ArticlePDF Available

Craving love? Enduring grief activates brain's reward center

Authors:

Abstract

Complicated Grief (CG) occurs when an individual experiences prolonged, unabated grief. The neural mechanisms distinguishing CG from Noncomplicated Grief (NCG) are unclear, but hypothesized mechanisms include both pain-related activity (related to the social pain of loss) and reward-related activity (related to attachment behavior). Bereaved women (11 CG, 12 NCG) participated in an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging scan, during grief elicitation with idiographic stimuli. Analyses revealed that whereas both CG and NCG participants showed pain-related neural activity in response to reminders of the deceased, only those with CG showed reward-related activity in the nucleus accumbens (NA). This NA cluster was positively correlated with self-reported yearning, but not with time since death, participant age, or positive/negative affect. This study supports the hypothesis that attachment activates reward pathways. For those with CG, reminders of the deceased still activate neural reward activity, which may interfere with adapting to the loss in the present.
Craving love? Enduring grief activates brain's reward center
Mary-Frances O'Connor
a,
, David K. Wellisch
b
, Annette L. Stanton
a,b,c
, Naomi I. Eisenberger
c
,
Michael R. Irwin
a
, Matthew D. Lieberman
c
a
Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University of California at Los Angeles, USA
b
Department of Psychiatry, Semel Institute for Neuroscience, University of California at Los Angeles, USA
c
Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles, USA
abstractarticle info
Article history:
Received 10 January 2008
Revised 8 April 2008
Accepted 29 April 2008
Available online 10 May 2008
Complicated Grief (CG) occurs when an individual experiences prolonged, unabated grief. The neural
mechanisms distinguishing CG from Noncomplicated Grief (NCG) are unclear, but hypothesized mechanisms
include both pain-related activity (related to the social pain of loss) and reward-related activity (related to
attachment behavior). Bereaved women (11 CG, 12 NCG) participated in an event-related functional magnetic
resonance imaging scan, during grief elicitation with idiographic stimuli. Analyses revealed that whereas both
CG and NCG participants showed pain-related neural activity in response to reminders of the deceased, only
those with CG showed reward-related activity in the nucleus accumbens (NA). This NA cluster was positively
correlatedwith self-reported yearning,but not with time since death, participantage, or positive/negativeaffect.
This study supports the hypothesis that attachment activates reward pathways. For those with CG, reminders of
the deceased still activate neural reward activity, which may interfere with adapting to the loss in the present.
© 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
There is no pain so great as the memory of joy in present grief.
~Aeschylus, founder of Greek tragedy
Introduction
Grief is one of life's most painful experiences. When
suffering the loss of a loved one, one can feel as if the attendant
sadness and longing will last indenitely. Although successful
adaptation to the loss is the most frequent response (Bonnano
et al., 2002), grief does not abate in a substantial minority;
rather, it develops into Complicated Grief (CG) (Ott et al., 2007).
CG, previously known as chronic, pathological or traumatic
grief, includes debilitating recurrent pangs of painful emo-
tions, with intense yearning, longing and searching for the
deceased, and preoccupation with thoughts of the loved one.
This syndrome has now been dened by an empirically-
derived set of criteria (Boelen and van den Bout, 2005) and is
being considered for inclusion in the DSM-V.
The neurocognitive mechanisms involved in CG are currently
unknown. Some have hypothesized that attachment may
activate reward pathways and that this neural response may
have addiction-like properties (Insel, 2003; Panksepp et al.,
2002). We hypothesize that a major neurocognitive difference
between CG and Noncomplicated Grief (NCG) is that reminders
ofthedeceasedmaystillactivateneuralrewardsforthosewith
CG. This reward activation may interfere with adapting to the
loss in the present. Supporting this idea are subjective reports
from CG patients indicating pleasurable reveries about the lost
love (during which the reality of the loss is ignored) in addition to
painful yearning (Shear et al., 2005). These self-report ndings
suggest the involvement of both reward and pain networks.
The nucleus accumbens (NA) is the region most commonly
associated with reward (Knutson et al., 2001). NA has also been
shown to play a role in social attachment, such as sibling and
maternal afliation, via neurotransmitters and peptides (e.g.,
dopamine and oxytocin) (You ng e t al., 20 01). The pain network,
by contrast, including the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC),
insula, and periaqueductal gray (PAG) has been previously
implicated in both physical (Rainville, 2002) and social pain
(Eisenberger et al., 2003; Rainville, 2002). The present analysis
specically investigates whether the CG group had greater
activity occur in the brain's reward or pain networks than the
NCG group.
Methods and materials
Participants
Women (11 CG, 12 NCG) who had experienced the death of
a mother or sister to breast cancer in the past 5 years were
NeuroImage 42 (2008) 969972
Corresponding author. Fax: (310) 794-9247.
