ArticlePDF Available
COVER PICTURE
Pink and blue: the color of gender
Paolo Frassanito &Benedetta Pettorini
Published online: 4 January 2008
#Springer-Verlag 2007
Assigning color to gender is mostly a twentieth-century
trait. It should be noted that it is a practice limited most
often to Western Europe and the Americas. It would also
seem that the effect of color-coded gender differences (pink
for girls, blue for boys) existed oppositely initially [10].
In fact, this reversal of what we consider normalwas
considered conventional, even in the early twentieth
century. The debate of when and why pink and blue came
into fashion to designate gender rages on, but almost every
argument alludes to a passage in the novel Little Women,
published in 1868: Amy ties a pink bow and a blue bow
on Megs twins Daisy and Demi, so people will know the
difference between the girl and the boy.This is said to be
done in the French style,suggesting that it might have
been possible in France that pink and blue were already
gender-specific.
However, there is evidence that this practice was not
always common or always done throughout much of
Europe. In fact, in the nineteenth century, parents dressed
infants in white dresses, suggesting that color and dresses
were not used to distinguish between girls and boys [3].
At one point, pink was considered more of a boys color,
as a watered-down, bold, dramatic red, which is a fierce
color. Instead, blue was considered more for girls. Probably
this choice was affected by the fact that blue, especially
dark blue, was associated with the Virgin Mary in Christian
Europe. In fact, painters often mixed lapis lazuli into paints
to depict what was considered the most sacred feminine
icon.
The Sunday Sentinal, an American newspaper, in 1914
advised mothers: If you like the color note on the little
ones garments, use pink for the boy and blue for the girl, if
you are a follower of convention(March 29, 1914).
Similarly, Ladies Home Journal informed: There has been
a great diversity of opinion on the subject, but the generally
accepted rule is pink for the boy and blue for the girl. The
reason is that pink being a more decided and stronger color
is more suitable for the boy, while blue, which is more
delicate and dainty, is prettier for the girl(June, 1918).
The current pink for girls and blue for boys was not
uniform until the 1950s [11]. It would seem that Nazi
Germany had something to do with the association of pink
with femininity: catholic traditions in Germany and
neighboring countries reverse the current color coding,
because of the strong association of blue with the Virgin
Mary; the Nazis in their concentration camps use a pink
triangle to identify homosexuals. The Nazis choice of pink
suggests that, by the 1930s, it was a color that in Germany
had become associate with girls.
Thus, Dress Maker Magazine stated: The preferred
color to dress young boys in was pink! Blue was reserved
for girls as it was considered the paler, more dainty of the
two colors, and pink was thought to be the stronger (akin to
red). It was not until WWII that the colors were reversed
and pink was used for girls and blue for boys...
After World War II, blue was used extensively for mens
uniforms. Therefore, blue became associated as more of a
masculine color. From the 1940s onward, pink was pushed
as a womans color. Think pinkwas the marketing slogan
to convince women to embrace their femininity.
The 1950s featured a virtual color explosion, not only in
clothing, but also in things like appliances and furniture.
Dressing children in pink and blue to specifically denote
gender suggested the rising middle class and above. In
Childs Nerv Syst (2008) 24:881882
DOI 10.1007/s00381-007-0559-3
P. Frassanito (*):B. Pettorini
Pediatric Neurosurgery, Catholic University,
Largo A. Gemelli, 8,
00168 Rome, Italy
e-mail: paolo.frassanito@gmail.com
other words, people who could afford to make the gender
assignment did so, since many infants appear somewhat
asexual when first born.
Another possible theory links pink and blue gender
references to the 1950s film Funny Face, which stars
Audrey Hepburn. Hepburn was thought an extremely
feminine woman, and her outfits in pink may have proven
inspiring. This explanation is somewhat unlikely, given that
the film was not released until 1957.
