ArticlePDF Available

Does Intrinsic Motivation Fuel the Prosocial Fire? Motivational Synergy in Predicting Persistence, Performance, and Productivity

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Researchers have obtained conflicting results about the role of prosocial motivation in persistence, performance, and productivity. To resolve this discrepancy, I draw on self-determination theory, proposing that prosocial motivation is most likely to predict these outcomes when it is accompanied by intrinsic motivation. Two field studies support the hypothesis that intrinsic motivation moderates the association between prosocial motivation and persistence, performance, and productivity. In Study 1, intrinsic motivation strengthened the relationship between prosocial motivation and the overtime hour persistence of 58 firefighters. In Study 2, intrinsic motivation strengthened the relationship between prosocial motivation and the performance and productivity of 140 fundraising callers. Callers who reported high levels of both prosocial and intrinsic motivations raised more money 1 month later, and this moderated association was mediated by a larger number of calls made. I discuss implications for theory and research on work motivation.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Does Intrinsic Motivation Fuel the Prosocial Fire? Motivational Synergy in
Predicting Persistence, Performance, and Productivity
Adam M. Grant
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Researchers have obtained conflicting results about the role of prosocial motivation in persistence,
performance, and productivity. To resolve this discrepancy, I draw on self-determination theory,
proposing that prosocial motivation is most likely to predict these outcomes when it is accompanied by
intrinsic motivation. Two field studies support the hypothesis that intrinsic motivation moderates the
association between prosocial motivation and persistence, performance, and productivity. In Study 1,
intrinsic motivation strengthened the relationship between prosocial motivation and the overtime hour
persistence of 58 firefighters. In Study 2, intrinsic motivation strengthened the relationship between
prosocial motivation and the performance and productivity of 140 fundraising callers. Callers who
reported high levels of both prosocial and intrinsic motivations raised more money 1 month later, and this
moderated association was mediated by a larger number of calls made. I discuss implications for theory
and research on work motivation.
Keywords: prosocial motivation, intrinsic motivation, persistence, job performance, work productivity
Why do employees go above and beyond the call of duty to
persist in performing their work effectively and productively?
Organizational scholars have begun to highlight prosocial motiva-
tion—the desire to benefit other people—as an important answer
to this question. Researchers have argued that prosocial motivation
facilitates enhanced persistence, performance, and productivity by
enabling dedication to a cause (Thompson & Bunderson, 2003) or
moral principle (Shamir, 1990), a commitment to the people who
benefit from one’s efforts (Grant, 2007), and a willingness to
accept and utilize negative feedback (Meglino & Korsgaard,
2004). Indeed, a number of studies using different conceptualiza-
tions and measures related to prosocial motivation suggest that
prosocial motivation is associated with higher levels of persis-
tence, performance, and productivity across various tasks, jobs,
and extrarole behaviors (e.g., Bing & Burroughs, 2001; Brewer &
Selden, 1998; Grant et al., 2007; Korsgaard, Meglino, & Lester,
1997; Naff & Crum, 1999; Rioux & Penner, 2001; see also Ilies,
Scott, & Judge, 2006).
However, several studies have returned less encouraging results,
suggesting that particular forms of prosocial motivation are not
significantly related to job performance evaluations (Alonso &
Lewis, 2001) or the performance of organizational citizenship
behaviors (Konovsky & Organ, 1996). One explanation for these
conflicting results is that researchers have overlooked an important
dimension along which prosocial motivation varies. Self-
determination theory suggests that prosocial motivation can be
based on different levels of autonomous regulation; the desire to
benefit others can be autonomously supported by feelings of
identification and value congruence or can be coerced by feelings
of pressure and obligation (Gagne´ & Deci, 2005; Ryan & Connell,
1989). This distinction may have critical implications for under-
standing whether and when prosocial motivation promotes persis-
tence, performance, and productivity.
In this article, I build on this core insight to develop and test a
contingency perspective on prosocial motivation. Drawing on self-
determination theory, I propose that prosocial motivation is most
likely to enhance persistence, performance, and productivity when
it is accompanied by intrinsic motivation. Two studies of firefight-
ers and fundraising callers provide support for the proposed mod-
erating role of intrinsic motivation, demonstrating a synergistic
interaction of prosocial and intrinsic motivations in predicting
higher levels of persistence, performance, and productivity. To-
gether, the studies advance existing knowledge about the boundary
conditions of prosocial motivation and fill a gap in self-
determination theory and research about the interactions of intrin-
sic motivation with other forms of regulation.
Intrinsic and Prosocial Motivations
Motivation is a foundational topic in psychology and orga-
nizational studies. Because it describes the reasons that drive
actions, an understanding of motivation is central to explaining
both individual and organizational behavior (e.g., Mitchell &
Daniels, 2003). Motivation refers to the psychological pro-
I am grateful to the National Science Foundation Graduate Research
Fellowship, the American Psychological Association Early Research
Award, and the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology Lee
Hakel Scholarship for providing financial support for the preparation of
this article.
I also appreciate constructive feedback on drafts of this article from
Sabine Sonnentag, Jeff Thompson, Teresa Cardador, and members of the
Impact Lab, especially Justin Berg. For assistance with data collection, I
thank Grace Chen, Wes Clark, Christy Flanagan, Howard Heevner, David
Lapedis, and Dave McIntyre.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Adam M.
Grant, Kenan-Flagler Business School, University of North Carolina at
Chapel Hill, Campus Box 3490, McColl Building, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-
3490. E-mail: agrant@unc.edu
Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association
2008, Vol. 93, No. 1, 48 –58 0021-9010/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.48
48
cesses that direct, energize, and sustain action (e.g., Latham &
Pinder, 2005) or “an inner desire to make an effort” (Dowling
& Sayles, 1978, p. 16).
Motivation researchers have long recognized that this desire to
make an effort can derive from different sources (e.g., Herzberg,
1966; Porter & Lawler, 1968; Staw, 1977). Early in the 20th
century, scholars and practitioners believed that external controls,
incentives, punishments, and rewards were necessary to motivate
persistence, performance, and productivity (see Heath, 1999;
Steers, Mowday, & Shapiro, 2004). The advent of the human-
relations movement paved the way for a new view of motivation.
Rather than assuming that employees dislike work, scholars began
to propose that work can be inherently interesting and enjoyable
(e.g., Herzberg, 1966; McGregor, 1960). This view is developed in
and represented by theories of self-determination and intrinsic
motivation (e.g., Deci, 1975; Deci & Ryan, 1985). Intrinsic moti-
vation refers to the desire to expend effort based on interest in and
enjoyment of the work itself (Amabile, Hill, Hennessey, & Tighe,
1994; Gagne´ & Deci, 2005; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Intrinsic moti-
vation is typically contrasted with extrinsic motivation—the desire
to expend effort to obtain outcomes external to the work itself,
such as rewards or recognition (Amabile, 1993; Brief & Aldag,
1977).
Prosocial motivation is the desire to expend effort to benefit
other people (Batson, 1987). Like intrinsic motivation (Amabile et
al., 1994), prosocial motivation has been conceptualized in both
traitlike and statelike terms. As a relatively enduring individual
difference, prosocial motivation is reflected in the personality trait
of agreeableness (Graziano, Habashi, Sheese, & Tobin, in press),
dispositions toward empathy and helpfulness (Penner, Dovidio,
Piliavin, & Schroeder, 2005), and values of concern for others (De
Dreu, 2006; Meglino & Korsgaard, 2004; Perry & Hondeghem, in
press; Schwartz, 1992). As a more temporary psychological state,
prosocial motivation involves a momentary focus on the goal of
protecting and promoting the welfare of other people, which is
typically prompted by contact with others who need help (Batson,
1987; Grant, 2007).
Differentiating Prosocial and Intrinsic Motivations
Because prosocial and intrinsic motivations have largely been
studied in separate literatures, researchers have made few attempts
to integrate understandings of the two motivations. What is the
relationship between the two motivations? On one hand, research-
ers have often assumed that prosocial motivation is a specific form
of intrinsic motivation. For instance, job design researchers have
classified opportunities to benefit others as intrinsic rewards
(Herzberg, Mausner, & Snyderman, 1967) that increase intrinsic
motivation (Hackman & Oldham, 1976).
On the other hand, the two forms of motivation reflect different
scholarly assumptions about the drivers of motivation. Intrinsic
motivation takes a hedonic perspective by emphasizing pleasure
and enjoyment as drivers of effort, whereas prosocial motivation
takes a eudaimonic perspective by emphasizing meaning and pur-
pose as drivers of effort (Kahn, 1990; McGregor & Little, 1998;
Ryan & Deci, 2001; Waterman, 1993). Moreover, psychologists
have demonstrated that prosocial and intrinsic motivations involve
different reasons for expending effort. For intrinsically motivated
individuals, effort is based on interest and enjoyment; for proso-
cially motivated individuals, effort is based on a desire to benefit
others (Ryan & Connell, 1989). This basic distinction highlights
three important differences between the two forms of motivation:
self-regulation (autonomous vs. introjected/identified), goal direct-
edness (process vs. outcome), and temporal focus (present vs.
future).
