability in early childhood is a strong predictor of later psychopathology as well as reduced school readiness, lower educational attainment, employment problems and involvement with the criminal justice system. Assessment of early language development is universally offered in many countries, but there has been little evaluation of assessment tools. We planned to compare the screening performance of two commonly used language assessment instruments. Methods A pragmatic diagnostic accuracy study was carried out in five areas of England comparing the performance of two screening tools (Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) and Sure Start Language Measure (SSLM)) against a reference test (Preschool Language Scale, 5th edition). Results Results were available for 357 children aged 23–30 months. The ASQ Communication Scale using optimal cut-off values had a sensitivity of 0.55, a specificity of 0.95 and positive and negative predictive values of 0.53 and 0.95, respectively. The SSLM had corresponding values of 0.83, 0.81, 0.33 and 0.98, respectively. Both screening tools performed relatively poorly in families not using English exclusively in the home. Conclusion The very widely used ASQ Communication Scale performs poorly as a language screening tool, missing over one-third of cases of low language ability. The SSLM performed better as a screening tool.
Article
Full-text available
This paper reports a longitudinal follow-up of 71 adolescents with a preschool history of speech-language impairment, originally studied by Bishop and Edmundson (1987). These children had been subdivided at 4 years into those with nonverbal IQ 2 SD below the mean (General Delay group), and those with normal nonverbal intelligence (SLI group). At age 5;6 the SLI group was subdivided into those whose language problems had resolved, and those with persistent SLI. The General Delay group was also followed up. At age 15–16 years, these children were compared with age-matched normal-language controls on a battery of tests of spoken language and literacy skills. Children whose language problems had resolved did not differ from controls on tests of vocabulary and language comprehension skills. However, they performed significantly less well on tests of phonological processing and literacy skill. Children who still had significant language difficulties at 5;6 had significant impairments in all aspects of spoken and written language functioning, as did children classified as having a general delay. These children fell further and further behind their peer group in vocabulary growth over time.
Article
Full-text available
The present investigation is a follow-up to a longitudinal study involving approximately 400 normally developing children begun in 1960. From this large database, two groups of subjects (now aged 32–34) were asked to participate in the present project: (a) a group of 24 adults with a documented history of a moderate phonological/language disorder that persisted through at least the end of first grade (probands), and (b) a group of 28 adults from the same birth cohort and schools who were known to have had at least average articulation skills over the same period (controls). As part of a larger project, these adults were interviewed about their educational and occupational accomplishments and those of their siblings. Results revealed that, in comparison to control subjects, the proband adults reported that they had received lower grades in high school, required more remedial academic services throughout their school careers, and completed fewer years of formal education. Occupationally, although the groups did not differ in employment status, the proband subjects tended to occupy jobs considered semiskilled or unskilled with a much greater frequency than both the control subjects and their gender-matched siblings. When asked to indicate general satisfaction with educational and occupational outcomes, however, subjects in both groups tended to rate themselves as either “very” or “fairly” satisfied.
Article
Full-text available
This paper reports a longitudinal follow-up of 71 adolescents with a preschool history of speech-language impairment, originally studied by Bishop and Edmundson (1987). These children had been subdivided at 4 years into those with nonverbal IQ 2 SD below the mean (General Delay group), and those with normal nonverbal intelligence (SLI group). At age 5;6 the SLI group was subdivided into those whose language problems had resolved, and those with persistent SLI. The General Delay group was also followed up. At age 15-16 years, these children were compared with age-matched normal-language controls on a battery of tests of spoken language and literacy skills. Children whose language problems had resolved did not differ from controls on tests of vocabulary and language comprehension skills. However, they performed significantly less well on tests of phonological processing and literacy skill. Children who still had significant language difficulties at 5;6 had significant impairments in all aspects of spoken and written language functioning, as did children classified as having a general delay. These children fell further and further behind their peer group in vocabulary growth over time.