E-mail address: mfoconnor@mednet.ucla.edu (M.-F. O'Connor).
1053-8119/$ see front matter © 2008 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.256
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
NeuroImage
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg
recruited to participate in an event-related fMRI study. Par-
ticipants were recruited from a clinic for women at familial risk
of breast cancer and from the community. Sixty-four persons
were screened by telephone, and 35 had an initial interview.
Participants were excluded for current Axis I disorder (includ-
ing major depression), as evidenced by a structured clinical
interview (Spitzer et al., 1994). CG was diagnosed in a struc-
tured clinical interview (Prigerson and Jacobs, 2001) using
empirically-derived criteria. The mean age of participants was
43.70 (SD= 10.00), they had an average of 16.57 (SD= 2.35)
years of school and 78.26% were white. The two groups did not
differ on any demographic characteristics as assessed by
ANOVA and chisquare analyses. The Institutional Review
Board at UCLA approved the study and all participants gave
written informed consent.
Procedure
Each participant provided a photograph of her deceased
loved one and these photos were matched with photos of a
stranger. Participants also shared an autobiographical narra-
tive of the death event and 15 grief-related idiographic words
were chosen from it. These words were matched for part of
speech, number of letters and frequency of usage in the En-
glish language with 15 neutral words. These photos and words
were made into 60 composites (Fig. 1), each consisting of
one picture (deceased or stranger) and one word (grief-related
or neutral). Empirical support for the grief-eliciting task has
been shown previously using both skin conductance metho-
dology and participants' subjective reports (Gündel et al.,
2003). Participants viewed composites through goggles in
randomized order. Each participant was given the same set of
instructions: Focus on any thoughts, feelings or memories
you have to the combination of the picture and the word on
the slide. Let yourself respond emotionally to each slide by
being aware of your feelings without trying to alter them.
fMRI methodology
Data were acquired on a Siemens Allegra 3T scanner. For
each participant, a high-resolution structural T2-weighted
echo-planar image (spin-echo; TR = 5000 ms; TE = 33 ms;
matrix size 128× 128; 36 axial slices; FOV= 20 cm; 3-mm
thick, skip 1-mm) was acquired coplanar with the functional
scan. A functional scan, lasting 5 min and 30 s, was acquired
(echo-planar T2-weighted gradient-echo, TR=2500 ms, TE =
25 ms, ip angle= 90, matrix size 64 × 64, 36 axial slices, FOV =
20 cm; 3-mm thick, skip 1-mm).
The imaging data were analyzed using statistical parametric
mapping (SPM'99; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neu-
rology, Institute of Neurology, London, UK). Images were re-
aligned, normalized, and smoothed with an 8 mm Gaussian
kernel, full width at half maximum. Analysis used the general
linear model in an event-related analysis. Effects at each voxel
were estimated using linear contrasts to compare regionally
specic effects. The contrasts for the individual subjects were
aggregated for single-group analysis and for between-group
analysis according to the random effect model in SPM. All
group comparison analyses (e.g., CGNNCG) were thresholded
using an uncorrected p-value of .005 combined with a cluster
size threshold of 10 voxels. Where indicated, parameter esti-
mates were extracted from group comparisons and reported
for each group separately. All coordinates are reported in MNI
format.
Results
Reward activation
Greater reward-related activity occurred in the CG relative
to the NCG individuals. Notably, the NA (x=10, y= 20, z=6;
Fig. 2A) was the only region of the brain that was more active in
response to grief-related words than neutral words among
those with CG compared to NCG (t= 3.51, pb0.001, 15 voxels).
Parameter estimates were extracted from this region and
reported separately for each group as shown in Fig. 2B. CG
individuals produced increased NA activity in response to grief
words (t=3.36, pb.01), whereas the NCG individuals did not
(t=1.82, pb.10). A comparison of the deceased and stranger
photos did not produce NA activity. This may have been due to
habituation effects as 15 different grief words were used, but
only a single deceased photograph was shown repeatedly. To
examine this possibility, we compared deceased and stranger
photos from the rst third of the trials only in order to isolate
pre-habituation activity. Here, as in the grief word comparison,
there was greater activation in the NA (x=14, y=4,z=16) for
the CG group than the NCG group (t=3.68, pb.001, 18 voxels).
Yearning
To clarify the functional role of NA activity in grief, cor-
relational analyses were performed with parameter estimates
of activity from the observed cluster (10, 20, 6). Greater NA
activation in the grief word comparison correlated signicantly
with more self-reported yearning for the deceased (r=.42,
pb.05) (Fig. 3). No correlation was observed between NA acti-
vation and length of time since the death, participant age, or
general positive or negative affect. Similarly, the NA activation
observed during the early trials of the deceased photo
comparison was also associated with yearning (r= .36, pb.05),
and was not correlated with length of time since the death,
participant age, or general positive or negative affect.