First, prosocial and intrinsic motivations involve different levels
of autonomy in self-regulation (Ryan & Deci, 2000). When intrin-
sically motivated, employees feel naturally drawn, or pulled, to-
ward completing their work. The decision to expend effort is based
on personal enjoyment and is thus fully volitional, self-determined
and autonomous (Kehr, 2004). When prosocially motivated, em-
ployees are more likely to push themselves toward completing
their work. The decision to expend effort is less autonomous, as it
is based more heavily on conscious self-regulation and self-control
to achieve a goal (Gagne´ & Deci, 2005). Prosocial motivation is
therefore characterized not as a state of pure intrinsic motivation,
but instead as a state of introjected or identified regulation. Em-
ployees are driven not by inherent interest in the work itself, but
rather by introjected goals of avoiding guilt and protecting self-
esteem or by identified goals of fulfilling core values and identities
(Ryan & Deci, 2000; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999; Sheldon & Houser-
Marko, 2001).
Second, prosocial and intrinsic motivations differ in terms of
goal directedness. When intrinsically motivated, employees are
process focused—they see the work as an end in and of itself
(Amabile, 1993; Bono & Judge, 2003; Wrzesniewski, McCauley,
Rozin, & Schwartz, 1997). When prosocially motivated, employ-
ees are outcome focused—they see the work as a means to the end
goal of benefiting others (Grant, 2007). Put differently, intrinsic
motivation is a paratelic state in which the work is inherently
enjoyable, whereas prosocial motivation is a telic state in which
the work is instrumental to a purpose or goal (Apter, 1984;
Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, 1988).
Third, the previous distinction highlights that prosocial and
intrinsic motivations differ in terms of temporal focus. When
intrinsically motivated, employees are present focused—they are
concerned with the experience of performing the work itself
(R. W. Quinn, 2005). When prosocially motivated, employees are
future focused—they are concerned with achieving a meaningful
outcome upon completing the work (Batson, 1998).
To illustrate, consider the case of a university professor present-
ing a lecture to students. When intrinsically motivated, the teach-
er’s effort is based on enjoyment of the task of lecturing, which
provides joy and pleasure in the process of performing. When
prosocially motivated, the teacher’s effort is based on a desire to
educate students, which provides meaning and fulfillment in the
outcome of student learning. In summary, prosocial and intrinsic
motivations differ along at least three dimensions. Intrinsic moti-
vation involves autonomous self-regulation and a focus on a pro-
cess in the present. Prosocial motivation involves self-regulation
introjected or identified with values and a focus on an outcome in
the future. These distinctions suggest that the two motivations can
be understood as relatively independent. As such, it possible for
the two motivations to interact, a possibility that is central to the
present research.
49
INTRINSIC AND PROSOCIAL MOTIVATIONS
Hypotheses
How do intrinsic and prosocial motivations interact to influence
persistence, performance, and productivity? Persistence refers to
the amount of time that employees invest in their efforts (e.g.,
Dweck & Gilliard, 1975; Sandelands, Brockner, & Glynn, 1988).
Performance refers to the degree to which employees’ behaviors
achieve organizational objectives (e.g., Beal, Cohen, Burke, &
McLendon, 2003; Campbell, 1990; McCloy, Campbell, & Cudeck,
1994; Motowidlo, 2003). Productivity describes an outcome of
performance—the quantity of output that results from performance
behaviors as well as external contextual and opportunity factors
(e.g., Blumberg & Pringle, 1982; Schmidt & Hunter, 1983).
In this section, I develop the hypothesis that intrinsic motivation
strengthens the positive association between prosocial motivation
and persistence, performance, and productivity. The central logic
underlying this hypothesis is that prosocial motivation takes on
different degrees of autonomous regulation depending on the level
of intrinsic motivation. Employees can be motivated to benefit
others because they want to help or because they feel that they
have to help (Cunningham, Steinberg, & Grey, 1980; Gagne´,
2003). As Ryan and Connell (1989, p. 757) explained, “A person
can have reasons for acting prosocially that can be variously
construed as external or introjected, or as outcomes of identifica-
tions.” When intrinsic motivation is high, prosocial motivation is
characterized by identified regulation: employees feel that com-
pleting their tasks is beneficial to their own self-selected goals, as
they enjoy the process of working and value the outcome of
helping others (Gagne´ & Deci, 2005). When intrinsic motivation is
low, prosocial motivation is characterized by introjected regula-
tion: employees do not enjoy the process of working, but they put
pressure on themselves to do so in order to help others (Ryan &
Connell, 1989).
Accordingly, self-determination theory suggests that when in-
trinsic motivation is high, prosocial motivation will be positively
associated with persistence, performance, and productivity. When
intrinsic motivation is present, because employees enjoy the pro-
cess of completing their tasks, they will feel volition, autonomy,
and free choice in their efforts to benefit others, experiencing
prosocial motivation as identified regulation (Ryan & Connell,
1989). Thus, when intrinsic motivation is high, prosocial motiva-
tion will increase effort by providing an important outcome goal—
helping others—for employees to pursue (Gagne´ & Deci, 2005).
As a result, prosocial motivation will increase employees’ willing-
ness to invest time and energy in their tasks, persisting to perform
them effectively and productively. Indeed, recent research on
self-determination theory suggests that intrinsically motivated ef-
forts to benefit others enable employees to fulfill their basic
psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness
(Sheldon, Arndt, & Houser-Marko, 2003). Employees experience
autonomy in acting freely to benefit others, competence in suc-
cessfully helping others, and relatedness in connecting their ac-
tions to outcomes that matter in the lives of other people (Grant,
2007). Accordingly, when intrinsic motivation is high, prosocial
motivation is likely to promote high levels of persistence, perfor-
mance, and productivity.
On the other hand, self-determination theory suggests that when
intrinsic motivation is low, prosocial motivation will be less pos-
itively associated with persistence, performance, and productivity.
In the absence of intrinsic motivation, because employees do not
enjoy the process of working, they will feel pressured to benefit
others, experiencing prosocial motivation as introjected regulation
(Ryan & Connell, 1989; see also Cunningham et al., 1980). A core
premise of self-determination theory is that employees prefer to be
originators of their own behavior rather than pawns of the will of
others (deCharms, 1968; Deci, 1971; Deci, Koestner, & Ryan,
1999). The experience of pressure threatens employees’ abilities to
fulfill their basic psychological needs for autonomy (Ryan & Deci,
2000). Employees can respond to this pressure in one of two ways.
First, employees can escape the pressure by directly reducing their
engagement and effort levels (Bazerman, Tenbrunsel, & Wade-
Benzoni, 1998). This self-determined choice will enable them to
regain their feelings of autonomy at the expense of persistence,
performance, and productivity. Second, employees can succumb to
the pressure by expending additional effort to fulfill their obliga-
tions to help others. As employees feel pressured to contribute over
and above what they find interesting and enjoyable, their feelings
of autonomy will be threatened (Gagne´ & Deci, 2005), and they
will be more likely to experience stress and role overload (Bolino
& Turnley, 2005). These psychological costs will undermine em-
ployees’ capabilities to persist in performing their tasks effectively
and productively. Thus, when intrinsic motivation is low, prosocial
motivation is less likely to enhance persistence, performance, and
productivity.
Hypothesis 1. Intrinsic motivation moderates the relationship
between prosocial motivation and persistence, performance,
and productivity. The higher the intrinsic motivation, the
stronger the positive association between prosocial motiva-
tion and persistence, performance, and productivity.
Overview of the Present Research
To test these hypotheses, I conducted studies in two occupa-
tions—firefighting and fundraising—in which I expected prosocial
motivation to be a psychologically meaningful variable. The first
study provides an initial test of the interaction between prosocial
and intrinsic motivations as a predictor of persistence (the number
of overtime hours worked by municipal firefighters). The second
study examines the interaction between prosocial and intrinsic
motivations as a predictor of the performance and productivity of
fundraising callers.
Study 1
Method
Participants and Procedure
Participants were 58 paid municipal firefighters (2 women, 56
men) at a fire department serving a community in the Midwest
U.S. Municipal firefighting is a complex job (e.g., Peterson, Bor-
man, Mumford, Jeannerete, & Fleishman, 1999). It requires ex-
tensive knowledge about service, safety, security, mechanics, med-
ical treatment, and construction, as well as strong skills in
coordination, critical thinking, problem-solving, monitoring, and
judgment and decision-making. The training chief allocated time
for firefighters to complete surveys during required monthly train-
ing sessions. I visited the organization, distributed consent forms,
50
GRANT
and assured firefighters that their responses would be confidential
and anonymous and would be shared only in aggregate form
without any personally identifying information.
Measures
Unless otherwise indicated, all items used 7-point Likert-type
scales with anchors of 1 (disagree strongly)to7(agree strongly).
Prosocial and intrinsic motivations. I measured prosocial and
intrinsic motivations with items adapted from self-regulation
scales developed by Ryan and Connell (1989). An introductory
question asked, “Why are you motivated to do your work?”. The
surveys contained four items measuring each form of motivation.
