ArticlePDF Available

Mirror, Mirror on the Wall: Enhancement in Self-Recognition

Authors:

Abstract

People's inferences about their own traits and abilities are often enhancing. A series of experiments suggests that this enhancement extends to more automatic and perceptual judgments as well, such that people recognize their own faces as being more physically attractive than they actually are. In each experiment, participants' faces were made more or less attractive using a morphing procedure. Participants were more likely to recognize an attractively enhanced version of their own face out of a lineup as their own, and they identified an attractively enhanced version of their face more quickly in a lineup of distractor faces. This enhancement bias occurred for both one's own face and a friend's face but not for a relative stranger's face. Such enhancement was correlated with implicit measures of self-worth but not with explicit measures, consistent with this variety of enhancement being a relatively automatic rather than deliberative process.
1159
Mirror, Mirror on the Wall:
Enhancement in Self-Recognition
Nicholas Epley
University of Chicago
Erin Whitchurch
University of Virginia
(Dunning & Cohen, 1992). Negative stereotypes about
others are selectively activated and applied to make one-
self look better (Kunda & Sinclair, 1999). Flattering infor-
mation about the self is accepted readily, whereas
threatening information is evaluated more critically and
ultimately derogated (Ditto & Lopez, 1992; Ditto, Munro,
Apanovitch, Scepansky, & Lockhart, 2003; Ditto,
Scepansky, Munro, Apanovich, & Lockhart, 1998). These
and other deliberate reasoning strategies (Chambers &
Windschitl, 2004) are the tools that frequently enable
people to form a more desirable image of their traits and
abilities than reality might allow.
We sought evidence in this research, however, for a
more direct and potentially automatic version of
enhancement, using a novel method to study biases in
the evaluation of relatively objective self-relevant stim-
uli. In particular, we investigated whether people would
show evidence of enhancement in the recognition of their
own facial image. Do people recognize themselves—their
very own faces—as being more desirable than they actu-
ally are? In addition, are people more calibrated evalu-
ating the attractiveness of strangers about whom they
know relatively nothing?
Authors’ Note: We thank Julia Crowell, Nicholas Josephowitz,
Megan Kolasinski, Julie Ragsdale, Aram Seo, and Audrey Tse for
assisting with data collection; Justin Kruger, Jasmine Kwong, Allen
McConnel, Gillian Rhodes, and Bernd Wittenbrink for helpful com-
ments; and National Science Foundation Grant SES0241544, the
University of Chicago Graduate School of Business, and the
Templeton Foundation for financial support. Please address corre-
spondence to Nicholas Epley, Graduate School of Business, University
of Chicago, 5807 South Woodlawn Avenue, Chicago, IL 60637; e-
mail: epley@chicagogsb.edu.
PSPB, Vol. 34 No. 9, September 2008 1159-1170
DOI: 10.1177/0146167208318601
© 2008 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
People’s inferences about their own traits and abilities
are often enhancing. A series of experiments suggests
that this enhancement extends to more automatic and
perceptual judgments as well, such that people recognize
their own faces as being more physically attractive than
they actually are. In each experiment, participants’ faces
were made more or less attractive using a morphing pro-
cedure. Participants were more likely to recognize an
attractively enhanced version of their own face out of a
lineup as their own, and they identified an attractively
enhanced version of their face more quickly in a lineup of
distracter faces. This enhancement bias occurred for both
one’s own face and a friend’s face but not for a relative
stranger’s face. Such enhancement was correlated with
implicit measures of self-worth but not with explicit mea-
sures, consistent with this variety of enhancement being a
relatively automatic rather than deliberative process.
Keywords: self-enhancement; recognition; implicit attitudes;
automatic evaluation; self-esteem
People tend to evaluate their own traits and abilities
more favorably than is objectively warranted.
Although debate exists about the magnitude and exact
nature of such biases across both individuals and cul-
tures (Heine, Lehman, Markus, & Kitayama, 1999;
Kwan, John, Kenny, Bond, & Robins, 2004; Sedikides,
Gaertner, & Toguchi, 2003), a large literature docu-
ments the variety of clever ways in which people can
create flattering images of themselves (Dunning, 1999).
Such self-enhancement effects are not simply mindless
attempts for people to “see” what they want to see but
rather represent a more thoughtful—albeit biased—
processing of self-relevant information. Ambiguous traits
are defined in ways that enable favorable self-evaluations
We predicted that people would indeed show an
enhancement bias in self-recognition based on the
emerging consensus that people’s automatic and intu-
itive associations to the self tend to be objectively posi-
tive (see Koole & DeHart, 2007, for a review). People,
for instance, tend to like the letters in their own name
more than letters that are not in their name (Koole,
Dijksterhuis, & van Knippenberg, 2001; Nuttin, 1985)
and are faster to identify positive words and slower to
identify negative words following a self-relevant sub-
liminal prime than a self-irrelevant prime (Spalding &
Hardin, 1999). As a result of such positive associations,
people tend to unknowingly prefer jobs, spouses, and
even grades that bear some resemblance to the self
(Nelson & Simmons, 2007; Pelham, Mirenberg, &
Jones, 2002). Because liking tends to be used as a cue
for familiarity, with liked others being recognized as
more familiar than disliked others (Monin, 2003), we
reasoned that such automatic liking for the self would
likewise bias recognition of one’s own facial image. The
more positive one’s intuitive and automatic assessment
of the self, the more they should recognize positively
enhanced images of their face as their actual face. Such
a bias should not, however, extend to relative strangers
that do not provoke intuitively positive assessments.
People are therefore likely to be more calibrated when
evaluating others’ images than when evaluating their
own image.
Such results—self-enhancement compared to both
reality and evaluations of others—would share many
hallmarks of existing self-enhancement results. We
believe, however, that such a bias in self-recognition
would not only be widely interesting but also scientifi-
cally important for at least three reasons. First, self-
enhancement in the recognition of one’s own image may
represent a distinct form of self-enhancement, one pro-
duced by relatively implicit or automatic psychological
mechanisms rather than by the existing list of deliberate
information-processing mechanisms. Visual recogni-
tion, including facial recognition, is a textbook example
of an automatic psychological process (Grill-Spector &
Kanwisher, 2005), occurring rapidly and intuitively
with little or no apparent conscious deliberation (e.g.,
Thorpe, Fize, & Marlot, 1996). As a result, enhance-
ment biases in self-recognition should be correlated
with automatic or implicit measures of self-worth that
gauge such automatic associations to the self rather
than with explicit and deliberate measures of self-
worth. This relationship with implicit measures would
contrast sharply with existing enhancement results that
are related to explicit measures of self-worth (e.g.,
Brown, 1986; Gramzow & Willard, 2006; Kobayashi
& Brown, 2003) and suggest there may be two rela-
tively independent sources of self-enhancement. One
source would be reflected in the automatic and implicit
associations people possess about themselves, and
another would be reflected in the more explicit and
deliberate attitudes people hold about themselves that
may distort the recruitment and evaluation of ambigu-
ous evidence. This research would therefore suggest that
both automatic and controlled mechanisms can under-
lie objective self-enhancement. In so doing, it would
also advance the developing and expanding literature
on the consequences and importance of implicit atti-
tudes in general, and the consequences and importance
of implicit self-esteem more specifically.
Second, many existing self-enhancement biases
emerge on relatively ambiguous stimuli that must be
constructed or recalled at the time of judgment—from
one’s relative standing on ambiguous traits (Dunning &
Cohen, 1992), to the quality of one’s relationships
(Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 2004), to the liking of
one’s initials (Koole et al., 2001), to the validity of self-
threatening information (Ditto et al., 2003). There is no
easy method for objectively identifying one’s true lead-
ership ability, the true quality of one’s romantic rela-
tionship, the actual attractiveness of the letter E, or the
true implications of negative performance feedback for
one’s self-evaluation. Other research has demonstrated
that people recall past behavior on objectively verifiable
stimuli in a self-serving fashion (such as prior SAT or
GPA scores; Gramzow & Willard, 2006), but such
results appear to stem from the effortful reconstruction
of memory or deliberately exaggerated self-reports
rather than from a distorted perception of a stimulus
that appears right before people’s eyes. Self-enhanced
recognition of one’s own face, in contrast, would
emerge on a concrete and verifiable feature of the self
that most people see multiple times every day of their
lives. Few aspects about the self are more readily
observable and objectively verifiable than one’s own
facial image. Nevertheless, top-down processes based
on motivations, goals, or expectations have been shown
to influence perception of objective and readily observ-
able stimuli. In one experiment, for instance, partici-
pants judged the slope of a hill to be steeper when they
were encumbered by a heavy backpack than when they
were not so encumbered (Bhalla & Proffitt, 1999). In
another, participants’ motivations to see a particular
object on a screen led them to actually perceive an
ambiguous stimulus in a manner consistent with their
motivated desires (Balcetis & Dunning, 2006). These
findings are consistent with the “new look” approach
to perception that has emphasized the ways in which
top-down processes can influence what people “see”
when they look at the external world. This research
would be the first within this tradition to look at how
top-down processes can influence people’s perceptions
1160 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
of their very own image. It would therefore be the first
to demonstrate that people, quite literally, see them-
selves as more desirable than they actually are.