Pain network activation
To examine painrelated neural activity, we examined: a)
whether each group showed neural activity in three regions
previously linked to pain processes (anterior cingulate cortex
Fig. 1. Example of picture/word composites presented to subject.
970 M.-F. O'Connor et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 969972
(ACC), insula, periaqueductal gray (PAG) (Peyron et al., 2000));
and b) whether there were signicant differences between
the groups in painrelated neural activity in those regions
(pb.005, 10 voxels). If one group showed signicant activation
in a region (pb.005, 10 voxels), we reported the activation from
that same coordinate in the other group (Table 1). Each group
showed signicant activity in pain-related regions (dACC, in-
sula, PAG) in the comparisons of the deceased vs. stranger
pictures and grief vs. neutral words (Table 1), as has been pre-
viously shown in NCG (Gündel et al., 2003). The only activity in
pain-related regions that differed between groups was in the
left insula (x=34, y=14,z= 18), which was more active in the
NCG group during the viewing of grief-related vs. neutral
words (t=4.20, pb0.001, 87 voxels).
Discussion
Two models of grief have been hypothesized: a detachment
model and a reunion model (Bowlby,1980). In the detachment
model, the grief emotion is believed to play a role in the
acceptance of the reality of the death and therefore assist in
recovery from the loss. In the reunion model, the grief is a form
of protest against the separation from the deceased, and serves
to promote reunion with the lost person, not detachment. After
the death, cues of the deceased (such as memories, photos,
etc.) persist, triggering yearning and grief. Coping with these
salient cues is a major task in adjusting to the death (Freed and
Mann, 2007). Freed and Mann hypothesize that if the detach-
ment model is correct, the pangs of grief would occur with
reduced NA activity over time, as the salience of the cues
Fig. 3. Positive correlation between self-reported yearning and BOLD activity in the NA
(10, 20, 6; N=23) for each subject.
Table 1
Effects in three regions of the pain network for both CG (N=11) and NCG (N=12) in
response to pictures of the deceased and grief-related words
Region Group MNI coordinates (x,y,z)Tp-value (voxels)
Grief-relatedNneutral (words):
ACC
NCG 2 34 8 5.12 0.001 (140)
CG 2 34 8 2.74 0.02
Insula
NCG 36 18 18 4.73 0.001 (44)
CG ––
PAG
NCG ––
CG ––
Deceased Nstranger (pictures)
dACC
NCG 2 24 24 4.17 0.001 (309)
CG 2 24 24 1.87 0.05
Insula
NCG 40 8 2 4.76 0.001 (235)
48 10 0 4.47 0.001 (277)
CG 40 8 2 1.70 0.02
48 10 0 2.31 0.03
PAG
NCG 10 18 16 9.74 0.00 01 (55)
CG 10 18 16 2.37 0.02
Signicant activations in one group (pb005, 10 voxels) were examined and reported for
the other group. Tand p-values are listed for each group separately. Cluster sizes were
determined at the threshold of .005, 10 voxels (if the signicance did not reach this
threshold, cluster sizes were not comparable and therefore not listed). dACC = dorsal
anterior cingulate cortex, PAG = periaqueductal gray.
Fig. 2. A) Nucleus accumbens activity (10, 20, 6) in response to grief-related vs. neutral
words that was signicantly greater in the Complicated Grief group compared to the
Noncomplicated Grief group (pictured at pb.05). B) Bar graph showing nucleus
accumbens activity (10, 20, 6) in response to grief-related vs. neutral words for those
with Complicated and Noncomplicated Grief.
971M.-F. O'Connor et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 969972
decreases and acceptance of the reality leads to detachment. If
the reunion model is correct, then the pangs of grief would
continue to occur with NA activity, with reward activity in
response to the cues motivating reunion with the deceased.
This study demonstrates that each of these models may be
accurate for distinct subgroups of bereaved individuals: relative
to non-grief-eliciting stimuli, those with CG produced sig-
nicant NA activations, whereas those with NCG produced NA
reductions during grief elicitation.
Findings suggest that both CG and NCG groups may have
felt pain upon presentation of grief-related stimuli, but only
those with CG also activated an area important for reward
processing when viewing cues of the deceased. The associa-
tion between NA activity and yearning, but not time since
death, participant age, or positive/negative affect provides
convergent and discriminative validity. The addiction-rele-
vant aspect of this neural response (Knutson et al., 2001) may
help to explain why it is hard to resist engaging in pleasurable
reveries about the deceased even though engaging in these
reveries may prevent those with CG from adjusting to the
realities of the present. Many who suffer from addiction-like
disorders experience them as afictions; similarly we are not
suggesting that reveries about the deceased are emotionally
satisfying, but rather may serve as craving responses that may
make adapting to the reality of the loss more difcult.