Thus, the pink and blue tradition is recent and relatively
exclusive to the Western world, but the girlspreference for
the color pink seems to have deeper roots. In a recent study,
the researchers report a preference for blue color on a
yellowblue scale both in males and in females, but a girls
preference for red on a greenred scale. This sex difference,
revealed by a rapid paired-comparison task, is robust and
cross-cultural [8]: could it have a biological basis or is it
only social imprinted?
A recent argument proposes a biological basis,
connected to evolved sex differences in specialized visual
pathways that allows females to better discriminate red
wavelengths. The huntergatherer theory proposes that
female brains should be specialized for gathering-related
tasks and is supported by studies of visual abilities [13].
Tricromacy and the second redgreen system (LM
opponent channel) are modernadaptations in primate
evolution thought to have evolved to facilitate the identi-
fication of ripe, yellow fruit or edible red leaves embedded
in green foliage [12]. It is therefore plausible that, in
specializing for gathering, the female brain honed the
trichromatic adaptations (and developed more the P-cell
pathway of vision), and these underpin the female prefer-
ence for objects reddish. Research on foraging in contem-
porary nonhuman primates [5] supports this hypothesis.
Whereas discrimination of red wavelengths appears to
facilitate identification of plant food, a preference for red
or pink appears to have an advantage for successful female
reproduction. This preference for reddish-pink is thought to
exist because infant faces compared to adult ones are
reddish-pink, and red or pink may signal approach
behaviors that enhance infant survival [7].
Similarly, evolutionary theorists have reasoned that selec-
tion pressures might have contributed to spatial abilities in
men that enhanced the hunt and capture of animals, such as
the identification of spatial position, object movement, and a
global analysis of visual scenes [6], that are processed by the
M-cell pathway, phylogenetically older [1]. Other findings in
studies on primates are consistent with androgen-dependent
effects on visual processing pathway structure at the level of
the cortex, supporting a biological basis of the preference for
the color [2].
On the other hand, the segregation of the anatomical and
functional properties of the M-cell and P-cell pathways at
the cortical level is less pronounced in infants, if compared
to adults [4], consistent with the proposal that parcellation
and specialization of the visual processing stream is
directed by experience in postnatal life and so enhancing
the social and cultural influences in addition to the
biological basis [9].
References
1. Alexander GM (2003) An evolutionary perspective on sex-typed toy
preferences: pink, blue and the brain. Arch Sex Behav 32:714
2. Bachevalier J, Hagger C (1991) Sex differences in the develop-
ment of learning abilities in primates. Psychoneuroendocrinology
16:177188
3. Chiu SW, Gervan S, Fairbrother C et al (2006) Sex-dimorphic
color preference in children with gender identity disorder: a
comparison in clinical and community controls. Sex Roles
55:385395
4. Dobkins KR, Anderson CM (2002) Color-based motion process-
ing is stronger in infants than in adults. Psychol Sci 13:7680
5. Dominy NJ, Lucas PW (2001) Ecological importance of trichro-
matic vision to primates. Nature 410:363366
6. Eals M, Silverman I (1994) The huntergatherer theory of spatial
sex differences: proximate factors mediating the female advantage
in location memory. Ethol Sociobiol 15:95105
7. Highley JD, Hopkins WD, Hirsch RM et al (1987) Preferences of
female rhesus monkeys (macaca mulatta) for infantile coloration.
Dev Psychobiol 20:718
8. Hurlbert AC, Ling Y (2007) Biological compnents of sex
differences in color preference. Curr Biol 17(16):623625
9. Johnson MH (2001) The development and neural basis of face
recognition: comment and speculation. Infant Child Dev 10:3133
10. Paoletti JB (1997) The gendering of infantsand toddlers
clothing in America. In: Martine KA, Ames KL (eds) The
material culture of gender, the gender of material culture.
University Press of New England, Hanover, NH
11. Paoletti JB, Kregloh C (1989) The childrens department. In:
Kidwell CB, Steele V (eds) Men and women: dressing the part.
Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC
12. Regan BC, Julliot C, Simmen B et al (2001) Fruits, foliage, and
the evolution of primate color vision. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B
Biol Sci 356:229283
13. Silverman I, Eals M (1992) Sex differences in spatial abilities:
evolutionary theory and data. Oxford Press, New York
882 Childs Nerv Syst (2008) 24:881882
... (Paoletti 2012: xviii) In the beginning of the 1900s, "blue" was accepted girly, while "pink" was a boyish color (Paoletti 2012). The best explanation we have found as to why blue was seen as a "girly" color is that it was so strongly associated with Virgin Mary (Frassanito and Pettorini 2008). In the 1940s, boys and girls had started to be dressed with sex-specific clothing and pink became the girls' color, blue the boys' color (Grannan 2021). ...
... Accordingly, any colorjust as any other metaphoris given different meanings in different situations and contexts. "Color" refers to as various frameworks as ethnicity (Williams 1966), legality (Smith 2019), gender (Frassanito and Pettorini 2008) and politics (Levendusky and Pope 2011), just to mention a few of the many dimensions and distinctions we have seen color refers to. The most well-spread ones of the color metaphorssuch as white-and blue-collar workersmay be more universal than other metaphors, although there is no reason to believe that there are no diverging meanings even of these well-spread metaphors. ...
Chapter
This chapter acknowledges color metaphors in organization studies, and focuses on one group of color metaphors that may be the most frequently used—the variety of color-coded versions of the collar metaphor, such as blue- and white-collar workers. The chapter is based on an unsystematic literature review, and identifies and discusses a set of fairly common color metaphors that have occurred in both more academic and more practitioner-oriented writings. The authors make efforts to trace how certain colors have been given certain meanings, as they have been used as metaphors in combination with “collar.” The authors give examples of the many different meanings that there are of any single color, discuss the universality of meaning of any particular color, and suggest that caution is exercised by any organizational researcher who wants to include color metaphors in their research.
... Tarihsel süreç içerisinde pembe, sertliği ve cesareti temsil eden kırmızı renginden üretildiği için erkeklere özgü olarak kabul edilmiştir. Bunun yanı sıra mavi rengi de dini temsillerin etkisinden (Hristiyanlıkta Meryem Ana'nın lacivert ile ilişkilendirilmesi) dolayı kadınlara özgü olarak atfedilmiştir (Frassanito & Pettorini, 2008). Ancak bu durum özellikle 20. ...
... Bu görsellerde et ürünleri yapanların erkekler olduğu görülürken, kadınların sebze gibi ürünlerde temsil edildiği görülmektedir. Diğer taraftan kadınların pembe renkle, erkeklerin ise mavi renkle temsil edildiği günümüz toplumunda (Frassanito & Pettorini, 2008;Pomerleau, Bolduc, Malcuit, & Cossette, 1990) üretilen yemeklerde de #foodporn görselleri bu durumu koruyan ve yayılmasını sağlayan içeriklerden oluşmaktadır. ...
Article
Full-text available
Food porn kavramı, yemeğin pornografik gösterim biçimleriyle sunulmaya başlamasıyla ortaya çıkmıştır. İçerisinde hazzı ve ulaşılmazlığı barındıran kavram, Instagram gibi görsel ağların yükselişiyle birlikte kendisine yeni bir varoluş alanı bulmuştur. Bugüne kadar Instagram’da #foodporn hashtagi kullanılarak yaklaşık olarak 300 milyon görsel paylaşılmıştır. Bu sayı oldukça yüksek ve yemeğin dijital çağda geçirdiği dönüşümü anlamak için oldukça önemlidir. Bu çalışma, Instagram’da #foodporn hashtagi kullanılarak paylaşılan görsellerin içerik, sunum ve çağrışımlarla nasıl anlatılar oluşturduğuna odaklanmaktadır. Bu amaçla #foodporn hashtagli görseller netnografik yöntem aracılığıyla incelenmiş ve sunulan imgelerle ilgili çeşitli alt kategoriler oluşturulmuştur. İlk kategori olan içerik, zararlı ve sağlıklı olmak üzere ikiye ayrılmaktadır. İkinci kategori olan sunum, yaratıcılık/ustalık, yerellik (kültürel) ve statü/kimlik üçe ayrılmaktadır. Üçüncü kategori olan çağrışım ise cinsellik ve toplumsal cinsiyet normları yansıtan ataerkil #foodporn içerikli görsellerden oluşmaktadır. Sonuç olarak bu çalışmada Instagram’da oluşturulan #foodporn görsellerin çeşitli şekillerde anlam ve çağrışım yaratan görseller sunduğu ve görsellerin toplumsal, kültürel ve ekonomik yapıyı temsil eden içeriklerden oluştuğu belirlenmiştir.