Third, our experiments involve having people recog-
nize themselves as well as a relative stranger and may
therefore provide additional demonstrations that
people’s evaluations of others can be more calibrated
than evaluations of themselves. For instance, people in
one set of experiments tended to overestimate the likeli-
hood that they would engage in a desirable behavior at
some point in the future, from buying a flower to bene-
fiting a charity to voting in a U.S. presidential election,
but were remarkably well calibrated when predicting
the behavior of a relative stranger (Epley & Dunning,
2000, 2006). The same results also emerged in predic-
tions of relationship longevity, in which those in the
midst of a relationship were more miscalibrated (in the
predictable direction) than were observers (see also
Gagne & Lydon, 2004). Similar miscalibration in this
experiment would perhaps be even more interesting
given that people so clearly have more exposure to, and
experience with, their own image than they do to others.
The miscalibration, however, does not come from the
lack of information about the self compared to others but
rather from the positively distorted nature of the infor-
mation and associations people have about themselves
that they do not have about relatively unknown others.
We tested our main hypotheses—that people would
show enhancement in the recognition of their own facial
image; that such enhancement would be correlated with
implicit, but not explicit, measures of self-worth; and
that people would not show such biases in the recogni-
tion of relative strangers—in three experiments using a
morphing procedure to create more and less attractive
versions of participants’ own facial image. Participants
were then asked to identify their actual face out of a
lineup (Experiments 1 and 2) and that of a relative
stranger and a close friend (Experiment 2), or to recog-
nize their own image from a series of distracters as
quickly as possible (Experiment 3). We predicted that
participants would recognize their own image as being
more desirable than it actually is, that this bias would
also emerge for well-liked friends but not for relative
strangers, and that people would also be faster to iden-
tify positively distorted images of themselves than to
recognize their actual image.
EXPERIMENT 1: SELF-RECOGNITION
Method
Caucasian participants (N=27, 18 female) posed for
a photograph at the end of an unrelated experiment.
The experimenter instructed participants to remove
their glasses and facial piercings and to pull back their
hair if it fell onto their faces. Participants were also
instructed to form a neutral expression for the picture.
These images were then cropped and subjected to a pro-
cedure designed to systematically alter their facial
attractiveness, namely, by morphing their photograph
with a highly attractive or unattractive face.
In this averaging procedure, participants’ facial images
were morphed in 10% increments (up to 50%) with a highly
attractive or unattractive same-gender target face. The
attractive targets (male and female) were composite
images of several dozen faces (obtained at http://www
.uni-regensburg.de/Fakultaeten/phil_Fak_II/Psychologie/
Psy_II/beautycheck/english/durchschnittsgesichter/
durchschnittsgesichter.htm), and the unattractive targets
were individuals suffering from craniofacial syndrome
(obtained at http://www.craniofacial.net/). This morphing
was done by matching up points on the participants’ face
with the identical points on the target’s face (corners of
mouth, shape of eyes, etc.). A 50% morph with the
attractive target would therefore represent a face that was
halfway between their own and the target’s on all of these
critical points. This procedure produced 11 faces, 5 mor-
phed with the attractive target (up to 50%), 5 morphed
with the unattractive target, and the actual photograph
(see Figure 1).1
Participants returned 2 to 4 weeks after taking their
photograph, were seated at a computer, reminded of the
photograph taken several weeks earlier, and told that
they would be presented with a series of images modi-
fied from their original picture. Participants were then
presented with a randomly ordered lineup containing all
11 faces and asked to identify their actual image by
pointing to it on the computer screen. We defined actual
image to participants as the image taken of them in the
prior session. This served as our primary measure of
self-recognition.
Participants then completed a second recognition
measure in which they were shown each picture in iso-
lation on the computer screen (in a random order) and
asked to estimate the likelihood that each image was
their actual image, on a scale ranging from 0% (not at
all likely) to 100% (certain). Finally, participants were
again shown each imagine in isolation (in the same ran-
dom order) and asked to indicate how much they liked
each image on a scale ranging from –5 (bad) to 5
(good).
Participants next completed an implicit measure of
self-worth (Spalding & Hardin, 1999) in which they
were asked to classify words as positive or negative fol-
lowing subliminal (15 ms) presentations of self-relevant
(me, myself) or self-irrelevant (two, manner) primes (see
also Hetts, Sakuma, & Pelham, 1999). The extent to
which self-relevant primes, compared to irrelevant primes,
Epley, Whitchurch / ENHANCED RECOGNITION 1161
speeds the identification of positive words and slows
identification of negative words is taken as an index of
implicit self-worth. Participants then completed the stan-
dard measure of explicit self-esteem—the Rosenberg
(1965) Self-Esteem scale—and a second measure of
implicit self-esteem—the name-letter effect (Nuttin,
1985). Liking one’s own initials more than different ini-
tials, compared to others with different initials, is taken as
a measure of implicit self-esteem (see Baccus, Baldwin, &
Packer, 2004, for a description of the scoring procedure).
We collected two measures of implicit self-esteem, because
of a lack of an accepted standard, to collapse them into a
more reliable composite measure of the underlying latent
variable (Cunningham, Preacher, & Banaji, 2001).
Results
All relevant means and correlations for this experiment
are presented in Table 1.
Face recognition and self-esteem. As predicted, par-
ticipants tended to select one of the faces morphed with
an attractive target as their own out of the lineup more
often than faces morphed with an unattractive target,
t(26) =2.63, p =.02, d=1.01. A frequency distribution
for this measure is presented in Figure 2.
To help identify the source of this bias in self-recognition,
we correlated the image participants recognized as their
own with the implicit and explicit measures of self-
worth. Because the two measures of implicit self-esteem
were significantly correlated with each other, r(25) =
.52, p=.01, we standardized each measure and then
averaged them into a single composite measure. As pre-
dicted, participants’ self-recognition was significantly
correlated with the composite measure of implicit self-
esteem, r(25) =.47, p =.01, but neither positively nor sig-
nificantly correlated with explicit self-esteem, r(25) =
–.26, p=.19. The stronger participants’ automatic
1162 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
Figure 1 Example of the averaging procedure.
Epley, Whitchurch / ENHANCED RECOGNITION 1163
Figure 2 Percentage of participants selecting each face as their own image in the averaging procedure from Experiment 1, Experiment 1
follow-up, and Experiment 2.
0%
5%
10%
15%
20%
25%
30%
35%
40%
50% 40% 30% 20% 10% Actual 10% 20% 30% 40% 50%
Percentage of Participants Selecting Face
Exp. 1
Exp. 1 Follow-Up
Exp. 2
Unattractive Morph Attractive Morph
TABLE 1: Average Enhancement in Self-Recognition and Its Relation to Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem Across Experiments
Experiment 1 Experiment 1 Follow-Up Experiment 2
M (SD)M (SD)M (SD)
Enhancement in recognition
Averaging procedure
Self 6.30% (12.45)* 12.00% (19.71)* 13.33% (16.33)*
Friend 10.09% (19.00)*
Stranger (experimenters)
Female 1.3% (18.72)
Male 3.3% (29.44)
Correlation with self-recognition
Implicit self-esteem
Name-letter preference 0.44* 0.15 0.44*
Automatic evaluation 0.30 0.52* 0.25
Composite average 0.47* 0.40 0.53*
Explicit self-esteem
Rosenberg Self-Esteem scale 0.26 0.11 0.03
NOTE: Enhancement in recognition (the top half of the table) is reported as the mean morph level selected by participants in the experiments,
with positive percentages indicating a mean selection morphed in the direction of the positive target (e.g., 10% indicates that participants, on
average, selected the 10% morph with the attractive target). Negative numbers indicate a mean selection morphed in the direction of a negative
target. Standard deviations are reported in parentheses.