The four items for prosocial motivation were “Because I care
about benefiting others through my work,” “Because I want to help
others through my work,” “Because I want to have positive impact
on others,” and “Because it is important to me to do good for
others through my work” (␣⫽.90). The four items for intrinsic
motivation were “Because I enjoy the work itself,” “Because it’s
fun,” “Because I find the work engaging,” and “Because I enjoy it”
(␣⫽.71).
Persistence. For the outcome measure, 2 months after the
surveys were completed, the training chief provided data on the
number of overtime hours that firefighters had worked in the
previous week. Overtime hours has been conceptualized as an
indicator of persistence, as it measures the amount of time that
employees invest in their work (Mitchell & Daniels, 2003). Fire-
fighters were allowed to sign up for overtime hours in advance,
thus demonstrating persistence.
Results
An exploratory factor analysis using principal axis factoring
with maximum likelihood estimation and an oblique rotation ex-
amined whether the prosocial and intrinsic motivation items were
distinct. The analysis revealed the expected two-factor solution
(eigenvalues 4.08 and 1.50, respectively), and the resulting
prosocial and intrinsic motivation factors were positively corre-
lated (r .41, p .001).
To test the hypothesis that the prosocial-intrinsic motivation
interaction would predict overtime, I followed the procedures
recommended by Aiken and West (1991; see also Cohen, Cohen,
West, & Aiken, 2003). I mean-centered the prosocial and intrinsic
motivation variables and multiplied the two centered variables to
create a continuous interaction term. I then conducted ordinary
least squares regression analyses predicting overtime from proso-
cial motivation, intrinsic motivation, and the interaction term. The
analysis showed that prosocial motivation did not significantly
predict overtime, ␤⫽.02, t(54) 0.14, p .89, but intrinsic
motivation did, ␤⫽.29, t(54) 2.13, p .04, as did the
interaction term, ␤⫽.35, t(54) 2.47, p .02. A hierarchical
regression showed that the addition of the interaction term in-
creased the variance explained in overtime significantly from r
2
.14 (f
2
.16) to r
2
.22 (f
2
.28), F(1, 54) 6.09, p .02.
1
Figure 1 displays the simple slopes for the relationship between
prosocial motivation and overtime at one standard deviation above
and below the mean of intrinsic motivation. Prosocial motivation
was positively associated with overtime when intrinsic motivation
was high (␤⫽.44) but was negatively associated with overtime
when intrinsic motivation was low (␤⫽⫺.53). Firefighters with
high levels of both prosocial and intrinsic motivations averaged
33.12 overtime hours per week, whereas all other firefighters
averaged 19.78 overtime hours per week. Thus, intrinsic motiva-
tion moderated (strengthened) the association between prosocial
motivation and overtime hours.
Discussion
The results provided general support for the role of intrinsic
motivation in moderating the association between prosocial moti-
vation and persistence. Firefighters who reported high levels of
both prosocial and intrinsic motivations worked more overtime
hours 2 months later. Although these results are promising, they
are subject to at least three important limitations. First, the study
focused on a small sample of employees in one occupation, raising
questions about generalizability. Second, I was only able to obtain
one outcome measure (overtime hours), which raises questions
about applications to the domain of effectiveness. Does intrinsic
motivation moderate the association between prosocial motivation
and performance and productivity in a larger sample from a
different occupation?
Third, an alternative explanation for the results is that intrinsic
motivation is a reflection of job satisfaction. Whereas intrinsic
motivation is a specific desire to expend effort based on enjoyment
1
In light of the small sample size, I examined standardized dfbeta and
Cook’s D influence statistics to determine whether the interaction was
dependent on a few cases. First, I used standardized dfbetas to assess the
change in the interaction term’s regression coefficient caused by deleting
each case. After eliminating the standardized dfbetas with absolute values
greater than 2 ⫼公 n (Belsley, Kuh, & Welsch, 1980), or .26 in this sample,
the interaction term was still statistically significant in the positive direc-
tion. Second, I used Cook’s D to assess the multivariate change across
regression coefficients caused by deleting each observation. After I reran
the analyses excluding the three cases that exceeded the conventional
cutoff point (Fox, 1991) of 4 (nk1), or 0.07 in this sample, the
interaction term was still statistically significant in the positive direction.
These analyses suggest that the interaction was robust even after eliminat-
ing influential cases.
0
10
20
30
40
Low High
Prosocial Motivation
Persistence (overtime hours)
Low Intrinsic
High Intrinsic
Figure 1. Study 1 regression slopes for overtime hours.
51
INTRINSIC AND PROSOCIAL MOTIVATIONS
of the tasks themselves (Amabile, 1993), job satisfaction is a more
global attitude—an evaluative judgment about the extent to which
one’s overall work experiences meet one’s expectations or stan-
dards (Harrison, Newman, & Roth, 2006; Warr, 2007; Weiss,
2002). It may not be the experience of autonomous regulation and
self-determination provided by intrinsic motivation, but rather a
generally favorable job attitude, that strengthens the prosocial
motivation-behavior relationship. According to this perspective,
job satisfaction might lead employees to perceive other people in
their work environments in a more favorable light, increasing the
willingness of prosocially motivated employees to expend addi-
tional effort in order to benefit these people (e.g., George, 1991).
Study 2
To address these three issues, I conducted a second study. I
examined intrinsic and prosocial motivations as predictors of the
performance and productivity of fundraising callers and measured
job satisfaction as an alternative explanation of the results.
Method
Participants
Participants were 140 paid fundraising callers (71 women, 69
men) at a call center at a public university in the Midwestern U.S.
All callers worked the same number of hours and shifts and were
responsible for contacting prospective alumni donors in order to
persuade them to give money to the university. The callers thus
provided a service to university faculty, staff, and students, who
benefited from the funds raised, and to alumni, who were updated
on recent events and activities at the university. Callers accessed
alumni names and phone numbers from a computerized database
provided by managers. They were required to use a standardized
script to make their pitches, which instructed them to ask for
donations three times over the course of a single call before
hanging up and turning to the next call.
Procedure
Managers provided time for callers to complete surveys during
their regularly scheduled shifts. I arrived at the organization,
distributed consent forms to the callers, and explained that re-
sponses would be confidential, reported only in the aggregate. I
then distributed surveys to the callers to measure prosocial moti-
vation, intrinsic motivation, and job satisfaction. One month after
callers completed their surveys, managers provided objective mea-
sures of their performance and productivity.
Measures
Unless otherwise indicated, all items used 7-point Likert-type
scales with anchors of 1 (disagree strongly)to7(agree strongly).
Prosocial and intrinsic motivations. Callers completed sur-
veys measuring prosocial motivation (␣⫽.91) and intrinsic mo-
tivation (␣⫽.93) using the same items as in Study 1.
Job satisfaction. Job satisfaction was measured with a four-
item scale developed by R. P. Quinn and Shepard (1974; see also
Eisenberger, Cummings, Armeli, & Lynch, 1997).
Performance and productivity. Managers in the call center
provided objective data for performance and productivity. Perfor-
mance was measured by the number of calls that callers made in a
1-week interval, which is one important element of performing the
job effectively. Productivity was measured by output, which was
defined as the amount of donation money that callers raised in the
same interval. Managers supplied both measures in a 1-week
interval 1 month after the survey data were collected. This week-
long interval was appropriate given that callers were drawing from
a common pool of prospective alumni donors, signifying that
different callers had similar opportunities to perform.
Results
Means, standard deviations, reliability coefficients, and correla-
tions for the measures appear in Table 1. To ensure that the
constructs were distinct, I conducted confirmatory factor analyses
using EQS software (version 6.1; Bentler, 1995) with maximum
likelihood estimation procedures, following recommendations in
the measurement literature (e.g., Bentler & Dudgeon, 1996; Kline,
1998). A one-factor model displayed very poor fit with the data,
2
(20, N 140) 312.82, normed fit index (NFI) .67,
nonnormed fit index (NNFI) .56, comparative fit index (CFI)
.68, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) .168. In
contrast, the expected two-factor model displayed very good fit
with the data,
2
(19, N 140) 60.84, NFI .94, NNFI .93,
CFI .96, SRMR .040. Supporting the distinctiveness of the
prosocial and intrinsic motivation measures, a chi-square differ-
ence test showed that the model fit improved significantly from the
one-factor to two-factor model,
2
(1, N 140) 251.98, p
.001.
I conducted additional confirmatory factor analyses including
the four job satisfaction items to examine whether intrinsic moti-
Table 1
Study 2 Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations
Variable MSD12 3 4 5
1. Performance (calls made) 48.65 40.48
2. Productivity (dollars raised) 436.73 565.56 .58
***
3. Intrinsic motivation 3.76 1.54 .10 .19
*
(.93)
4. Prosocial motivation 4.48 1.41 .08 .11 .55
***
(.91)
5. Job satisfaction 4.90 1.34 .10 .18
*
.60
***
.36
***
(.81)
Note. Coefficient alphas appear across the diagonal in parentheses.
*
p .05.