Prior work has demonstrated a relationship between auto-
nomic arousal and posterior cingulate (PCC) activity during
grief elicitation (O'Connor et al., 2007). O'Connor et al. hypo-
thesized a role for PCC in those with CG, given that those with
CG may have greater arousal. However, there were no sig-
nicant differences in PCC activity between the NCG and CG
groups in the present study. Future work is needed to more
carefully explore whether the PCC plays a role in CG in a larger
sample. In addition, connectivity analyses in the prior study
showed that PCC activation was associated with subgenual ACC
activation. Similarly, future studies should investigate connec-
tivity between NA and prefrontal or limbic regions important in
emotion regulation.
Understanding the reward processes activated in those with
CG could substantially change treatment of this disorder.
Therapies such as behavioral interventions that target reward
processes may confer benet and preferentially aid in adapting
to the loss (Shear et al., 2005). Likewise, dopaminergic inter-
ventions that alter reward sensitivity could theoretically be
more effective in treating CG than serotonergic interventions,
which have failed to alter grief intensity (Zygmont et al., 1998).
Addressing the continued craving of past relationships may
assist those with CG in adapting to the loss.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the UCLA Brain Mapping Center for
their assistance. This research was supported by funds from
the California Breast Cancer Research Program Grant Number
10IB-0048. This work was also supported in part bygrant T32-
MH19925, the Cousins Center for Psychoneuroimmunology
and the Friends of the Semel Institute for Neuroscience and
Human Behavior.
References
Boelen, P.A., van den Bout, J., 2005. Complicated grief, depression, and anxiety as
distinct postloss syndromes: a conrmatory factor analysis study. Am. J. Psychiatry
162 (11), 21752177.
Bonnano, G.A., Wortman, C.B., Lehman, D.R., Tweed, R.G., Haring, M., Sonnega, J., et al.,
2002. Resilience to loss and chronic grief: a prospective study from preloss to 18-
months postloss. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83 (5), 11501164.
Bowlby, J.,1980. Attachment and loss. Loss, Sadness and Depression, vol 3. Basic Books,
New York.
Eisenberger, N.I., Lieberman, M.D., Williams, K.D., 2003. Does rejection hurt? An fMRI
study of social exclusion. Science 302 (5643), 290292.
Freed, P.J.,Mann, J.J., 20 07. Sadness and loss: Toward a neurobiopsychosocialmodel. Am.
J. Psych. 164 (1), 2834 .
Gündel,H.,O'Connor,M.-F.,Littrell,L.,Fort,C.,Lane,R.D.,2003.Functional
neuroanatomy of grief: an fMRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry 160 (11), 19461953.
Insel, T.R., 2003. Is social attachment an addictive disorder? Physiol. Behav. 79 (3), 351.
Knutson, B., Adams, C.M., Fong, G.W., Hommer, D., 2001. Anticipation of increasing
monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 21 (16) RC159.
O'Connor, M.-F., Gündel, H., McRae, K., Lane, R.D., 2007. Functional neuroanatomical
correlates of emotion al arousal during grief. Neuropsychopharmacology 32,
21842189.
Ott, C.H., Lueger, R.J., Kelber, S.T., Prigerson, H.G., 2007. Spousal bereavement in older
adults: common, resilient, and chronic grief with dening characteristics. J. Nerv.
Ment. Disord. 195 (4), 332341.
Panksepp, J., Knutson, B., Burgdorf, J., 2002. The role of brain emotional systems in
addictions: a neuro-evolutionary perspective and new self-reportanimal model.
Addiction 97 (4), 459469.
Peyron, R., Laurent, B., Garcia-Larrea, L., 2000. Functional imaging of brain responses to
pain. A review and meta-analysis. Clin. Neurophysiol. 30 (5), 263288.
Prigerson, H.G., Jacobs, S.C., 2001. Traumatic grief as a distinct disorder: a rationale, con-
sensuscriteria, and preliminaryempirical test.In: Stroebe,M.S., Hansson,R.O., Stroebe,
W., Schut, H. (Eds.), Handbook of Bereavement Research: Consequences, Coping and
Care. American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., pp. 613645.
Rainville, P., 2002. Brain mechanisms of pain affect and pain modulation. Curr. Opin.
Neurobiol. 12 (2), 195.
Shear, K., Frank, E., Houck, P.R., ReynoldsIII, C.F., 2005. Treatment of complicated grief: a
randomized controlled trial. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 293 (21), 26012608.
Spitzer, R.L., Williams, J.B.W., Gibbons, M., First, M.D.,1994. Structured Clinical Interview
of the DSM-IV. American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC.
Young, L.J., Lim, M.M., Gingrich, B., Insel, T.R., 2001. Cellular mechanisms of social
attachment. Horm. Behav. 40 (2), 133138.