*p< .05.
positive associations to the self (indexed by implicit self-
esteem) was, the more enhanced an image participants
recognized as their own.
Likelihood and liking. Participants’ ratings of the
likelihood that each face, rated in isolation, was their
own showed the same conceptual pattern as the self-
recognition measure already reported, with participants
indicating that the more desirable morphs were more
likely to be their own. As can be seen in the top panel of
Figure 3, the average likelihood judgments across the 11
faces showed a roughly bell-shaped curve, with the peak
shifted toward the attractive morphs. Participants indi-
cated that the 20% attractive morph, for instance, was
most likely to be their own (M=65.93%, SD =20.24),
a figure significantly higher than the likelihood ratings
for their actual image (M=54.07%, SD =26.93), paired
t(26) =2.17, p=.04, d=.85. The average estimated like-
lihood assigned to the five attractive morphs was signifi-
cantly higher (M=36.15, SD =15.23) than that assigned
to the five unattractive morphs (M=19.56, SD =12.48),
paired t(26) =6.06, p<.001, d=2.38.
In addition to making these likelihood estimates, par-
ticipants also saw each image in isolation and indicated
how much they liked each image. As can be seen in the
bottom panel of Figure 3, participants tended to like the
images morphed with the attractive faces more than
those morphed with the unattractive faces. More impor-
tant, the average correlation for each participant
between these likelihood and liking ratings was signifi-
cantly positive (M=.42, SD =.30), t(26) =7.38, p<
.001, d=2.89, as was the overall correlation across par-
ticipants between the average likelihood estimate
assigned to each of the 11 images and the average liking
of those images, r(26) =.58, p<.001. These results are
consistent with our proposed mechanism that auto-
matic liking of one’s image is used as a guide for self-
recognition and are consistent with the significant
1164 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
50% 40% 30% 20% 10% Actual 10% 20% 30% 40% 50%
Estimated Likelihood That Face Is One's Own
Unattractive Morph Attractive Morph
–5
–4
–3
–2
–1
0
1
2
3
50% 40% 30% 20% 10% Actual 10% 20% 30% 40% 50%
Liking for Face
hpr
o
M evit
c
arttAhproM
e
vitcar
tt
anU
Figure 3 Average estimated likelihood and liking for each image presented in isolation (Experiment 1).
NOTE: Likelihood estimates were made on a scale ranging from 0% to 100%. Liking estimates were made on a scale ranging from –5 (bad) to
5 (good).
implicit self-esteem correlation observed on the direct
self-recognition measure.
Discussion
As predicted, participants tended to recognize their
own facial image as being more desirable than it actu-
ally was. What is more, this bias was correlated with
implicit measures of self-worth in two procedures, but it
was not correlated with explicit measures of self-worth.
This suggests that automatic positive associations to the
self guide the enhancement observed in this experiment
rather than more deliberate and controlled assessments
of the self, a finding further corroborated by the signifi-
cant relationship between assessed likelihood that an
image is one’s own and liking for that image.
Although consistent with our hypotheses, readers
might be concerned that this experiment used partici-
pants’ actual photograph rather than their mirror image,
and they may entertain alternative interpretations of our
results based on this feature (Mita, Dermer, & Knight,
1977). We see no theoretical reason to expect this to be
an important boundary condition for self-enhanced
recognition. To examine this generalizability directly, we
simply conducted a small follow-up with 15 partici-
pants from the University of Chicago using their mirror
image in the same morphing procedure described in
Experiment 1. As before (see Table 1), participants
tended to select an enhanced image as their own actual
image, t(14) =2.36, p=.03, d =1.26, with an effect size
very similar to that observed in Experiment 1 on this
procedure. This image selection was again positively
correlated (with similar effect size, but nonsignificantly
given the smaller sample size) with the composite mea-
sure of implicit self-esteem, r(12) =.40, p=.15, but not
with explicit self-esteem, r(12) =.11, p=.71 (see Table
1 for all relevant means and correlations for this exper-
iment). Using participants’ actual photograph rather
than their mirror image does not seem to explain the
results of Experiment 1.
EXPERIMENT 2: FRIENDS AND STRANGERS
One hallmark of enhancement biases is that one’s
close friends and relatives get caught up in the act as
well. People in satisfying relationships, for instance, rate
their romantic partners more favorably than those very
partners rate themselves (Murray et al., 2004) and
sometimes even rate their friends more desirably than
people rate themselves (Kenny & Kashy, 1994). Given
such findings for liked others, we expected that the pat-
terns of enhancement in recognition we observed in
Experiment 1 would not be restricted just to one’s own
image, but would extend to close friends as well. It
should not, however, extend to relative strangers. We
tested this prediction directly in Experiment 2.
Method
Harvard University undergraduates (N=24, 17
female) participated in exchange for course credit. As
part of an unrelated study, participants were asked to
bring along a close friend. This two-part experiment
was conducted by three experimenters, one male and
one female in the first session and a new female experi-
menter for the second session. Participants completed
this experiment individually (each participant com-
pleted the first part with either the male or female
experimenter and then finished the study with the
remaining experimenter). At the end of this unrelated
experiment, participants posed for a photograph
against a white wall. Photographs of both participants
as well as both experimenters were then morphed using
the averaging procedure described for Experiment 1.
Participants were invited to return to the lab alone 3
to 4 weeks later by a new experimenter. In this second
session, participants completed the same recognition
procedure used in Experiment 1 for their own image,
for their friend’s image, and for both of the experi-
menters’ images’ (in random order). Finally, partici-
pants completed the three self-esteem measures from
Experiment 1.
Results
Because of computer malfunctions, the composite
self-esteem scores for 2 participants are based only on
the name-letter measure.
As can be seen in Table 1, participants showed evi-
dence of enhancement when identifying their own
image, t(23) =4.12, p <.001, d =1.72, as well as their
friend’s image, ts(23) =2.69, p =.01, d=1.12, but not
when identifying either of the experimenters’ images,
both ts <1. Averaging across the two experimenters’
images, this difference across targets was reliable, F(2,
20) =3.65, p=.04, η2=.27. Follow-up contrasts
revealed that the observed recognition bias did not dif-
fer between self and friend, F<1; was significantly dif-
ferent between self and the average of the
experimenters’ images, F(1, 21) =5.47, p=.03, η2=
.21; and was marginally significant between the friend’s
image and the average of the experimenters’ images,
F(1, 21) =3.95, p=.06, η2=.16.
As in Experiment 1, self-enhancement was signifi-
cantly correlated with the composite measure of implicit
self-esteem, r(19) =.53, p <.01, but not with explicit
self-esteem, r(19) =–.03, ns. Given that the self was the
target of these measures (rather than the friend or the
Epley, Whitchurch / ENHANCED RECOGNITION 1165
experimenters), it is not surprising that neither implicit
nor explicit measures of self-esteem correlated system-
atically with enhancement of one’s friend or the experi-
menters (ps >.4).
Discussion
Experiment 2 again provides support for our main pre-
diction that people would show evidence of an enhance-
ment bias in self-recognition, a bias that was related to
implicit or automatic measures of self-worth rather than
to explicit or deliberate measures. Experiment 2 also
demonstrates that such enhancement bias extends to
familiar and liked others as well, in this case to one’s close
friends, but does not extend to unfamiliar strangers.
Consistent with other existing research, participants
tended to show evidence of enhancement when evaluating
their close friends to the same extent as when evaluating
themselves (Kenny & Kashy, 1994).
There is one lower level ambiguity that this experi-
ment does not completely address, namely, whether the
enhancement in recognition observed in this experiment
was due to liking or familiarity. People tend to like both
themselves and their friends, but they are also more
familiar with both themselves and their friends (com-
pared to strangers). These two variables are close trav-
eling companions in daily life, as people tend to feel
they are familiar with liked others (Monin, 2003) and
simple exposure to others tends to increase liking
(Moreland & Zajonc, 1982). Although the simple com-
parison between friends and strangers in this experi-
ment cannot distinguish between these two mechanistic
accounts, the relationship between implicit self-esteem
and self-recognition clearly suggests that liking is play-
ing a larger role in the effects observed in these experi-
ments than simple familiarity (see also Note 1).