***
p .001.
52
GRANT
vation was distinct from job satisfaction. A two-factor model with
the prosocial motivation items loading on one factor and the
intrinsic motivation and job satisfaction items loading together on
the second factor did not fit the data well,
2
(53, N 140)
188.08, NFI .84, NNFI .85, CFI .88, SRMR .076. The
expected three-factor model displayed good fit,
2
(51, N 140)
113.79, NFI .90, NNFI .93, CFI .94, SRMR .046, which
supported the empirical distinctiveness of intrinsic motivation and
job satisfaction. A chi-square difference test showed that the model
fit improved substantially from the two-factor to three-factor
model,
2
(2, N 140) 74.29, p .001.
Predicting Performance and Productivity
I used hierarchical ordinary least squares regression analyses to
examine whether the prosocial-intrinsic motivation interaction sig-
nificantly predicted performance and productivity, following the
same procedures as in Study 1. I mean-centered the prosocial and
intrinsic motivation variables and multiplied the two centered
variables to create a continuous interaction term. The results of
these analyses indicated that neither prosocial motivation nor
intrinsic motivation independently predicted performance or pro-
ductivity. However, the interaction between prosocial and intrinsic
motivations was a significant predictor of both performance and
productivity (see Table 2, first two columns). To facilitate the
interpretation of these results, I plotted the simple slopes for the
relationship of prosocial motivation with performance (see Figure
2) and productivity (see Figure 3) at one standard deviation above
and below the mean of intrinsic motivation. Prosocial motivation
was positively associated with performance when intrinsic moti-
vation was high (␤⫽.41) but not low (␤⫽.00). Prosocial
motivation was also positively associated with productivity when
intrinsic motivation was high (␤⫽.31) but not low (␤⫽⫺.20).
Callers with high levels of both prosocial and intrinsic motivations
averaged 51.82 calls and $510.58 in donations as compared with
an average of 40.26 calls and $308.10 in donations for the other
callers. Thus, intrinsic motivation moderated (strengthened) the
association between prosocial motivation and performance and
productivity.
Mediated Moderation Analyses
I conducted supplementary analyses to examine whether perfor-
mance mediated the association between prosocial–intrinsic moti-
vation interaction and productivity, following the procedures for
mediated moderation recommended by Muller, Judd, and Yzerbyt
(2005). The first criterion, for the interaction between the moder-
ator and the independent variable to significantly predict the de-
pendent variable, was met by prior analyses showing that the
interaction between prosocial and intrinsic motivations signifi-
cantly predicted productivity. The second criterion, for the inter-
action between the moderator and the independent variable to
significantly predict the mediator, was also met by prior analyses
showing that the interaction between prosocial and intrinsic moti-
vations significantly predicted performance. The third criterion,
for the mediator to significantly predict the dependent variable
30
40
50
60
Low High
Prosocial Motivation
Performance (calls made)
Low Intrinsic
High Intrinsic
Figure 2. Study 2 regression slopes for performance.
260
310
360
410
460
510
560
Low High
Prosocial Motivation
Productivity ($$ raised)
Low Intrinsic
High Intrinsic
Figure 3. Study 2 regression slopes for productivity.
Table 2
Study 2 Regressions for Performance, Productivity, and
Mediated Moderation
Variable
Performance
Productivity
Step 1
Productivity
Step 2
t(136) t(136) t(135)
Prosocial
motivation
.110 1.09 .090 0.92 .002 0.02
Intrinsic motivation .030 0.31 .130 1.36 .080 0.93
Prosocial
Motivation
Intrinsic
Motivation
.280 3.31
**
.290 3.41
**
.090 1.25
Performance .450 5.97
***
Performance
Intrinsic
Motivation
.230 2.85
**
Note. The explained variance was r
2
.09 for performance (f
2
.10),
r
2
.11 for productivity in Step 1 (f
2
.12), and r
2
.41 for productivity
in Step 2 (f
2
.69).
**
p .01.
***
p .001.
53
INTRINSIC AND PROSOCIAL MOTIVATIONS
while controlling for the interactions between the moderator and
the independent variable and between the moderator and the me-
diator, was met by an additional regression analysis. Performance
significantly predicted productivity while controlling for the two
interaction terms (see Table 2, third column). Finally, the associ-
ation between the independent variable and the dependent variable
decreased significantly after entering the mediator, as the associ-
ation between the prosocial–intrinsic motivation interaction and
productivity decreased from ␤⫽.29, p .001 to ␤⫽.09, p .21
(see Table 2, second and third columns). A Sobel test using the
critical values recommended by MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoff-
man, West, and Sheets (2002) showed that this decrease was
statistically significant, z 3.68, p .01. Thus, performance
mediated the relationship between the prosocial–intrinsic motiva-
tion interaction and productivity.
Job Satisfaction Analyses
Finally, I examined job satisfaction as an alternative explanation
for the results by conducting moderated regression analyses pre-
dicting the two dependent variables from the interactions of proso-
cial motivation with both intrinsic motivation and job satisfaction.
The results, which are displayed in Table 3, show that intrinsic
motivation, but not job satisfaction, interacted with prosocial mo-
tivation to predict both performance and productivity. Thus, it
appears that the specific experience of intrinsic motivation, rather
than the more general positive attitude of job satisfaction, is
responsible for the moderating patterns observed.
Discussion
These findings build on and complement the results from Study
1, which showed that intrinsic motivation strengthened the asso-
ciation between prosocial motivation and persistence in firefight-
ing. Study 2 extended this key finding to the domain of effective-
ness, supporting the role of intrinsic motivation in strengthening
the association between prosocial motivation and performance
productivity in fundraising. Moreover, performance mediated the
interactive relationship between prosocial and intrinsic motiva-
tions and productivity, and the moderating pattern was unique to
intrinsic motivation—it did not hold for job satisfaction. The two
studies thereby provide convergent support for the role of intrinsic
motivation in strengthening the association between prosocial mo-
tivation and persistence, performance, and productivity.
General Discussion
A core question in motivation research concerns what drives
employees to persist in their tasks toward effective performance
and productivity. Although recent research has pointed to prosocial
motivation as an important answer to this question, little research
has examined its boundary conditions. I drew on self-
determination theory to propose that prosocial motivation is more
likely to predict persistence, performance, and productivity when it
is accompanied by intrinsic motivation. Across two field studies, I
found support for this hypothesis: intrinsic motivation strength-
ened the association between prosocial motivation and persistence,
performance, and productivity in firefighting and fundraising.
Although the two studies displayed consistent results for the
synergistic interaction between prosocial and intrinsic motivations,
an interesting discrepancy emerged between the findings for in-
trinsic motivation as an independent predictor of the outcome
variables. Intrinsic motivation independently predicted the number
of overtime hours that firefighters worked in Study 1 but did not
independently predict the performance and productivity of fund-
raising callers in Study 2. A plausible explanation for this discrep-
ancy is based on differences in the variety and complexity of the
work. Whereas the firefighters completed varied, complex tasks
that supported intrinsic motivation, the fundraising callers com-
pleted comparatively repetitive, simple tasks that offered limited
opportunities for intrinsic motivation. Firefighters experienced
higher levels of intrinsic motivation (M 6.09, SD 0.77) than
fundraising callers (M 3.76, SD 1.54), increasing the likeli-
hood that intrinsic motivation would influence their behaviors
(e.g., Steel & Ko¨nig, 2006). This interpretation is consistent with
evidence that intrinsic motivation is difficult to sustain in repetitive
tasks (Hackman & Oldham, 1976) and more likely to increase
effort in varied than repetitive tasks (Koestner & Losier, 2002).
The two studies also revealed a surprising trend: a negative
relationship between prosocial motivation and the outcomes of
persistence (Study 1) and productivity (Study 2) when intrinsic
motivation was low. A likely theoretical explanation for this trend
suggested earlier is that when intrinsic motivation is low, the
experience of pushing oneself to complete the task in the absence
of enjoyment leads to stress and overload (e.g., Bolino & Turnley,
2005). From this perspective, prosocial motivation without intrin-
sic motivation may deplete employees’ psychological resources
for self-regulation (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000), leading to
exhaustion and thereby decreased persistence and productivity.
Theoretical Contributions
This article helps to resolve conflicting results about the role of
prosocial motivation in persistence, performance, and productivity.
Together, the results for firefighters and fundraisers suggest that
prosocial motivation is most likely to contribute to these outcomes
when it is accompanied by high levels of intrinsic motivation. In
the absence of intrinsic motivation, however, prosocial motivation
may not be sufficient to enhance persistence, performance, and
productivity. These findings highlight intrinsic motivation as an
Table 3
Study 2 OLS Regressions Predicting Performance and
Productivity With Job Satisfaction
Variable
Performance Productivity
t(134) t(134)
Prosocial motivation .11 1.07 .08 0.84
Intrinsic motivation .01 0.07 .08 0.72
Prosocial Motivation
Intrinsic Motivation
.26 2.29
*
.32 2.84
**
Job satisfaction .07 0.64 .09 0.89
Prosocial Motivation
Job Satisfaction
.03 0.25 .05 0.45
Note. The explained variance was r
2
.09 for performance (f
2
.10)
and r
2
.12 for productivity (f
2
.13). OLS ordinary least squares.