Zygmont, M., Prigerson, H., Houck, P., Miller, M., Shear, M., Jacobs, S., et al., 1998. A post
hoc comparison of paroxetine and nortriptyline for symptoms of traumatic grief.
J. Clin. Psychiatry 59 (5), 241245.
972 M.-F. O'Connor et al. / NeuroImage 42 (2008) 969972
... При РТГ у порівнянні з нормальним горюванням достовірно більше активуються ділянки прилеглого ядра. З попередніх досліджень відомо, що демонстрація образів романтичного кохання (партнер проти незнайомця) та батьківської любові (своєї дитини проти іншої дитини) також показують активність тієї самої ділянки головного мозку [49,50]. Оскільки активність прилеглого ядра є високою як у відповідь на живих близьких так й в тих, хто страждає від ускладненого горя, ймовірно, активація в цьому регіоні у відповідь на нагадування про померлого з часом зменшується при неускладненому горі. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective. Due to the war in Ukraine the problem of loss has become critical. Significant bereavement cause grief. At the core of grief is separation distress which is accompanied by loss-related experiences. They trigger a grieving process that helps adjust to bereavement and requires different strategies in different areas of life. If the burning is prolonged, it maladapts the person and pathological burning is formed. Purpose. The purpose of the article is to conduct an analysis of the concept of "normal grief" and "pathological grief", a critical review of modern researches on this problem. Methods. In order to achieve the purpose, a thorough qualitative and quantitative content analysis of scientific researches devoted to bereavement, normal and pathological grief was carried out. The article search was made using international databases such as PubMed, Google Scholar, Web of Science and CrossRef, for the period 1997-2024. Conclusions. Considering the ongoing war in Ukraine, studies related to pathological grief are relevant. In the first place are diagnostic criteria and a clear distinction between normal and pathological reactions to bereavement, identification of risk factors, detection of subtle physiological and psychological mechanisms of prolongation of the grieving process.
... Individuals with damage to these brain regions often report a lack of distress from physical pain, despite still being able to feel it (Berthier, Starkstein, & Leiguarda, 1988). Furthermore, research has shown that these same neural regions are involved in processing experiences of social rejection or loss in humans (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003;O'Connor et al., 2008). Given this overlap in neural systems, it is reasonable to assume that factors reducing physical pain would also affect social pain. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social pain is emotional distress caused by harm or threat to social connections that results in social exclusion, rejection, or loss. Social Pain is also a potentiator of physical pain. Supportive social relationships are widely recognized for their impact on maintaining health and well-being. The Passion of Jesus Christ serves as a quintessential example of social pain (i.e., desertion, betrayal, denial) potentiating physical pain (i.e., beatings, Crown of Thorns, crucifixion). Christ opts to forgive. Although forgiveness is one solution to reduce social pain, other interventions exist. This review seeks to identify and summarize interventions associated with reducing social pain. We conducted a systematic review using Medline (PubMed), Google Scholar, and Cochrane CENTRAL to identify relevant articles. Results: The database searches produced 548 articles. Fourteen randomized controlled trials (RCTs) were included in this systematic review. Acetaminophen, both deceptive and open-label placebos, mindfulness training, and psilocybin were found to reduce social pain. Of note, the combination of acetaminophen and forgiveness yielded superior results compared to either acetaminophen or forgiveness alone. Pharmacological interventions operate on the premise that the neural pathways responsible for physical pain also play a role in social pain. Both pharmacological and non-pharmacological interventions are available for reducing social pain.
... One of these found a relationship between autonomic arousal and posterior cingulate activity during grief elicitation (O'Connor et al. 2007). A subsequent study by O'Connor et al. (2008) looked at the activation patterns of recently bereaved women. The experimenters selected 23 participants and conducted clinical interviews with them (based on Prigerson and Jacobs 2001), which led to eleven of the women being diagnosed with "complicated grief." ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper explores the limitations of neurobiological approaches to human emotional experience, focusing on the case of grief. We propose that grief is neither an episodic emotion nor a longer-term mood but instead a heterogeneous, temporally extended process. A grief process can incorporate all manner of experiences, thoughts, and activities, most or all of which are not grief-specific. Furthermore, its course over time is shaped in various different ways by interpersonal, social, and cultural environments. This poses methodological challenges for any attempt to relate grief to the brain. Grief also illustrates wider limitations of approaches that conceive of emotions as brief episodes, abstracted from the dynamic, holistic, longer-term organization of human emotional life.
