ArticlePDF Available

The Hidden Impact of Conspiracy Theories: Perceived and Actual Influence of Theories Surrounding the Death of Princess Diana

Taylor & Francis
The Journal of Social Psychology
Authors:

Abstract

The authors examined the perceived and actual impact of exposure to conspiracy theories surrounding the death of Diana, Princess of Wales, in 1997. One group of undergraduate students rated their agreement and their classmates' perceived agreement with several statements about Diana's death. A second group of students from the same undergraduate population read material containing popular conspiracy theories about Diana's death before rating their own and others' agreement with the same statements and perceived retrospective attitudes (i.e., what they thought their own and others' attitudes were before reading the material). Results revealed that whereas participants in the second group accurately estimated others' attitude changes, they underestimated the extent to which their own attitudes were influenced.
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
1
Running head: THE HIDDEN IMPACT OF CONSPIRACY THEORIES
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and actual influence of theories
surrounding the death of Princess Diana
Karen M. Douglas & Robbie M. Sutton
University of Kent
Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to:
Karen Douglas
Department of Psychology
University of Kent
Canterbury, Kent, CT2 7NP
United Kingdom
E-mail: k.douglas@kent.ac.uk
Uncorrected manuscript
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
2
Abstract
The present research examined the perceived and actual impact of exposure to
conspiracy theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. Undergraduate students
rated their agreement with a number of statements about Diana’s death. They also rated
their classmates’ perceived agreement with the statements. From the same
undergraduate population, a second group of students read some material containing
popular conspiracy theories about Diana’s death. They then rated their own and others’
agreement with the same statements, as well as perceived retrospective attitudes (i.e.,
what they thought their own and others’ attitudes were before reading the material).
Results revealed that while their estimates of others’ attitude change were accurate,
participants underestimated the extent to which their own attitudes were influenced.
Keywords: attitude change or persuasion, conspiracy theories, self-other bias,
third-person effect
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
3
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and actual influence of theories
surrounding the death of Princess Diana
On Sunday, August 31
st
1997, the death of Princess Diana shocked the world.
Almost immediately after the event, people began to question the incidents surrounding
the accident that killed her and her partner, Dodi Fayed. Was the limousine driver
Henri Paul drunk and, if so, was his dangerous driving a contributory factor in the
crash? Did the papparazzi chase the limousine into the Paris tunnel, forcing Henri Paul
to drive too fast? Not surprisingly, people wanted to understand the factors that
contributed to the death of their princess. However, in addition to plausible
explanations for the crash, there soon emerged a series of less plausible accounts that
fall under the banner of the popular term conspiracy theories. Generally, the conspiracy
theories surrounding Diana’s death invoke ‘bigger’, more powerful, or more sinister
explanations for the events than most likely occurred. For example, one theory
implicates the British Secret Service in a plot to assasinate the princess. Another
suggests that Diana staged her death so that she and Dodi Fayed could retreat into
isolation (LondonNet, 2005).
Scholars characterize conspiracy theories as attempts to explain the ultimate
cause of an event (usually a political or social event) as a secret plot by a covert alliance
of powerful individuals or organizations, rather than as an overt activity or natural
occurrence (e.g., McCauley & Jacques, 1979). Attempts to explain why people believe
conspiracy theories have focused on people’s need to explain events that are beyond
their control. In particular, some researchers view conspiracy theories as a response to
‘powerlessness’; in the face of increasingly vast and anonymous bureaucratic forces,
conspiracy theories allow people to come to terms with the possibility that these
underlying forces shape their future (e.g., Melley, 2002). Similarly, others view
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
4
conspiracy theories as a means for less powerful individuals to imagine themselves in
posession of powerful, or secret information (e.g., Mason, 2002). The belief in
conspiracy theories perhaps fulfils people’s need to explain uncontrollable situations
(McCauley & Jaques, 1979).
The belief in conspiracy theories is often seen as foolish and illogical (e.g.,
Shermer, 1997; Melley, 2002; Willman, 2000), and indeed the term itself is somewhat
dismissive or pejorative. Nonetheless, the popularity of conspiracy theories often grows
with time and theories also become more elaborate over time (McHoskey, 1995).
Conspiracy theories surround many other historical and social events such as the origins
of A.I.D.S. (e.g., Simmons & Parsons, 2005; Parsons, Simmons, Shinhoster & Kilburn,
1999), and the assasination of President John F. Kennedy (e.g., McCauley & Jacques,
1979; McHoskey, 1995). The belief in conspiracy theories is also associated with
psychological variables such as perceived power. In particular, the more influence
African American people believe themselves to have over political processes, the less
likely they are to believe conspiracy theories against African Americans (Parsons et al.,
1999). Belief in conspiracy theories among African Americans has also been linked to
system blame or perceived societal prejudice against them (Crocker, Luhtanen,
Broadnax & Blain, 1999). It has also been linked generally to lack of trust (Goertzel,
1994). Further, people who believe one conspiracy theory are more likely to believe in
others (Goertzel, 1994). Given the popularity of conspiracy theories and the
relationships between belief in such theories and psychological variables, it is therefore
surprising that little research has examined the psychological impact of exposure to
conspiracy theories. That is, to what extent do conspiracy theories actually influence
people’s attitudes? Also, are people generally aware of the impact of conspiracy
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
5
theories on their attitudes, or do they think themselves invulnerable? We addressed
these questions in the current study.
Research on the third-person effect provides a starting point to our research.
The third-person effect or TPE (Davison, 1983) is the tendency for people to believe
that persuasive media influence others more than themselves. Much research has
replicated this finding in a variety of contexts including politics and news (e.g., Duck,
Hogg & Terry, 1995) and advertising (e.g., David & Johnson, 1998; Duck, Hogg &
Terry, 1998, 1999; Gibbon & Durkin, 1995; Gunther & Thorson, 1992; Innes & Zeitz,
1988). This research demonstrates that people generally feel ‘others’ to be more
gullible than the self and that others should therefore be protected from potentially
damaging attempts to change their attitudes and behaviors.