*
p .05.
**
p .01.
54
GRANT
important boundary condition for the benefits of prosocial moti-
vation, advancing existing knowledge about the conditions under
which prosocial motivation is likely to contribute to desirable
employee behaviors. These findings also accentuate the value of
conceptualizing prosocial motivation in terms of varying degrees
of autonomous regulation, rather than studying prosocial motiva-
tion as a unitary construct, as has been common in prior research.
For example, Rioux and Penner’s (2001) measure of prosocial
motives included items such as “To have fun with my coworkers,”
“Because I like interacting with my coworkers,” and “Because I
feel it is important to help those in need.” These items appear to
capture a relatively intrinsic or identified, rather than introjected,
form of prosocial motivation. In contrast, the items utilized here
allowed for prosocial motivation to vary independently with in-
trinsic motivation, making it possible to examine whether the
association between prosocial motivation and persistence, perfor-
mance, and productivity outcomes is contingent on intrinsic mo-
tivation.
Further, by examining prosocial and intrinsic motivations in
tandem, this article offers two central contributions to self-
determination theory and research. First, the findings expand cur-
rent understandings of the interactions between intrinsic motiva-
tion and other forms of self-regulation, an important but largely
neglected issue in psychological and organizational research. Self-
determination theory has traditionally treated different forms of
self-regulation as mutually exclusive, with little attention to inter-
actions among them (Gagne´ & Deci, 2005). In contrast, I have
argued that employees experience prosocial motivation as a form
of identified regulation when intrinsic motivation is high and
experience prosocial motivation as a form of introjected regulation
when intrinsic motivation is low. The findings across the two
studies regarding the interactions between intrinsic and prosocial
motivations thereby provide new insights into the potential syn-
ergy between intrinsic and identified forms of self-regulation. It
appears that the combination of enjoying the process and valuing
the outcome can enable higher levels of persistence, performance,
and productivity. These results thus provide empirical support for
theoretical perspectives proposing that intrinsic motivation can
interact constructively with other forms of motivation (Amabile,
1993; Gagne´ & Deci, 2005; Staw, 1977).
The second contribution to self-determination theory lies in
integrating research on intrinsic and prosocial motivations.
Whereas researchers have primarily studied prosocial and intrinsic
motivations in separate literatures, I have taken conceptual and
empirical steps to clarify the nature of and relationship between
prosocial and intrinsic motivations. Conceptually, the theoretical
development differentiates between the two motivations in terms
of self-regulation (introjected/identified vs. autonomous), goal di-
rectedness (outcome vs. process), and temporal focus (future vs.
present). Empirically, the results demonstrate that the two moti-
vations are positively related but clearly distinguishable and that
they can thereby interact to predict important outcomes in organi-
zational life. This attention to prosocial motivation takes a step
toward contextualizing intrinsic motivation research. The vast
majority of intrinsic motivation research has been conducted in
laboratories with individuals working on isolated, independent
tasks (Ambrose & Kulik, 1999). As Shamir (1990, p. 321) wrote,
“A more social concept of intrinsic motivation is needed.” Con-
sistent with this recommendation, my research focused on individ-
uals working in social, interdependent contexts on tasks that af-
fected the welfare of other people. The examination of both
intrinsic and prosocial motivations in these contexts answers re-
cent calls to move beyond task-focused and self-focused motiva-
tion perspectives toward more other-focused, relational motivation
perspectives (Grant, 2007; Meglino & Korsgaard, 2004; Shamir,
1991).
Limitations and Future Directions
The studies are subject to several limitations that can be ad-
dressed in further research. First, although the use of temporally
delayed outcome variables strengthens causal inferences, to pro-
vide more conclusive evidence about causal relationships, it is
necessary for future research to experimentally manipulate proso-
cial and intrinsic motivations and/or utilize cross-lagged longitu-
dinal designs that measure motivations and outcomes together at
multiple time intervals. Second, I did not directly assess the
proposed mechanism for explaining the results—that intrinsic mo-
tivation influences whether prosocial motivation is experienced as
identified or introjected regulation. It will be important for future
research to investigate psychological mechanisms that mediate the
link between prosocial and intrinsic motivations and persistence,
performance, and productivity. For example, it may be the case
that when prosocial motivation is low but intrinsic motivation is
high, employees’ attempts to force themselves to engage in the
task lead to cognitive interference that undermines their abilities to
perform effectively (e.g., Bazerman et al., 1998; Beal, Weiss,
Barros, & MacDermid, 2005). This mechanism may complement
persistence in explaining the association between the interaction of
prosocial and intrinsic motivations and the outcomes of perfor-
mance and productivity. Third, I was not able to control for other
established predictors of persistence, performance, and productiv-
ity, such as conscientiousness, perceived job characteristics, and
positive affect; I suggest that future research include these types of
control variables to examine the relative contributions of intrinsic
and prosocial motivations to the outcomes of interest.
Finally, the use of self-report measures of prosocial and intrinsic
motivations at single points in time raises questions about whether
employees’ responses reflect enduring orientations, temporary
states, or both (e.g., Amabile et al., 1994). Assessing the two
motivations as both enduring orientations and temporary states
using experience-sampling and daily diary methodologies will
provide insight into this issue by illuminating the relative roles of
between-individual and within-individual variations in predicting
outcomes. Such designs will also help to adjudicate claims that
multiple motivations cannot coexist in the same moment (Apter,
1984; Kuhl, 1992). Also potentially worthwhile are implicit mea-
sures of the motivations, which may transcend some of the limi-
tations of explicit self-reports (Schultheiss & Brunstein, 2001).
Practical Implications and Conclusion
The findings across the two studies have important practical
implications. The results suggest that employees display higher
levels of persistence, performance, and productivity when they
experience prosocial and intrinsic motivations in tandem. Manag-
ers may draw on these findings to tailor selection and socialization
practices toward prosocial and intrinsic motivations. From a se-
55
INTRINSIC AND PROSOCIAL MOTIVATIONS
lection standpoint, managers may measure prosocial and intrinsic
motivational orientations to hire employees who display disposi-
tional tendencies to experience high levels of both motivations.
From a socialization standpoint, managers may design work con-
texts to cultivate both prosocial and intrinsic motivations. For
example, task significance is thought to enhance intrinsic motiva-
tion (Hackman & Oldham, 1976), but because task significance
provides opportunities to benefit others, it may simultaneously
enhance prosocial motivation (Grant, 2008). Similarly, empower-
ment interventions are thought to increase intrinsic motivation
(Thomas & Velthouse, 1990), but because they provide employees
with expanded opportunities to contribute and have an impact, they
may simultaneously increase prosocial motivation. Thus, the
knowledge that both prosocial and intrinsic motivations play a
significant role in persistence, performance, and productivity may
motivate managers to both select and socialize these motivations,
with the potential to increase effectiveness outcomes.
In conclusion, E. B. White wrote, “I arise every morning torn
between the desire to improve the world and the desire to enjoy it.
This makes it hard to plan the day” (Kennedy, 2006). In contrast,
the findings presented here suggest that employees who experience
the desire to both improve and enjoy the world are motivated to
act, persisting in completing their tasks effectively and produc-
tively. By suggesting that synergy between prosocial and intrinsic
motivations may enhance persistence, performance, and produc-
tivity, this article advances existing theory, research, and practice
related to work motivation.
References
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Alonso, P., & Lewis, G. B. (2001). Public service motivation and job
performance: Evidence from the public sector. American Review of
Public Administration, 31, 363–380.
Amabile, T. M. (1993). Motivational synergy: Toward new conceptualiza-
tions of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in the workplace. Human
Resource Management Review, 3, 185–201.
Amabile, T. M., Hill, K. G., Hennessey, B. A., & Tighe, E. M. (1994). The
Work Preference Inventory: Assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motiva-
tional orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66,
950 –967.
Ambrose, M. L., & Kulik, C. T. (1999). Old friends, new faces: Motivation
in the 1990s. Journal of Management, 25, 231–292.
Apter, M. J. (1984). Reversal theory and personality: A review. Journal of
Research in Personality, 18, 265–288.
Batson, C. D. (1987). Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? In L.
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 20,
pp. 65–122). New York: Academic Press.
Batson, C. D. (1998). Altruism and prosocial behavior. In D. T. Gilbert,
S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology
(4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 282–316). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Bazerman, M. H., Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Wade-Benzoni, K. (1998). Nego-
tiating with yourself and losing: Making decisions with competing
internal preferences. Academy of Management Review, 23, 225–241.
Beal, D. J., Cohen, R. R., Burke, M. R., & McLendon, C. L. (2003).
Cohesion and performance in groups: A meta-analytic clarification of
construct relations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 989 –1004.
Beal, D. J., Weiss, H. M., Barros, E., & MacDermid, S. M. (2005). An
episodic process model of affective influences on performance. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 90, 1054 –1068.