... Neurobiological mechanisms, specifically reward pathways in the brain, may also explain this strong relationship. Yearning and longing for a deceased loved one is characteristic of PGD, and neuroimaging has linked this feature with reward-related activity in the nucleus accumbens (O'Connor et al., 2008), similar to craving and neural activation among individuals with alcohol use disorder (Galandra et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Problematic alcohol use (PAU) is highly prevalent in the United States. Although bereavement, a highly stressful and ubiquitous experience across the lifespan, is believed to increase the risk for PAU based on a small number of studies, research using large diverse samples of bereaved adults has yet to be conducted. Therefore, relations between PAU and bereavement remain poorly understood, hampering the reach and effectiveness of alcohol interventions. The current study addresses this limitation by investigating rates and correlates of PAU and service utilization among a large national sample of bereaved adults. Methods Participants were adults who reported the death of a significant other in their lifetime (N = 1529). Most participants identified as female (69.1%) and White (68.2%), with an average age of 44.7 (SD = 16.29). Online self‐report surveys assessed the prevalence of PAU using the AUDIT‐C, mental health service utilization, and associated characteristics. Results Nearly one‐third (n = 463; 30.3%) screened positive for PAU, which surpasses rates found in the general US population. After accounting for other characteristics, time since the death (OR, 3.63; 95% CI, 2.59–5.08) and meeting presumptive criteria for depression (OR, 2.28; 95% CI, 1.64–3.18) and prolonged grief disorder (PGD; OR, 1.66; 95% CI, 1.13–2.25) significantly increased risk for PAU among the bereaved. Approximately half (n = 244; 52.7%) of bereaved adults with PAU received any mental health service since the death. Time since the death (OR, 4.19; 95% CI, 2.38–7.48) and presumptive depression (OR, 2.16; 95% CI, 1.25–3.74) were associated with service utilization after accounting for other characteristics. Conclusions The high prevalence of PAU among bereaved adults, particularly among those with a diagnosis of PGD, and limited use of support services underscore the need for greater empirical attention and integrated substance use care for bereaved adults.
Article
Full-text available
Background Researchers explore the biology of painful experiences not primarily felt in the body (‘non-physical pain’), sometimes referred to as mental, social or emotional pain. A critical challenge lies in how to operationalise this subjective experience for biological research, a crucial process for translating findings into clinical practice. Aims To map studies investigating biological features of non-physical pain, focusing on their conceptual features (i.e. terms and definitions of non-physical pain) and methodological characteristics (e.g. experimental paradigms and measures). Method This methodological systematic review searched reports of primary research on the biological features of non-physical pain across Embase, MEDLINE and Web of Science. Using a meta-research approach, we synthetised results on terms, definitions, populations, experimental paradigms, confounders, measures of non-physical pain and investigation methods (e.g. functional magnetic resonance imaging). Results We identified 92 human studies, involving 7778 participants. Overall, 59.1% of the studies did not report any definition of non-physical pain, and 82% of studies did not use a specific measure. Regarding the possibility of translating results to clinical settings, most of the human studies involved only healthy participants (71.7%) and the seven different experimental paradigms used to induce non-physical pain had unknown external validity. Confounders were not considered by 32.4% of the experimental studies. Animal studies were rare, with only four rodent studies. Conclusions Biomedical studies of non-physical pain use heterogeneous concepts with unclear overlaps and methods with unknown external validity. As has been done for physical pain, priority actions include establishing an agreed definition and measurement of non-physical pain and developing experimental paradigms with good external validity.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to investigate the treatment of acceptance and commitment therapy based on grief disorder and distress among coronary heart disease nurses in the School of Nursing and Midwifery of Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences in Tehran. The present study was a quasi-experimental design of pre-test-post-test with a control group. The experimental sample included 30 bereaved nurses in 1399. They were selected by available sampling method and divided into experimental and control groups. The experimental group underwent group acceptance and commitment therapy training of Professor Steven (1986) for 8 sessions of 60 minutes. The Anxiety Scale (Educational Attendance, Social Attendance, and Cognitive Attendance), Kessler et al. (2002), and the Mourning Questionnaire (1975) were used to collect data on nurses suffering from coronary heart disease due to the spread of the coronavirus online. Data were analyzed using multivariate analysis of covariance in two stages of post-test and follow-up, significance level p <0.05. The results in the post-test and follow-up stages showed that the acceptance and commitment therapy training group was significantly effective on the components of distress and long grief disorder. Based on the results of this study, it seems that by teaching acceptance and commitment therapy, the amount of distress and disorder to prolonged mourning caused by coronary artery can be reduced
Article
Full-text available
The U.S. Federal Aviation Administration (FAA) recently provided detailed instructions on how Aviation Medical Examiners (AMEs) should assess and evaluate pilots for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The European, Australian and International Civil Aviation Organization guidelines for the assessment of PTSD in aviation are general guidelines and do not address the unique and specific circumstances of a flight crew per se. The starting point of the U.S. FAA’s guidance is an already-established clinical PTSD diagnosis since it is known that PTSD compromises aviation safety and has been related to fatal aviation accidents. According to the FAA’s guidance, a PTSD assessment is undertaken based on whether the condition is symptomatic and medicated, or whether more than 2 years have elapsed since showing symptoms and receiving medication. The International Classification of Diseases (ICD) criteria for stress disorders have changed between versions ICD-10 and the soon-to-be-released ICD-11. The new ICD-11 criteria are discussed in this article in the context of aviation health. Additionally, PTSD, potentially caused by an incident of turbulence, is discussed in the context of aviation mental health. There are currently no published studies on turbulence-caused mental trauma. We have identified in this article potential factors which are related to pilots’ and cabin crew’s stressors in incidents of severe and extreme turbulence. Three recommendations are provided: (1) harmonize assessment practices of PTSD internationally; (2) healthcare professionals taking care of traumatized flight crew should have a follow-up guide that takes specific and local conditions into account, and ensures the identification of patients who require follow-up treatment as early as possible; and (3) aviation health care professionals should consider ICD-11 diagnostic criteria as the information may be more useful in the assessment and diagnosis of aviation-related trauma.