Existing research demonstrates that the positive associ-
ations indexed by these implicit self-esteem measures are
more than simple measures of exposure or familiarity, and
instead index favorable conditioned associations toward a
target (in this case, toward the self; Baccus et al., 2004;
Dijksterhuis, 2004; Jones, Pelham, Mirenberg, & Hetts,
2002). Distinguishing the impact of familiarity versus lik-
ing in enhanced recognition for others would require mea-
suring automatic evaluative associations to these others or
identifying targets that are equivalent in terms of liking
but vary in familiarity or who vary in liking but are equal
in familiarity. The self-recognition results from these
experiments, and their relation to implicit measures of
self-worth, suggest that implicitly disliked targets, for
instance, should show evidence of derogation in recogni-
tion. Indeed, one recent experiment using a procedure
nearly identical to Experiment 2 showed that people
who tend to dislike themselves—namely, those with body
dysmorphic disorder—tend to recognize themselves as
being less attractive than they actually are (Clerkin &
Teachman, in press). Whether implicitly disliked targets
show evidence for negative distortion is an interesting
avenue for further research and may shed light on likely
sources of variability and boundary conditions for recog-
nition biases in judgment.
EXPERIMENT 3: SPEED OF RECOGNITION
Although we find the influence of familiarity on
recognition biases to be interesting, we felt it more pru-
dent in the scope of the present research to provide
stronger evidence for the phenomenon of enhanced self-
recognition itself. In particular, all of the results pre-
sented thus far have relied on self-reported recognition
as an index of actual recognition. People are willing to
report that an attractively enhanced image is truly their
own, but we sought more evidence in Experiment 3 that
people truly believe what they report. Although such
self-report biases would not explain the pattern of cor-
relations with implicit and explicit self-esteem, they
might serve as an alternative account for the overall
enhancement bias observed in self-recognition. In par-
ticular, we sought evidence for self-enhancement from a
less reactive measure that uses the speed with which
people can identify an image as an indication of their
recognition. In this experiment we asked participants to
identify, as quickly as possible, their own, an attrac-
tively enhanced, or an unattractively enhanced photo of
themselves out of a series of lineups composed of dis-
tracter faces. We reasoned that people would recognize
a face that they truly believed to be their own more
quickly than a face that they did not truly believe was
their own. Given the results of Experiments 1 and 2, we
predicted a linear trend—that participants would iden-
tify the attractively enhanced version the fastest, their
actual image more slowly, and their unattractively
altered version the slowest.
This new methodology also allows us to test the
plausibility of an alternative interpretation of the results
of Experiments 1 and 2 based on the similarity between
the extreme targets used in the morphing procedure and
participants’ actual faces. In particular, it is possible that
participants’ actual faces resemble the highly attractive
target more than they resemble the highly unattractive
target. If one’s actual image is more similar to the attrac-
tive morph than it is to the unattractive morph, the range
of morphs produced with the attractive target may sim-
ply be smaller than the range produced with the unat-
tractive target. Any error in recognition is therefore
likely to lead to biased responses in the direction of the
attractive target. This alternative interpretation would
1166 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
presumably apply to all faces rather than simply one’s
own, and the difference in recognition for self versus the
relative stranger in Study 2 therefore seems inconsistent
with this alternative. So too are the likelihood estimates
reported in Figure 2 inconsistent with this interpretation,
as the range of likelihood estimates is directionally larger
across the attractive faces than it is across the unattrac-
tive faces, showing a clear ability to differentiate
between the images. Nevertheless, the new procedure in
Experiment 3 allows us to test this alternative directly
by measuring, through reaction times, participants’
ability to distinguish among the morphed images. If the
range of attractive morphs is small and is therefore hard
to distinguish among them, participants should show a
smaller difference in recognition speed between the
attractive morph and their actual face (or perhaps no
difference at all) than between their unattractive morph
and their actual face.
Method
University of Chicago undergraduates (N=20, 17
female) participated in exchange for $6. As in the pre-
vious experiments, participants arrived in the labora-
tory for an unrelated experiment and had their
photograph taken before leaving. Participants’ facial
images were then morphed using the averaging proce-
dure from Experiments 1 and 2. After returning to the
lab, participants were seated in front of a blank com-
puter screen, told that they would be shown an array of
12 photographs (1 of which was their own), that they
were to identify their own image as quickly as possible,
and that they would see a total of 12 such arrays. Each
array included 1 image of the actual participant (in a
random location)—either their actual image, the 20%
attractive morph, or the 20% unattractive morph—and
11 same-gender distracter faces (taken from 33 partici-
pants in previous experiments). Each of the three target
images (attractive, actual, unattractive) was therefore
presented four times in a predetermined random order.
Participants were told to press the space bar as soon as
they spotted their own image, and to then point to their
image on the screen that they recognized to be their
own. We used the speed with which participants pressed
the space bar as an indication of their recognition speed.
When finished with all 12 arrays, participants were
thanked, paid, and dismissed.
Results and Discussion
Reaction times more than 3 SD from the mean of a
particular trial were removed on an item-by-item basis
(3.5% of observations in total). Participants saw each of
the target images four times, and we calculated the average
speed with which they recognized each of the target
images.
A repeated measures ANOVA on participants’ aver-
age reaction times to recognize the three target faces
revealed a significant effect consistent with our pre-
dicted linear trend, F(2, 18) =4.43, p=.03, η2=.33.
Participants recognized their attractive morph most
quickly (M=1,892.09 ms, SD =442.53), their actual
image more slowly (M=2,068.44 ms, SD =559.47),
and their unattractive morph the most slowly (M=
2,133.12 ms, SD =460.11). Follow-up contrasts
revealed that participants were marginally faster to rec-
ognize the attractive morph than their actual image,
F(1, 19) =3.35, p=.08, η2=.15, and were significantly
faster to recognize the attractive morph than the unat-
tractive morph, F(1, 19) =6.47, p=.02, η2=.25. The
difference in reaction times for participants to recognize
their actual image and the unattractive morph was non-
significant, F<1. Notice that this result is not only
inconsistent with what an alternative interpretation
based on range restrictions between participants’ actual
image and the morphed targets would predict, it is pre-
cisely the opposite of what such an alternative would
predict. People recognize objects more quickly when
those objects match their mental representations, and
these results clearly suggest that people’s mental repre-
sentations of their own facial image are, on average,
more desirable than their image actually appears in
reality.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
These experiments demonstrate that people tend to
show evidence of self-enhancement in self-recognition,
that such self-enhancement extends to close friends but
not relative strangers, and that it is related to automatic
and implicit measures of self-worth rather than deliber-
ate and explicit measures. These experiments demon-
strate that self-enhancement can emerge during the
relatively automatic and perceptual process of face
recognition, and the pattern of correlations with
implicit measures of self-worth suggest a version of self-
enhancement potentially distinct from existing demon-
strations of positive illusions that are consistently
correlated with explicit measures.
The methodology used to derive these conclusions is
novel and immediately seems to raise at least two inter-
esting issues. First, the methodology in these experiments
bears some relation to measures of implicit self-esteem
and raises questions about whether these results reflect
another measure of implicit self-esteem or a consequence
of implicit self-esteem. We believe it is a consequence,
Epley, Whitchurch / ENHANCED RECOGNITION 1167
rather than another measure, of implicit self-esteem.
Psychological measures of latent constructs are designed
to tap the central and defining features of the construct
itself. Implicit self-esteem is theoretically defined as the
positivity of automatic or intuitive associations to the self
(Koole & DeHart, 2007) and is therefore indexed by
measures of liking for things associated with the self
(such as liking for the initials of one’s name; Dijksterhuis,
2004; Koole et al., 2001) or by the valence of associations
automatically activated when primed with the self-con-
cept (e.g., Hetts et al., 1999; Spalding & Hardin, 1999).
Because liking is used as a cue for recognition (Monin,
2003), we believe that enhancement bias in self-recognition
is one causal step removed from the more basic underly-
ing measure of implicit self-regard and therefore is better
conceptualized as a psychological phenomenon rather
than as a psychological measure.
