ArticlePDF Available

When to Care for, Abandon, or Eat Your Offspring: The Evolution of Parental Care and Filial Cannibalism

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Parental care and filial cannibalism (the consumption of one's own offspring) co-occur in many animals. While parental care typically increases offspring survival, filial cannibalism involves the killing of one's young. Using an evolutionary ecology approach, we evaluate the importance of a range of factors on the evolution of parental care and filial cannibalism. Parental care, no care/total abandonment, and filial cannibalism evolved and often coexisted over a range of parameter space. While no single benefit was essential for the evolution of filial cannibalism, benefits associated with adult or offspring survival and/or reproduction facilitated the evolution of cannibalism. Our model highlights the plausibility of a range of alternative hypotheses. Specifically, the evolution of filial cannibalism was enhanced if (1) parents could selectively cannibalize lower-quality offspring, (2) filial cannibalism increased egg maturation rate, (3) energetic benefits of eggs existed, or (4) cannibalism increased a parent's reproductive rate (e.g., through mate attractiveness). Density-dependent egg survivorship alone did not favor the evolution of cannibalism. However, when egg survival was density dependent, filial cannibalism invaded more often when the density dependence was relatively more intense. Our results suggest that population-level resource competition potentially plays an important role in the evolution of both parental care and filial cannibalism.
Content may be subject to copyright.
vol. 170, no. 6 the american naturalist december 2007
When to Care for, Abandon, or Eat Your Offspring: The
Evolution of Parental Care and Filial Cannibalism
Hope Klug
1,
*
and Michael B. Bonsall
2,
1. Department of Zoology, University of Florida, Gainesville,
Florida 32611;
2. Mathematical Ecology Research Group, Department of Zoology,
University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom
Submitted November 8, 2006; Accepted July 20, 2007;
Electronically published October 1, 2007
Online enhancement: appendix.
abstract: Parental care and filial cannibalism (the consumption
of one’s own offspring) co-occur in many animals. While parental
care typically increases offspring survival, filial cannibalism involves
the killing of one’s young. Using an evolutionary ecology approach,
we evaluate the importance of a range of factors on the evolution
of parental care and filial cannibalism. Parental care, no care/total
abandonment, and filial cannibalism evolved and often coexisted over
a range of parameter space. While no single benefit was essential for
the evolution of filial cannibalism, benefits associated with adult or
offspring survival and/or reproduction facilitated the evolution of
cannibalism. Our model highlights the plausibility of a range of
alternative hypotheses. Specifically, the evolution of filial cannibalism
was enhanced if (1) parents could selectively cannibalize lower-qual-
ity offspring, (2) filial cannibalism increased egg maturation rate, (3)
energetic benefits of eggs existed, or (4) cannibalism increased a
parent’s reproductive rate (e.g., through mate attractiveness).
Density-dependent egg survivorship alone did not favor the evolution
of cannibalism. However, when egg survival was density dependent,
filial cannibalism invaded more often when the density dependence
was relatively more intense. Our results suggest that population-level
resource competition potentially plays an important role in the evo-
lution of both parental care and filial cannibalism.
Keywords: parental care, filial cannibalism, sexual selection, infanti-
cide, density-dependent egg survivorship.
Parental investment, and more specifically parental care,
* Corresponding author; e-mail: hklug@zoo.ufl.edu.
E-mail: michael.bonsall@zoo.ox.ac.uk.
Am. Nat. 2007. Vol. 170, pp. 886–901. 2007 by The University of Chicago.
0003-0147/2007/17006-42207$15.00. All rights reserved.
DOI: 10.1086/522936
is one of the most well-studied topics in life-history evo-
lution. Adaptive theories of evolution typically suggest that
parents should strive to increase offspring survival, and
parental care is one way in which parents are thought to
achieve this (reviewed by Clutton-Brock [1991]). Although
parental care is assumed to increase offspring survival, filial
cannibalism, the consumption of one’s own viable off-
spring, commonly co-occurs with parental care. Indeed,
filial cannibalism is prevalent in a range of taxa exhibiting
parental care (Polis 1981; Elgar and Crespi 1992). For
example, caring females consume some of their young in
the bank vole (Clethrionomys glareolus; Klemme et al.
2006), the house finch (Carpodacus mexicanus; Gilbert et
al. 2005), and the wolf spider (Pardosa milvina; Anthony
2003), and both parents of the burying beetle (Nicrophorus
orbicollis) are known to consume their offspring (Bartlett
1987). Filial cannibalism has been particularly well doc-
umented in fish species with paternal care during the egg
stage (reviewed in Manica 2002). Indeed, because of the
prevalence of filial cannibalism in fish systems, most the-
oretical and empirical work on the subject has focused on
fish (but see Bartlett 1987; Thomas and Manica 2003;
Creighton 2005). While early ethologists considered filial
cannibalism a social pathology with little or no evolu-
tionary significance, it is now typically thought to reflect
an adaptive trade-off between current and future repro-
ductive success (e.g., Manica 2002, 2004). However, de-
spite much theoretical development and empirical work
during the past few decades, the evolutionary significance
of filial cannibalism remains unclear in many systems.
The most widely accepted hypothesis of filial cannibal-
ism as an adaptive strategy suggests that energetic need is
the primary factor leading to filial cannibalism and that a
caring parent gains energy and nutrients from consuming
its offspring that are then reinvested into future repro-
duction, thereby increasing net reproductive success
(Rohwer 1978; Sargent 1992). Specifically, whole-clutch
cannibalism (i.e., the consumption of all offspring during
a given reproductive bout) is assumed to be an investment
in future reproduction, whereas partial-clutch cannibalism
(i.e., the consumption of only some offspring present) can
Evolution of Parental Care and Filial Cannibalism 887
represent an investment in either current or future repro-
duction. This energy-based hypothesis predicts that can-
nibalism will increase as food availability decreases and
when parental condition is poor (Rohwer 1978; Sargent
1992). While food availability and/or parental condition
affect the amount of cannibalism in some species (e.g.,
Stegastes rectifraenum [Hoelzer 1992], Pomatoschitus mi-
crops [Kvarnemo et al. 1998], and Abudefduf sexfasciatus
[Manica 2004]), it has no effect in others (e.g., Gasterosteus
aculeatus [Belles-Isles and Fitzgerald 1991] and Etheostoma
flabellare [Lindstro¨m and Sargent 1997]), and in two sys-
tems, cannibalism declines as male condition or food avail-
ability decreases (Jordanella floridae [Klug and St. Mary
2005] and Pomatoschistus minutus [Klug et al. 2006]).
Other studies have examined whether eggs can provide a
caring parent with sufficient energy to offset the costs of
care. Again, the evidence is mixed: two studies concluded
that energy attained from filial cannibalism is sufficient to
offset costs related to care (Kume et al. 2000; Thomas and
Manica 2003), while in another study, energy from eggs
was found to be insufficient (Smith 1992). Thus, parental
energetic need alone cannot explain the prevalence of filial
cannibalism.
Alternatively, Payne et al. (2002) and Klug et al. (2006)
suggested that filial cannibalism is mediated by density-
dependent egg survivorship and that by consuming some
eggs in their nests, caring parents can improve the sur-
vivorship of the remaining eggs and increase their net
reproductive success. Such density-dependent egg survi-
vorship is potentially related to the physical environment
(e.g., oxygen availability; Payne et al. 2002) or increased
benefits of parental care to the remaining offspring. The
hypothesis of filial cannibalism mediated by density-
dependent egg survivorship has received support in two
fish species (Stegastes leucostictus [Payne et al. 2002] and
P. minutus [Klug et al. 2006]) but has, in general, received
little further empirical or theoretical examination (but see
Payne et al. 2004). Likewise, some have suggested that filial
cannibalism is a mechanism by which parents reduce
brood size in response to anticipated resource competition
among their adult offspring (Bartlett 1987; Creighton
2005) or kill offspring of reduced quality (Forbes and
Mock 1998; see also Kozlowski and Stearns 1989). While
the former hypothesis has received some attention in the
burying beetle (Creighton 2005), neither of these hypoth-
eses of filial cannibalism has been explicitly evaluated.
Because of the mixed empirical support for the energy-
based hypothesis and the lack of empirical evidence re-
garding alternative hypotheses, filial cannibalism remains
an evolutionary conundrum. Indeed, previous work sug-
gests that a parent’s energetic need (Rohwer 1978; Sargent
1992; Manica 2002), expectations regarding offspring sur-
vival or reproductive value (Payne et al. 2002; Neff 2003;
Klug et al. 2006), competition for mates (Sikkel 1994;
Kondoh and Okuda 2002), and anticipated offspring re-
source competition (Creighton 2005) are potentially im-
portant factors for explaining the adaptive significance of
filial cannibalism. However, previous theory has tended to
focus on each of these factors in separate theoretical con-
texts (e.g., energetic benefits of consuming offspring
[Rohwer 1978; Sargent 1992], variation in offspring quality
[Forbes and Mock 1998], mate availability [Kondoh and
Okuda 2002], and expectations regarding offspring sur-
vivorship [Payne et al. 2004]), despite empirical evidence
suggesting that it is unlikely that any single factor alone
can explain the prevalence of filial cannibalism (e.g., Man-
ica 2004; Klug et al. 2006).
Here, we develop a model of parental care, total off-
spring abandonment (i.e., no care), and filial cannibalism
to begin to isolate the pivotal factors affecting the evolution
of care and filial cannibalism. First, we determine the gen-
eral conditions under which we would expect these strat-
egies (i.e., care, no care/total abandonment, filial canni-
balism) to evolve alone or in combination. We then
evaluate the plausibility of multiple alternative hypotheses
within a single theoretical context by assessing the im-
portance of a range of potential costs and benefits of care
and cannibalism. Specifically, we focus on costs and ben-
efits related to energetics, offspring survival and quality,
mate competition, and general resource competition.
Methods
The model is set up as an ecological problem in which a
rare mutant with a unique life-history strategy is allowed
to invade a resident population (e.g., Vincent and Brown
2005). Specifically, we assume that the resident strategy is
in equilibrium and that an alternative mutant strategy in-
vades from rare into the resident population. We assume
a system in which individuals develop through an egg stage
and a juvenile stage and then mature and reproduce as
adults. While in the egg stage, individuals can be aban-
doned by parents, receive parental care, suffer filial can-
nibalism, or receive parental care and suffer filial canni-
balism (fig. 1). We outline the dynamics of a system in
which a mutant with care and/or cannibalism invades a
resident population that either lacks or provides parental
care. We then use mutual invasion analysis to explore the
effects of costs and benefits of varying strategies on lifetime
fitness and the evolution (i.e., invasion from rare and sub-
sequent fixation) of parental care and/or filial cannibalism.