Article
The dissolving of social bonds is disruptive and leads to increased stress responsivity and a strong desire for reunion. The oxytocin (OXT) system is critical for the formation of social attachments, such as pair bonds, and is also involved in social recognition, social memory, and social vigilance. Therefore, long-term changes in the OXT system resulting from cohabitation and pair bonding may contribute to reunion-seeking behavior. Here, we employed social conditioned place preference (SCPP) and the forced swim test (FST) to examine sensitivity to partner-associated contexts and passive stress coping following a period of partner separation. We found that opposite-sex cohabitation led to SCPP formation only in male prairie voles with a strong preference for their partner, and this SCPP was maintained following short-term loss of a pair bonded partner. Furthermore, pair bonded males that were separated from their partner displayed more passive stress-coping than those that were not bonded to their lost partner, suggesting that differences in prairie vole mating tactics (i.e. formation of a bond or not) influence the behavioral response to partner separation. Finally, we found changes in OXTR binding that may reflect variation in loss-related behavioral phenotypes based on different mating strategies.
Article
Full-text available
This chapter begins with an evaluation of recent research that has accumulated about traumatic grief (TG) symptomatology. More specifically, the available evidence is reviewed to determine whether it satisfies requirements for the establishment of TG as a distinct clinical entity. Following this review, the authors discuss some advantages and disadvantages of developing standardized criteria for TG. Given their belief that the advantages outweigh the disadvantages, they then describe the logic behind the consensus criteria for TG proposed by a panel of experts. Results of a preliminary test of the proposed criteria set for TG are reported, directions for their further refinement are discussed, and assessment tools for TG are presented. The authors conclude by proposing ways to distinguish between normal and TG reactions and by suggesting directions for future research. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Brain responses to pain, assessed through positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) are reviewed. Functional activation of brain regions are thought to be reflected by increases in the regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in PET studies, and in the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal in fMRI. rCBF increases to noxious stimuli are almost constantly observed in second somatic (SII) and insular regions, and in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and with slightly less consistency in the contralateral thalamus and the primary somatic area (SI). Activation of the lateral thalamus, SI, SII and insula are thought to be related to the sensory-discriminative aspects of pain processing. SI is activated in roughly half of the studies, and the probability of obtaining SI activation appears related to the total amount of body surface stimulated (spatial summation) and probably also by temporal summation and attention to the stimulus. In a number of studies, the thalamic response was bilateral, probably reflecting generalised arousal in reaction to pain. ACC does not seem to be involved in coding stimulus intensity or location but appears to participate in both the affective and attentional concomitants of pain sensation, as well as in response selection. ACC subdivisions activated by painful stimuli partially overlap those activated in orienting and target detection tasks, but are distinct from those activated in tests involving sustained attention (Stroop, etc.). In addition to ACC, increased blood flow in the posterior parietal and prefrontal cortices is thought to reflect attentional and memory networks activated by noxious stimulation. Less noted but frequent activation concerns motor-related areas such as the striatum, cerebellum and supplementary motor area, as well as regions involved in pain control such as the periaqueductal grey. In patients, chronic spontaneous pain is associated with decreased resting rCBF in contralateral thalamus, which may be reverted by analgesic procedures. Abnormal pain evoked by innocuous stimuli (allodynia) has been associated with amplification of the thalamic, insular and SII responses, concomitant to a paradoxical CBF decrease in ACC. It is argued that imaging studies of allodynia should be encouraged in order to understand central reorganisations leading to abnormal cortical pain processing. A number of brain areas activated by acute pain, particularly the thalamus and anterior cingulate, also show increases in rCBF during analgesic procedures. Taken together, these data suggest that hemodynamic responses to pain reflect simultaneously the sensory, cognitive and affective dimensions of pain, and that the same structure may both respond to pain and participate in pain control. The precise biochemical nature of these mechanisms remains to be investigated.