While the belief in conspiracy theories may not be considered damaging per se,
or a direct attempt at influencing people’s attitudes for capital gain or to obtain votes,
research on the TPE is useful in considering the impact of exposure to conspiracy
theories on people’s attitudes. Just like persuasive media such as advertising, people
may not want to admit that they are influenced by conspiracy theories because this may
make them appear vulnerable, easily ‘led astray’, or weak-minded to others (Shermer,
1997). Admitting that they are influenced by conspiracy theories may also be
threatening to their self-esteem, just as admitting that they are vulnerable to persuasive
advertising (Duck & Mullin, 1995; Duck et al., 1995; Perloff, 1989). Therefore, people
might be happy to assume that others are influenced by conspiracy theories, but not so
ready to admit to being swayed by these theories themselves.
However, recent research demonstrates that, while people may not admit to
being influenced by persuasive messages, they nevertheless are influenced (Douglas
and Sutton, 2004). Even more intriguing, sometimes people appear to be completely
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
6
oblivious to their attitude change. Douglas and Sutton examined the perceived and
actual impact of messages about gun control and global warming. Results revealed that,
while people were accurate in judging the attitude change for others, they significantly
underestimated the extent to which their own attitudes were influenced (see also Bem &
McConnell, 1970; Markus, 1986; Wixon & Laird, 1976). They were therefore unaware
that the persuasive messages had an impact on their attitudes.
In the current study, we used Douglas and Sutton’s (2004) method to examine
the perceived and actual impact of conspiracy theories surrounding the death of Princess
Diana. To illustrate our procedure, undergraduate students were assigned to a control or
experimental group. The participants were drawn from the same population and were
randomly allocated to the groups, so that the groups did not differ demographically.
The control group were asked to rate their own (baseline self) and their classmates’
(baseline other) perceived agreement with a list of statements about the events
surrounding Diana’s death. In the experimental group, participants were first asked to
read some information containing popular conspiracy theories about Diana’s death.
They were then asked to rate their own (current self) and their classmates’ (current
other) perceived agreement with the same items. In addition to this, they were asked to
rate their retrospective attitudes (i.e., what they perceive their attitudes to have been
before reading the material – retrospective self) and the same for their classmates
(retrospective other).
Using this design, it was possible to compare the perceived (current –
retrospective) attitude change for self and others, with participants’ actual attitude
change (current self – baseline self). We could therefore assess how accurate
participants were about their attitude change. By sampling from the same
undergraduate student population, our participant group is also the comparison group
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
7
enabling us to also identify whether participants’ perceptions of others were accurate
with confidence. This strategy has been successfully applied to other self-serving biases
(e.g., Krueger & Dunning, 1999), and contrasts with many other studies that have asked
participants to compare themselves to broader, remote groups such as ‘other university
students’ (e.g., Cohen, Mutz, Price & Gunther, 1988, Gunther, 1991; Gunther &
Thorson, 1992)
1
.
If indeed there is a hidden impact of conspiracy theories, then people should
perceive their attitudes to be unchanged as a result of exposure to conspiracy theories.
That is, participants should rate their ‘current self’ and ‘retrospective self’ attitudes to be
the same. However, comparing perceived attitude change for others (current –
retrospective) with participants’ actual attitude change (current self – baseline self)
should reveal that while people are accurate in judging the impact of conspiracy theories
on others, that they significantly underestimate their impact on themselves. They will
therefore be unaware of the impact of exposure to conspiracy theories on their attitudes.
Method
Participants and design
A total of 96 undergraduate students at a British university participated in the
experiment. Of these, half were male and half were female. Participants’ median age
was 20.9. Participants were approached whilst at leisure on campus, and were rewarded
with sweets. The experiment consisted of a 2 (rated person: self/other) x 2 (attitude
rating: retrospective/current) within-subjects design for the experimental group. In the
control group, rated person (self/other) was manipulated within-subjects. Participants
were randomly assigned to the control or experimental groups. As would be expected,
given random assignment there were no significant differences in the distribution of age
and gender across conditions.
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
8
Materials and Procedure
Participants in the control group were informed that they were going to be asked
to read some statements about the incidents surrounding the death of Princess Diana.
They were also informed that they would be asked to rate their own agreement with
each statement, and how much they thought other undergraduate students at their
university would agree with each statement. The term ‘conspiracy theory’ was not
mentioned. Participants were then presented with five statements relating to five
popular conspiracy theories about Diana’s death. These were taken from a news archive
on the website:
(http://www.londonnet.co.uk/ln/talk/news/diana_conspiracy_theories.html).
These were as follows:
“One or more rogue ‘cells’ in the British Secret Service constructed and carried
out a plot to kill Diana.”
“There was an official campaign by MI6 to assassinate Diana, sanctioned by
elements of the establishment.”
“Diana faked her own death so she and Dodi could retreat into isolation.”
“Business enemies of Dodi and his father Mohammed Al Fayed assassinated
Dodi, with the death of Diana a cover up for their operation.”
“Diana had to be killed because the British government could not accept that the
mother of the future king was involved with a Muslim Arab.”
For each statement, participants were asked to rate either their own, or others’
agreement on a seven-point scale from 1 ‘strongly disagree’ to 7 ‘strongly agree’.
Those participants who rated their own attitudes first were asked to rate others’ attitudes
second and vice versa so that order was counterbalanced. The scale reliabilities for self
(
α
= .73) and others (
α
= .85) were both acceptable in accordance with Nunnally’s
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
9
(1977) recommendation that a scale alpha of .70 should be considered acceptable in
social psychological research. On completion, participants were debriefed and thanked
for their participation.