Model Dynamics
We consider a stage-structured system (which is appro-
priate for many fish, bird, and insect systems) in which
888 The American Naturalist
Figure 1: The model: individuals develop through an egg and a juvenile
stage and reproduce as adults. Eggs die (at rate d
E
), are consumed by
their parent(s) (at rate b), or mature into juveniles (at rate m
E
). Indi-
viduals survive and pass through the juvenile stage (at rate ), wherem 7 j
EJ
t represents the time spent in the juvenile stage. As adults, individuals
either die (at rate d
A
) or reproduce (at rate , where Kr[1 (A(t)/K)]
represents the population carrying capacity).
individuals pass through egg (E), juvenile, and adult (A)
stages. Eggs increase as adults reproduce and decrease as
eggs mature and die, such that
dE A(t)
p r 7 A(t) 7 1 ⫺⫺d 7 E(t) m 7 E(t), (1)
EE
()
dt K
where r is the rate of egg fertilization (i.e., mean repro-
ductive rate of adults), d
E
is the death rate of eggs, and
m
E
is the rate at which eggs mature. We assume logistic
population growth, where K represents population car-
rying capacity and density dependence associated with re-
source competition affects adult reproduction (i.e., the rate
of fertilization). Adults in the population increase as eggs
mature and survive the juvenile stage and decrease as
adults die, such that
dA
p m 7 E(t t) 7 j d 7 A(t), (2)
EJA
dt
where t is a time delay representing the juvenile stage, j
J
is survival rate through the juvenile stage, and d
A
is the
death rate of adults (fig. 1). The equilibrial densities are
thus
d 7 A
A
E p ,(3)
m 7 j
EJ
(d 7 d )/(m 7 j ) (d /j )
EA EJ AJ
A p K 7 1 .(4)
{[ ]}
r
Resident and Mutant Trade-Offs
To explore the fixation of different strategies, we allowed
rare mutants with different life histories to invade a res-
ident population. Specifically, we considered the following
cases: (1) a rare mutant who provides parental care invades
a resident population with no care (and no cannibalism),
(2) a rare mutant who practices filial cannibalism invades
a resident population with no care (and no cannibalism),
(3) a rare mutant who provides parental care and practices
filial cannibalism invades a resident population with no
care and no cannibalism, and (4) a rare mutant who pro-
vides parental care and practices filial cannibalism invades
a resident population that provides parental care (but does
not cannibalize). The different life-history strategies are
represented through the incorporation of appropriate
trade-offs into the model (described in table 1), and the
model was analyzed using linear additive trade-offs and
nonlinear trade-offs (table 1).
In cases in which parental care was provided (either by
individuals in the resident population and/or by the rare
mutant), we assumed that parental care increases the sur-
vivorship of eggs (i.e., as d
E
decreases, parental care in-
creases) and that receiving parental care during the egg
stage increases an individual’s likelihood of surviving
through the juvenile stage (i.e., the level of care received
as an egg affects quality such that j
J
increases as d
E
de-
creases). Providing parental care is assumed to be costly
to the parent providing it, and thus, we assumed that the
reproductive rate of adults (i.e., their rate of producing
fertilized eggs) decreases and that the death rate of adults
increases as care increases (i.e., r decreases and d
A
increases
as d
E
decreases). Furthermore, in all cases in which care
is provided, a decrease in the maturation rate of the eggs
was associated with an increase in the reproductive rates
of adults: the less time that an individual has to spend
caring for a clutch of eggs, the greater that individual’s
reproductive rate will be (i.e., as m
E
decreased, r increased).
When we considered the case of a rare mutant practicing
filial cannibalism, we assumed some energetic benefit of
cannibalism, such that the death rate of adults decreased
and the reproductive rate of adults increased as canni-
balism increased (i.e., d
A
decreases and r increases as the
rate of cannibalism b increases). A further goal of these
analyses was to determine whether cannibalism could
evolve in the absence of a substantial benefit of canni-
balism. In these analyses, we assume no direct benefit of
cannibalism (i.e., there is no effect of b on d
A
or r) and
no indirect benefit of cannibalism in relation to canni-
balism freeing up other resources and reducing compe-
tition among adults (i.e., for these comparisons).K p K
m
We analyzed the model both assuming that egg survi-
vorship was density independent and for the case in which
Table 1: Trade-off functions
Parameter and trade-offs
Strategy
No parental care and no
filial cannibalism Parental care only Filial cannibalism only Parental care and filial cannibalism
Reproductive rate r and r
m
:
(1) Reproductive rate decreases as caring increases
(i.e., r or r
m
decreases as d
E
or d
Em
decreases); (2)
reproductive rate increases as maturation rate of
eggs increases (for a carer only; i.e., r or r
m
in-
creases as m
E
or m
Em
increases); (3) reproductive
rate increases as cannibalism increases (i.e., r
m
in-
creases as b increases)
Linear:
r p r
0
Nonlinear:
r p r
0
Linear:
r
m
p 7 (1 d
Em
m
Em
)r
m
0
Nonlinear:
(d m )
Em Em
r p r 7
mm
0
(1d m )
Em Em
Linear:
r
m
p 7 (1 b)r
m
0
Nonlinear:
b
r p r 7
mm
0
(1b)
Linear:
r
m
p 7 (1 d
Em
m
Em
b)r
m
0
Nonlinear:
(d m b)
Em Em
r p r 7
mm
0
(1d m b)
Em Em
Juvenile survival rate j
J
and j
Jm
:
Juvenile survival rate increases as care increases (i.e.,
j
J
or j
Jm
increases as d
E
or d
Em
decreases)
Linear:
j
J
p j
J
0
Nonlinear:
j
J
p j
J
0
Linear:
j
Jm
p j
Jm0
7 (1 d
Em
)
Nonlinear:
(1d )
Em
j p j 7
Jm Jm
0
d
Em
Linear:
j
Jm
p j
Jm
0
Nonlinear:
j
Jm
p j
Jm
0
Linear:
j
Jm
p 7 (1 d
Em
)j
Jm
0
Nonlinear:
(1d )
Em
j p j 7
Jm Jm
0
d
Em
Adult death rate d
A
and d
Am
:
(1) Adult death rate increases as caring increases (i.e.,
d
A
or d
Am
increases as d
E
or d
Em
decreases); (2)
adult death rate decreases as cannibalism increases
(i.e., d
A
or d
Am
decreases as b increases)
Linear:
d
A
p d
A
0
Nonlinear:
d
A
p d
A
0
Linear:
d p d 7 (1 d )
Am Am Em
0
Nonlinear:
(1d )
Em
d p d 7
Am Am
0
d
Em
Linear:
d
Am
p 7 (1 b)d
Am
0
Nonlinear:
(1b)
d p d 7
Am Am
0
b
Linear:
d
Am
p 7 (1 d
Em
b)d
Am
0
Nonlinear:
(1d b)
Em
d p d 7
Am Am
0
d 7b
Em
Note: The following trade-off functions were used to reflect the unique life histories of individuals who provide parental care and/or practice filial cannibalism. The death rate of eggs is assumed to be a
function of the parental care provided (i.e., as d
E
decreases, care is presumed to increase), and thus egg death rate is our proxy for care.
890 The American Naturalist
egg survivorship was density dependent. For the cases in
which egg survivorship was assumed to be density depen-
dent, the death rate of eggs follows an increasing function
in E, and we considered two functions,
2
d 7 E ,(5)
E
1
d 7 (1 q 7 E), (6)
E
where q is the strength of density dependence (following
Bellows 1981).
Invasion Dynamics and Fitness
Under the density-independent egg survivorship scenario,
the dynamics of the rare mutant are thus given by the
following equations and by incorporating relevant trade-
offs (table 1):
dE A
m
p r 7 A (t) 7 1 ⫺⫺d 7 E (t)
mm Emm
()
dt K
m
m 7 E (t) b 7 E (t) 7 A (t), (7)
Em m m m
dA
m
p m 7 E (t t) 7 j d 7 A (t), (8)
Em m Jm Am m
dt
where E
m
and A
m
are the egg and adult strategies for the
invading strategy; m
Em
, d
Em
, j
Jm
, and K
m
are the egg mat-
uration rate, egg death rate, juvenile survival rate, and
carrying capacity for the alternative invading strategy, re-
spectively; and b is the mutant’s rate of cannibalism (b
equals an average rate of cannibalism, which could rep-
resent some combination of whole- and partial-clutch can-
nibalism; if the mutant does not cannibalize). Theb p 0
mutant is assumed to be rare in the population, and thus,
density dependence operating on adult mutant reproduc-
tion occurs through competition with the resident. The
lifetime fitness of the mutant can then be found from the
determinant of

A
l d m b r 7 1
Em Em m
()
K
m
.(9)

m exp (l7 t) 7 jl d

Em Jm Am
Hence, while some life-history parameters (e.g., fertil-
ization rate of eggs r) will be correlated with lifetime fitness
under some scenarios, the real measure of fitness in this
model is the eigenvalues of the invasion matrix. To evaluate
the invasion and replacement dynamics of a rare mutant
that provides parental care and/or practices filial canni-
balism, we used the fitness function of the mutant to cal-
culate the evolutionarily stable state(s) (i.e., when the rate
of change in fitness is 0). We then performed mutual in-
vasion analyses by evaluating when the fitness function is
10 (using a Newton-Raphson algorithm with the resident
dynamics A
set at equilibrium) for different values of a
life-history trait (see table 1; appendix in the online edition
of the American Naturalist). We evaluated and present
pairwise invasion boundaries for different values of the
maturation rate of eggs. Comparing the invasion potential
with regard to the maturation rate of eggs is ideal because
(1) it allows us to represent a wide range of life-history
strategies, including faster and slower reproducers, and (2)
lifetime fitness is highly sensitive to maturation rate. Spe-
cifically, we illustrate (1) the conditions for which the mu-
tant would invade and outcompete the resident; (2) the
boundaries for which the resident would invade and out-
compete the mutant; (3) the putative coexistence range,
in which the strategies have the potential to coexist; (4) a
region of nonpersistence, where neither strategy will per-
sist; and (5) a region in which neither strategy will persist
or initial conditions of the model determine the strategy
that invades. Local stability analyses were performed and
are described in the appendix; for the parameter ranges
considered, the invasion dynamics were always stable. Nu-
merical simulations confirm that strategy coexistence oc-
curs when the dynamics are stable and that regions of
parameter space exist (labeled NP/IC and NP in figs. 2–
6) where neither strategy persists or where the outcome
is based on initial conditions. We evaluated the invasion
potential of the rare mutant for several biologically relevant
scenarios by changing the value(s) of a single life-history
parameter of interest for the mutant and/or resident
populations.