Second, novel methodologies such as the one used
here raise concerns about procedural artifacts that may
provide alternative interpretations of an experiment’s
results. Although we took great care to avoid obvious
confounds, readers may be concerned that the images
may have somehow differed in luminance, blurriness, or
some other aspect systematically across the experi-
ments. We have no evidence to suggest such procedural
artifacts but notice that any such procedural concerns
would need to account for the breadth of findings
across the entire set of experiments—not simply the
mean-level bias in recognitions observed for self and
friends (Experiments 1 and 2), for one’s actual image
and one’s mirror image (follow-up to Experiment 1), or
in the speed of recognition (Experiment 3), but also for
the consistent pattern of correlations with implicit ver-
sus explicit measures of self-worth (and the symmetry
procedure mentioned in Note 1).
One alternative interpretation that might systemati-
cally be able to explain this entire pattern, and one we
therefore believed required additional empirical investiga-
tion, involves people’s actual attractiveness. People who
are actually more attractive may have more difficulty dis-
tinguishing between images morphed with the attractive
target simply because the objective difference between an
attractive person and the attractive target used in these
experiments would be smaller. Attractive participants
might therefore false alarm to the attractively enhanced
images more often. If attractive people also have higher
implicit self-worth, such an alternative could account for
the pattern we obtained. Although we mentioned that a
similar alternative interpretation does not seem to account
for the pattern of results observed in Experiment 3 across
all participants, our concern here has more to do with the
individual-level correlations between participants.
We examined this alternative by having separate
groups of participants (between 20 and 24 for each
experiment) rate the actual attractiveness of each origi-
nal image used in the experiments. Raters evaluated the
participants from only one of the experiments included
in this article, with images of the original participants
appearing in a random order. No consistent relationship
emerged between average ratings of actual attractive-
ness and the photo participants originally selected as
their own (rs =–.37, –.15, and .29, for Experiment 1,
Experiment 1 follow-up, and Experiment 2, respec-
tively, all ps >.05) or in the speed with which they iden-
tified any of the images in Experiment 3 (ps >.3).
Average ratings of attractiveness were also not consis-
tently related to the composite measure of implicit self-
esteem across the three experiments (ps >.2). Differences
in actual attractiveness do not seem to account for the
results we observed.
Although these results are generally consistent with
the general picture of self-enhancement that emerges
from the social psychological literature, they appear at
odds with empirical results regarding body satisfaction
more generally. In particular, both men and women tend
to show consistent dissatisfaction with their body image
(Powell, Matacin, & Stuart, 2001) and may even hold
negatively distorted perceptions of their own bodies.
Women (especially those with eating disorders) asked to
represent their waist circumference with a rope, for
instance, tend to create a circle that may be as little as
half the size of their actual waist (for a review, see Cash
& Deagle, 1997). The resolution, we suspect, may come
in the automatic and implicit versus relatively con-
trolled and deliberate nature of the measures used in
these differing experiments. The self-enhancing results we
report here emerge on recognition measures that are pro-
totypic examples of automatic psychological processes,
whereas body dissatisfaction results emerge in more
deliberate and explicit measures. Similar dissociations
between implicit and explicit self-views have emerged in
cross-cultural studies of self-enhancement between
Asian and Western Europeans. Although explicit mea-
sures show consistent evidence of self-criticism and dep-
recation among East Asians compared to Western
Europeans (Heine et al., 1999), their implicit self-assess-
ments are predictably favorable (Hetts et al., 1999;
Kitayama & Karasawa, 1997; Yamaguchi et al., 2007).
Whether a similar dissociation would emerge in percep-
tions of body image more generally is an interesting
avenue for further research.
More generally, the relation between self-enhance-
ment and implicit self-worth in these experiments sug-
gests a potentially distinct version of self-enhancement
from existing demonstrations of positive illusions that
are consistently correlated with explicit measures.
Implicit attitude measures tend to reflect consistent and
repeated associations in the environment, producing
1168 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
automatic associations through classical conditioning
that can then extend to related but unconditioned stim-
uli. People structure their lives in ways that tend to
increase the likelihood of positive associations to the
self (or close friends and loved ones), and stimuli asso-
ciated with the self are therefore automatically evalu-
ated positively (Beggan, 1992; Pelham, Carvallo, &
Jones, 2005). The experiments here suggest that such
associations can create positive distortions of the self as
well, a top-down influence that not only influences how
people think about themselves but also what people see
when they look directly at themselves. It is perhaps of
little wonder, then, that people so rarely seem to like the
photographs taken of themselves. The image captured
by the camera lens just does not match up to the image
captured in the mind’s eye.
NOTE
1. Participants also completed a second procedure originally
intended to allow a conceptual replication of the correlation with
implicit versus explicit self-esteem. In particular, we created a series of
morphed images increasing in symmetry. Existing research demon-
strates that people tend to like symmetrical images of themselves
(Little & Jones, 2003), and symmetry is generally perceived as being
attractive (Rhodes, 2006). In this symmetry procedure, participants’
facial images were split in half vertically, the right side flipped to cre-
ate a perfectly symmetrical face, and participants’ actual image was
morphed with this symmetrical image in 10% increments up to 90%,
resulting in 11 total images. Because this procedure only increases
symmetry rather than decreases it, mean biases in the recognized
image cannot be taken as clear evidence of self-enhancement, as any
random error in the selection of faces can only lead to a selection of
an enhanced face. We included this procedure only to serve as a pos-
sible conceptual replication for the relation between recognition and
implicit versus explicit self-esteem.
Recent evidence, however, suggests that this chimeric procedure
can slightly decrease the apparent attractiveness of the face due to
anomalies introduced by asymmetrical facial features (for a review,
see Rhodes, 2006). We tried to eliminate these concerns by removing
three people who had obvious facial marks that would appear
bizarre with this procedure, but we cannot rule out artifacts alto-
gether. This procedure, unbeknown to us at the time, therefore con-
tains some ambiguities that may make it less than ideal for our
purposes.
That said, results from this symmetry procedure were at least con-
sistent with our predictions. As with the averaging procedure, partic-
ipants’ tendency to select the more symmetrical self-images was
significantly correlated with the composite measure of implicit self-
esteem, r(22) =.40, p =.05, but not with explicit self-esteem, r=.18,
p=.40. Participants tended to select a face that was significantly more
enhanced than their own (M=26.25% morph with perfectly sym-
metrical image), t(23) =4.68, p<.001, d=1.95.
REFERENCES
Baccus, J. R., Baldwin, M. W., & Packer, D. J. (2004). Increasing
implicit self-esteem through classical conditioning. Psychological
Science, 15, 498-502.
Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2006). See what you want to see:
Motivational influences on visual perception. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 612-625.
Beggan, J. K. (1992). On the social nature of nonsocial perception:
The mere ownership effect. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 62, 229-237.
Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geo-
graphical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1076-1096.
Brown, J. D. (1986). Evaluations of self and others: Self-enhancement
biases in social judgments. Social Cognition, 4, 353-376.
Cash, T. F., & Deagle, E. A., III (1997). The nature and extent of body
image disturbances in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 22, 107-125.
Chambers, J. R., & Windschitl, P. D. (2004). Biases in social compar-
ative judgments: The role of nonmotivated factors in above-aver-
age and comparative optimism effects. Psychological Bulletin,
130, 813-838.
Clerkin, E. M., & Teachman, B. A. (in press). Perceptual and cogni-
tive biases in individuals with body dysmorphic disorder symp-
toms. Cognition and Emotion.
Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit
attitude measures: Consistency, stability, and convergent validity.
Psychological Science, 12, 163-170.
Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). I like myself but I don’t know why:
Enhancing implicit self-esteem by subliminal evaluative condition-
ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 345-355.
Ditto, P. H., & Lopez, D. F. (1992). Motivated skepticism: The use of dif-
ferential decision criteria for preferred and nonpreferred conclusions.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,568-584.
Ditto, P. H., Munro, G. D., Apanovitch, A. M., Scepansky, J. A., &
Lockhart, L. K. (2003). Spontaneous skepticism: The interplay of
motivation and expectation in responses to favorable and unfa-
vorable medical diagnoses. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 29, 1120-1132.
Ditto, P. H., Scepansky, J. A., Munro, G. D., Apanovich, A. M., &
Lockhart, L. K. (1998). Motivated sensitivity to preference-incon-
sistent information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
75, 53-69.