Participants in the experimental group were informed that they would be asked
to read some material about the incidents surrounding the death of Princess Diana, and
to answer some questions. At this point, participants were given a sheet of paper
outlining some points about the incidents surrounding Diana’s death. This was prefaced
with the following statement:
“Many believe that Princess Diana’s death was not an accident. Additional
information has been discussed that casts doubt on the conclusion that Diana’s
death was accidental. Some of this information is presented below.”
The term ‘conspiracy theory’ was not mentioned in the information. The information
that followed was a series of eight points outlining arguments for the position that
Diana’s death was not an accident. These were popular conspiracy theories such as the
concern over the rapid disposal of Diana and Dodi’s bodies, the missing Fiat Uno that
was said to be involved in the accident, and the suggestion that witnesses heard a bomb
blast immediately prior to the crash. For example, one conspiracy theory was worded
as follows:
“Immediately after the crash news was broadcast, witnesses appeared on U.S.
TV saying that they heard an explosion or bang before they heard the car crash.
Was this a gunshot, or a bomb?”
After participants read the information, they were presented with the same five-
item scale as for the control group. Participants were asked to respond to these items
four times by rating: (a) their current agreement or disagreement with each statement
(current self,
α
= .82), (b) how much they agreed or disagreed with each statement
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
10
before reading the material (retrospective self,
α
= .81), (c) how much they think their
classmates would currently agree or disagree with each statement (current other,
α
=
.86), and (d) how much they think their classmates would have agreed or disagreed with
each statement before reading the material (retrospective other,
α
= .79). Again,
participants were asked to respond to each item on a seven-point scale from 1 ‘strongly
disagree’ to 4 ‘neutral’ to 7 ‘strongly agree’. Question order was blocked for ‘self’ and
‘other’ and then alternated across time (retrospective or current), such that there were
four different versions of the questionnaire. In the four versions, ratings were made in
the following orders:
1. Self current self retrospective other current other retrospective
2. Self retrospective self current other retrospective other current
3. Other current other retrospective self current self retrospective
4. Other retrospective other current self retrospective self current
Participants were randomly assigned to one of the four questionnaires and again, there
were no differences in the distribution of participants’ age and gender across conditions.
On completion, participants were debriefed and thanked for their participation.
Results
The analyses were carried out in accordance with those of Douglas and Sutton
(2004). Results for the experimental group were entered into a 2 (rated person:
self/other) x 2 (attitude rating: retrospective/current) repeated measures ANOVA.
There was no main effect for rated person. Overall, participants did not rate their
classmates as endorsing the statements about Diana’s death more (M = 2.77) than
themselves (M = 2.61), F(1, 47) = 1.35, p = .251, η
2
= .03. Results did however reveal a
main effect for attitude rating, such that mean ratings of agreement with the statements
across self and others increased from retrospective (M = 2.48) to current (M = 2.90),
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
11
F(1, 47) = 38.91, p = .000, η
2
= .45. Finally, as predicted the interaction between rated
person (self/other) and attitude rating, was significant, F(1, 47) = 11.4, p = .001, η
2
=
.20. All means, standard deviations and significant between-cell differences are
displayed in Table 1.
Perceived attitude change for self and others
We compared current and retrospective attitudes and results revealed that
attitude change was perceived to occur for others, t(47) = 5.02, p = .000, d = 0.76, and
the self, t(47) = 2.52, p = .015, d = 0.14. As expected, the difference scores between
current and retrospective attitudes for self and others revealed that attitude change was
judged to be greater for others than the self, (Ms = 0.71 and 0.15), t(47) = 3.38, p =
.001, d = 0.75.
Actual attitude change
As predicted, participants were more in agreement with the statements about
Diana’s death in the experimental group than in the control group, t(47) = 5.23, p =
.000, d = 1.07. Also, as expected, participants’ perceptions of their own change (current
self – retrospective self, M = 0.15) were significantly lower than actual attitude change
(current self – baseline self, M = 0.93); a test of the difference between these scores
revealed that participants underestimated the extent to which they were influenced, t(47)
= 13.52, p = .000, d = 0.87.
Accuracy of attitude change perceptions
The actual attitude change for the sample (current self – baseline self) was no
different to the attitude change that participants in the experimental group attributed to
their classmates (current other – retrospective other), t(47) = 1.57, p = .124, d = 0.20.
This finding teamed with the finding that participants significantly underestimated their
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
12
attitude change, is further evidence that participants are more accurate about others’
attitude change than their own (Douglas and Sutton, 2004).
The experimental group’s retrospective ratings of their attitudes (retrospective
self) were more favoring of the statements than those of the control group (baseline
self), t(47) = 4.52, p = .000, d = 0.91. This suggests that participants misremembered
what their original attitudes were (cf. Bem & McConnell, 1970; Markus, 1986; Wixon
& Laird, 1976).
Discussion
The current results suggest that there may indeed be a hidden impact of
conspiracy theories. While participants in our study were prepared to admit to being
influenced by conspiracy theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana – the
difference between ‘current self’ and ‘retrospective self’ attitudes was significant –
participants significantly underestimated the extent of their own attitude change.
Participants were therefore unaware of the extent of the impact of the conspiracy
theories on their own attitudes. In contrast, their estimates of how much others’
attitudes had changed were accurate. This finding supports research in the TPE
literature demonstrating that people underestimate the extent of their own persuasibility,
rather than overestimating the extent of others’ persuasibility (e.g., Cohen et al., 1988;
Douglas & Sutton, 2004). This study also replicates the finding that people are
sometimes unaware, upon exposure to information, that their previous attitudes have
changed (Douglas & Sutton, 2004; see also Bem & McConnell, 1970; Markus, 1986;
Wixon & Laird, 1976).