Biologically Relevant Comparisons
In addition to the fixed trade-offs reflecting varying life-
history strategies (table 1), we explicitly considered the
effects of varying selective regimes (e.g., differential mating
success associated with a particular strategy, effects of care
or cannibalism on population resources and, hence, car-
rying capacity) on the invasion dynamics. To do this, we
used pairwise comparisons in which we altered the mag-
nitude of one (or more) parameter(s) to reflect a biological
scenario of interest.
First, we evaluated the importance of offspring survival
benefits of care on the invasion patterns of varying strat-
egies. Empirically, parental care has been shown to reduce
the death rate of offspring (discussed in Clutton-Brock
1991), and thus, we compared the invasion patterns of the
caring mutant for a range of cases in which care was ef-
Evolution of Parental Care and Filial Cannibalism 891
Figure 2: Invasion of parental care. Parental care invades and/or coexists with no care more often (A) when parental care increases the maturation
rate of eggs, (B) when parental care increases parental reproductive rate ( , ), (C) when parental care is associated with a decreasedr p 1.0 r p 1.2
m
carrying capacity ( , ), and (D) when the caring mutant is able to cannibalize (i.e., for the mutant, for the residents).K p 20 K p 15 b p 0.01 b p 0
m
Invasion boundaries are shown for the maturation rate of the eggs, and unless noted above, , , ,r p r p 1.0 d p d p 0.9 d p d p 0.5
mEEmAAm
, , , . The mutant invades the resident in the regions labeled “care” (AC) or “care & cannibalism” (D),j p j p 0.5 K p K p 20 b p 0 t p 0.1
JJm m
the resident invades the mutant in the region labeled “no care” (AC) or “no care or cannibalism” (D), and both strategies coexist in the region
labeled “coexistence.” Neither strategy will persist in the region labeled “NP.” The region labeled “NP/IC” is a region in which neither strategy will
persist or where the outcome is dependent on initial conditions of the model. See main text for definition of variables.
fective (i.e., ) with those in which it was ineffectived ! d
Em E
(i.e., ).d p d
Em E
Second, sexual selection has been hypothesized to be a
major force in the evolution and fixation of parental care
(Baylis 1981; Andersson 1994). In some systems, mate
choice for a partner who will provide care is thought to
affect the reproductive rate of the nonlimiting sex (e.g.,
the number of eggs a caring parent receives per repro-
ductive bout or over the course of the breeding season is
correlated with parental care: Jordanella flordiae [St. Mary
et al. 2001] and Pomatoschistus minutus [Pampoulie et al.
2004]). Likewise, filial cannibalism has been shown to in-
crease the attractiveness of a caring parent’s nest in some
cases (e.g., Sikkel 1994) and might be preferred during
mate choice if there are benefits of cannibalism to re-
maining offspring (e.g., through density-dependent egg
survivorship). With regard to the model, if care or filial
cannibalism is a trait that is preferred by one sex, we would
expect the mutant exhibiting care or cannibalism to receive
more fertilizations per time period (e.g., a breeding season
or lifetime) than a resident who does not exhibit care. In
this sense, r
m
(i.e., egg fertilization rate in eq. [7]) of a
mutant who exhibits a preferred trait would be expected
to be, on average, greater than that of a resident who does
not exhibit the preferred trait. To incorporate this aspect
of sexual selection, we compared invasion patterns for
cases in which caring and/or cannibalism increased the
reproductive rate of the caring mutant relative to the res-
ident (i.e., ) with those in which the magnitude ofr
1 r
m
the reproductive rate did not differ between the mutant
and the resident (i.e., ).r p r
m
Likewise, it is possible that filial cannibalism creates
892 The American Naturalist
Figure 3: Invasion of filial cannibalism. A mutant that provides parental care and practices filial cannibalism invades and coexists with a resident
that provides care only (A) more often when cannibalism increases the maturation rate of eggs, (B) more often when cannibalism increases the
parent’s reproductive rate ( , ), (C) less often when cannibalism decreases the parent’s reproductive rate ( , ), andr p 0.5 r p 0.6 r p 0.6 r p 0.5
m m
(D) more often when parents are able to selectively cannibalize offspring with reduced future survival ( , , , ).d p 0.2 j p 0.9 d p 0.1 j p 0.95
EJEmJm
Invasion boundaries are shown for the maturation rate of the eggs, m
E
and m
Em
, and unless otherwise noted, , ,r p r p 0.5 d p d p 0.2
mEEm
, , , , . The mutant invades the resident in the region labeled “care & cannibalism,”d p d p 0.5 j p j p 0.9 K p K p 20 b p 0.015 t p 0.1
AAm JJm m
the resident invades the mutant in the region labeled “care only,” and both strategies coexist in the region labeled “coexistence.” Neither strategy
will persist in the region labeled “NP.” The region labeled “NP/IC” is a region in which neither strategy will persist or where the outcome is dependent
on initial conditions of the model. See main text for definition of variables.
reproductive conflict between parents. In this case, one
would expect noncannibalistic individuals to be favored
during mate choice (Kraak 1996; Lindstro¨m 2000). To
assess the importance of mate preference for a noncan-
nibalistic partner, we compared cases in which the mutant
has a reduced reproductive rate relative to the resident
(i.e., ) with the case in which the mutant and residentr
! r
m
have equal reproductive rates (i.e., ).r p r
m
Parental care, no care, and filial cannibalism might affect
population-level resources in different ways. For example,
providing care might necessitate greater per capita re-
sources (e.g., increased energetic need and nesting re-
sources per individual) than not providing care (i.e, K
1
for a caring mutant invading a resident without care).K
m
Similarly, the ability to cannibalize while providing care
might, in a sense, free up other resources and increase the
carrying capacity of a population (i.e., for a mutantK
! K
m
who can cannibalize and care invading a resident who can
only care). To begin to evaluate the importance of such
resource-related effects at the population level, we com-
pared cases in which carrying capacity varies between the
mutant and the resident population. Likewise, for cases in
which the mutant and residents have equal carrying ca-
pacities, we compared patterns for a range of carrying
capacities to determine whether a relatively productive
ecosystem (i.e., system with a large carrying capacity) or
unproductive ecosystem (i.e., system with a relatively small
carrying capacity) favors the invasion of a particular
strategy.
Finally, to further evaluate patterns of cannibalism evo-
lution, we compared the fitness boundaries for cases in
which parents were allowed to selectively cannibalize eggs
Evolution of Parental Care and Filial Cannibalism 893
Figure 4: Effect of density-dependent egg survivorship on the evolution of parental care and filial cannibalism. Parental care and no care invade
and/or coexist over a large range of parameter space when (A) the rare mutant does not cannibalize ( ). The range over which parental careb p 0
invades decreases when (B) the rare mutant cannibalizes ( ). However, increasing the strength of density dependence (q in eq. [6]) increasesb p 0.01
the range over which care and cannibalism invades: care with cannibalism invades more often when (C) the strength of the density dependence is
greater ( ) in comparison to (B) the case in which it is relatively weak ( ). Invasion boundaries are shown for the maturation rateq p 0.9 q p 0.6
of the eggs, m
E
and m
Em
, and unless otherwise noted, , , , , , ,r p r p 3 d p 0.9 d p 0.3 d p d p 0.5 j p j p 0.5 K p K p 20 b p
mE Em AAm JJm m
, , . The mutant invades the resident in the region labeled “care” (A) or “care & cannibalism” (B, C), the resident invades the0.01 t p 1 q p 0.6
mutant in the region labeled “no care” (A) or “no care or cannibalism” (B, C), and both strategies coexist in the region labeled “coexistence.” The
region labeled “NP/IC” is a region in which neither strategy will persist or where the outcome is dependent on initial conditions of the model. See
main text for definition of variables.
with reduced future survivorship (i.e., andd ! d
Em E
) with those of parents that could not selectivelyj 1 j
Jm J
cannibalize (i.e., and ).d p d j p j
Em E Jm J
Results
All of the strategies considered (i.e., parental care, no care/
total offspring abandonment, filial cannibalism) evolved
over a range of parameter space in all analyses. While the
evolution of parental care and/or filial cannibalism was
favored by benefits to adults and/or offspring, such benefits
were not essential for the invasion of a particular strategy,
highlighting the plausibility of a range of non–mutually
exclusive alternative hypotheses (table 2). In all cases con-
sidered, the coexistence dynamics were stable (appendix).
While incorporating nonlinear trade-off functions (table
1) into the model altered the results quantitatively, there
were no qualitative effects of these functions (i.e., the pat-
terns were the same), and thus, we present only results in
which linear trade-offs were used.
Invasion of Parental Care
Effects of Egg Maturation Rate, Egg Death Rate, Adult Re-
productive Rate, and Carrying Capacity. A mutant with
parental care invaded or coexisted with a resident popu-
lation lacking care over a wide range of life-history pa-
rameters (fig. 2A), particularly when care was effective at
decreasing the death rate of eggs (i.e., when ),d
! d
Em E
when it increased survivorship through the juvenile stage
(i.e., when ), and when caring increased maturationj
1 j
Jm J
rate of the eggs (i.e., when ; fig. 2A). Similarly,m 1 m
Em E
894 The American Naturalist
Figure 5: Effect of energetic benefits on the evolution of filial cannibalism. Parental care with filial cannibalism is more likely to invade and coexist
with no care if filial cannibalism is (A) beneficial to a parent’s survival and reproduction versus (B) the case where there are no benefits of cannibalism.
Likewise, care with cannibalism is more likely to invade a state of only care when (C) adult survival and reproductive benefits of egg eating exist
versus (D) the case where such benefits are absent. Invasions boundaries are shown for the maturation rate of the eggs, m
E
and m
Em
. Unless otherwise
noted, , , , , , , for A and B, and ,r p r p 1.0 d p d p 0.9 d p d p 0.5 j p j p 0.5 K p K p 20 b p 0.01 t p 0.1 r p r p 0.5 d p
mEEmAAmJJm m mE
,,,,,forC and D. The mutant invades the resident in the region labeledd p 0.2 d p d p 0.5 j p j p 0.9 K p K p 20 b p 0.015 t p 0.1
Em A Am J Jm m
“care & cannibalism,” the resident invades the mutant in the region labeled “no care or cannibalism” (A, B) or “care only” (C, D), and both strategies
coexist in the region labeled “coexistence.” Neither strategy will persist in the region labeled “NP.” The region labeled “NP/IC” is a region in which
neither strategy will persist or where the outcome is dependent on initial conditions of the model. See main text for definition of variables.
the range over which care invaded or coexisted with no
care increased when parental care was associated with an
increased rate of egg fertilization (e.g., if it was a preferred
trait, such that ; fig. 2A vs. fig. 2B) and when carer
1 r
m
was associated with a decreased carrying capacity relative
to the resident population (fig. 2A vs. fig. 2C). The evo-
lution of parental care was relatively insensitive to changes
in carrying capacity for cases in which the resident and
mutant had equal carrying capacities.