Belsley, D. A., Kuh, E., & Welsch, R. E. (1980). Regression diagnostics:
Identifying influential data and sources of collinearity. New York: John
Wiley.
Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS structural equations program manual. Encino,
CA: Multivariate Software.
Bentler, P. M., & Dudgeon, P. (1996). Covariance structure analysis:
Statistical practice, theory, and directions. Annual Review of Psychology,
47, 563–592.
Bing, M. N., & Burroughs, S. M. (2001). The predictive and interactive
effects of equity sensitivity in teamwork-oriented organizations. Journal
of Organizational Behavior, 22, 271–290.
Blumberg, M., & Pringle, C. D. (1982). The missing opportunity in
organizational research: Some implications for a theory of work perfor-
mance. Academy of Management Review, 7, 560 –569.
Bolino, M. C., & Turnley, W. H. (2005). The personal costs of citizenship
behavior: The relationship between individual initiative and role over-
load, job stress, and work-family conflict. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 90, 740 –748.
Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Self-concordance at work: Toward
understanding the motivational effects of transformational leaders.
Academy of Management Journal, 46, 554–571.
Brewer, G. A., & Selden, S. C. (1998). Whistle blowers in the federal civil
service: New evidence of the public service ethic. Journal of Public
Administration Research and Theory, 8, 413–439.
Brief, A. P., & Aldag, R. J. (1977). The intrinsic-extrinsic dichotomy:
Toward conceptual clarity. Academy of Management Review, 2, 496
500.
Campbell, J. P. (1990). Modeling the performance prediction problem in
industrial and organizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M.
Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology
(2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 687–732). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists
Press.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple
regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (Eds.). (1988). Optimal
experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Cunningham, M. R., Steinberg, J., & Grey, R. (1980). Wanting to and
having to help: Separate motivations for positive mood and guilt-
induced helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38,
181–192.
deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal affective determi-
nants of behavior. New York: Academic Press.
Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic
motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105–115.
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review
of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic
motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627– 668.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination
in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
De Dreu, C. K. W. (2006). Rational self-interest and other orientation in
organizational behavior: A critical appraisal and extension of Meglino
and Korsgaard (2004). Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1245–1252.
Dowling, W. F., & Sayles, L. R. (1978). How managers motivate: The
imperatives of supervision. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Dweck, C. S., & Gilliard, D. (1975). Expectancy statements as determi-
nants of reactions to failure: Sex differences in persistence and expect-
ancy change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 1077–
1084.
Eisenberger, R., Cummings, J., Armeli, S., & Lynch, P. (1997). Perceived
organizational support, discretionary treatment, and job satisfaction.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 812–820.
Fox, J. (1991). Regression diagnostics. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
56
GRANT
Gagne´, M. (2003). The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation
in prosocial behavior engagement. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 199
223.
Gagne´, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self-determination theory and work
motivation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 331–362.
George, J. M. (1991). State or trait: Effects of positive mood on prosocial
behaviors at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 299 –307.
Grant, A. M. (2007). Relational job design and the motivation to make a
prosocial difference. Academy of Management Review, 32, 393– 417.
Grant, A. M. (2008). The significance of task significance: Job perfor-
mance effects, relational mechanisms, and boundary conditions. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 93, 108 –124.
Grant, A. M., Campbell, E. M., Chen, G., Cottone, K., Lapedis, D., & Lee,
K. (2007). Impact and the art of motivation maintenance: The effects of
contact with beneficiaries on persistence behavior. Organizational Be-
havior and Human Decision Processes, 103, 53– 67.
Graziano, W. G., Habashi, M. M., Sheese, B. E., & Tobin, R. M. (in press).
Agreeableness, empathy, and helping: A Person Situation perspective.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1976). Motivation through the design of
work: Test of a theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor-
mance, 16, 250 –279.
Harrison, D. A., Newman, D. A., & Roth, P. L. (2006). How important are
job attitudes? Meta-analytic comparisons of integrative behavioral out-
comes and time sequences. Academy of Management Journal, 49, 305–
325.
Heath, C. (1999). On the social psychology of agency relationships: Lay
theories of motivation overemphasize extrinsic incentives. Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 25– 62.
Herzberg, F. (1966). Work and the nature of man. Cleveland, OH: World
Publishing.
Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B. B. (1967). The motivation to
work (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Ilies, R., Scott, B. A., & Judge, T. A. (2006). The interactive effects of
personal traits and experienced states on intraindividual patterns of
citizenship behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 49, 561–575.
Kahn, W. A. (1990). Psychological conditions of personal engagement and
disengagement at work. Academy of Management Journal, 33, 692–724.
Kehr, H. M. (2004). Integrating implicit motives, explicit motives, and
perceived abilities: The compensatory model of work motivation and
volition. Academy of Management Review, 29, 479 499.
Kennedy, E. M. (2006, May 14). Commencement address of Senator
Edward M. Kennedy at Springfield College. Retrieved December 21,
2006, from http://kennedy.senate.gov/newsroom/speech.cfm?id
88f8cc0c-c49e-4832-a663–142c328c9968
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation mod-
eling. New York: Guilford Press.
Koestner, R., & Losier, G. F. (2002). Distinguishing three ways of being
internally motivated: A closer look at introjection, identification, and
intrinsic motivation. In E. L. Deci, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of
self-determination research (pp. 101–121). Rochester, NY: University
of Rochester Press.
Konovsky, M. A., & Organ, D. W. (1996). Dispositional and contextual
determinants of organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Organi-
zational Behavior, 17, 253–266.
Korsgaard, M. A., Meglino, B. M., & Lester, S. W. (1997). Beyond
helping: Do other-oriented values have broader implications in organi-
zations? Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 160 –177.
Kuhl, J. (1992). A theory of self-regulation: Action versus state orientation,
self-discrimination, and some applications. Applied Psychology: An In-
ternational Review, 41, 97–129.
Latham, G. P., & Pinder, C. C. (2005). Work motivation theory and
research at the dawn of the twenty-first century. Annual Review of
Psychology, 56, 495–516.
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., &
Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other
intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104.
McCloy, R. A., Campbell, J. P., & Cudeck, R. (1994). A confirmatory test
of a model of performance determinants. Journal of Applied Psychology,
79, 493–505.
McGregor, D. (1960). The human side of the enterprise. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
McGregor, I., & Little, B. R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and
meaning: On doing well and being yourself. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 74, 494 –512.
Meglino, B. M., & Korsgaard, M. A. (2004). Considering rational self-
interest as a disposition: Organizational implications of other orienta-
tion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 946 –959.
Mitchell, T. R., & Daniels, D. (2003). Motivation. In W. C. Borman, D. R.
Ilgen, & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of psychology, Vol. 12:
Industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 225–254). New York:
John Wiley.
Motowidlo, S. J. (2003). Job performance. In W. C. Borman, D. R. Ilgen,
& R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Industrial and
organizational psychology, Vol. 12: Industrial and organizational psy-
chology (pp. 39 –53). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Muller, D., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2005). When moderation is
mediated and mediation is moderated. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 89, 852– 863.
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of
limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological
Bulletin, 126, 247–259.
Naff, K. C., & Crum, J. (1999). Working for America: Does public service
motivation make a difference? Review of Public Personnel Administra-
tion, 19, 5–16.
Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., & Schroeder, D. A. (2005).
Prosocial behavior: Multilevel perspectives. Annual Review of Psychol-
ogy, 56, 365–392.
Perry, J. L., & Hondeghem, A. (Eds.). (in press). Public service motivation:
State of the science and art. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Peterson, N. G., Borman, W. C., Mumford, M. D., Jeannerete, P. R., &
Fleishman, E. A. (1999). An occupational information system for the
21st century: The development of O*NET. Washington, DC: APA.
Porter, L. W., & Lawler, E. E. III. (1986). Managerial attitudes and
performance. Homewood, IL: Richard D. Irwin.
Quinn, R. P., & Shepard, L. G. (1974). The 1972—1973 quality of em-
ployment survey. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, University of
Michigan.
Quinn, R. W. (2005). Flow in knowledge work: High performance expe-
rience in the design of national security technology. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 50, 610 641.
Rioux, S. M., & Penner, L. A. (2001). The causes of organizational
citizenship behavior: A motivational analysis. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 86, 1306 –1314.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and
internalization: Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749–761.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the
facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being.
American Psychologist, 55, 68 –78.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). To be happy or to be self-fulfilled: A
review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. In S. Fiske
(Ed.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 52, pp. 141–166). Palo Alto,
CA: Annual Reviews.
Sandelands, L. E., Brockner, J., & Glynn, M. A. (1988). If at first you don’t
succeed, try, try again: Effects of persistence-performance contingen-
57
INTRINSIC AND PROSOCIAL MOTIVATIONS
cies, ego involvement, and self-esteem on task persistence. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 73, 208 –216.
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1983). Individual differences in produc-
tivity: An empirical test of estimates derived from studies of selection
procedure utility. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 407– 414.