Dunning, D. (1999). A newer look: Motivated social cognition and
the schematic representation of social concepts. Psychological
Inquiry, 10, 1-11.
Dunning, D., & Cohen, G. L. (1992). Egocentric definitions of traits
and abilities in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 63, 341-355.
Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling “holier than thou”: Are
self-serving assessments produced by errors in self or social
prediction? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,
861-875.
Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2006). The mixed blessings of self-knowledge
in behavioral prediction: Enhanced discrimination but exacerbated
bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 641-655.
Gagne, F. M., & Lydon, J. E. (2004). Bias and accuracy in close rela-
tionships: An integrative review. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 8, 322-338.
Gramzow, R. H., & Willard, G. (2006). Exaggerating current and
past performance: Motivated self-enhancement versus reconstruc-
tive memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32,
1114-1125.
Grill-Spector, K., & Kanwisher, N. (2005). Visual recognition: As
soon as you know it is there, you know what it is. Psychological
Science, 16, 152-160.
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999).
Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological
Review, 106, 766-794.
Hetts, J. J., Sakuma, M., & Pelham, B.W. (1999). Two roads to posi-
tive regard: Implicit and explicit self-evaluation and culture.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 512-559.
Jones, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Hetts, J. J. (2002).
Name letter preferences are not merely mere exposure: Implicit ego-
tism as self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
38, 170-177.
Kenny, D. A., & Kashy, D. A. (1994). Enhanced coorientation in the
perception of friends: A social relations analysis. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1024-1033.
Epley, Whitchurch / ENHANCED RECOGNITION 1169
Kitayama, S., & Karasawa, M. (1997). Implicit self-esteem in Japan:
Name letters and birthday numbers. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 23, 736-742.
Kobayashi, C., & Brown, J. D. (2003). Self-esteem and self-enhancement
in Japan and America. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34,
567-580.
Koole, S. L., & DeHart, T. (2007). Self-affection without self-reflec-
tion: Origins, models, and consequences of implicit self-esteem. In
C. Sedikides & S. Spencer (Eds.), The self in social psychology
(pp. 36-86). New York: Psychology Press.
Koole, S. L., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2001). What’s
in a name: Implicit self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 80, 614-627.
Kunda, Z., & Sinclair, L. (1999). Motivated reasoning with stereo-
types: Activation, application, and inhibition. Psychological
Inquiry, 10, 12-22.
Kwan, V. S. Y, John, O. P., Kenny, D. A., Bond, M. H., & Robins, R. W.
(2004). Reconceptualizing individual differences in self-enhance-
ment bias: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Review, 111,
94-110.
Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2003). Evidence against perceptual bias views
for symmetry preferences in human faces. Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London Series B–Biological Sciences, 270, 1759-1763.
Mita, T. H., Dermer, M., & Knight, J. (1977). Reversed facial images
and the mere-exposure hypothesis. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 35, 597-601.
Monin, B. (2003). The warm glow heuristic: When liking leads to
familiarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85,
1035-1048.
Moreland, R. L., & Zajonc, R. B. (1982). Exposure effects in person
perception: Familiarity, similarity, and attraction. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 395-415.
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (2004). The benefits of
positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfaction
in close relationships. In H. T. Reis & C. E. Rusbult (Eds.), Close
relationships: Key readings (pp. 317-338). Philadelphia: Taylor &
Francis.
Nelson, L. D., & Simmons, J. P. (2007). Moniker maladies: When
names sabotage success. Psychological Science, 18, 1106-1112.
Nuttin, J. M. (1985). Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: The
name letter effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15,
353-361.
Pelham, B. W., Carvallo, M., & Jones, J. T. (2005). Implicit egotism.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 106-110.
Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Jones, J. K. (2002). Why Susie
sells seashells by the seashore: Implicit egotism and major life deci-
sions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 469-487.
Powell, J. L., Matacin, M. L., & Stuart, A. E. (2001). Body esteem:
An exception to self-enhancing illusions? Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 31, 1951-1978.
Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty.
Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 199-226.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self-
enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 60-79.
Spalding, L. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1999). Unconscious unease and self-
handicapping: Behavioral consequences of individual differences in
implicit and explicit self-esteem. Psychological Science, 10, 535-539.
Thorpe, S., Fize, D., & Marlot, C. (1996). Speed of processing in the
human visual system. Nature, 381, 520-522.
Yamaguchi, S., Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Murakami, F., Chen,
D., Shiomura, K, et al. (2007). Apparent universality of positive
implicit self-esteem. Psychological Science, 18, 498-500.
Received July 7, 2007
Revision accepted February 1, 2008
1170 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
... Based on these understandings, researchers offered several methods to measure self-recognition for the study of the self-concept (e.g. Epley & Whitchurch, 2008;Maister et al., 2021;Moon et al., 2020;Walker & Keller, 2019 ...
... They were more likely to select the face with higher levels of agency and communion than persons with high explicit self-esteem. However, Epley and Whitchurch (2008) show using self-report measures suggest that self-esteem does not moderate self-enhancement. ...
... We found that self-esteem does not moderate self-recognition distortions, the finding consistent with earlier work (Stephan et al., 2015). As we mention in the introduction, the literature is inconclusive regarding the role of self-esteem (Epley & Whitchurch, 2008;Moon et al., 2020). Future research may use state measures of selfesteem (e.g. ...
Article
Typically, individuals are motivated to see themselves in a flattering light regarding their physical and personality characteristics. We suggest that visual self-perception is anchored in the true self which is largely the self-enhancing self, and may also reflect the privileged access to one's traits and internal states. Here, we examine the impact and limits of self-enhancement motivation and egocentric bias on individuals’ recognition of variations of their portraits in terms of the Big Five personality dimensions. We predict that individuals recognise themselves better in positively altered portraits, namely, portraits looking more agreeable, conscientious and open. We also predict that self-enhancement is anchored in reality: Individuals recognise themselves less in strongly altered than in slightly or non-altered portraits. In Study 1, we measure the directions of self-recognition distortions by investigating in which random variations of their portraits individuals recognise themselves. In Study 2, relying on a mouse tracking paradigm, we additionally measure the degree of distortion, and the conflict individuals experience during the self-recognition task. Results of both studies mainly demonstrate self-recognition distortions towards agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness and neuroticism. Moreover, results from Study 2 reveal that self-recognition is anchored in reality. Findings of two studies applying different methods convergently show systematic biases in self-recognition regarding the Big Five dimensions, both enriching understanding of the self-enhancement processes and demonstrating that the methods applied might be useful for future research focusing on self-recognition.
... Several studies have examined the effect of attitudes towards a face on recognition-that is, whether the face is recognised as being more or less attractive depending on the attitude towards the face. Epley and Whitchurch (2008) reported that, when participants were asked to select the image of their own face from presented images of their own face, morphs of an unattractive face, and morphs of an attractive face, they tended to select the morphed face that was more attractive than they actually were. Moreover, this biased recognition was positively correlated with implicit self-esteem. ...
... Our results showed that distortion in face recognition was affected by judged attractiveness level: when a face was initially judged as moderately and highly attractive, more attractive images were recognised as the target face than when it was initially judged to be less attractive. The results extend previous studies that have revealed a recognition bias, wherein people recognise the faces of themselves, their friends, and romantic partners as more attractive (Epley & Whitchurch, 2008;Penton-Voak et al., 2007). This shows that such a bias is not merely due to one's own face recognition or the relationship between the participant and target face, but also occurs when one judges strangers to be highly attractive. ...
... Previous studies on biased face recognition examined this from the perspective of feelings towards the self and relationship with others (Clerkin & Teachman, 2008;Epley & Whitchurch, 2008;Penton-Voak et al., 2007). The present study revealed that faces with positive impressions, even towards strangers, were recognised as being more attractive than faces with negative impressions. ...
Article
We investigated the influence of perceived impression on the recognition of strangers’ faces. Two experiments were conducted to examine whether face recognition distortion varies depending on perceived attractiveness and distinctiveness. Participants judged the attractiveness or distinctiveness of a target face presented in a video. Subsequently, they were shown a set of five images in which attractiveness or distinctiveness were manipulated and asked to select the original target face from the five images. Results showed that when participants initially judged a target face to be highly attractive, the more attractive image in the set was recognised as the target face than when participants judged the target face to be low in attractiveness. Furthermore, when participants judged a target face to be highly distinct, the more distinctive image in the set was recognised as the target face than when participants judged a target face to be low in distinctiveness. These findings were discussed in terms of the labelling effect and the verbal overshadowing effect.