Effect of Cannibalism on the Evolution of Care. To evaluate
whether the ability to practice filial cannibalism affects the
evolution of parental care, we compared the case in which
a mutant with only parental care was allowed to invade a
resident population with no parental care and no canni-
balism with the scenario in which the mutant could care
and cannibalize (fig. 2A vs. fig. 2D). Indeed, filial canni-
balism facilitated the evolution of care. When the caring
mutant was allowed to cannibalize (fig. 2D), parental care
(and filial cannibalism) evolved over a wider range of pa-
rameter space and coexisted more often with no care than
when the mutant was not allowed to cannibalize (fig. 2A).
Invasion of Filial Cannibalism (with and
without Parental Care)
Effects of Egg Maturation Rate, Reproductive Rate, and Se-
lective Cannibalism. Parental care with filial cannibalism
was more likely to invade and/or coexist with a state of
only care when it increased the maturation rate of eggs
(i.e., when ; fig. 3A) and when filial cannibalismm
1 m
Em E
allowed a parent to improve the quality of care provided
for remaining offspring (i.e., cannibalism decreases d
Em
relative to d
E
). Care and cannibalism invaded and coexisted
Evolution of Parental Care and Filial Cannibalism 895
Figure 6: Effect of carrying capacity on the evolution of filial cannibalism. Parental care with filial cannibalism invades and coexists with care over
a range of parameter space when (A) cannibalism does not affect carrying capacity ( ). However, cannibalism invades over a greaterK p K p 20
m
range of parameter space when (B) cannibalism increases carrying capacity ( , ). Likewise, care with cannibalism invades no careK p 10 K p 20
m
over a range of parameter space when (C) care and cannibalism do not affect carrying capacity ( ), but it invades or coexists moreK p K p 20
m
often when (D) care and cannibalism decrease carrying capacity ( , ). For cases in which the resident and mutant have equal carryingK p 20 K p 10
m
capacities, parental care and cannibalism are more likely to invade when (E) carrying capacity is relatively small ( ) versus the case inK p K p 10
m
which it is relatively large (A). In contrast, care with cannibalism is more likely to invade no care/no cannibalism when (F) carrying capacity is
relatively large ( ) versus (C) the case in which it is relatively small. Invasions boundaries are shown for the maturation rate of theK p K p 50
m
eggs, m
E
and m
Em
. Unless otherwise noted, , , , , , for A, B, and E, andr p r p 0.5 d p d p 0.2 d p d p 0.5 j p j p 0.9 b p 0.01 t p 0.1
mEEmAAmJJm
,,,,,forC, D, and F. The mutant invades the resident in the regionr p r p 1.0 d p d p 0.9 d p d p 0.5 j p j p 0.5 b p 0.01 t p 0.1
mEEmAAmJJm
labeled “care & cannibalism,” the resident invades the mutant in the region labeled “care only” (A, B, E) or “no care or cannibalism” (C, D, F),
and both strategies coexist in the region labeled “coexistence.” Neither strategy will persist in the region labeled “NP.” The region labeled “NP/IC
is a region in which neither strategy will persist or where the outcome is dependent on initial conditions of the model. See main text for definition
of variables.
more often when filial cannibalism increased the repro-
ductive rate of the caring parent(s) (e.g., care and/or can-
nibalism are preferred in mate choice, such that ;r
1 r
m
fig. 3A vs. fig. 3B) but invaded less often if it decreased
the reproductive rate of adults (e.g., noncannibalism is
preferred, such that ; fig. 3A vs. fig. 3C). Similarly,r
! r
m
care with cannibalism evolved more often when parents
could selectively cannibalize their offspring. Specifically, if
parents cannibalized offspring with a higher egg death rate
and a lower juvenile survival rate (fig. 3D), cannibalism
invaded more often than in cases in which parents were
not capable of selectively cannibalizing (fig. 3A). These
patterns were consistent when we considered parental care
and filial cannibalism evolving from a state of care or a
Table 2: Alternative hypotheses regarding the evolutionary significance of filial cannibalism (FC)
Hypothesis Description Model findings Related previous findings
1. Selective filial cannibalism Offspring with particular characteristics
(e.g., reduced survival, decreased
maturation rate) are preferentially
consumed
Evolution of FC facilitated by selective
cannibalism of offspring with lower
maturation rates, lower egg survival,
and/or lower juvenile survival
FC affected by certainty of paternity in
some systems (Neff 2003; Frommen
et al. 2007; Gray et al. 2007) but not
in others (Svensson et al. 1998); ef-
fect of other aspects of offspring
quality on FC largely unknown
2. Filial cannibalism speeds up egg
development By increasing costs associated with re-
maining in the egg stage, FC is a
mechanism by which parents increase
maturation rate of eggs (i.e., FC de-
creases the time it takes for eggs to
develop)
Evolution of FC more likely if cannibal-
ism increases egg maturation rate
Not previously examined, to our knowl-
edge; potentially relevant for systems
in which parent-offspring conflict ex-
ists over the optimal amount of care
provided/received
3. Energy-based filial cannibalism FC provides energy that offsets costs of
care and is reinvested into current
and/or future reproduction
Energetic benefit of eggs facilitated evo-
lution of FC
Substantial energetic benefit of FC and/
or effect of energetic need on FC
found in several systems (reviewed in
Manica 2002)
4. Density-dependent egg-survivor-
ship-mediated filial cannibalism Density-dependent egg survival medi-
ates FC; by consuming some young,
parents increase survival of remaining
offspring
Density-dependent egg survival alone
did not facilitate the evolution of FC;
more intense density dependence fa-
cilitated evolution of FC in compari-
son to weaker density dependence
FC is affected by density-dependent egg
survivorship in two species (Payne et
al. 2002, 2004; Klug et al. 2006)
5. Mate-choice-mediated filial
cannibalism FC is preferred in mate choice, thereby
increasing relative reproductive rate
If FC increases relative reproductive rate
of cannibals, FC evolves more often
FC increases nest attractiveness (and
consequently eggs received) in some
cases (Sikkel 1994)
6. Sexual-conflict-mediated filial
cannibalism FC is a nonpreferred trait and decreases
relative reproductive rate
If FC decreases reproductive rate, FC
evolves less often
Sexual conflict can inhibit FC in some
cases (Lindstro¨m 2000); sexual con-
flict regarding FC not well studied
empirically (but see Kraak 1996)
7. Filial cannibalism driven by re-
source competition FC is driven by population-level re-
source competition among adults
Evolution of FC is sensitive to popula-
tion-level carrying capacity
Mate availability (Kondoh and Okuda
2002) and other resource competition
(Creighton 2005) affects FC in some
cases; effects of general resource
availability on FC not well known
Note: We present several nonmutually exclusive hypotheses and briefly describe the findings of our model, and those of some previous work, in relation to these hypotheses.
Evolution of Parental Care and Filial Cannibalism 897
state of no care. Likewise, filial cannibalism (without care)
invaded and/or coexisted with no care/no cannibalism over
a greater range of parameter space if filial cannibalism
improved offspring survival (i.e., or ) ord
! d j 1 j
Em E Jm J
increased the maturation rate of eggs ( ) or whenm 1 m
Em E
parents were able to practice selective filial cannibalism of
offspring with reduced survival during the egg and/or ju-
venile stage.
Effects of Density-Dependent Egg Survivorship. When egg
survivorship was density dependent, parental care and/or
filial cannibalism evolved and coexisted over a wide range
of parameter space. However, in the absence of any other
benefits of filial cannibalism, density-dependent egg sur-
vivorship alone did not facilitate the evolution of canni-
balism. In fact, allowing a mutant that provides care to
cannibalize decreased the range over which care and can-
nibalism evolved, in comparison to the case in which the
mutant could not cannibalize (fig. 4A vs. fig. 4B). However,
when the mutant provided care and cannibalized, parental
care and cannibalism invaded over a greater range of pa-
rameter space as the strength of density dependence (i.e.,
q) increased (fig. 4B vs. fig. 4C). In other words, the evo-
lution of filial cannibalism was not facilitated by density-
dependent egg survivorship per se, but relatively intense
density dependence allowed cannibalism to evolve more
often in comparison to weaker density dependence (i.e.,
as q in eq. [6] increased, the range over which care with
cannibalism and no care could invade and/or coexist in-
creased). These patterns were the same for both density-
dependent functions considered.
Effects of Energetic Benefits of Consuming Offspring. En-
ergetic benefits of filial cannibalism (i.e., benefits associ-
ated with d
Am
and r
m
) increased the range over which care
and cannibalism could invade and coexist with no care
(fig. 5A vs. fig. 5B). However, in this scenario (i.e., con-
sidering care and cannibalism invading from a state of no
care or cannibalism), it is possible that cannibalism could
be thought of as simply hitchhiking in with care. Thus,
we considered the case in which cannibalism and care
invaded a resident who already provides care. While en-
ergetic benefits of cannibalism to adult reproduction and
survival increased the range of invasion and coexistence
(fig. 5C), filial cannibalism (which, in this case, is equiv-
alent to simply killing offspring, or abandoning offspring
that have no chance of surviving alone, during the course
of care) was still able to invade in the absence of benefits
to adults (fig. 5D).
Effects of Carrying Capacity. For cases in which cannibal-
ism alters the population carrying capacity, filial canni-
balism with parental care was more likely to evolve from
a state of only care if cannibalism increased the carrying
capacity. In other words, filial cannibalism was more likely
to invade if it somehow increased the productivity of the
system (fig. 6A vs. fig. 6B). In contrast, for the case of filial
cannibalism with parental care evolving from a state of no
care, cannibalism and care were more likely to evolve if
they were associated with a decrease in the population
carrying capacity (i.e., if care with cannibalism decreased
the productivity of the system; fig. 6C vs. fig. 6D). Likewise,
for the case of filial cannibalism (without parental care)
invading a resident state of no care/no cannibalism, filial
cannibalism invaded and coexisted over a greater range of
parameter space if it increased carrying capacity.
If carrying capacities were equal for the resident pro-
viding parental care and the mutant providing care and
practicing filial cannibalism (i.e., productivity was equiv-
alent for mutant and resident), cannibalism invaded and/
or coexisted more often when carrying capacity was rel-
atively low (i.e., in relatively unproductive systems; fig. 6A
vs. fig. 6E). This trend (i.e., more invasion and coexistence
of the mutant at lower carrying capacities) was consistent
for the case in which filial cannibalism (with no parental
care) invaded a resident with no care/no cannibalism.