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Assessment of implicit
motives with a research version of the TAT: Picture profiles, gender
differences, and relations to other personality measures. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 77, 71– 86.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values:
Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1– 65).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Shamir, B. (1990). Calculations, values, and identities: The sources of
collectivistic work motivation. Human Relations, 43, 313–332.
Shamir, B. (1991). Meaning, self and motivation in organizations. Orga-
nization Studies, 12, 405– 424.
Sheldon, K. M., Arndt, J., & Houser-Marko, L. (2003). In search of the
organismic valuing process: The human tendency to move towards
beneficial goal choices. Journal of Personality, 71, 835– 869.
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need-satisfaction, and
longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 76, 482–497.
Sheldon, K. M., & Houser-Marko, L. (2001). Self-concordance,
goal attainment and the pursuit of happiness: Can there be an up-
ward spiral? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,
152–165.
Staw, B. M. (1977). Motivation in organizations: Toward synthesis and
redirection. In B. M. Staw & G. Salancik (Eds.), New directions in
organizational behavior. (pp. 55–95) Chicago: St. Clair.
Steel, P., & Ko¨nig, C. J. (2006). Integrating theories of motivation. Acad-
emy of Management Review, 31, 889–913.
Steers, R., Mowday, R., & Shapiro, D. (2004). The future of work moti-
vation theory. Academy of Management Review, 29, 379 –387.
Thomas, K. W., & Velthouse, B. A. (1990). Cognitive elements of em-
powerment: An “interpretive” model of intrinsic task motivation. Acad-
emy of Management Review, 15, 666681.
Thompson, J. A., & Bunderson, J. S. (2003). Violations of principle:
Ideological currency in the psychological contract. Academy of Man-
agement Review, 28, 571–586.
Warr, P. (2007). Work, happiness, and unhappiness. Mahwah, NJ: Erl-
baum.
Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of
personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 678 691.
Weiss, H. M. (2002). Deconstructing job satisfaction: Separating evalua-
tions, beliefs and affective experiences. Human Resource Management
Review, 12, 173–194.
Wrzesniewski, A., McCauley, C. R., Rozin, P., & Schwartz, B. (1997).
Jobs, careers, and callings: People’s relations to their work. Journal of
Research in Personality, 31, 21–33.
Received January 24, 2007
Revision received June 6, 2007
Accepted July 26, 2007
58
GRANT
... Individual self-versus other-oriented motivations can limit founders' perceptions because they act as a filter through which they may select and interpret information (Kunda, 1990). Self-oriented motivation has been found to stimulate information scanning for, and processing of, individuallevel attributes and self-relevant consequences, leading to prioritization of a competitive mindset and wealth distribution (De Dreu & Nauta, 2009;Grant, 2008). In contrast, other-oriented motivation information facilitates scanning for and processing of group-level attributes, social cues, and other relevant consequences, leading to prioritization of cooperation and wealth distribution benefiting mostly others (De Dreu & Nauta, 2009;Grant, 2008). ...
... Self-oriented motivation has been found to stimulate information scanning for, and processing of, individuallevel attributes and self-relevant consequences, leading to prioritization of a competitive mindset and wealth distribution (De Dreu & Nauta, 2009;Grant, 2008). In contrast, other-oriented motivation information facilitates scanning for and processing of group-level attributes, social cues, and other relevant consequences, leading to prioritization of cooperation and wealth distribution benefiting mostly others (De Dreu & Nauta, 2009;Grant, 2008). On the basis of the above, we argue that founders' self-and otheroriented motivations can be relevant to wealth distribution decisions, including employee pay. ...
... Bolino and Grant (2016) argue that other-oriented motivation and prosocial motivation scales are different operationalizations of the same construct. Accordingly, we measured other-oriented motivation with the frequently used four-item prosocial motivation scale developed by Grant (2008). The 7-point scale was anchored at disagree strongly and agree strongly. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study adds to the emergent stream of work examining the micro-level antecedents of pay dispersion by focusing on how business founders’ personal characteristics influence pay dispersion in their organizations. We leverage stakeholder theory and the motivated information processing perspective to predict pathways between founders’ self- versus other-oriented motivations, their perceptions of employee and shareholder salience, and pay dispersion in their organizations. We test our hypotheses on data from a two-wave survey of founders. We find that a high level of motivation to benefit others on the part of a founder reduces the salience of shareholder concerns in decision-making, which in turn reduces pay dispersion. In contrast, a high level of motivation to benefit oneself heightens the salience of shareholder concerns in decision-making, increasing pay dispersion. Our results inform the debate on pay dispersion by elucidating the role played by founders’ self- versus other-oriented motivations and stakeholder salience perceptions.
... Prosocial impact was assessed with the 3-item scale developed by Grant [29]. A sample item was "I am aware of how my work today will help others (e.g., colleagues, patients and their family)." ...
... Specifically, social impact positively affected all dimensions of need satisfaction, including competence, autonomy, relatedness, and benevolence. These findings align with previous research, emphasizing the importance of social impact in fostering positive work experiences and outcomes [29,38]. The findings of this study suggest that when individuals perceive their work as having a positive impact on others and society, they may experience heightened levels of need satisfaction, which could be associated with improved innovative work behavior and enhanced creativity. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In recent research, a growing body of empirical evidence, suggests that prosocial impact at work can play a significant role in enhancing creativity and innovativeness. Drawing from Self Determination Theory, we hypothesized that basic psychological needs and benevolence satisfaction could serve as a mediating factor in the relation between employees perceived social impact and innovative work behavior and creativity, thus illuminating the manner in which contentment of psychological needs fosters inventive proclivities within the organizational milieu. Results from a study in Greece and Canada (N=528) showed that both perceived social impact and prosocial motivation are positively associated with innovative work behavior and creativity while autonomy and competence satisfaction mediate the relation between perceived social impact and the work outcomes examined within this study. Moreover, prosocial motivation was found to moderate the relation between benevolence satisfaction and innovativeness. Findings extend prior research on the role of prosociality on creative behavior at work and provide supporting evidence for the organizations that encourage and support employees’ initiatives to make a positive difference in the lives of others.
... Role of positive emotion in motivation [40] 41 Theoretical Review Pink [30] presents the surprising truth about what motivates us, emphasizing intrinsic motivation. ...
Article
This review explores the multifaceted field of motivation within organizational behavior by synthesizing insights from some research. The research is founded on an extensive literature review encompassing various studies that address motivation-related issues within organizational behavior. Spanning foundational theories to contemporary paradigms, the review delves into intrinsic and extrinsic motivational factors, justice perceptions, emotional influences, and the interplay of creativity. It highlights the evolution of motivational theories, ranging from Adams' equity theory to modern constructs like self-determination theory and goal-setting theory, emphasizing the dual nature of motivation. Justice perceptions emerge as crucial influencers of behavior, while transformational leadership behaviors amplify motivation, job satisfaction, and commitment. The relationship of emotions adds another layer, where positive affect enhances expectancy and motivation. The temporal dynamics of motivation and its creative potential are also explored. The review culminates in a nuanced understanding of motivation's essence, guiding organizations toward fostering engagement and productivity. It serves as a foundation for further exploration, reflecting the intricate variety of motivational factors that influence organizational behavior.
... According to Carsrud and Brännback (2011), extrinsic motivations are linked to wealth, status, and external rewards that arise from entrepreneurship, while intrinsic motivation is linked to success and achievement of goals. Intrinsic motivations are associated with prosocial motivations (Grant, 2008), the desire to mobilize efforts based on concern for others (Batson, 1995). Social entrepreneurs are characterized by a strong relationship with intrinsic motivation through interest and passion for social entrepreneurship, dissatisfaction with previous work, the courage social entrepreneurs to focus their resources and efforts on creating value for disadvantaged groups as: own, related to the emotional antecedent of passion and frustration, described as achievements, autonomy, relationship, and influence; and motivations of others, in emotional antecedents of empathy and solidarity, described as altruism, cultivation, social justice, and sense of obligation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social entrepreneurs have several motivations that lead them to open and continue their companies, which vary according to the stage of the social enterprise’s life cycle. This study seeks to analyze the motivations throughout the stages of a social enterprise’s life cycle. To do this, we carried out a qualitative research with primary data collected through interviews with experts and social entrepreneurs and secondary data collected through documents and websites. The results indicate three types of motivational factors: essential factors, building factors, and solidifying factors. Each type plays different roles during life cycle. All essential factors are present across all stages of the life cycle. The building factors are found mainly in the early stages, while different solidifying factors are distributed throughout the stages of the life cycle. The essential factors are the main difference between social and traditional entrepreneurs. This research brings new insights by linking motivational factors to the life cycle stages of social enterprises. Entrepreneurs can use this information to identify their stage and leverage their motivators for development, while training programs can be tailored to better align with the life cycle stage of social enterprises. Keywords: motivation; life cycle stages; social enterprises; social entrepreneurship
... With the perspective of SIT, supervisors' KBT in their subordinates may stimulate as a response to the employee's PPPI and thereof S-CB actions. The high perceptions of PPPI will be regarded as meaningful and beneficial efforts to leaders and should lead leaders to develop trust in employees (Grant, 2008a(Grant, , b, 2012. Based on the above arguments, PPPI and S-CB are expected to serially mediate the relationship between S-KH and S-KBT. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose-Past studies have largely focused on leaders' influence on employees' attitudes and behaviors, largely ignoring the followership and its consequences. This study investigates the social impact that followers induce on leaders through their intentions and actions. Following social impact theory (SIT), this study contributes to the growing research on supervisory knowledge hiding (KH) and related positive consequences beyond the traditional leader-centered approach. This paper investigates the serial mediation link between supervisory KH and supervisory knowledge-based trust (KBT) via perceived prosocial impact and supervisor directed citizenship behavior. Design/methodology/approach-Time-lagged dyadic data of 348 employees working in a bank under 54 supervisors were collected from Saudi Arabia. Findings-The findings suggest that supervisory KH entails a potential prosocial impact on employees to engage in supervisor-directed citizenship behavior that builds the KBT in supervisors about subordinates. The empirical support provides an understanding of the social impact of subordinates' influence on supervisors above and beyond traditional leadership literature by depicting the active role of followers in influencing leaders' behavior in building trust in knowledge management. Theoretical and practical implications of the findings are also discussed. Originality/value-By studying the followership effect on leadership, this study extends the social impact process beyond a social phenomenon to the workplace in a supervisor-subordinate relationship. Moreover, examining the positive framing of a leader's KH to transform such behaviors through active followership role provides a new insight into positive consequences of supervisory behavior through social impact.
