ArticlePublisher preview available

Pathogens, Personality, and Culture: Disease Prevalence Predicts Worldwide Variability in Sociosexuality, Extraversion, and Openness to Experience

American Psychological Association
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Previous research has documented cross-cultural differences in personality traits, but the origins of those differences remain unknown. The authors investigate the possibility that these cultural differences can be traced, in part, to regional differences in the prevalence in infectious diseases. Three specific hypotheses are deduced, predicting negative relationships between disease prevalence and (a) unrestricted sociosexuality, (b) extraversion, and (c) openness to experience. These hypotheses were tested empirically with methods that employed epidemiological atlases in conjunction with personality data collected from individuals in dozens of countries worldwide. Results were consistent with all three hypotheses: In regions that have historically suffered from high levels of infectious diseases, people report lower mean levels of sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness. Alternative explanations are addressed, and possible underlying mechanisms are discussed.
Pathogens, Personality, and Culture: Disease Prevalence Predicts
Worldwide Variability in Sociosexuality, Extraversion, and Openness to
Experience
Mark Schaller and Damian R. Murray
University of British Columbia
Previous research has documented cross-cultural differences in personality traits, but the origins of those
differences remain unknown. The authors investigate the possibility that these cultural differences can be
traced, in part, to regional differences in the prevalence in infectious diseases. Three specific hypotheses
are deduced, predicting negative relationships between disease prevalence and (a) unrestricted sociosex-
uality, (b) extraversion, and (c) openness to experience. These hypotheses were tested empirically with
methods that employed epidemiological atlases in conjunction with personality data collected from
individuals in dozens of countries worldwide. Results were consistent with all three hypotheses: In
regions that have historically suffered from high levels of infectious diseases, people report lower mean
levels of sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness. Alternative explanations are addressed, and possible
underlying mechanisms are discussed.
Keywords: culture, disease prevalence, extraversion, openness to experience, sociosexuality
People’s personalities differ, and some of that individual vari-
ability is geographically clumped. But why is that so? How are we
to understand the origins of regional differences in personality? A
complete response to that question will surely require attention to
many different processes operating at different levels of analysis.
Here, we focus on one previously unidentified contributor to those
differences. We report empirical results indicating that specific
kinds of cross-cultural differences in personality result, in part,
from regional differences in the prevalence of infectious disease.
Cross-Cultural Differences in Personality
In recent years, several ambitious programs of research have
collected data from dozens of countries worldwide as a means of
documenting cross-cultural differences on various kinds of per-
sonality traits. These investigations assessed personality traits at
the individual level of analysis, with standard trait assessment
instruments, and on the basis of these data computed mean trait
scores at the regional level of analysis.
For example, Schmitt (2005) and his collaborators in the Inter-
national Sexuality Description Project assessed worldwide vari-
ability in chronic tendencies toward either a “restricted” or “unre-
stricted” sociosexual style. (Among other things, more highly
unrestricted individuals seek more sexual variety, are more com-
fortable with casual sexual encounters, and have more sexual
partners across their lifetime.) The results document considerable
regional variability on this trait. In addition, several international
teams of researchers have assessed worldwide variability along the
Big Five personality traits of agreeableness, conscientiousness,
extraversion, neuroticism, and openness to experience (McCrae,
2002; McCrae, Terracciano, & 79 members of the Personality
Profiles of Cultures Project, 2005; Schmitt et al., 2007). The
results document regional differences along all five fundamental
dimensions of personality.
These results, and others like them, are invaluable assets to the
scientific study of individual differences and culture. Newer in-
vestigations have been addressing the consequences that these
cultural differences may have (e.g., on health outcomes and social
policy decisions; McCrae & Terracciano, 2008). To date, however,
almost no research whatsoever has addressed why these worldwide
differences in personality profiles exist in the first place.
Psychology, Ecology, and the Origins of Culture
Many factors may have contributed to cross-cultural differences
in personality. Some of those causes may be idiosyncratic to the
unique histories of specific populations. Although important, anal-
yses of idiosyncratic circumstances don’t easily yield conclusions
that are predictive on a more global scale. It is toward discovering
a more complete (and more broadly predictive) explanation for the
origins of cross-cultural differences that it is useful to consider
how basic human tendencies, operating in conjunction with vary-
ing ecological conditions, can produce cross-cultural differences
Mark Schaller and Damian R. Murray, Department of Psychology,
University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.