It is also interesting to note that participants were not only unaware of the extent
of their attitude change. Indeed, it also appeared that participants misremembered what
their original attitudes were because their perceived retrospective attitudes did not
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
13
match the original attitude ratings given by the control group. In other words,
participants inaccurately reported what their retrospective attitudes were. This
adjustment was not made for others. This ‘revision’ of previous attitudes perhaps
creates the illusion for participants that attitude change occurred less for themselves
than for others. Akin to the hindsight bias (Fischhoff, 1975; Hawkins & Hastie, 1990),
participants seemingly misremembered their previous attitudes so that they appeared
closer to their current attitudes. Taken together, our findings suggest that the conspiracy
theories about the death of Princess Diana influenced participants without awareness;
while participants perceived themselves to be relatively invulnerable when they were
clearly not, they were more than happy to suggest that others members of their class
were more influenced.
In replicating the results of Douglas and Sutton (2004) in the domain of
conspiracy theories, the current research extends previous work to a different form of
social influence. Here, the information given to participants is unlikely to change their
behavior. However, participants still underestimated the extent to which they were
influenced. Previously, this bias had only been demonstrated for blatant influence
attempts such as messages about global warming (attempts to persuade people to
conserve fuel; Douglas & Sutton, 2004) and defamatory messages about politicians
(attempts to influence voting behavior; Cohen et al., 1988). Therefore, from these
results we can conclude that the underestimation of personal influence extends beyond
intentionally persuasive content domains. Perhaps people more generally assume that
they are resistant to attempts to persuade them.
So, how far would this bias extend? Future research may be designed to
examine other areas where people are unaware of the influence they experience. In
particular, while our results extend previous findings to conspiracy theories (a new
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
14
content domain), future research may also examine new media. For example, the
Internet is a venue of many concerted attempts to influence the public, by interests
ranging from companies attempting to sell products to right-wing and racist groups
attempting to recruit new members. Research suggests that people are concerned about
the influence of this material (e.g., Beckles, 1997; Zickmund, 1997), and respond
strongly to it (e.g., Douglas & McGarty, 2001, 2002). However, beliefs about the
impact of harmful Internet messages on self and others, and the actual impact of this
material on people, have not yet been investigated.
Future research might also investigate the underlying mechanisms that lead
people to underestimate the extent to which they are influenced by conspiracy theories
and other forms of influence. In the TPE literature, the assumption that others are more
influenced than the self has been linked to a motivation to maintain positive self-esteem
(e.g., Duck & Mullin, 1995; Duck et al., 1995; Perloff, 1989). Perhaps therefore,
denying the true extent of influence on the self might protect people from losing ‘face’
with others, or from feeling gullible. Both are likely to have an impact on self-esteem.
The extent to which it is normatively acceptable to be influenced by media content has
also been linked with the TPE (Duck et al., 1999). It may be normatively unacceptable
to admit being influenced by elaborate and illogical conspiracy theories, and this could
potentially explain why people do not admit the full extent of their personal influence.
Finally, our findings call into question current theorizing about the function of
conspiracy theories. If indeed conspiracy theories are a means to provide explanations
for uncertain events (Melley, 2002), or are a response to powerlessness (e.g., Melley,
2002; Mason, 2002), then it is perhaps surprising that people are not prepared to fully
accept that they have been influenced by them. We may expect people to be reluctant to
agree that they are influenced by advertising because it has a material impact on their
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
15
lives, but if conspiracy theories are adaptive, then why are they so widely dismissed as
foolish and controversial? Future research may therefore attempt to uncover other
reasons why people overtly reject conspiracy theories but perhaps privately accept them
to be true. Related to this, it would be useful to examine the potential impact of
conspiracy beliefs on other socially significant variables such as locus of control
(Rotter, 1966), and just world beliefs (e.g., Lerner, 1980; Lipkus, Dalbert & Siegler,
1996; Sutton & Douglas, 2005).
In summary, the current study provides a first examination of the real impact of
conspiracy theories. Indeed, conspiracy theories about the death of Princess Diana were
influential on our British participants. However, these very participants were oblivious
to the impact that these conspiracy theories had on their attitudes. They correctly
assumed that others from the same demographic as themselves were influenced but
denied the full extent of influence on themselves.
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
16
Author note
The authors thank Lisa Huggins who collected the data for this study.
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
17
Footnotes
1. Douglas and Sutton (2004) used both a cross-sectional design
(control/experimental groups) as in the current study, and a longitudinal design
where one group of participants was tested in two phases. Their findings were
the same using both methods.
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
18
References
Beckles, C. (1997). Black struggles in cyberspace: Cyber-segregation and cyber-nazis.
The Western Journal of Black Studies, 21, 12-19.
Bem, D.J., & McConnell, H.K. (1970). Testing the self-perception explanation of
dissonance phenomena: On the salience of premanipulation attitudes. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 23-31.
Cohen, J., Mutz, D., Price, V., & Gunther, A. (1988). Perceived impact of defamation:
An experiment on third-person effects. Public Opinion Quarterly, 52, 161-173.
Crocker, J., Luhtanen, R., Broadnax, S., & Blaine, B.E. (1999). Belief in U.S.
government conspiracies against Blacks among Black and White college
students: Powerlessness or system blame? Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 25, 941-953.
David, P., & Johnson, M.A. (1998). The role of self in third-person effects about body
image. Journal of Communication, 48, 37-58.
Davison, W.P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 47, 1-15.
Douglas, K.M., & McGarty, C. (2002). On computers and elsewhere: A model of the
effects of Internet identifiability on communicative behaviour. Group Dynamics,
6, 17-26.
Douglas, K.M., & McGarty, C. (2001). Identifiability and self-presentation: Computer-
mediated communication and intergroup interaction. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 40, 399-416.
Douglas, K.M., & Sutton, R.M. (2004). Right about others, wrong about ourselves?
Actual and perceived self-other differences in resistance to persuasion. British
Journal of Social Psychology 43, 585-603.
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
19
Duck, J.M., Hogg, M.A., & Terry, D.J. (1995). Me, us and them: Political
identification and the third-person effect in the 1993 Australian federal election.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 195-215.
Duck, J.M., Hogg, M.A., & Terry, D.J. (1998). Perceived self-other differences in
persuasibility: The effects of interpersonal and group-based similarity. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 1-21.