... However, if we compare this finding with earlier research, it becomes evident that the relationship between color and gender is culturally constructed. In the nineteenth century, pink was regarded as a symbol of masculinity, while blue was considered more for girls; In the 1940s, both genders exhibited high agreement in color preferences [39,44,50]. Therefore, the notion that pink signifies femininity and blue signifies masculinity merely reflects contemporary gender culture. ...
Article
Full-text available
With the rapid development of technology, VR stores are introduced into retail industry in which virtual shopping assistants(VSAs) gradually replace sales personnel to interact with consumers. Existing research has focused on the impact of sensory elements of service assistants on purchase intentions, but the exploration of multi-sensory interactions remains relatively insufficient. Based on stereotype content model(SCM) and gender consistency theories, the aim of this research is to investigate how visual-audio cues of virtual shopping assistants influence young consumers’ purchase intentions. A 2 (color: pink vs. blue) * 3(voice type: male vs. female vs. child) * 2 (voice pitch: high vs. low) online experiment (n = 507) was conducted. The results revealed that gender consistency influenced young consumers’ attitudes and behaviors when a virtual shopping assistant talked in high-pitch female voice or low-pitch male voice. Moreover, when a virtual shopping assistant talked in high-pitch female voice, the relationship between color and purchase intention is significantly mediated by competence, sociability, morality and discomfort. When a virtual shopping assistant talked in low-pitch male voice, the relationship between color and purchase intention is significantly mediated by competence, sociability and morality. However, young consumers didn’t evaluate virtual shopping assistants in children or neutral voice based on the principle of gender consistency, indicating a loosening of traditional gender norms among youth. These findings enrich theoretical research on cross-modal sensory marketing and develop SCM theory from the perspective of gender stereotype. Additionally, this study further emphasizes the necessity for retailers to prioritize gender consistency in the design of VSAs, as an optimal design has the potential to shape positive initial impressions and subsequently influence consumers’ purchasing decisions.
... First is the use of the color pink, carefully selected by the make-up artist to appeal to her primary clientele, young women. Research shows that pink and blue colors are stereotypically gendered and appeal to females and males respectively (e.g., Koller, 2008;Frassanito & Pettorini, 2008;LoBue & DeLoache, 2011). Koller (2008), in particular, stressed that the color pink is a semiotic resource of femininity that accentuates the sexuality of women. ...
... Renklere bugün uygulandığı haliyle kadınsı/erkeksi anlamların yüklenmesi sadece 20. Yüzyılın başından beri mevcut olan bir uygulamadır ve köken itibariyle de batı dünyasına özgüdür (Frassanito ve Pettorini, 2008). Sanayi devrimi ile yaygınlaşan bu uygulama önce moda ve tekstil endüstrisinde kendini göstermiş, renkler çeşitlendirilerek kız ve erkek çocuklarına özel renkler önerilmeye başlanmıştır. ...
Article
Integrating insights from several different theoretical perspectives, this research formulates and tests a conceptualization of how men’s use of products containing features stereotypically associated with women (e.g., pink-colored products) induces them to become more supportive of women. This effect is held to arise because men’s use of pink products leads them to have an experience similar to the one associated with women, encouraging male users to adopt the perspective of women. In turn, this perspective-taking increases their willingness to engage in behaviors supportive of women. Across six studies, we found that when men used pink-colored products (e.g., pens, headphones, folders), they subsequently showed greater support for women, such as writing supportive messages, sharing information about gender equality, donating money, and volunteering. However, this effect diminished when men merely saw the pink products rather than used them, when their gender identity was threatened, or when the support target was not women. We also discussed the implications of these findings.