This research was supported by research grants from the University of
British Columbia Hampton Fund and the Social Sciences and Humanities
Research Council of Canada. Thanks to Emma Buchtel, Steve Heine, and
Ara Norenzayan for helpful comments and suggestions. Thanks also to
Robert McCrae, Antonio Terracciano, and their collaborators on the Per-
sonality Profiles of Cultures Project for generously providing us with the
opportunity to do secondary analyses on their facet-level findings prior to
the publication of those findings.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mark
Schaller, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2136
West Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada. E-mail:
schaller@psych.ubc.ca
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association
2008, Vol. 95, No. 1, 212–221 0022-3514/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.212
212
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... This claim is supported by the finding that Openness to Experience is lower within populations of countries with higher rates of infectious diseases (r = -.59; Schaller and Murray, 2008). However, while a negative relationship between Openness to Experience and Disgust was found in a sample of 132 undergraduate students in the United States (Haidt et al., 1994), this result was not replicated in an Italian sample (Giampietro et al., 2019). ...
... Research had found that people are more likely to be lower in Openness to Experience within countries that have higher levels of disease prevalence (Schaller & Murray, 2008). The results here suggest that this relationship may be due to other factors outside Disgust sensitivity. ...
Preprint
IntroductionMost research investigating relationships between the Big Five and emotional states has focused on how emotional attributes relate to Extraversion and Neuroticism. However, the potential for discrete emotional states to enable a richer understanding of the emotive nature of all Big Five traits and their sub-traits has been neglected.Methods Participants (N = 203) completed the Big Five Aspects Scale, watched six emotionally stimulating video clips, and self-reported their experience of basic emotions before (Baseline) and after (Reaction) each video. Spearman correlations identified state-trait relationships, followed by regression analyses to assess the unique contribution of each trait to emotional experiences.Results Conscientiousness negatively correlated with Baseline Sadness, while Agreeableness positively correlated with Reaction Disgust, Fear, and Sadness. Extraversion predicted higher Joy, and Neuroticism was linked to greater Fear and Sadness.Conclusion Findings reinforce Extraversion and Neuroticism’s links to positive and negative emotionality, respectively, while also showing that Agreeableness predicts heightened sensitivity to negative affect. Conscientiousness, particularly Orderliness, appears protective against Baseline Sadness, and Openness to Experience, especially Intellect, is linked to lower sensitivity to Surprise. Potential mechanisms underlying these relationships are discussed.Keywords: Personality, Emotion, Basic Emotions, Five Factor Model, Big Five Aspects Scale
... However, there are results that show that state-dependent models are indeed plausible. The data suggests that the behavior guided by lower openness and extraversion may be more adaptive in countries with higher prevalence of infective diseases (Schaller & Murray, 2008). Environments affected by violent intergroup conflicts moderate the link between personality and mating, highlighting the adaptive role of extraversion and emotional stability (Međedović, 2018b). ...
... This process exists even in preindustrial populations where fitness benefits of personality characteristics depend on the specific locations where individuals live (Gurven et al., 2014). Fitness outcomes of personality may also be dependent upon the incidence of pathogens in a given population (Schaller & Murray, 2008). The variation in selection gradients on personality (i.e., regression slopes) that we obtained in the present data are in accordance with environmentally-driven state-dependent models of personality evolution. ...
Article
Full-text available
The effects of natural selection on personality traits are still understudied, which is in contrast with their importance for analyzing the evolution of personality. In the present research, we analyzed natural selection on the Big Five personality traits by estimating the relations between personality characteristics and evolutionary fitness (reproductive success: i.e., number of children) using the World Values Survey wave 6 data ( N = 22,636; 17 countries). Using multilevel Poisson regression models with random slopes, we obtained a positive linear association between conscientiousness and reproductive success. We also detected a nonlinear association between openness and the criterion measure: additional graphical and ANOVA analyses showed that nonlinearity emerged from the fact that only individuals with above average openness had lower reproductive success. The effect sizes of the associations between personality traits and reproductive success were low. Finally, we detected variation in selection gradients (i.e., differences in fixed model coefficients) across the countries—coefficients with both positive and negative signs are estimated for extraversion, neuroticism, and openness. This variation is in accordance with the state-dependent models of personality evolution, where environment is viewed as an external state—the environment's moderating effect on the personality-fitness link may preserve inter-individual variation in behavior within and between the populations.