However, when we considered the case in which canni-
balism with care invaded a state of no care (and assumed
that the carrying capacities were equal for resident and
mutant), cannibalism and care were more likely to invade
or coexist with no care/no cannibalism when carrying ca-
pacity was relatively large (i.e., when the system was rel-
atively more productive; fig. 6C vs. fig. 6F).
Discussion
We have shown that parental care, filial cannibalism, and
no care/total offspring abandonment can evolve over a
wide range of life-history parameters. Our results suggest
that the ability to abandon or consume offspring during
the course of parental care can actually facilitate the evo-
lution of parental care and that offspring abandonment/
no care, parental care, and filial cannibalism often have
the potential to coexist. Even in the absence of direct ben-
efits of filial cannibalism, such as energetic gain or in-
creased survival of remaining offspring, filial cannibalism
invaded (and coexisted with) noncannibalistic strategies
in multiple contexts (i.e., with or without care, across vary-
ing resident strategies, over a range of life-history param-
eters). In the absence of such benefits, cannibalism is
equivalent to simply killing (or abandoning offspring that
will subsequently die) during the course of care. Our re-
sults suggest that the evolutionary dynamics of filial can-
nibalism are likely comparable to those of simple offspring
abandonment (which provides no immediate benefits,
898 The American Naturalist
such as energetic gain, to parents). However, our results
suggest that the evolution and fixation of filial cannibalism
are favored by a variety of evolutionary and ecological
factors. While no single benefit of consuming eggs was
essential for the invasion of filial cannibalism to occur,
several potential benefits facilitate the evolution of filial
cannibalism.
In particular, our model highlights the plausibility of
several non–mutually exclusive alternative hypotheses fa-
voring the evolution of filial cannibalism (table 2). The
ability to selectively cannibalize eggs facilitated the evo-
lution of cannibalism in all contexts. The ability to selec-
tively cannibalize offspring allows parents to alter the phe-
notypes of the offspring they produce after fertilization
and on a relatively fine timescale, which might be partic-
ularly beneficial in a variable environment. Selective can-
nibalism of clutches of lower reproductive value has been
demonstrated in relation to uncertainty of paternity (i.e.,
possible cuckolding events) in some fishes (Lepomis ma-
crochirus [Neff 2003], Telmatherina sarasinorum [Gray et
al. 2007], and Gasterosteus aculeatus [Frommen et al.
2007]), and the elimination of low-quality offspring has
been a focus in other contexts (e.g., allowing lower-quality
offspring to be eliminated by siblicide [Stearns 1987] and
spontaneous and selective abortion in humans and sex
ratio adjustment in red deer [Stearns 1987; Kozlowski and
Stearns 1989]). However, selective filial cannibalism of vi-
able offspring in relation to other aspects of offspring qual-
ity has received little empirical attention. In particular, we
hypothesize that, in some contexts, filial cannibalism of
offspring with (1) reduced expected future survival or (2)
slower maturation rates during the period in which care
is being provided can be an adaptive strategy.
Alternatively, it is possible that filial cannibalism itself
increases the development rate of eggs. Our model suggests
that parental fitness is highly sensitive to the maturation
rate of eggs. If filial cannibalism increases the maturation
rate of eggs relative to those of noncannibalistic parents,
filial cannibalism evolves over a greater range of parameter
space (fig. 3A). Indeed, for cases in which parent-offspring
conflict exists over the optimal duration of parental care,
filial cannibalism might be a way in which parents speed
up the developmental rate of their eggs, thereby allowing
them to reduce per offspring costs of care or reenter the
mating pool faster. According to this hypothesis, caring
parents potentially benefit by providing care for a shorter
duration of time if filial cannibalism creates an environ-
ment in which offspring are eager to escape the egg stage
(e.g., because of increased risk of death). To our knowl-
edge, this idea of filial cannibalism speeding up egg de-
velopment has not previously been considered and, as
mentioned, is likely to be relevant for cases in which par-
ents and offspring differ in the optimal amount of care
they provide/receive.
Incorporating an energetic benefit of cannibalism fa-
cilitated the invasion of filial cannibalism. This finding is
consistent with previous theoretical and empirical work
suggesting that energetic need affects filial cannibalism
(e.g., Rohwer 1978; Sargent 1992; Kraak 1996; reviewed
by Manica [2002]). However, some empirical work sug-
gests that the effects of energetic need on filial cannibalism
are not always straightforward: in some species, canni-
balism increases as parental energetic need increases (e.g.,
Thomas and Manica 2003), whereas in other species, an
opposite pattern is observed (e.g., Klug et al. 2006). More-
over, in other systems, there appear to be no effects of
parental condition on filial cannibalism under some con-
ditions (e.g., Lindstro¨m and Sargent 1997), and in other
species, the relationship between energetic need and can-
nibalism differs in varying contexts (H. Klug and K. Lind-
stro¨m, unpublished data). Furthermore, some have sug-
gested that the energetic benefits of cannibalism are not
sufficient to explain the prevalence of filial cannibalism
(Smith 1992). In our model, filial cannibalism invaded
over a range of parameter space even when we removed
the benefits of cannibalism, suggesting that substantial en-
ergetic benefit of cannibalism is not necessarily essential
for the evolution of cannibalism. That said, there is no
doubt that filial cannibalism provides a caring parent with
energy and/or nutrients and such benefits are critical in
systems where parents are unable to feed during the course
of providing parental care (Manica 2002, 2004). Indeed,
energetic benefits certainly favor the evolution of filial can-
nibalism (fig. 5; previous work by Rohwer [1978] and
Sargent [1992]; reviewed in Manica 2002).
Likewise, increasing the strength of density-dependent
egg survivorship increased the parameter space over which
filial cannibalism evolved. However, density-dependent
egg survivorship alone did not facilitate the evolution of
filial cannibalism. Indeed, it seems unlikely that density-
dependent egg survivorship per se would lead to the evo-
lution of filial cannibalism in the absence of other trade-
offs associated with egg number. If animals can track their
environment, they would simply be expected to adjust the
number of eggs they produce according to expected egg
survivorship (i.e., they should lay at densities that maxi-
mize survival). Further work is needed to evaluate the
importance of density-dependent egg survivorship when
other trade-offs are associated with the number of off-
spring produced or when the environment is variable. Spa-
tial and temporal variation in the environment has been
hypothesized to influence patterns of cannibalism ob-
served in nature (e.g., Payne et al. 2004), but additional
work is needed to understand more fully the importance
of such stochasticity at varying scales.
Evolution of Parental Care and Filial Cannibalism 899
Sexual selection via mate choice and/or sexual conflict
also affected the invasion and fixation of filial cannibalism
and/or parental care. Our model suggests that the evo-
lution and fixation of parental care from a state of no care
can be facilitated by differential reproductive success if
parental care or filial cannibalism increases the reproduc-
tive rate of individuals exhibiting care or cannibalism (e.g.,
if parental care or filial cannibalism is preferred during
mate choice). This finding is consistent with some previous
work. For example, Pampoulie et al. (2004) and Lindstro¨m
et al. (2006) recently demonstrated mating preferences for
parental care, suggesting a potentially larger role for sexual
selection in the evolution of care than previously thought.
Additionally, filial cannibalism is possibly favored by sex-
ual selection if cannibalism directly benefits a choosing
mate or when it makes a caring parent more attractive in
some other way (Sikkel 1994; Lindstro¨m 2000). Likewise,
if a mating preference exists for noncannibals, the param-
eter space over which filial cannibalism evolves decreases.
Interestingly, the role of sexual conflict has received rel-
atively little theoretical or empirical attention previously
(but see Kraak and van den Berghe 1992; Kraak 1996;
Lindstro¨m 2000). In fishes, where filial cannibalism is typ-
ically practiced by caring fathers, the focus of almost all
work has been on costs and benefits of cannibalism to
caring males. One must also wonder whether benefits to
noncannibalistic females exist, and if such benefits are ab-
sent, why do females tolerate filial cannibalism? More em-
pirical work is needed to better understand the costs and
benefits of filial cannibalism to a parent who does not
practice filial cannibalism but has a mate that does.
Finally, population-level resource competition likely
plays a role in the evolution of both parental care and
filial cannibalism. When care and/or cannibalism affected
population carrying capacity in our model, parental care
was more likely to evolve if caring was associated with a
reduction in the carrying capacity (e.g., if productivity was
decreased), whereas filial cannibalism was more likely to
invade if it increased the carrying capacity (e.g., if can-
nibalism increased the productivity of the system). Ad-
ditionally, the evolution of filial cannibalism (with or with-
out parental care) was affected by population carrying
capacity, even for the case in which the carrying capacities
of the mutant and residents were equal. It is unclear how
parental care and filial cannibalism potentially alter
population-level dynamics and resulting carrying capaci-
ties in nature, but this idea warrants further attention. For
example, it is possible that the ability to cannibalize in-
creases resource availability to caring parents, thereby free-
ing up other resources and increasing the productivity of
a system. Regardless, understanding the ecological dynam-
ics of a system (i.e., intensity of resource competition and
population growth parameters such as carrying capacity)
is likely to be critical for understanding the evolution of
parental care and filial cannibalism across animal taxa.
While previous work has sometimes incorporated popu-
lation-level growth dynamics in parental care theory (e.g.,
McNamara et al. 2000), this is not a common approach.
In summary, our results suggest that parental care and
filial cannibalism can evolve over a range of life-history
patterns and ecological conditions and that multiple strat-
egies often have the potential to coexist. Coexistence, while
not well studied (but see Webb et al. 1999), is prevalent
in nature (e.g., maternal- or paternal-only care in many
taxa [reviewed in Clutton-Brock 1991], care and no care
with total offspring abandonment following egg fertiliza-
tion: Jordanella floridae [R. E. Hale, personal communi-
cation], the white stickleback Gasterosteidae spp. [Blouw
1996]; care and care followed by abandonment: Hypop-
tychus dybowskii [Narimatsu and Munehara 2001]). Like-
wise, there are many cases in which caring parents never
or rarely consume or abandon their offspring. Even in
fishes, where care with filial cannibalism has been well
documented, there are still many species exhibiting pa-
rental care in which filial cannibalism is absent (e.g., Mi-
cropterus dolomieui; Gillooly and Baylis 1999). For species
exhibiting filial cannibalism, there is a great deal of vari-
ation in the patterns of cannibalism observed among spe-
cies and within and between individuals (e.g., how many
eggs are consumed, who practices cannibalism and when;
Petersen and Marchetti 1989; Okuda and Yanagisawa 1996;
Lindstro¨m and Sargent 1997; Lissa˚ker et al. 2002; Klug
and St. Mary 2005; Klug et al. 2005). Understanding such
within- and between-species variation in filial cannibalism
and parental care will require more detailed theoretical
and empirical work that simultaneously considers multiple
factors (such as variation in offspring quality, energetic
needs of parents, mating preferences and sexual conflict,
general resource competition). Additionally, it will also be
important to assess the importance of environmental het-
erogeneity in the evolution of filial cannibalism. From this
study, our approach and results provide a novel basis for
further developing this theme of whether to care for or
consume one’s own offspring.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to four referees, whose insightful com-
ments greatly improved this manuscript. Funding for this
work was provided by a National Science Foundation
(NSF) Graduate Research Fellowship and a NSF Doctoral
Dissertation Improvement Grant (Division of Integrative
Organismal Biology and the Office of International Science
and Engineering) to H.K. M.B.B. is a Royal Society Uni-
versity Research Fellow.