Article
Full-text available
Cinsiyet, günümüzde pazar bölümlendirmesinde yaygın kullanılan bir kriterdir. Süt, anahtarlık, telefon veya otomobil gibi çeşitli ürün kategorilerinde yapılan cinsiyete dayalı pazar bölümlendirmenin temelinde, bireylerin biyolojik cinsiyetlerinden ziyade toplumun yarattığı "toplumsal cinsiyet" anlayışı bulunmaktadır. Ancak, konunun önemi ile kıyaslandığında ürünlerdeki görsel iletişim öğelerine ilişkin toplumsal cinsiyet kalıp yargılarının ve bu yargıların ürün tercihlerindeki rolünün yeterince incelenmediği görülmektedir. Bu eksiklikten yola çıkarak, bu araştırmada üründeki görsel iletişim öğelerinin tüketicilerin algılamalarına ve satın alma davranışlarına etkileri, toplumsal cinsiyet bağlamında ele alınarak incelenmiştir. Nicel yöntem kullanılarak, doğrudan ve dolaylı ölçüm içeren bir anket formu ile 715 katılımcıdan kolayda örnekleme yoluyla veri toplanmıştır. Sonuçlar, tüketicilerin ürünlerdeki görsel iletişim öğelerini değerlendirirken toplumsal cinsiyet kalıp yargılarından yoğun biçimde faydalandığını göstermekte ve marka logo tasarımlarıyla birlikte ürün ambalajının tasarımı ve renginin toplumsal cinsiyet kalıp yargılarıyla tüketicilerin satın alma tercihi arasındaki ilişkiyi daha iyi anlamamıza katkı sağlamaktadır. Çalışmanın bulguları, toplumsal cinsiyet kalıp yargılarının tüketici davranışlarına etkisini anlamamıza ve segmentasyon, konumlandırma ve pazarlama iletişim stratejileri açısından önemli bilgiler sunmaktadır.
Article
Colors have been reported to be associated with genders (e.g., reddish color-females). Here, we investigated color-gender associations (red-female/green-male) among Japanese participants using two Stroop-word categorization tasks. Ten Japanese gendered words were chosen as visual stimuli. In Experiment 1 (N = 23), participants were instructed to indicate whether a target word presented in either red, green, or gray font color was a masculine or feminine word. Results showed a congruency effect of red-female association that red font color facilitated the categorization of feminine words and inhibited the categorization of masculine words compared to other colors. No significant effect of green-male association was observed. Experiment 2 (N = 23 newly recruited participants) examined whether the congruency effect of color-gender associations could bias perceptual font color categorization. Participants were asked to discriminate whether the font color, with low saturation, was red or green while ignoring the word's meaning. Results showed that participants responded faster and made fewer errors when categorizing red font colors for feminine words than masculine words. Additionally, a congruent effect of green-male association on performance accuracy was observed, although it did not affect response times. These results indicate the existence of an automatically activated red-female association, which can influence both conceptual gender categorization and perceptual color processing.
Article
Kawaii is a Japanese cultural uniqueness that attracts attention around the world. This study focused on pink as a typical kawaii color. The goal is to clarify the relation of preferences in pink colors and fashion taste. Four pink colors were selected and used in our questionnaire to collect data about most kawaii and most favorite pink colors as well as the behavior in using pink items in 2020 and 2021. From the questionnaire results, we obtained tendencies of the pink colors for the most kawaii and the most favorite as well as their relationship with fashion trend. Finally, we clarified the pink color that have constant trend over time and the one that tends to be influenced by annual trend. The results suggest that different pink colors may give different impressions according to the annual trend. The results from this study contribute to the fashion industry.