Duck, J.M., Hogg, M.A., & Terry, D.J. (1999). Social identity and perceptions of
media persuasion: Are we always less influenced than others? Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 29, 1879-1899.
Duck, J.M., & Mullin, B.A. (1995). The perceived impact of the mass media:
Reconsidering the third person effect. European Journal of Social Psychology,
25, 77-93.
Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight / foresight: The effect of outcome knowledge on
judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 1, 288-299
Gibbon, P., & Durkin, K. (1995). The third person effect: Social distance and
perceived media bias. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 597-602.
Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731-742.
Gunther, A.C., & Thorson, E. (1992). Perceived persuasive effects of product
commercials and public service announcements. Communication Research, 19,
574-596.
Hawkins, S.A. & Hastie, R. (1990). Hindsight: Biased judgements of past events after
the outcomes are known. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 311-327.
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
20
Innes, J.M., & Zeitz, H. (1988). The public’s view of the impact of the mass media: A
test of the ‘third person’ effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 457-
463.
Lerner, M.J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York:
Plenum Press.
Lipkus, I.M., Dalbert, C., & Siegler, I.C. (1996). The importance of distinguishing the
belief in a just world for self versus others: Implications for psychological well-
being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 666-677.
LondonNet. Princess Diana: The conspiracy theories (2005). Retrieved on 27 July 2006
from http://www.londonnet.co.uk/ln/talk/news/diana_conspiracy_theories.html.
Markus, G.B. (1986). Stability and change in political attitudes: Observed, recalled
and “explained”. Political Behavior, 8, 21-44.
Mason, F. (2002). A poor person’s cognitive mapping. In P. Knight (Ed.), Conspiracy
nation: The politics of paranoia in postwar America (pp. 40-56). New York:
New York University Press.
McCauley, C., & Jacques, S. (1979). The popularity of conspiracy theories of
presidential assassination: A Bayesian analysis. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 37, 637-644.
McHoskey, J.W. (1995). Case closed? On the John F. Kennedy assassination: Biased
assimilation of evidence and attitude polarization. Basic and Applied Social
Psychology, 17, 395-409.
Melley, T. (2002). Agency, panic and the culture of conspiracy. In P. Knight (Ed.),
Conspiracy nation: The politics of paranoia in postwar America (pp. 57-81).
New York: New York University Press.
Nunnally, J.C. (1977). Psychometric theory (2
nd
ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
21
Parsons, S., Simmons, W., Shinhoster, F., & Kilburn, J. (1999). A test of the grapevine:
An empirical examination of conspiracy theories among African Americans.
Sociological Spectrum, 19, 201-222.
Perloff, R.M. (1989). Ego-involvement and the third person effect of televised news
coverage. Communication Research, 16, 236-262.
Rotter, J.B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of
reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80.
Shermer, M. (1997). Why people believe weird things: Pseudoscience, superstition, and
other confusions of our time. New York: Freeman.
Simmons, W.P., & Parsons, S. (2005). Beliefs in conspiracy theories among African
Americans: A comparison of elites and masses. Social Science Quarterly, 86,
582-598.
Sutton, R.M., & Douglas, K.M. (2005). Justice for all, or just for me? More support for
the self-other distinction in just-world beliefs. Personality and Individual
Differences, 39, 637-645.
Willman, S. (2002). Spinning paranoia: The ideologies of conspiracy and contingency
in postmodern culture. In P. Knight (Ed.), Conspiracy nation: The politics of
paranoia in postwar America (pp. 21-39). New York: New York University
Press.
Wixon, D.R., & Laird, J.D. (1976). Awareness and attitude change in the forced-
compliance paradigm: The importance of when. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 34, 376-384.
Zickmund, S. (1997). Approaching the radical other: The discursive culture of
cyberhate. In S.G. Jones (Ed.), Virtual culture: Identity and communication in
cybersociety, (pp. 185-205). London: Sage.
The hidden impact of conspiracy theories
22
Table 1. Mean (and standard deviation) control, retrospective and current attitudes
towards for self and other. Higher values indicate greater rated endorsement of
the statements.
Attitude rating
Control Retrospective Current Perceived Actual
attitude change attitude change
______________________________________________________________________________
Self 1.75 (0.62)aa 2.53 (1.05)b 2.68 (1.06)dd 0.15 (0.40)f
Person 0.93 (1.21 )g
Other 2.15 (0.75)c 2.42 (0.84)bc 3.13 (1.01)ee 0.71 (0.98)g
______________________________________________________________________________
Means that share a subscript are not significantly different at p < .05.
... In the very first studies we conducted on the psychology of conspiracy theories, we were interested in the extent to which conspiracy theories affect what people think about important events (Douglas & Sutton, 2008). That is, do they change people's attitudes about what might have happened? ...
... Means that share a subscript do not differ significantly from p < .05. Data from Douglas and Sutton (2008). Douglas and Sutton (2020) and is used under the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. ...
Chapter
Conspiracy theories are a common feature of social and political debate, and of growing concern for governments and policymakers. Awareness of the importance of conspiracy theories has been facilitated by a rapid expansion of empirical research in the past 20 years, and the bulk of this research comes from social psychology. In the current chapter, we review this significant research progress, focusing on the antecedents of conspiracy beliefs and their consequences. As we review this literature, we refer to examples from our ongoing research programme. We then articulate what we perceive to be the main limitations of the research to date focusing on measurement, sampling and theoretical focus, and propose avenues for future research. Finally, we briefly discuss interventions to address the effects of conspiracy theories.
... Like previous meta-analyses on similar topics (Bowes et al., 2023;Golec de Zavala et al., 2022;Goreis & Voracek, 2019;Stasielowicz, 2022a;Stojanov & Halberstadt, 2020), we also took into account the possibility that effect size magnitudes may depend on the type of conspiracy beliefs being studied. For example, researchers often measure belief in well-known specific conspiracy theories, such as those surrounding the deaths of President John F. Kennedy or Princess Diana (e.g., Douglas & Sutton, 2008;Swami et al., 2011). Other scales have been constructed to capture belief in more general notions of conspiracies. ...