900 The American Naturalist
Literature Cited
Andersson, M. 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton University Press,
Princeton, NJ.
Anthony, C. D. 2003. Kinship influences cannibalism in the wolf
spider, Pardosa milvina. Journal of Insect Behavior 16:23–36.
Bartlett, J. 1987. Filial cannibalism in burying beetles. Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology 21:197–183.
Baylis, J. R. 1981. The evolution of parental care in fishes, with
reference to Darwin’s rule of male sexual selection. Environmental
Biology of Fishes 6:223–251.
Belles-Isles, J. C., and G. J. Fitzgerald. 1991. Filial cannibalism in
sticklebacks: a reproductive management strategy. Ethology Ecol-
ogy and Evolution 3:49–62.
Bellows, T. S. 1981. The descriptive properties of some models for
density dependence. Journal of Animal Ecology 51:139–156.
Blouw, D. M. 1996. Evolution of offspring desertion in a stickleback
fish. Ecoscience 3:18–24.
Clutton-Brock, T. H. 1991. The evolution of parental care. Princeton
University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Creighton, J. C. 2005. Population density, body size, and phenotypic
plasticity of brood size in a burying beetle. Behavioral Ecology 16:
1031–1036.
Elgar, M. A., and B. J. Crespi, eds. 1992. Cannibalism: ecology and
evolution among diverse taxa. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Forbes, S. L., and D. W. Mock. 1998. Parental optimism and progeny
choice: when is screening for offspring quality affordable? Journal
of Theoretical Biology 192:3–14.
Frommen, J. G., C. Brendler, and T. C. M. Bakker. 2007. The tale
of the bad stepfather: male three-spined sticklebacks Gasterosteus
aculeatus L. recognize foreign eggs in their manipulated nest by
egg cues alone. Journal of Fish Biology 70:1295–1301.
Gilbert, W. M., P. M. Nolan, A. M. Stoehr, and G. E. Hill. 2005.
Filial cannibalism at a house finch nest. Wilson Bulletin 117:413–
415.
Gillooly, J. F., and J. R. Baylis. 1999. Reproductive success and the
energetic cost of parental care in male smallmouth bass. Journal
of Fish Biology 54:573–584.
Gray, S. M., L. M. Dill, and J. S. McKinnon. 2007. Cuckoldry incites
cannibalism: male fish turn to cannibalism when perceived cer-
tainty of paternity decreases. American Naturalist 169:258–263.
Gurney, W. S. C., and R. M. Nisbet. 1998. Ecological dynamics.
Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Hoelzer, G. A. 1992. The ecology and evolution of partial-clutch
cannibalism by parental Cortez damselfish. Oikos 65:113–120.
Klemme, I., J. A. Eccard, and I. Ylo¨nen. 2006. Do female bank voles
(Clethrionomys glareolus) mate multiply to improve on previous
mates? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 60:415–421.
Klug, H., and C. M. St. Mary. 2005. Reproductive fitness conse-
quences of filial cannibalism in the flagfish, Jordanella floridae.
Animal Behaviour 70:685–691.
Klug, H., A. Chin, and C. M. St. Mary. 2005. The net effects of
guarding on egg survivorship in the flagfish, Jordanella floridae.
Animal Behaviour 69:661–668.
Klug, H., K. Lindstro¨m, and C. M. St. Mary. 2006. Parents benefit
from eating offspring: density-dependent egg survivorship com-
pensates for filial cannibalism. Evolution 60:2087–2095.
Kondoh, M., and M. Okuda. 2002. Mate availability influences filial
cannibalism in fish with paternal care. Animal Behaviour 63:227–
233.
Kozlowski, J., and S. C. Stearns. 1989. Hypotheses for the production
of excess zygotes: models of bet-hedging and selective abortion.
Evolution 43:1369–1377.
Kraak, S. B. M. 1996. Female preference and filial cannibalism Aid-
ablennius sphynx (Teleostei, Blenniidae): a combined field and lab-
oratory study. Behavioural Processes 36:85–97.
Kraak, S. B. M., and E. P. van den Berghe. 1992. Do females assess
paternal quality by means of test eggs? Animal Behaviour 43:865–
867.
Kume, G., A. Yamaguchi, and T. Taniuchi. 2000. Filial cannibalism
in the paternal mouthbrooding cardinalfish Apogon lineatus: egg
production by the female as the nutrition source for the mouth-
brooding male. Environmental Biology of Fishes 58:233–236.
Kvarnemo, C., O. Svensson, and E. Forsgren. 1998. Parental behav-
iour in relation to food availability in the common goby. Animal
Behaviour 56:1285–1290.
Lindstro¨m, K. 2000. The evolution of filial cannibalism and female
mate choice strategies as resolutions to sexual conflicts in fishes.
Evolution 54:617–627.
Lindstro¨m, K., and R. C. Sargent. 1997. Food access, brood size, and
filial cannibalism in the fantail darter, Etheostoma flabellare. Be-
havioral Ecology and Sociobiology 40:107–110.
Lindstro¨m, K., C. M. St. Mary, and C. Pampoulie. 2006. Sexual
selection for male parental care in the sand goby, Pomatoschistus
minutus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 60:46–51.
Lissa˚ker, M., C. Kvarnemo, and O. Svensson. 2002. Effects of a low
oxygen environment on parental effort and filial cannibalism in
the male sand goby, Pomatoschistus minutus. Behavioural Ecology
14:374–381.
Manica, A. 2002. Filial cannibalism in teleost fish. Biological Reviews
77:261–277.
———. 2004. Parental fish change their cannibalistic behaviour in
response to the cost-to-benefit ratio of parental care. Animal Be-
haviour 67:1015–1021.
McNamara, J. M., T. Szekely, J. N. Webb, and A. I. Houston. 2000.
A dynamic game-theoretic model of parental care. Journal of The-
oretical Biology 205:605–623.
Narimatsu, Y., and H. Munehara. 2001. Territoriality, egg desertion,
and mating success of a paternal care fish, Hypoptychus dybowskii.
Behavior 138:85–96.
Neff, B. D. 2003. Paternity and condition affect cannibalistic behavior
in nest-tending bluegill sunfish. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobi-
ology 54:377–384.
Okuda, N., and Y. Yanagisawa. 1996. Filial cannibalism by the mouth-
brooding cardinalfish, Apogon doederlini, in relation to their phys-
ical condition. Environmental Biology of Fishes 45:397–404.
Pampoulie, C., K. Lindstro¨m, and C. M. St. Mary. 2004. Have your
cake and eat it too: male sand gobies show more parental care in
the presence of female partners. Behavioral Ecology 15:199–204
Payne, A. G., C. Smith, and A. Campbell. 2002. Filial cannibalism
improves survival and development of beaugregory damselfish em-
bryos. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 269:
2095–2102.
———. 2004. A model of oxygen-mediated filial cannibalism in
fishes. Ecological Modelling 174:253–266.
Petersen, C. W., and K. Marchetti. 1989. Filial cannibalism in the
Cortez damselfish Stegastes rectifraenum. Evolution 43:158–168.
Polis, G. A. 1981. The evolution and dynamics of intraspecific pre-
dation. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 12:225–251.
Evolution of Parental Care and Filial Cannibalism 901
Rohwer, S. 1978. Parent cannibalism of offspring and egg raiding as
a courtship strategy. American Naturalist 112:429–440.
Sargent, R. C. 1992. Ecology of filial cannibalism in fish: theoretical
perspective. Pages 38–62 in M. A. Elgar and B. J. Crespi, eds.
Cannibalism: ecology and evolution among diverse taxa. Oxford
University Press, Oxford.
Sikkel, P. C. 1994. Filial cannibalism in a paternal-caring marine fish:
the influence of egg developmental stage and position in the nest.
Animal Behaviour 47:1149–1158.
Smith, C. 1992. Filial cannibalism as a reproductive strategy in care-
giving teleosts. Netherlands Journal of Zoology 42:607–613.
St. Mary, C. M., C. G. Noureddine, and K. Lindstro¨m. 2001. Effects
of the environment on male reproductive success and parental
care in the Florida flagfish, Jordanella floridae. Ethology 107:1035–
1052.
Stearns, S. C. 1987. The selection arena hypothesis. Pages 337–349
in S. C. Stearns, ed. The evolution of sex and its consequences.
Birkhauser, Basel.
Svensson, O., C. Magnhagen, E. Forsgren, and C. Kvarnemo. 1998.
Parental behaviour in relation to the occurrence of sneaking in
the common goby. Animal Behaviour 56:175–179.
Thomas, L. K., and A. Manica. 2003. Filial cannibalism in an assassin
bug. Animal Behaviour 66:205–210.
Vincent, T., and B. Brown. 2005. Evolutionary game theory, natural
selection, and Darwinian dynamics. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Webb, J. N., A. I. Houston, J. M. McNamara, and T. Szekely. 1999.
Multiple patterns of parental care. Animal Behaviour 58:983–993.
Associate Editor: William G. Wilson
Editor: Michael C. Whitlock
... Caring males adjust their consumption of offspring based on food availability, which may be an adaptive strategy to increase own fitness (Klug and Bonsall, 2007). In some cases, caring males consume some or all of their offspring, thus seemingly reducing their own fitness. ...