Article
Full-text available
Primates are apparently unique amongst the mammals in possessing trichromatic colour vision. However, not all primates are trichromatic. Amongst the haplorhine (higher) primates, the catarrhines possess uniformly trichromatic colour vision, whereas most of the platyrrhine species exhibit polymorphic colour vision, with a variety of dichromatic and trichromatic phenotypes within the population. It has been suggested that trichromacy in primates and the reflectance functions of certain tropical fruits are aspects of a coevolved seed–dispersal system: primate colour vision has been shaped by the need to find coloured fruits amongst foliage, and the fruits themselves have evolved to be salient to primates and so secure dissemination of their seeds. We review the evidence for and against this hypothesis and we report an empirical test: we show that the spectral positioning of the cone pigments found in trichromatic South American primates is well matched to the task of detecting fruits against a background of leaves. We further report that particular trichromatic platyrrhine phenotypes may be better suited than others to foraging for particular fruits under particular conditions of illumination; and we discuss possible explanations for the maintenance of polymorphic colour vision amongst the platyrrhines.
Article
Full-text available
The present study was designed to assess sex-dimorphic color preferences in children with gender identity disorder (47 boys, 18 girls), clinical controls (65 boys, 35 girls), and community controls (65 boys, 35 girls). The mean age of the children was 7.63years (range = 3–12years). Children were shown a hexagon-shaped display of 144 colors extracted from PowerPoint™. Each child was asked to choose his or her three favorite colors (Trials 1–3) by pointing to them, naming them, and then to provide a justification for each choice. From the entire array, children labeled a total of 11 different colors: black, blue, brown, gray, green, orange, pink, purple, red, white, and yellow. For three of the colors (blue, pink/purple, and red), there was evidence for normative sex differences in color preference, and, for the colors blue and pink/purple, the gender-referred children showed inverted patterns of color preference. For the color blue, luminance values showed that the gender-referred boys and control girls preferred lighter shades, whereas the gender-referred girls and control boys preferred darker shades. Qualitative analysis indicated that gender-specific justifications were uncommon, even for the sex-dimorphic colors. Gender-referred children showed inverted gender-stereotyped color preferences, which are likely related to their more general pattern of cross-gender identification. Principles of gender-differentiated development derived from gender-schema theory are used to explain the group differences in color preferences.
Article
Full-text available
Trichromatic colour vision, characterized by three retinal photopigments tuned to peak wavelengths of approximately 430 nm, approximately 535 nm and approximately 562 nm (refs 1, 2), has evolved convergently in catarrhine primates and one genus of New World monkey, the howlers (genus Alouatta). This uniform capacity to discriminate red-green colours, which is not found in other mammals, has been proposed as advantageous for the long-range detection of either ripe fruits or young leaves (which frequently flush red in the tropics) against a background of mature foliage. Here we show that four trichromatic primate species in Kibale Forest, Uganda, eat leaves that are colour discriminated only by red-greenness, a colour axis correlated with high protein levels and low toughness. Despite their divergent digestive systems, these primates have no significant interspecific differences in leaf colour selection. In contrast, eaten fruits were generally discriminated from mature leaves on both red-green and yellow-blue channels and also by their luminance, with a significant difference between chimpanzees and monkeys in fruit colour choice. Our results implicate leaf consumption, a critical food resource when fruit is scarce, as having unique value in maintaining trichromacy in catarrhines.
Article
Full-text available
Large sex differences in children's toy preferences are attributed to gender group identification and social learning. The proposal outlined in this paper is that contemporary conceptual categories of "masculine" or "feminine" toys are also influenced by evolved perceptual categories of male-preferred and female-preferred objects. Research on children exposed prenatally to atypical levels of androgens and research on typically developing infants suggest sex-dimorphic preferences exist for object features, such as movement or color/form. The evolution and neurobiology of mammalian visual processing--and recent findings on sex-dimorphic toy preferences in nonhuman primates--suggest further that an innate bias for processing object movement or color/form may contribute to behaviors with differential adaptive significance for males and females. In this way, preferences for objects such as toys may indicate a biological preparedness for a "masculine" or "feminine" gender role-one that develops more fully as early perceptual preferences are coupled with object experiences imposed by contemporary gender socialization.