Article
Purpose To promote gender equality, world leaders at the UN came up with Sustainable Development Goal 5 (SDG5). It proposes to eliminate discrimination against women by providing them with similar opportunities for reaching leadership positions. Hence, this study aims to examine the contribution made by women transformational leaders to their employees’ performance. The study seeks to emphasize the role played by female leaders in the on-job performance of employees and their mental well-being by encouraging intrinsic motivation among them. Design/methodology/approach For the purpose of data collection, a questionnaire was sent through Google Forms to the employees who had females as their superiors or in the decision-making position. Data from a final sample of 517 respondents was gathered, on which SEM was applied to analyze the direct impact of transformational women’s leadership on employee performance and the indirect impact through the mediation of intrinsic motivation. Findings The study found that by having feminine traits, women are stronger transformational leaders as they encourage individuals to be self-motivated instead of getting stimulated because of some external incentive. Also, such a leadership style ensures better work performance and mentally healthier employees. In addition, transformational women's leadership creates a better work environment by inspiring a teamwork culture instead of individualism. Practical implications The study has implications for not just researchers but other stakeholders as well. The study is useful for organizations as it directs them to hire and promote more women for leadership positions. Also, the results hint that people prefer working for women-led organizations as it will ensure a healthier work atmosphere. Originality/value There are many studies from earlier times related to transformational leadership. However, female leadership and the role it plays for employees have not gotten their fair share of attention. Thus, to the best of the authors’ knowledge, the present work is one of the very few where contributions made by women transformational leaders have been assessed.
Article
Purpose The purpose of this study is to investigate the impact of family motivation (FM) on promotive voice behavior (VBPm) and knowledge hiding (KH). The study uses moral disengagement (MD) role as a mediator to see how FM shapes moral engagement leading to participate in promotive voice and knowledge sharing. Design/methodology/approach The hypothesized model was tested using partial least squares structural equation modeling. The authors used convenience sampling and collected data in two phases. The authors have a final sample of 257 faculty members for analysis, with an overall response rate of 42.8%. Findings Study findings reveal a negative relationship between FM with MD and a positive relationship with VBPm. The relationship between FM and (KHKH results did not show the expected effects. At the same time, mediation of MD between FM and voice behavior and FM and (KHKH show the expected results. Originality/value The study finds that family factors have practical consequences for companies in recognizing the value of familial elements in cultivating employee voice and engagement behaviors. Since family is a powerful motivation to work, it provides valuable insights for HRM strategies and organizational studies to encourage employee voice and moral engagement in the workplace. The study is one of the few studies investigating the impact of FM on promotive voice and KH and enhancing the knowledge of mediating role of MD.
Article
Existing studies on MOOCs examine learners’ engagement processes but have not explored links between them and motivations to enroll. In our previous work, we identified three motivation groups in a MOOC for educators: intrinsic, professional, and prosocial. In the current study, we used process mining to compare the course engagement patterns of these three groups. We found that throughout the course, the intrinsic group was the most engaged, but the prosocial group became the most engaged by the end of the course. We also identified rarely visited pages and page sequences that do not follow the intended course structure. Our findings enhance existing research on motivation and engagement in MOOCs by showing how motivation relates to fine-grained engagement metrics. They suggest that MOOC developers may want to consider why some groups are less engaged and why some pages appear less engaging and change the course structure accordingly.
Article
Full-text available
Employee voice is perceived as an approach with both significant advantages and disadvantages. The current study aims to analyse the behaviours of supervisors when endorsing the ideas of employees who raise promotive voices and consider the employees positively for the development of quality relationships. Specifically, 970 employees were recruited from various small and medium enterprises (SMEs), including manufacturing and electronics companies, in Pakistan for the current study participation. Data collection was conducted in two stages through two sources (supervisors and subordinates) before examining the collected data on the Mplus software for random coefficient regression analyses. Subordinates’ promotive voice was significantly and positively correlated to the Leader-Member-Exchange (LMX) quality and creative idea endorsement when supervisors interpreted the voice as driven more by performance enhancement motives instead of impression management motives. The findings suggested that performance enhancement motives and impression management motives would produce contrasting material consequences for subordinates. The present study limitations and future research directions were also discussed.
Article
Purpose The authors intended to find out what motivates employees at the workplace. For this purpose, the authors examined family motivation and intrinsic motivation influences over work attitudes such as organizational citizenship behavior (individual) and affective commitment. Design/methodology/approach In the current research, the authors adopted time-lagged approach to collect a total of 352 responses from managers in the hospitality sector. This approach was adopted to avoid common method issues related to survey research. Findings The findings suggest positive association of family motivation with intrinsic motivation, affective commitment and organizational citizenship behavior (individual). The authors also found intrinsic motivation positively related to affective commitment and organizational citizenship behavior (individual). The findings also found statistical support pertaining to the mediating role of intrinsic motivation on family motivation's positive relationship with affective commitment and organizational citizenship behavior (individual). Practical implications Considering important role of family motivation, the authors ask managers to think through ways that could help employees feel better about their family's wellbeing. The authors also suggest organizations upsurge intrinsic motivation of their employees by engaging them in decision-making process, allow employees to craft their jobs because through these a higher level of organizational citizenship behavior for individuals and affective commitment could be generated. Originality/value The authors extend the core assumption of self-determination theory that work motivation (intrinsic in specific) is autonomously determined, deeply rooted within individuals, and gratifying. It works on the pleasure principle and mirrors a hedonic standpoint. In such a situation, employees work merely based on their interest and joy; they focus and enjoy the process.
Article
Two studies used the self-concordance model of healthy goal striving (K. M. Sheldon & A. J. Elliot, 1999) to examine the motivational processes by which people can increase their level of well-being during a period of time and then maintain the gain or perhaps increase it even further during the next period of time. In Study I, entering freshmen with self-concordant motivation better attained their 1st-semester goals, which in turn predicted increased adjustment and greater self-concordance for the next semester's goals. Increased self-concordance in turn predicted even better goal attainment during the 2nd semester, which led to further increases in adjustment and to higher levels of ego development by the end of the year. Study 2 replicated the basic model in a 2-week study of short-term goals set in the laboratory. Limits of the model and implications for the question of how (and whether) happiness may be increased are discussed.
Book
I: Background.- 1. An Introduction.- 2. Conceptualizations of Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination.- II: Self-Determination Theory.- 3. Cognitive Evaluation Theory: Perceived Causality and Perceived Competence.- 4. Cognitive Evaluation Theory: Interpersonal Communication and Intrapersonal Regulation.- 5. Toward an Organismic Integration Theory: Motivation and Development.- 6. Causality Orientations Theory: Personality Influences on Motivation.- III: Alternative Approaches.- 7. Operant and Attributional Theories.- 8. Information-Processing Theories.- IV: Applications and Implications.- 9. Education.- 10. Psychotherapy.- 11. Work.- 12. Sports.- References.- Author Index.
Article
Existing theory fails to provide strong and consistent prediction of individual job performance. This paper argues that the failure stems from a neglect of an important dimension of performance—the opportunity to perform—and the interaction of opportunity with known correlates of performance. A three dimensional interactive model of work performance is proposed; suggestions for future research and for managerial practice are offered.
Article
This article presents a cognitive model of empowerment. Here, empowerment is defined as increased intrinsic task motivation, and our subsequent model identifies four cognitions (task assessments) as the basis for worker empowerment: sense of impact, competence, meaningfulness, and choice. Adopting an interpretive perspective, we have used the model also to describe cognitive processes through which workers reach these conclusions. Central to the processes we describe are workers' interpretive styles and global beliefs. Both preliminary evidence for the model and general implications for research are discussed.