... This is demonstrated by the fact that one responds faster to one's own face than to the faces of others, a phenomenon known as the 'self-face advantage' (see [2] for a review). Interestingly, people generally hold a self-enhancement bias whereby they perceive their own face as more attractive than it might be [3][4][5]. The underlying mechanism of this phenomenon is not fully understood, but one possibility could be the identification with attractive others through blurring of self-other boundaries [6]. ...
... Traditionally, selfface recognition with another face has been long explored in studies where participants' faces were morphed with attractive others. For instance, a seminal study by Epley and Whitchurch [5] found that participants were more likely to recognise a more attractive version of their face as their own, suggesting that this could be a form of self-enhancement, driven by implicit and automatic psychological mechanisms. This bias also correlated with measures of self-worth, hinting at a top-down effect where positive self-associations lead to positive self-distortions. ...
... In fact, the perceptual threshold corresponding to a 50% positive response rate was 80.76%, that is participants had more difficulties to detect facial changes and required images that contained more "other" to recognize themselves. These results are in keeping with Epley and Whitchurch [5]' study which showed that when photographs of participant's faces are morphed with attractive faces, they were more likely to identify the attractive morphs as the own face compared to their actual face. This is also consistent with previous studies using similar self-other facial morphing tasks which reported an egocentric bias, according to which facial morphs are judged to look more like "self" when morphed with a more attractive relative to a less attractive face [5,6]. ...
Article
Full-text available
People naturally exhibit a self-serving bias which can be observed in their tendency to judge their own physical attractiveness more favourably than that of others. Despite this positive self-perception, minimally invasive cosmetic injectable procedures for facial rejuvenation and enhancement are becoming increasingly common. It remains unclear, however, whether recognizing an altered version of one’s own face, enhanced cosmetically, correlates with a positive view of cosmetic surgery and excessive preoccupations about physical characteristics perceived as defects (body dysmorphic concerns). In this study, 30 healthy female participants, aged 18–24 years (Mage = 21.1 years, SD = 1.6), engaged in a face recognition task during which their faces were digitally morphed with that of gender-matched unfamiliar women who had undergone cosmetic enhancements, specifically lip and cheek fillers. The duration of exposure to these modified faces varied with short (500 msec) and long (2000 msec) viewing periods. Participants were asked to identify whether the digital morphs represented themselves or the other woman. Self-reports regarding acceptance of cosmetic surgery and dysmorphic concerns were collected. Participants PSE indicated a tendency towards self-bias under short presentation times, shifting towards the other as presentation times lengthened. Interestingly, this effect was associated with greater acceptance of cosmetic surgery and higher body dysmorphic concerns. This study underscores the importance of understanding how perceptions of others’ physical appearances can influence self-recognition and attitudes towards cosmetic surgery, which may have both positive and potentially harmful implications.
... All images were then collated into an online questionnaire via the online survey platform Qualtrics. To minimise bias, an independent community sample of 60 women (30 Caucasian; 30 Eastern/Southeastern Asian) ethnicitymatched to models rated images for facial attractiveness, facial adiposity, likeability, and emotional expression as these factors are known/suggested to bias enfacement illusion effects [57][58][59][60][61]. For example, facial adiposity was assessed using a scale from 1 (very underweight) to 7 (very overweight). ...
... Stimulation video Over a videoconferencing platform (Zoom), the 10 selected models recorded 150 s videos alternating between exaggerated smiles and a neutral expression every 10 s (see Fig. 2). This is consistent with prior protocols [50,65] and research demonstrating that enfacement is stronger for models with positivelyvalenced faces [57,59]. Videos were slightly longer than prior protocols which were typically 120 s [49] to increase the stimulation phase and likelihood of enfacement. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Remote research methods and interventions for mental health disorders have become increasingly important, particularly for conditions like eating disorders (EDs). Embodiment illusions, which induce feelings of ownership over another person?s body or body parts, offer valuable insights into the mechanisms underlying self-perception issues in EDs and potential interventions. However, existing research using these illusions has been limited to face-to-face settings. We illustrate a novel online protocol to induce the enfacement illusion (embodiment illusion principles applied to one’s face) in an ED-based sample. Methods Participants complete a 2-hr virtual session with a researcher. First, baseline trait/state ED psychopathology measures and a self-face recognition task occur. Second, participants experience two testing blocks of the enfacement illusion involving synchronously and asynchronously mimicking a pre-recorded actor’s facial expressions. After each block, subjective and objective enfacement illusion measures occur alongside state ED psychopathology reassessment. Discussion Successfully inducing enfacement illusions online could provide an affordable, accessible virtual approach to further elucidate the mechanistic role of self-perception disturbances across psychopathologies such as EDs. Moreover, this protocol may represent an innovative, remotely-delivered intervention strategy, as ‘enfacement’ over another face could update negative self-representations in a cost-effective, scalable manner.
... Tone evenness of female participants by age group and ethnicity. overestimate their attractiveness may reflect self-deception [62], that is, the process of denying opposing evidence and logical arguments [63]. Self-deception has been described as a self-serving bias. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective Research documents effects of skin features on assessments of age, health and attractiveness of female faces. Ethnic variation also has been reported for the impact of age‐related changes in skin features on face assessments. Here, we investigate women's self‐ratings across age cohorts and ethnic groups and discrepancies with (non‐expert) assessor ratings of facial appearance together with age‐dependent changes in skin features. Methods Faces of women 20–65 years from five ethnic groups (each n = 36) were imaged. Participants provided self‐ratings of age, health and attractiveness, and were judged on these attributes by members of the same ethnic group (each n = 120). Digital image analysis was used to quantify skin gloss, tone evenness, wrinkling and sagging. Age‐dependent changes in ratings and skin features within and between ethnic groups were assessed by comparing information from 10‐year cohorts. We also tested whether menopausal status could be predicted by self‐ratings, assessor ratings and image‐based skin features. Results Women of all ethnic groups judged themselves younger and higher in attractiveness and health compared to third‐party assessors, with the largest discrepancies for age in French women and for attractiveness and health in South African women. In Indian and South African women, specular gloss and skin tone evenness were lower compared to other participants, and sagging was higher in Indian, Japanese and South African women compared to Chinese and French women. Women's menopausal status could be predicted from assessor ratings and image‐based skin features but not from self‐ratings. Conclusion There are differences between women's self‐ratings and assessor ratings of facial appearance. These discrepancies vary with female age and ethnicity. Age and ethnicity effects also are evident in age‐dependent changes in skin features within and across ethnic groups, which together with assessor (but not self‐) ratings of facial appearance predict menopausal status.
Article
In recent years, digital face beautification has emerged as a trend in social networks, particularly among female user groups. This has exacerbated the gap in self-awareness between the ideal and actual selves. Digital face beautification has also increased positive emotions and happiness. In this study, we explored the impact of digital face beautification on females through three studies. Study 1 used text analysis to reveal the attention goals, emotional trends, and attitude evaluations of digital face beautification actors in social network scenarios (N = 519). Study 2 innovatively used the frequency and degree of difference as two key indicators of beautification behavior to explore the relationship between beautification behavior and female emotional experiences (N = 140). Studies 3A (N = 55) and 3B (N = 60) used behavioral experimental methods and mediation analysis to further verify the positive impact of digital face beautification behavior on female emotions and happiness. The results indicate that although digital face beautification behavior widens the gap between the ideal self and the actual self, moderate digital face beautification increases positive emotions, improves feelings of engagement and control, and boosts females’ happiness.
Article
Ten studies ( N = 4192) demonstrated that individuals depicted as prosocial were judged to be more physically beautiful. This evaluation of prosocial individuals as more beautiful is influenced by a motivation to be associated with prosocial others. This phenomenon was observed in real‐world settings (Study 1) and applied to both men and women, both as targets and observers (Studies 2a–2b). The effect persisted in scenarios where participants imagined the target without any visual aid (Study 2c) and extended beyond metaphorical interpretations of beauty (Study 3). The effect weakened when prosocial behaviour was an isolated incident, not indicative of the target's prosocial personality (Study 4). The influence of prosociality on beauty evaluations surpassed that of other positive traits such as intelligence or humour (Study 5) and remained significant despite physical imperfections in the target's appearance (Study 6). The effect diminished in situations where forming a relationship was not feasible, thus supporting the motivated cognition rationale (Studies 7–8). These findings highlight the substantial role of prosocial behaviour in influencing evaluations of physical beauty, a crucial element in social interactions and relationship formation, often outweighing other attributes typically linked to physical appearance evaluation.