... Yet, since caring males may lose weight and fat reserves while caring, obtaining energy and nutrients from consuming their offspring actually may improve their parental condition, thus allowing them to better care for the remaining offspring and/or increasing future reproductive success. Therefore, brood reduction may increase parental fitness if males can use some of their own offspring as a nutritional resource (Lindström, 2000;Klug and Bonsall, 2007;Sagebakken et al., 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Syngnathid fishes often experience a reduction in brood size and an increase in immunity during pregnancy. Researchers have studied the polygamous broad-nosed pipefish (Syngnathus typhle) and revealed that some eggs from low-quality females in the male’s brood pouch serve as nurse eggs that are absorbed by the father. It is unclear whether nurse eggs also exist in other syngnathid fishes, especially in monogamous syngnathid fishes. In monogamous syngnathid fishes, the male pouch only carries eggs from a single female. Thus, the question remains: do some eggs of monogamous syngnathid fishes also serve as nurse eggs? And if so, are these nurse eggs impaired eggs, or are they viable eggs that are consumed by the brooding father? In the present study, we used the monogamous lined seahorse (Hippocampus erectus) and asked whether nurse eggs exist in this species. We also explored whether nurse eggs potentially originate from viable versus inviable eggs. Using isotope labeling, we found that nutrients from embryos could be transferred to brooding fathers. Furthermore, we also found that brooding fathers with limited food had a higher isotope content and a smaller brood size compared to individuals with sufficient food. These results have demonstrated that nurse eggs exist in the lined seahorse, and also suggested that the brooding fathers actively consume viable embryos to absorb nutrients in response to low food availability. These findings help us better understand parent–embryo conflict, filial cannibalism, and male-only care in teleost fishes.
... Animals that take care of offspring sometimes exhibit filial cannibalism (Klug and Bonsall 2007;Van Meyel et al. 2019). If adult female G. liturivorus predated newly hatched non-offspring larvae, their presence would not result in increased survival of non-offspring even when they had previously protected non-offspring eggs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Parental care is costly to parents because it consumes energy and reduces the available time for future reproduction. Hence, in theory, parents should not care for the eggs or offspring of unrelated individuals. However, parental care for unrelated offspring has been demonstrated for some animal species, although it has been unclear whether such animals distinguish their offspring from unrelated individuals. Here, we examined protection of non-offspring eggs by adult females of the predatory mite Gynaeseius liturivorus, which discriminate eggs laid by other females, against the predatory mite Neoseiulus californicus. Adult female G. liturivorus protect their eggs against predators. Several adult females of this species are sometimes found with clustered eggs in field; hence, G. liturivorus mothers may protect non-offspring eggs. When G. liturivorus eggs were kept with N. californicus in the presence of conspecific adult females that were not mothers of the eggs, their survival was higher than in the absence of conspecific females. Although adult female G. liturivorus oviposited during experiments, the addition of eggs did not increase the survival of non-offspring eggs. Adult females did not cannibalise newly hatched non-offspring larvae in the presence of food, suggesting that the presence of adult females did not affect larval survival. Furthermore, adult females protected their own eggs laid during experiments, suggesting that protection of non-offspring eggs was not costly to the females. Our results also suggest that adult female G. liturivorus may protect conspecific eggs when abundant food is available. Significance statement Parental care is observed in a wide range of taxa and commonly provided for offspring and/or kin individuals. However, some animal species take care of unrelated individuals. This might be because such species do not discriminate between their own offspring and those of conspecifics. In this study, we experimentally demonstrated that non-offspring eggs were protected from predators by adult females of the predatory mite, Gynaeseius liturivorus, which distinguish their own eggs from conspecific eggs. Newly hatched non-offspring larvae were not cannibalised by adult female mites in the presence of food. Adult females also protected their own eggs irrespective of the presence of conspecific eggs. Our results suggest that adult female predatory mites protect non-offspring when food availability is high.
... Such a strategy can support large bass biomass even under variable environmental conditions, enabling increased individual and population growth rates (Dassow et al. 2018;Riesch et al. 2022). This energy-based hypothesis predicts that cannibalism will increase as food availability decreases and parental condition deteriorates (Rohwer 1978;Klug and Bonsall 2007). Since preying on conspecifics is energetically costly, according to Optimal Foraging Theory it only becomes advantageous when competition for other food items is intense (Riesch et al. 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Although biological invasions are becoming more frequent, their underlying ecological mechanisms often remain unknown. One of the most poorly understood aspects is the relationship between ontogenesis and the trophic role of alien species in invaded ecosystems. The aim of this study was to investigate the role of littoral habitat complexity in the shift from benthivory to piscivory of Micropterus salmoides, one of the most widely introduced and invasive fish species. Specifically, populations from two habitats differing in terms of aquatic vegetation cover within the same ecosystem were studied. Specimens of M. salmoides and its potential prey collected in both habitats were analyzed for C and N stable isotopes. The consumption of macroinvertebrates decreased with body size, but in the less complex habitat, M. salmoides shifted its diet to piscivory at an earlier stage of its life cycle. In this habitat, the size-based food web appears highly connected, as largemouth bass have diffuse weak interactions with multiple prey species occupying a range of trophic levels. This may lead bass to threaten the native fish not only by competition but also by predation. Large piscivorous individuals preferred conspecific fry as prey, on which they concentrated particularly in the more complex habitats, where diet specialization was marked. Since preying on conspecifics is energetically costly, according to Optimal Foraging Theory it only becomes advantageous when competition for other food items is intense. This evident trophic plasticity may favor the success of bass in invaded ecosystems and should be considered when deciding management policy, which should also include the preservation of habitat complexity.
... This could be attributed to the wildness in the animal since the cannibalization took place in the early days of acclimatization when the animal gave birth. This observed parent-offspring cannibalism in this present study is also in tandem with the observation of Klug and Bonsall (2007) who also reported that parents of many species readily cannibalise offspring under their care. ...
Article
Full-text available
African giant rats are recognized as one of the important small mammal species in Nigeria. They are highly priced and as an essential food item that is consumed as bush meat and a cheaper alternative protein source for the common man, especially in rural settings. As delicious as this particular species is there are some associated taboos with this animal because of their strange behaviour. This study therefore evaluates some behavioral attitudes associated with captive African giant rats. Thirty-six adult African giant rats (18 males and 18 females) were captured from the wild and their behaviour was studied for 8-10 weeks. They were monitored for feeding, sleeping, cannibalism, and reproductive behaviour. Feeding behaviour indicated that the animals manifested postures such as sitting on their haunches, standing on their legs, gnawing and acting coprophagy while eating. Sleeping attitudes revealed behaviour such as; assuming the shape of the letter O, maintaining a curve-shaped position, and lying on their back while sleeping. Commonly cannibalism behaviour observed includes; adult male-female cannibalism, mother-pup's cannibalism, and hunger-induced cannibalism. The animals also showed some peculiar reproductive behavior; these include; courtship, mating, and nursing attitudes. The study has been able to elucidate the behavioural characteristics of African giant rats and concludes that some behaviours and attitudes of captive African giant rats were strange and unique indeed. The study recommends that these characteristics should be factored into any plan for the domestication of African giant rats, and further studies to evaluate reasons for some of these behaviours.
... Cannibalism, or the killing and consumption of an individual of the same species, is well documented across a diverse array of taxa in the animal kingdom (Polis 1981). There are multiple forms of cannibalism including filicide (cannibalism of one's own young; Klug and Bonsall 2007), siblicide (cannibalism of siblings; Robbins et al. 2013, Anderson et al. 2015, and egg cannibalism (Clark andJackson 1994, Weldon et al. 2014). Many studies note the consumption of young and eggs by conspecifics. ...
... Although it would not be adaptive for victims to be eaten, there may be an overall benefit of kin cannibalism when available food is scarce and/or victims are unlikely to reproduce (Eickwort, 1973;Osawa, 1992;Pfennig, 1997). Hence, costs and benefits for cannibals and victims would depend on their kinship and food availability (Klug & Bonsall, 2007). ...
... This finding underscores that parental behavior strategies are significantly context-dependent. However, the plasticity of egg-guarding behavior under various circumstances remains unexplored despite numerous studies examining the benefits and costs of egg-guarding behavior for offspring and parents in many animals [4,13,[18][19][20][21]. Hence, it would be important to investigate flexible egg-guarding behavior in animals to fully understand the net effects of egg-guarding and its related ecological consequences. ...
Article
Full-text available
Parental care behavior has evolved as a life history strategy to improve reproductive success, particularly in organisms facing challenging environments. However, the variation in maternal care, such as egg-guarding behavior in response to the social environment and the associated ecological consequence of competition, remains largely unknown. This study addresses a gap in current knowledge by examining the plasticity of maternal care behavior in the predatory mite C. eruditus and its impact on offspring survival and intra- and interspecific competition. Our results demonstrated that the reproductive females frequently exhibit egg-guarding behaviors, with enhanced maternal care efforts when the interspecific competitor is present. Egg masses are significantly more vulnerable to predation in the absence of maternal care. Guarding females increased egg survival rates and adversely influenced the survival of both con- and heterospecific competitors, with higher mortality rates being detected. Our findings highlight the ecological significance of maternal care behaviors and suggest that releasing C. eruditus and Neoseiulus cucumeris (Oudemans) together is not recommended for pest management in storage products.
Chapter
Throughout this book I have considered the Gothic through the lens of the maternal gaze to offer new possibilities for both the genre’s critical direction and gaze theory. While there is a strong heritage of writing about mothers in the Female Gothic, seeing the Gothic mother as a tyrant who exerts her control over the domestic space through incarceration and surveillance via her filicidal gaze challenges the Gothic archetype of the paternal tyrant and affords the mother a more active position than previous spectral or victim readings allowed her. Understanding her tyranny as executed through a maternal gaze issued from the womb also redresses the imbalance of gaze theory which privileges the phallus, where the male gaze is perceived as the active, fetishising one and the female as passive and lacking or mediated by that of the male.
Article
Populations of the Eastern Hellbender, (a large‐bodied, fully aquatic salamander) inhabiting stream reaches with low catchment‐wide riparian forest cover upstream, have experienced population declines and a shift toward a geriatric population age structure. These population declines and demographic shifts might be attributed to reduced embryo viability. Reduced egg quality/viability could negatively affect recruitment and has also been known to trigger filial cannibalism in other species. Therefore, we hypothesized that in comparison to high forest cover sites, hellbender eggs collected from low forest cover sites would have a greater incidence of developmental abnormalities and lower overall viability, and that this would predict whole‐clutch cannibalism by the attending male. We collected a subset of eggs (~20–35) from 99 clutches across sites with variable upstream riparian forest cover and reared these eggs through hatching in stream water under controlled laboratory conditions. At the same time, we monitored the fate of the remaining eggs from the same clutches in the field to document the frequency of whole‐clutch filial cannibalism. We found that eggs collected from sites with lower upstream forest cover had significantly shorter embryonic development times and produced a lower percentage of viable hatchlings (hatchlings with normal development times and morphology). The average modelled viability of hatchlings was 70% higher in sites with the highest forest cover compared to our sites with the lowest forest cover. In contrast to our predictions, we did not find evidence to suggest that egg viability in the lab predicted whole‐clutch cannibalism in the field. Although forest cover was a significant predictor of egg viability and underdevelopment, substantial variance in embryonic developmental traits was unaccounted for in our models suggesting that traits associated with adults (e.g., egg and/or sperm quality) may also play a role in determining developmental outcomes. Further experiments are needed to identify what factors (e.g., egg quality, water quality) disrupt the embryonic development of hellbenders as well as the proximate stimulus that causes adult male hellbenders to eat their young. Our results emphasise the importance of restoring and protecting riparian forest cover to conserve sensitive stream species.