Article
ABSTRACTS The purpose of the present study was to investigate the development of and interrelations between the language proficiencies and reading abilities of children learning to read in either a first language or a second language. The authors compared the reading‐comprehension, word‐decoding, and oral‐language skills of both high and low SES Dutch third and fourth graders to the skills of low SES minority third and fourth graders from a Turkish or Moroccan background living in the Netherlands. Several tests of reading comprehension, word decoding, oral text comprehension, morphosyntactic knowledge, and vocabulary knowledge were administered at the beginning of third grade, the end of third grade, and the end of fourth grade. The results showed the minority children to be faster decoders than the Dutch low SES children. With respect to reading comprehension and oral language proficiency, however, the minority children were found to lag behind the Dutch children in all respects. With respect to the interrelations between oral‐language skills and reading skills, the development of reading comprehension was found to be influenced more by top‐down comprehension‐based processes than by bottom‐up word‐decoding processes for both the first‐ and second‐language learners. The oral Dutch skills of the minority children played a more prominent role in the explanation of their reading‐comprehension skills than the oral‐language skills of the Dutch children, however.
Article
Trials which randomize intact social groups, or clusters, to different interventions are becoming increasingly widespread. Although statistically less efficient than trials which randomize individuals, such designs are often preferred from a practical or ethical point of view, particularly in the evaluation of health care or educational strategies. We discuss selected issues that arise in the conduct of such trials, including the choice of design, ethical implications, sample size estimation and approaches to the analysis. The discussion is closely tied to methodological issues that have arisen in a recent evaluation trial of a new antenatal care programme, as sponsored by the Special Programme of Research, Development and Research Training in Human Reproduction of the World Health Organization.
Book
When a child is brought to a pediatrician because language is not developing normally, there are several investigations that are typically carried out. The child’s hearing is tested, and cognitive and social development are assessed. The medical history is scrutinized to see if there is any evidence of neurological disease or trauma early in life, and the child is examined for neurological signs and abnormalities of the speech apparatus. The parents are interviewed about the family circumstances. Although in many cases one might find some explanation for the child’s language disorder in the course of these investigations, all too often this is not the case. The child appears to be physically normal with adequate hearing, good nonverbal ability and normal social relationships, and the medical history and home background are unremarkable. Traditionally the terms “developmental aphasia” or “developmental dysphasia” have been used to describe cases of abnormal language development of unknown origin. However, this terminology is not entirely satisfactory. Use of a neurological label implies similarities with adult acquired dysphasia, but this is usually misleading since, as we shall see, there is seldom any indication of brain damage in these children. Furthermore, the label suggests a clear-cut syndrome, whereas in fact a wide range of problems is included in this definition. For these reasons, more neutral terms such as “specific developmental language disorder” or “specific language impairment” are frequently preferred (Bishop and Rosenbloom, 1987).
Article
The practitioner should attempt to identify the infant and young child with developmental delay as early as possible, so that appropriate services can be provided. Ongoing surveillance is required, rather than one-time screening. The practitioner should also serve as an advocate for children with developmental delay. He or she should ensure that appropriate services exist within the child's community and that they are readily accessible. This requires ongoing communication not only with the child and the family, but also with schools and community agencies.
Article
The present investigation is a follow-up to a longitudinal study involving approximately 400 normally developing children begun in 1960. From this large database, two groups of subjects (now aged 32-34) were asked to participate in the present project: (a) a group of 24 adults with a documented history of a moderate phonological/language disorder that persisted through at least the end of first grade (probands), and (b) a group of 28 adults from the same birth cohort and schools who were known to have had at least average articulation skills over the same period (controls). As part of a larger project, these adults were interviewed about their educational and occupational accomplishments and those of their siblings. Results revealed that, in comparison to control subjects, the proband adults reported that they had received lower grades in high school, required more remedial academic services throughout their school careers, and completed fewer years of formal education. Occupationally, although the groups did not differ in employment status, the proband subjects tended to occupy jobs considered semiskilled or unskilled with a much greater frequency than both the control subjects and their gender-matched siblings. When asked to indicate general satisfaction with educational and occupational outcomes, however, subjects in both groups tended to rate themselves as either "very" or "fairly" satisfied.