Preprint
Belief in conspiracy theories has been linked to harmful consequences for individuals and societies. In an effort to understand and mitigate these effects, researchers have sought to explain the psychological appeal of conspiracy theories. This article presents a wide-ranging systematic review and meta-analysis of the literature on conspiracy beliefs. We analyzed 971 effect sizes from 279 independent studies (Nparticipants = 137,406) to examine the relationships between psychological motives and conspiracy beliefs. Results indicated that these relationships were significant for all three analyzed classes of motivation: epistemic (k = 114, r = .14), existential (k = 121, r = .16), and social motivations related to the individual, relational, and collective selves (k = 100, r = .16). For all motives examined, we observed considerable heterogeneity. Moderation analyses suggest that the relationships were weaker, albeit still significant, when experimental (vs. correlational) designs were used, and differed depending on the conspiracy measure used. We statistically compare the absolute meta-analytic effect size magnitudes against each other and discuss limitations and future avenues for research, including interventions to reduce susceptibility to conspiracy theories.
... Incorrect information, frequently disseminated with the intent to mislead (Ecker et al., 2022), threatens the epistemic potential of democratic decision-making as it hinders access to accurate information (Brown, 2018 Participatory Conspiracy theories Are understood as an attempt to make sense of events by proposing a secret plot between powerful individuals or organizations that aims to accomplish sinister ends through continuing deception of the public (Douglas and Sutton, 2008;Goertzel, 1994). Conspiracy theories are often vague, are not inherently true or false, and tolerate internal contradictions (Wood et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite a long history of research on democratic backsliding, the process itself − in which the executive branch amasses power and undermines democratic processes and institutions − remains poorly understood. We seek to shed light on the underlying mechanisms by studying democratic near misses: cases in which a period of autocratic governance is quickly reversed or full backsliding is prevented at the last minute. Building on the literature on near misses in sociotechnical systems such as nuclear power plants, we adapt the drift-to-danger model to the study of democratic systems. Two key findings emerge: First, democratic backsliding is often triggered by political elites pushing the boundaries of their power by violating norms, which are crucial yet vulnerable safeguards for democracy. Second, democratic backsliding is unpredictable and non-linear, being driven by the interaction between political elites and the public. Norm-violating elites may feel legitimized by a supportive public that sees norm violations as justified. At the same time, political elites may signal that norm-violating behaviour is acceptable, potentially leading the public to adopt anti-democratic beliefs and behaviours. We identify risk factors that make norm violations more likely and outline behavioural sciences-based interventions to address these violations.
... These conspiracy beliefs can be very specific, for example, when people assume that a certain group (e.g., a political party) or a specific individual (e.g., Bill Gates) engages in conspiratorial acts. When people are confronted with a conspiracy theory, they subsequently tend to agree with it more than people who have not been exposed (Jolley & Douglas, 2014a, 2014bPummerer, Böhm, et al., 2022), even if they are not aware that their endorsement of the conspiracy theory has increased (Douglas & Sutton, 2008). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
2024) The appraisal model of conspiracy theories (AMCT): Applying appraisal theories to understand emotional and behavioral reactions to conspiracy theories. Psychological Inquiry. ISSN 1047-840X. (In press) Kent Academic Repository Downloaded from https://kar.
... As a conspiracy theory becomes more prevalent, it can deeply embed itself within a culture, in some cases through material artefacts (Boltanski 2014). This widespread dissemination increases the likelihood that more individuals will encounter these ideas, potentially leading to greater acceptance and belief (Douglas and Sutton 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
This article explores the pervasive influence of conspiracy theories, specifically the New World Order (NWO) and Golden Billion theories, within the context of the ongoing conflict between Russia and Ukraine. These theories form key narrative frameworks in Russian state media and global conspiracy communities, shaping perceptions of geopolitical events. This study dissects four pivotal episodes within the Russia-Ukraine conflict to illustrate how conspiracy theories shape public perception and policy direction, further entrenching ideological divides. In the first episode of the 2022 full-scale invasion, narratives of the Golden Billion were utilised to justify the attack, presenting Russia as a bastion against the Western elite's plans to dominate the global economy and resources. The second episode examines the attack on Mariupol in 2022, framed by Russian propaganda as a necessary act to thwart the supposed expansion of NATO and the EU, underpinned by the NWO agenda aiming to dilute Russian influence in Eastern Europe. The third episode analyses the Nord Stream pipeline sabotage in 2022, interpreted by some conspiracy theorists as an act by the NWO to destabilise Europe's energy security, thus consolidating control over energy routes and resources. The fourth episode delves into the 2024 Moscow terrorist attacks, which were seen by some as either a false flag operation conducted by Western powers or as a legitimate repercussion of Western encroachment orchestrated to weaken Russia's resolve and international standing. Each episode is contextualised within a broader conspiratorial framework, highlighting the dualistic nature of the NWO and Golden Billion theories that paint the conflict not merely as territorial disputes but as a clash between fundamentally opposing worldviews and global orders. This narrative analysis not only underscores the role of conspiracy theories in shaping geopolitical discourse but also demonstrates their utility in mobilising domestic support, framing international criticism, and justifying military actions. Our findings suggest that these conspiratorial narratives provide a resilient, albeit misleading, lens through which supporters of the Kremlin's policies can rationalise the war, attributing complex sociopolitical dynamics to the malevolent machinations of a global elite. This study contributes to understanding how modern conflicts are interpreted through ancient conspiratorial lenses, impacting national and international policy and public opinion.