Article
Full-text available
Comparative studies have implicated the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) in the anticipation of incentives, but the relative responsiveness of this neural substrate during anticipation of rewards versus punishments remains unclear. Using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging, we investigated whether the anticipation of increasing monetary rewards and punishments would increase NAcc blood oxygen level-dependent contrast (hereafter, "activation") in eight healthy volunteers. Whereas anticipation of increasing rewards elicited both increasing self-reported happiness and NAcc activation, anticipation of increasing punishment elicited neither. However, anticipation of both rewards and punishments activated a different striatal region (the medial caudate). At the highest reward level ($5.00), NAcc activation was correlated with individual differences in self-reported happiness elicited by the reward cues. These findings suggest that whereas other striatal areas may code for expected incentive magnitude, a region in the NAcc codes for expected positive incentive value.
Article
Full-text available
The evolutionary significance of neurochemical events in the brain has received minimal attention in the field of addiction research. Likewise, the general failure of neuroscientists to postulate how basic brain circuits might mediate emotional urges has retarded the development of scientific perspectives that could inform new inquiries into the underlying dynamics and treatment of addictions. In this paper, we revisit the argument that prototypically abused substances activate or alter specific emotional brain systems that were evolutionarily designed to signal potential increments or decrements in fitness. We then discuss two distinct emotional systems (reward seeking and separation distress) which may track different types of potential changes in fitness. Based on this evolutionarily inspired approach, we illustrate how a mammalian model of emotion (i.e. rodent ultrasonic vocalizations) may enable scientists to predict drug-related phenomena such as abuse potential, anatomical location of mediating neural substrates, and the psychological impact of withdrawal. We conclude by discussing some therapeutic and social implications of examining drug addiction processes with multiple emotional brain systems in mind.
Article
This report presents the results of an open-trial pilot study of paroxetine for symptoms of traumatic grief, compared with the effects of nortriptyline in an archival contrast group. Data are presented on 15 subjects (4 men, 11 women), ranging in age from 40 to 79 years (mean age = 57 years), who experienced the loss of a spouse (N = 8), child (N =5), grandchild (N = 1), or parent (N = 1). Subjects were required to have a baseline score on the Inventory of Complicated Grief (ICG) of > or = 20. Treatment with paroxetine began at a median of 17 months (range, 6-139 months) after the loss. Paroxetine-treated subjects received a psychotherapy tailored for traumatic grief. Depressive symptoms were assessed by using the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D). The ICG and the HAM-D were administered weekly over 4 months of paroxetine treatment (median dose = 30 mg/day). The group receiving paroxetine were then compared with a group (N = 22) participating in a separate trial of nortriptyline (median dose = 77.5 mg/day) for treatment of bereavement-related major depressive episodes. Level of traumatic grief symptoms (ICG) decreased by 53%, and depression ratings (HAM-D) decreased by 54% in paroxetine-treated subjects. Nortriptyline showed clinical effects comparable to those of paroxetine. Paroxetine may be an effective agent in the treatment of traumatic grief symptoms. A comparison of the paroxetine-treated group with a nortriptyline-treated group suggests that both agents have comparably beneficial effects on the symptoms of traumatic grief (as well as those of depression). However, the higher rate of diagnostic comorbidity in the paroxetine-treated group, together with the greater chronicity of their symptoms and the greater safety of paroxetine in overdose, leads us to favor paroxetine over nortriptyline for traumatic grief symptoms in general psychiatric practice. Further controlled evaluation of paroxetine for traumatic grief is necessary.
Article
Pharmacological studies in prairie voles have suggested that the neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin play important roles in behaviors associated with monogamy, including affiliation, paternal care, and pair bonding. Our laboratory has investigated the cellular and neuroendocrine mechanisms by which these peptides influence affiliative behavior and social attachment in prairie voles. Monogamous prairie voles have a higher density of oxytocin receptors in the nucleus accumbens than do nonmonogamous vole species; blockade of these receptors by site-specific injection of antagonist in the female prairie vole prevents partner preference formation. Prairie voles also have a higher density of vasopressin receptors in the ventral pallidal area, which is the major output of the nucleus accumbens, than montane voles. Both the nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum are key relay nuclei in the brain circuits implicated in reward, such as the mesolimbic dopamine and opioid systems. Therefore, we hypothesize that oxytocin and vasopressin may be facilitating affiliation and social attachment in monogamous species by modulating these reward pathways.
Article
Recent animal studies reveal ascending nociceptive and descending modulatory pathways that may contribute to the affective-motivational aspects of pain and play a critical role in the modulation of pain. In humans, a reliable pattern of cerebral activity occurs during the subjective experience of pain. Activity within the anterior cingulate cortex and possibly in other classical limbic structures, appears to be closely related to the subjective experience of pain unpleasantness and may reflect the regulation of endogenous mechanisms of pain modulation.