Chapter
Anthropologists have long recognized that cultural evolution critically depends on the transmission and generation of information. However, between the selection pressures of evolution and the actual behaviour of individuals, scientists have suspected that other processes are at work. With the advent of what has come to be known as the cognitive revolution, psychologists are now exploring the evolved problem-solving and information-processing mechanisms that allow humans to absorb and generate culture. The purpose of this book is to introduce the newly crystallizing field of evolutionary psychology, which supplied the necessary connection between the underlying evolutionary biology and the complex and irreducible social phenomena studied by anthropologists, sociologists, economists, and historians.
Article
While developmental researchers have long noted that infants of many mammalian species, including primates, have characteristic physical features, such as a distinctive coat or skin color, and some authors have further suggested that these features serve to elicit caretaking and solicitous behavior, few systematic investigations of the actual salience of such features for adult conspecifics have been carried out to date. The present series of 3 studies sought to determine whether natal coat and/or facial skin coloration might provide desirable visual stimulation for rhesus monkey adult females. In these studies the faces and/or fur of 6-month-old rhesus monkeys were dyed to simulate the normal coloration of rhesus monkey neonates. Adult females varying in parity and rearing history were then tested for their relative preference between these infantile-colored 6-month-olds and both normally colored 6-month-olds and other differentially colored control stimulus monkeys. Results indicated that regardless of parity or rearing history the adult female subjects exhibited consistent preferences for the stimulus animals with neonatal-like reddish-pink facial skin coloration.
Article
Based on their theory that sex differences in spatial abilities originated in human evolution as a function of division of labor, Silverman and Eals (1992) demonstrated in a series of studies that females consistently surpassed males in recall of locations of objects in a spatial array. The present studies were replications of the above, but with the inclusion of uncommon objects, for which subjects would not possess verbal labels. Female superiority for recall of locations of common objects as observed in Silverman and Eals was replicated across incidental and directed learning conditions. The female advantage occured as well for uncommon objects, but only under incidental learning conditions. Conjectures are offered regarding sex differences in attentional and imagery processes that could account for this pattern of results.
Article
The role of steroid gonadal hormones in promoting sex differences in reproductive behaviors has been thoroughly studied in numerous mammalian species. More recent experiments have indicated that the presence or absence of steroid hormones during the critical period of brain differentiation likewise might promote the development of sex differences in cognitive abilities. Studies in infant rhesus monkeys have demonstrated that there exist sex differences in learning abilities that can be altered by perinatal hormonal manipulations, suggesting that testosterone might be a crucial factor responsible for the development of sex differences in cognitive styles. In addition, neonatal lesion studies have shown that the cortical areas mediating specific learning abilities mature at different rates in male and female infant monkeys. These findings support the view that the perinatal hormonal environment can affect the rate of brain maturation by influencing neuronal connectivity at the cortical level. The combined data from sex differences in learning abilities in human infants and their reversibility in endocrinological syndromes suggest that testosterone may affect the maturation of the human brain in a manner similar to that demonstrated in nonhuman primates.
Article
One hallmark of vision in adults is the dichotomy between color and motion processing. Specifically, areas of the brain that encode an object's direction of motion are thought to receive little information about object color We investigated the development of this dichotomy by conducting psychophysical experiments with human subjects (2-, 3-, and 4-month-olds and adults), using a novel red-green stimulus that isolates color-based input to motion processing. When performance on this red-green motion stimulus was quantified with respect to performance on a luminance (yellow-black) standard, we found stronger color-based motion processing in infants than in adults. These results suggest that color input to motion areas is greater early in life, and that motion areas then specialize to the adultlike state by reweighting or selectively pruning their inputs over the course of development.