Article
Full-text available
Science denialism is at the heart of many conspiracy theory beliefs. We propose that such beliefs are manifestations of a distal social process: spite. In three pre‐registered studies, we test the hypothesis that established predictors of these beliefs (epistemic, existential, and social motives) are specific cues of competitive disadvantage that provoke a common facultative “spiteful” psychological response, making a person more open to believing in conspiracy theories. Study 1 ( N = 301; UK representative Prolific sample), found that spite mediated the relationship between realistic threat and in‐group narcissism (social motives), political powerlessness (existential motive), and intolerance for uncertainty (epistemic motive), and conspiracy theory belief and COVID‐19 conspiracies. This pattern was replicated in Study 2 ( N = 405; UK representative Prolific sample). In Study 3 ( N = 405; UK representative Prolific sample), we found that those who engaged in a spite‐inducing task reported higher levels of spite which indirectly resulted in stronger beliefs in conspiracy theories. The overall pattern of results provides initial evidence that spite may play a role in why people engage with false information. Research and policy implications of these findings are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
A few research studies in public relations have identified significant associations between relational and situational variables in the context of a specific issue involving an organization. Despite this, no research to date has explained why and how these associations occur. Therefore, this study tests the roles of conspiratorial thinking and responsibility attribution in influencing the confluence of publics' perceptions of an organization with their perceptions about an issue involving the organization. Situating the study in the context of the Australian Government and the issue of high-rise overdevelopment, an online survey was conducted with a nationally representative sample of 400 Australian citizens. The results showed that publics with high levels of distrust and political cynicism toward the government also reported high conspiratorial thinking about the government. Conspiratorial thinking was significantly associated with responsibility attribution to the government for causing the issue of high-rise overdevelopment. Subsequently, publics developed situational perceptions and motivations about the issue. The findings showed that even if individuals attribute responsibility to the government for causing an issue, as long as they have high situational activeness in the issue, they will engage in proactive communicative behaviors to seek, forefend and forward information from the government. URL (free open access):
Article
Full-text available
Belief in conspiracy theories has been linked to harmful consequences for individuals and societies. In an effort to understand and mitigate these effects, researchers have sought to explain the psychological appeal of conspiracy theories. This article presents a wide-ranging systematic review and meta-analysis of the literature on conspiracy beliefs. We analyzed 971 effect sizes from 279 independent studies (Nparticipants = 137,406) to examine the relationships between psychological motives and conspiracy beliefs. Results indicated that these relationships were significant for all three analyzed classes of motivation: epistemic (k = 114, r = .14), existential (k = 121, r = .16), and social motivations related to the individual, relational, and collective selves (k = 100, r = .16). For all motives examined, we observed considerable heterogeneity. Moderation analyses suggest that the relationships were weaker, albeit still significant, when experimental (vs. correlational) designs were used, and differed depending on the conspiracy measure used. We statistically compare the absolute meta-analytic effect size magnitudes against each other and discuss limitations and future avenues for research, including interventions to reduce susceptibility to conspiracy theories.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
انتشرت في العالم حديثًا، وفي أوساط مجتمعنا العربيّ خصوصًا، جماعاتٌ عدّة تروّج لسطحيّة الأرض وادِّعاء أنّ حقيقة كروِيّة الأرض نظريّة باطلة، معتقدين أنّ العالم هو ضحيّة خداعٍ كبيرٍ ومؤامرةٍ مدبّرةٍ بشأن كثيرٍ من القضايا المعاصرة، خصوصًا "شكل الأرض"، وتزعم أنّ الأرض مسطّحة، وغير ذلك كذبٌ وافتراء. على الرّغم من بطلان هذا الادّعاء والحقيقة العلميّة الجليّة التي تدلّ على كرويّة الأرض، إلاّ أنّ اعتقاد أنّ الأرض مسطّحة الشّكل، المبنيّ على علمٍ زائف، قد تبنّته فئةٌ معتبرةٌ في مجتمعاتنا من مستخدمي وسائل التّواصل الاجتماعيّ واليوتيوب، وتزداد هذه الظّاهرة توسّعًا وانتشارًا مريبين، يدعوان إلى القلق والتّفاعل، حيث تدع الحليم حيرانًا. المثير للدّهشة والغرابة أنّ من مروّجي وأتباع هذه النّظريّة الباطلة طبقةً مثقّفةً من خرّيجي المعاهد والجامعات، بيد أنّه لم يكن منهم علماءُ ذوو شأن. حيث نظّموا مؤتمراتٍ وملتقياتٍ دفاعًا عن نظريّتهم، ونشروا فيديوهاتٍ، وحرّروا عشراتِ المنشوراتِ والمقالاتِ المدعومة غالبًا بصورٍ وخرائطَ تدّعي أنّ الأرض مسطّحة وليست كرويّة، وأنّ العلماء قديمًا وحديثًا، خاصةً وكالة الفضاء الأميركية "ناسا"، ضلّلوا النّاس بكروية الأرض، وأنّ صور الأرض، وكذا نزول رواد الفضاء على سطح القمر، مجرّد صور "فوتوشوب" ومشاهد مفبركة ومزيّفة من صنع الشركات السينمائية الأمريكية المتواطئة. لم تأخذ هذه الظّاهرة حظًّا وافرًا من الدّراسة والوقوف على أبعادها التّاريخية والاجتماعية والعلمية، وحتى السّياسية، رغم وجود بعض الدّراسات والبحوث حول أسباب ودوافع الاعتقاد بأنّ الأرض مسطّحة واعتبارها مؤامرةً تُحاك ضدّ الإنسان المعاصر، إلاّ أنّ هناك عواملَ وأسبابًا مشتركةً تقف وراء التّرويج لها ولغيرها من الاعتقادات الباطلة، تتطلّب الاهتمام وبذلَ جهدٍ للتّعمّق في دراستها ومناقشتها والحدّ منها. سنعالج في هذه الورقة البحثّية ظاهرة انتشار نظريّة "مؤامرة كروية الأرض" في العالم، من خلال نشأتها والوقوف على عوامل وأسباب تفشّيها في العالم، وخاصة في الوطن العربي حديثًا، كما سنعرض ونناقش مقترحات حلول وآليّاتٍ للحدّ من انتشار الظّاهرة بين أوساط المجتمع.