Article
The biological world is full of phenomena that seem to run counter to Darwin's insight that natural selection can lead to the appearance of design. For instance, why do organisms in some species divide reproductive labor? The existence of non-reproducing organisms in such 'eusocial' species looks to be at odds with an evolutionary theory which posits traits exist because they help organisms survive and reproduce. What is the evolutionary advantage of an insect being distasteful to its predators? The distastefulness appears designed to deter predators, but can only affect the predator's actions when the insect is eaten; it is hard to see how such a trait could be passed on. This Element will cover the shared foundations of evolutionary explanations for these and other seemingly puzzling phenomena, focusing on the concepts of inclusive fitness and kin selection.
Article
All of life is a game and evolution by natural selection is no exception. The evolutionary game theory developed in this book provides the tools necessary for understanding many of nature's mysteries, including co-evolution, speciation, extinction and the major biological questions regarding fit of form and function, diversity, procession, and the distribution and abundance of life. Mathematics for the evolutionary game are developed based on Darwin's postulates leading to the concept of a fitness generating function (G-function). G-function is a tool that simplifies notation and plays an important role developing Darwinian dynamics that drive natural selection. Natural selection may result in special outcomes such as the evolutionarily stable strategy (ESS). An ESS maximum principle is formulated and its graphical representation as an adaptive landscape illuminates concepts such as adaptation, Fisher's Fundamental Theorem of Natural Selection, and the nature of life's evolutionary game. © Cambridge University Press, 2009 and T. L. Vincent and J. S. Brown 2005. All rights reserved.
Article
Parental care is expected to evolve when its principal benefit, increased survival of offspring, outweighs the cost of diminished future reproduction of the care giver(s). All sticklebacks (Gasterosteidae) show male parental care, but the males of a newly described form with iridescent white sexual colors ("white" stickleback) are here shown to be emancipated; they actively remove embryos from the nest after fertilization and disperse them over plumes of filamentous algae. Experiments show that the emancipation behaviors are heritable and that desertion apparently imposes little cost in environments with abundant filamentous algae. This evolutionary reversal from paternal care to desertion is the first reported for fishes. It is consistent with models of the adaptive evolution of parental care, and it illustrates that traits which exhibit a long phylogenetic stasis may nevertheless retain sufficient underlying variance to permit adaptive evolution under appropriate ecological conditions.
Article
Male three-spined sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatuis, are violently territorial when breeding. Even so, their stomachs commonly contain nonspecific eggs. Curiously, this conflict between the inviolability of territories and the consumption of nonspecific eggs is not resolved by the regular observations of male sticklebacks raiding each other's nests for eggs. This is so because stolen eggs are not eaten. Instead, they are invariably transferred to the thief's nest. Sticklebacks apparently do not recognize their own eggs, so stolen eggs must not serve primarily as food. My evidence that males do not recognize their own eggs is based on the success of van lersel's (1953) nest-switching experiments and on van den Assem's (1967) demonstration that males will hatch eggs that they did not fertilize. Both were laboratory studies, so the possibility that the strange eggs were recognized but not consumed because of an abundance of other food cannot be ruled out. So the plot has thickened. There is an illicit trafficking of eggs between nests, but this trafficking apparently does not explain the observed egg cannibalism. Furthermore, the egg raids themselves are complex in that they are often presaged by a behavioral sequence termed "sneaking" (van den Assem 1967). Here the prospective raider, usually a neighbor, swims to his victim's territorial boundary and then sinks to the bottom and shortly loses his vivid nuptial colors. He then stealthily approaches his victim's nest, creeping millimeter by millimeter along the bottom (Morris 1952; van den Assem 1967; Wootton 1971). Sneaking is often timed to coincide with courtship, and, when the female enters the nest to spawn, the waiting invader suddenly rushes to cover the remaining distance. The sudden "appearance" of the invader in this rush often enables him to pass through the nest, presumably spawning over the eggs, before the resident male does so. This seems to happen because the resident becomes preoccupied with attacks upon the invader (Morris 1952; van den Assem 1967, p. 104). Sneaking may appear to facilitate cuckoldry, and Barlow (1967) has so interpreted such "pseudofemale" behavior in a South American leaf fish, Polycentrus schomburgkii. But the plot is thicker. Additional observations suggest that stolen spawnings may be a subsidiary adaptation in sticklebacks and that sneaking primarily facilitates egg raiding. First, invaders that succeed
Article
Male cortez damselfish, Stegastes rectifraenum, in a central Gulf of California population, ate a large percentage (28.3%) of the clutches they received. This high rate of filial cannibalism permitted the testing of several predictions concerning the types of clutches that should be preferentially eaten and the mating tactics females should employ to reduce cannibalism rates. Males ate clutches that were smaller than average and that were at early stages of development. Experimentally reduced clutches were consumed at higher rates than controls. When multiple clutches were present, males preferentially ate the younger, smaller clutches. Females were more likely to deposit eggs with males who were caring for other early-stage eggs and to avoid males with late-stage eggs. This pattern was accentuated in females that deposited smaller clutches. These results provide evidence that filial cannibalism represents an adaptive response to clutches that do not provide adequate benefits to warrant the costs of parental care.
Article
Custodial male Stegastes rectifraenum frequently take bites from the clutches they guard. Although the function of this type of behavior (i.e., filial cannibalism) is unclear, an increasing number of studies have documented this behavior in fishes. Two distinct forms of filial cannibalism are recognized: whole-clutch cannibalism and partial-clutch cannibalism. The goals of this study were to establish (1) whether foraging is a function of partial-clutch filial cannibalism in S. rectifraenum, (2) whether it provides males with appreciable energetic benefits, and (3) whether feeding on embryos affects male behavior and subsequent reproductive success. An experimental approach was used to examine the reponse of custodial males to enrichment of their food supply. The subjects were fed eggs from other S. rectifraenum nests and the rate at which they fed on their own brood was examined. The group fed eggs had significantly fewer cannibalized eggs in their stomachs than the controls, supporting the hypothesis that males use the eggs as a food source. The stomach contents of control animals indicated that the average male parent consumes about 5.0% of an average-sized clutch, representing about 13% of a male's total caloric intake. A second egg-feeding experiment suggested that S. rectifraenum does not gain a short-term mating advantage through partial-clutch filial cannibalism. It is likely that the energy gained through this behavior is channeled into growth and survival. The implications of these results for the evolution of male parental care in fishes is discussed.
Article
Two hypotheses can explain the overproduction of zygotes. Bet-hedging assumes that optimal brood size varies unpredictably among breeding attempts. Excess zygotes are produced so that the number of independent offspring can be flexibly adjusted downward to the optimum number for that attempt. Selective abortion suggests that parents overproduce zygotes, identify those with the highest fitness expectations, then kill or abandon those with lower fitness in order to concentrate investment in those with the best prospects. Both hypotheses for the overproduction of zygotes work in principle, alone or together, and can lead to impressive levels of zygote overproduction. For both hypotheses, high levels of zygote overproduction are only attained when the unit cost of an aborted embryo is low relative to the cost of an independent offspring. Under bet-hedging, it is also important that the variability of environmental conditions important for breeding success be high. The two hypotheses together make clear when a parent could increase its fitness by killing or abandoning its offspring.
Article
Filial cannibalism has been described in many fish species with male parental care, and has typically been explained as a response to high energetic costs of brood defence and decreased feeding opportunities during the period of care. We investigated filial cannibalism in an insect, the assassin bug Rhinocoris tristis. In this species, males guard eggs of a number of females, cannibalizing some of their offspring within the brood. We monitored guarding males in both the field and the laboratory. Males typically ate eggs around the periphery of the brood, which were those most likely to have been parasitized by wasps. However, cannibalism persisted in the laboratory in the absence of parasites, and the number of cannibalized eggs was related to the length of care and overall brood size, suggesting that males use eggs as an alternative source of food. This conclusion was further supported by the fact that males in the field did not lose weight while guarding, despite being unable to forage efficiently while caring. Males were also observed to adopt broods, but in a laboratory experiment did not eat more eggs from adopted than from their own broods.
Article
We investigated the effects of male nest guarding on egg survival in the flagfish, a species in which paternal care is thought to be relatively recently evolved. Thus, we characterized the fitness consequences of a major component of paternal care when secondary adaptations to care are likely to be minimal. The effects of guarding under three predation regimes were examined by exposing eggs to treatments in which no predators, conspecific females, and conspecific females and the egg predator Gambusia affinis were present. These were crossed with the presence and absence of parental males. We expected that guarding would result in an increase in egg survival in the presence of egg predators, but considered that in their absence the benefit might be decreased or absent, as a result of filial cannibalism. We also examined effects on egg survivorship stemming from filial cannibalism and egg-directed components of care (i.e. cleaning and fanning); the effects of these behaviours were examined through treatments in which males had full access to eggs (i.e. complete care), males were separated from their eggs by a screen (i.e. egg fanning and no cannibalism), and males were absent (i.e. no care and no cannibalism). In accordance with our expectations, guarding increased egg survivorship in the presence of females and Gambusia. However, egg survivorship when males were alone with eggs was low. Indeed, significantly more eggs were eaten than became diseased, suggesting that males cannibalize healthy eggs. Neither fanning nor cleaning resulted in an increase in egg survivorship. Thus, in flagfish, there may be costs of care when predators are absent. We discuss our findings in relation to sexual selection and the early evolution of parental care.
Article
The territoriality of parental males and the hatching rate of egg masses in their territories were examined in Hypoptychus dybowskii, a species in which territorial males care for egg masses only just after mating, and sneaker males are known to occur. Most males deserted the territories with egg masses, but the hatching rates of abandoned and attended egg masses were similar. A large percentage of the deserted territories with egg masses were taken over by non-territorial males. Comparison of the mating success between males that deserted territories and those that attended territories showed that territory desertion occurred when males experienced fewer chances to mate. These results indicate that territorial males contributed little to embryonic survivorship after egg mass hardening, and deserted their territories depending on the mating rate. Such an unique reproductive strategy may be adaptive when there are no parental costs after arrangement of egg masses, high hatching rates of abandoned egg masses, and a high chance of sneaking.