... Boldness is positively associated to a beneficial childhood environment, especially to functional family relations. Having in mind the hypothesized negative links between psychopathy and beneficial environment, this result is intriguing, but it is in line with the data of more cautious and restricted personality traits in dangerous ecologies (Schaller & Murray, 2008); being bold, sociable, and stimulus-seeking in hostile environments may not be adaptive. Boldness is linked to intrasexual competition primarily through Self-Promotion: this is a competitive behavior where an individual shows one's best qualities in order to attract a mate and some scholars regard it as the most effective of competition tactics (Schmitt & Buss, 1996). ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research of psychopathic traits in the context of life history were limited by using single-score psychopathy measures and analyzing only some aspects of human life history. In the present study, we explored the associations between triarchic psychopathy traits (Meanness, Boldness, and Disinhibition) and a large number of life history indicators (childhood environment, age of menarche and first sexual intercourse, short and long-term mating, intra-sexual competition, and reproductive motivation) using a data from a community sample (N=486). We used network analysis to estimate the associations between the variables and their centrality indices in the network. The results were mostly in line with the hypothesis that psychopathy represents a set of behavioral traits that depicts a faster pace of life. However, some differences between psychopathic traits in the life history traits also emerged, showing different associations between psychopathy traits and life history indicators, especially regarding Boldness trait. Furthermore, Boldness showed a markedly higher centrality in the network compared to other psychopathy traits. Taken together, the results suggest that Boldness has the highest adaptive potential of all psychopathy traits. The findings are implicative for understanding psychopathy's pace of life and adaptive potential of psychopathic characteristics.
... Other research shows that chronic concerns with disease-and experimental manipulations of disease threat-predict prejudice towards novel cultures (Faulkner et al., 2004;Hodson & Costello, 2007;McGovern et al., 2024) or towards immigrants who refuse to assimilate into ingroup cultural norms (Karinen et al., 2019; though cultural neophobia may be better predicted by other non-disease-related motivational systems, e.g., Kerry et al., 2020). Experimentally priming disease threat leads to greater risk aversion (Prokosch et al., 2019), and at the cross-cultural level, actual levels of disease threat are negatively associated with Openness to Experience (Schaller & Murray, 2008)-the Big Five personality trait most strongly associated with novelty seeking and risk taking (Gocłowska et al., 2019;Lauriola & Levin, 2001). Given the implications of disease threat for people's novelty aversion and cautiousness, aversion toward unfamiliar food sources may have an especially strong relationship with disease transmission concerns. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Infectious diseases have posed an existential threat to humans throughout history, resulting in a complex system of evolved psychological and behavioral mechanisms designed to help mitigate infection. Given that food consumption represents a significant route through which humans can be exposed to illness-causing pathogens, further research into the relationship between disease avoidance motivations and novel food avoidance (i.e., food neophobia) is warranted. Methods Across three studies (total N = 736), we investigated the relationship between trait disease avoidance motivation (assessed by the Perceived Vulnerability to Disease scale) and food neophobia. Results and Conclusions Results from each of the three studies indicated that greater dispositional germ aversion significantly predicted greater food neophobia, whereas the relationship between dispositional perceived infectability and food neophobia was positive but more variable across the studies. Additionally, Study 3 revealed that while greater dispositional food neophobia predicted greater likelihood of avoiding foreign foods, experimentally priming disease threat was not associated with food choice. Lastly, an internal meta-analysis revealed that both germ aversion and perceived infectability were both uniquely positively associated with food neophobia. Limitations, conceptual issues, and avenues for future research are discussed.
... The level of students with good immunity impacts students' ability to produce more literacy programs compared to extrovert students with low immunity levels. This is in line with theoretical analyzes showing that individual differences in the strength of the biological immune response can be associated with individual differences in personality traits that function as behavioral immune responses (Schaller & Murray, 2008;Thornhill et al., 2010;Schaller, 2011;Mengelkoch et al., 2022;Namaziandost et al., 2023). According to this theoretical approach, individuals who have relatively weak biological immune responses are hypothesized to exhibit stronger behavioral immune responses such as avoidance of strangers, reduced exploratory behaviors, and greater exploratory behaviors. ...