Article
Full-text available
Surveyed 348 residents of southwestern New Jersey and found that most believed that several of a list of 10 conspiracy theories were at least probably true. Ss who believed in 1 conspiracy were more likely also to believe in others. Belief in conspiracies was correlated with anomia, lack of interpersonal trust, and insecurity about employment. Blacks and Hispanics were more likely to believe in conspiracy theories than were Whites. Younger Ss were slightly more likely to believe in conspiracy theories, but there were few significant correlations with gender, educational level, or occupational category. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
This research examines the prevalence of belief in conspiracy theories among African Americans in one Deep South state and identifies the factors related to these beliefs . Overall , there is a surprisingly strong belief in most conspiracy theories involving government . Over 85 % of the respondents agreed or strongly agreed that African Americans are harassed by police because of their race and that the criminal justice system is not fair to Blacks . The theories with the least support involved transracial adoption , family planning , and needle - exchange programs as genocide . Through factor analysis , the 11 conspiracy theory questions were combined into conceptual scales . The theories grouped into two distinct factors - malicious intent and benign neglect , with benign theories the more prevalent of the two . Suprisingly , age , gender , and education were not significant in explaining beliefs in malicious intent or benign neglect conspiracy theories . Among the interesting differences between the two groups of theories , church attendance was not significantly related to support for malicious intent theories , whereas it was negatively related to support for benign theories . The most important variable for explaining belief in conspiracies was the perceived involvement by African Americans in government . Those who believed that Blacks could influence the political process were less likely to believe in conspiracy theories . This finding suggests that such beliefs in conspiracy theories will not be reduced until African Americans perceive that they have more of a role to play in their government .
Article
Full-text available
The third-person effect hypothesis suggests that people systematically overestimate the extent to which others are affected by mass media messages. It also proposes that people act in accordance with their estimates of effects on others. This study set out to test both estimations and actions and to explain the phenomenon in terms of attribution theory. An experimental design manipulating the trustworthiness of the source of a defamatory newspaper article produced a third-person effect. Subjects' estimates of the damaging effects of the article, however, did not correspond to their assessment of a penalty against the newspaper. They took more account of source motivation than of impact on the audience when taking action.
Article
Full-text available
A recent but robust phenomenon described in communication literature has been the third-person effect—the finding that in response to mass media messages, such as news stories and programs, people estimate themselves to be less affected than others. The present experiment asked whether this self-other pattern would characterize responses to two types of product commercials (i.e., those that did and those that did not engender emotion) and to public service announcements (PSAs). The authors were also concerned with how accurately people could estimate the effects of these types of ads on themselves and others. Results indicated that for neutral ads, people estimated themselves to be more resistant than others, but for emotional ads, people estimated themselves to be more yielding to influence than others. For PSAs, there were no differences in perceived self and other influence. In addition, judgments of persuasive influence on self and others were markedly overestimated. Perhaps most interestingly, there was both a directional (yielding vs. resistance) and a magnitudinal impact of emotion on the influence estimates.
Article
K. Douglas and C. McGarty (2001) demonstrated that being identifiable to an in-group audience in a computer-mediated communication (CMC) setting leads people to describe anonymous out-group targets in more abstract or stereotypical ways. On the basis of these findings and the social identity model of deindividuation effects (SIDE; S. Reicher, R. Spears, & T. Postmes, 1995), the authors aimed to test a model of the effects of identifiability on communicative behavior in and beyond CMC. Participants in 3 studies, I CMC and 2 pen and paper, were asked to write responses to controversial messages. In all 3 studies, communicators who were identifiable to an in-group audience used more stereotypical language to describe anonymous out-group targets. Studies 2 and 3 suggested that rather than being strategic, this may result from more subtle or implicit communicative processes.
Article
This study examines the perceived effect of idealized media images on self and classmates for three levels of outcome undesirability: perception of ideal body weight, effect on self-esteem, and likelihood of developing an eating disorder. A significant third-person effect was observed, which widened as the outcome increased in social undesirability. Those with high self-esteem exhibited stronger third-person effect than those with low self-esteem. The overall pattern of findings suggests that two related but distinct processes might be involved in the third-person effect: (a) a general process associated with self-esteem, which explains perceived effect of media both on self and others; and (b) a specific process tied to situational personal vulnerability, which explains perceived effect on self, but does not explain perceived effect on others.
Article
1. The Belief in a Just World.- 2. The First Experiment: The Effect of Fortuitous Reward.- 3. The Second Experiment: Observers' Reactions to the "Innocent Victim".- 4. The Third Experiment: The Martyred and Innocent Victims.- 5. Three Experiments That Assess the Effects of Sex and Educational Background of Observers, Experimenter and Observer Influence on One Another, and the Reactions of "Informed" and Nonimplicated Observers.- 6. Reactions to the Belief in a Just World Theory and Findings: The "Nay-Sayers".- 7. Condemning the Victimized.- 8. The Assignment of Blame.- 9. The Response to Victimization: Extreme Tests of the Belief in a Just World.- 10. Who Believes in a Just World: Dimension or Style.- 11. Deserving versus Justice.- References.
Article
People tend to hold overly favorable views of their abilities in many social and intellectual domains. The authors suggest that this overestimation occurs, in part, because people who are unskilled in these domains suffer a dual burden: Not only do these people reach erroneous conclusions and make unfortunate choices, but their incompetence robs them of the metacognitive ability to realize it. Across 4 studies, the authors found that participants scoring in the bottom quartile on tests of humor, grammar, and logic grossly overestimated their test performance and ability. Although their test scores put them in the 12th percentile, they estimated themselves to be in the 62nd. Several analyses linked this miscalibration to deficits in metacognitive skill, or the capacity to distinguish accuracy from error. Paradoxically, improving the skills of the participants, and thus increasing their metacognitive competence, helped them recognize the limitations of their abilities. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)