Article
Mate choice requires navigating an exploration-exploitation trade-off. Successful mate choice requires choosing partners who have preferred qualities; but time spent determining one partner’s qualities could have been spent exploring for potentially superior alternatives. Here I argue that this dilemma can be modeled in a reinforcement learning framework as a multi-armed bandit problem. Moreover, using agent-based models and a sample of k = 522 real-world romantic dyads, I show that a reciprocity-weighted Thompson sampling algorithm performs well both in guiding mate search in noisy search environments and in reproducing the mate choices of real-world participants. These results provide a formal model of the understudied psychology of human mate search. They additionally offer implications for our understanding of person perception and mate choice.
Article
This work addresses the link between motivation and self-perception by systematically studying visual self-representations. We propose that the way individuals perceive themselves may be associated with dispositional and situationally induced approach and avoidance motivation. First, we investigate how dispositional differences in approach/avoidance motivation and self-esteem relate to self-perception. Second, we investigate how state differences in approach/avoidance motivation relate to self-perception. Based on motivation literature, we predicted that self-perception would reflect psychological equanimity at the dispositional level (seeing the self in a favorable light), and motivational flexibility at the situational level (situational avoidance motivation results in a more realistic view of one's qualities). We use up-to-date face modeling methods to measure individuals' self-perceptions in a systematic, nuanced, and implicit way: Participants are repeatedly asked to indicate which of two portrait versions better represents themselves. Then we relate distortions in self-perceptions to agency and communion dimensions. We demonstrate that (1) participants low in self-esteem show more communion enhancement than participants high in self-esteem; (2) participants in an avoidance state show less agency enhancement (i.e., more realistic self-perceptions) than participants in an approach state. This research is first to demonstrate regularities in visual self-perception that are linked to approach and avoidance motivation.
Article
Full-text available
It is assumed that people seek positive self-regard; that is, they are motivated to possess, enhance, and maintain positive self-views. The cross-cultural generalizability of such motivations was addressed by examining Japanese culture. Anthropological, sociological, and psychological analyses revealed that many elements of Japanese culture are incongruent with such motivations. Moreover, the empirical literature provides scant evidence for a need for positive self-regard among Japanese and indicates that a self-critical focus is more characteristic of Japanese. It is argued that the need for self-regard must be culturally variant because the constructions of self and regard themselves differ across cultures. The need for positive self-regard, as it is currently conceptualized, is not a universal, but rather is rooted in significant aspects of North American culture. Conventional interpretations of positive self-regard are too narrow to encompass the Japanese experience.
Article
Full-text available
The culture movement challenged the universality of the self-enhancement motive by proposing that the motive is pervasive in individualistic cultures (the West) but absent in collectivistic cultures (the East). The present research posited that Westerners and Easterners use different tactics to achieve the same goal: positive self-regard. Study 1 tested participants from differing cultural backgrounds (the United States vs. Japan), and Study 2 tested participants of differing self-construals (independent vs. interdependent). Americans and independents self-enhanced on individualistic attributes, whereas Japanese and interdependents self-enhanced on collectivistic attributes. Independents regarded individualistic attributes, whereas interdependents regarded collectivistic attributes, as personally important. Attribute importance mediated self-enhancement. Regardless of cultural background or self-construal, people self-enhance on personally important dimensions. Self-enhancement is a universal human motive.
Article
Full-text available
Because most people possess positive associations about themselves, most people prefer things that are connected to the self (e.g., the letters in one's name). The authors refer to such preferences as implicit egotism. Ten studies assessed the role of implicit egotism in 2 major life decisions: where people choose to live and what people choose to do for a living. Studies 1-5 showed that people are disproportionately likely to live in places whose names resemble their own first or last names (e.g., people named Louis are disproportionately likely to live in St. Louis). Study 6 extended this finding to birthday number preferences. People were disproportionately likely to live in cities whose names began with their birthday numbers (e.g., Two Harbors, MN). Studies 7-10 suggested that people disproportionately choose careers whose labels resemble their names (e.g., people named Dennis or Denise are overrepresented among dentists). Implicit egotism appears to influence major life decisions. This idea stands in sharp contrast to many models of rational choice and attests to the importance of understanding implicit beliefs.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we review evidence showing that both the activation and the application of stereotypes may be influenced by motivation. When an applicable stereotype supports their desired impression of an individual, motivation can lead people to activate this stereotype, if they have not already activated it spontaneously. Motivation can also lead people to apply this stereotype to individuals to whom they might not have applied it otherwise. On the other hand, when an applicable stereotype casts doubt over their desired impression of an individual, motivation can lead people to inhibit the activation of this stereotype. Even if people are unable to inhibit its activation, motivation may still lead them to inhibit its application to this individual. People pick and choose among the many stereotypes applicable to an individual, activating those that support their desired impression of this individual and inhibiting those that interfere with it.
Chapter
Full-text available
Over the last 10 years or so, the study of implicit self-esteem has developed into a burgeoning area of psychological research. Our goal in the present chapter is to summarize some of the conclusions that have emerged from this work. In the following paragraphs, we begin by outlining the major methods and models that have guided implicit self-esteem research. We then consider recent findings with regard to three important aspects of implicit self-esteem. First, what are the social and developmental origins of implicit self-esteem? Second, what is the relation between implicit and explicit self-esteem? Third, what are the consequences of implicit self-esteem for psychological functioning? After covering these various issues, we summarize the main findings on implicit self-esteem and suggest possible avenues for future implicit self-esteem research.
Article
Three investigations are reported that examined the relation between self-appraisals and appraisals of others. In Experiment 1, subjects rated a series of valenced trait adjectives according to how well the traits described the self and others. Individuals displayed a pronounced “self-other bias,” such that positive attributes were rated as more descriptive of self than of others, whereas negative attributes were rated as less descriptive of self than of others. Furthermore, in contrast to C. R. Rogers's (1951) assertion that high self-esteem is associated with a comparable regard for others, the tendency for individuals to evaluate the self in more favorable terms than they evaluated people in general was particularly pronounced among those with high self-esteem. These findings were replicated and extended in Experiment 2, where it also was found that self-evaluations were more favorable than were evaluations of a friend and that individuals with high self-esteem were most likely to appraise their friend...
Article
Although the self-enhancement motive is commonly held to be a universal human motivation, some theorists have recently argued that it does not operate in Japan. In an attempt to shed light on this issue, the authors conducted an investigation that explored the relation between self-esteem and self-enhancement in Japan and America. In both cultures and to the same degree, high self-esteem people were more apt to display evidence of self-enhancement than were low self-esteem people. The correspondence between the two cultures suggests that the self-enhancement motive does operate in Japan.
Article
In this article, we review evidence showing that both the activation and the application of stereotypes may be influenced by motivation. When an applicable stereotype supports their desired impression of an individual, motivation can lead people to activate this stereotype, if they have not already activated it spontaneously. Motivation can also lead people to apply this stereotype to individuals to whom they might not have applied it otherwise. On the other hand, when an applicable stereotype casts doubt over their desired impression of an individual! motivation can lead people to inhibit the activation of this stereotype. Even if people are unable to inhibit its activation, motivation may still lead them to inhibit its application to this individual. People pick and choose among the many stereotypes applicable to an individual, activating those that support their desired impression of this individual and inhibiting those that interfere with it.
Article
In this article, I argue that the basic building blocks of social cognition, the schemata people possess of social traits and concepts, are shaped by motivations to retain flattering images of the self As such, motivational influences on social cognition are more subtle and pervasive than usually acknowledged in the social cognitive literature. I review research showing that people possess self-flattering schemata of social concepts. I describe experimental work demonstrating that it is, indeed, the motivation to maintain self-worth prompting the self-serving nature of these schemata, detailing how these studies withstand the usual cognitive reinterpretations offered for motivational findings. Finally, I suggest that the field of social cognition reconsider the issues and insights of the New Look tradition, which concerned itself first and foremost with how people reconciled incoming social information with the perceiver's goals, wishes, fears, and desires.