Article
The development of literary culture in schools needs a literate ecosystem and full support from literacy activists in schools. This study aims to describe and explore the profile of literacy activists in madrasahs in terms of their level of immunity and personality traits. This study investigates the effect of student immunity as literacy activists in madrasahs with extroverted and introverted personality types on the ability to develop a literary culture. The parameters used to investigate the immunity levels in extroverted and introverted students were by examining the number of leukocytes, types of leukocytes, and the results of the IL-6 examination using the ELISA method. The results show that extroverted students have higher immunity than introverted students. The average leukocyte level of extroverted students is 24,350 cells/mm3 and introverted students is 50 cells/mm3. Meanwhile, the results of the IL-6 examination show the highest is 163,863 and the lowest is 43,984. Extroverted students can design various literacy programs that are challenging for students and are better able to build networks in developing literacy programs compared to introverted students. So, it can be concluded that extroverted students have a higher level of immunity, and also have the ability to explore and develop literacy programs better than introverted students.
... 9 The prevalence of infectious diseases has been linked to cross-cultural differences between regions, 10,11 which span from gastronomic variations 12 to distinctions of personality traits. 13 Of relevance for this secondary analysis, poor self-reported health was found in individuals not only with chronic diseases 14,15 but also among those with infectious diseases. 16 Self-reported health is a general health indicator widely used in public health studies. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: Infectious diseases are often associated with decline in quality of life. The aim of this study is to analyze the relationship between personal history of communicable, i.e., infectious and parasitic diseases and self-rated health. Study design: Secondary analysis of a large dataset multi-country observational study. Methods: We used a four-pronged analysis approach to investigate whether personal history of infectious and parasitic diseases is related to self-reported health, measured with a single item. Results: Three of the four analyses found a small positive effect on self-reported health among those reporting a history of pathogen exposure. The meta-analysis found no support but large heterogeneity that was not reduced by two classifications of countries. Conclusion: Personal history of infectious and parasitic diseases does not reduce self-reported health across a global sample.
... In this study, fear is assumed to be a driver for human control actions; so, other fear-control relationships are not within the scope of this work (e.g., large fear may cause stigma, reducing or delaying control efforts in some individuals [47]). Future studies could explore what types of social media, what kinds of educational programs, and what other psychosocial characteristics (e.g., personality [48,49], personal concerns [50], risk-prone or -averse behavior [51], rebellious mentality [52,53], social networks [54]) may produce equal or even opposite effects on the public's control actions beyond fear emotion. ...
Article
Full-text available
One major challenge in preventing infectious diseases comes from human control behaviors. In the context of vector-borne diseases (VBDs), I explored how the waxing and waning of a human psychological emotion—fear—can generate diverse control actions, which, in turn, influence disease dynamics. Fear may diminish over time after being triggered but can also be reinforced when new triggers emerge. By integrating fear dynamics into a generic Ross–MacDonald model tailored for the Zika virus, I found that an increase in initial fear can enhance control efforts, thereby reducing the number of infected individuals and deaths. Once initial fear becomes strong enough to deplete the mosquito population, any further increase in fear no longer impacts disease dynamics. When initial fear is at an intermediate level, the increase in disease caused by greater decay in fear can be counterbalanced by increasing the frequency of fear triggers. Interestingly, when the control period is short and initial fear is at an intermediate level, increasing the frequency of fear reinforcement can lead to a “hydra effect”, which increases disease transmission. These findings help explain variations in human control efforts and provide insights for developing more effective disease control strategies that account for the fear dynamics of local communities. This work also contributes to advancing the theory at the intersection of human behavior, disease ecology, and epidemiology.
Article
Introduction Most research investigating relationships between the Big Five and emotional states has focused on how emotional attributes relate to Extraversion and Neuroticism. However, the potential for discrete emotional states to enable a richer understanding of the emotive nature of all Big Five traits and their subtraits has been neglected. Methods Participants ( N = 203) completed the Big Five Aspects Scale, watched six emotionally stimulating video clips, and self‐reported their experience of basic emotions before (Baseline) and after (Reaction) each video. Spearman correlations identified state–trait relationships, followed by regression analyses to assess the unique contribution of each trait to emotional experiences. Results Conscientiousness negatively correlated with Baseline Sadness, while Agreeableness positively correlated with Reaction Disgust, Fear, and Sadness. Extraversion predicted higher Joy, and Neuroticism was linked to greater Fear and Sadness. Conclusion Findings reinforce Extraversion and Neuroticism's links to positive and negative emotionality, respectively, while also showing that Agreeableness predicts heightened sensitivity to negative affect. Conscientiousness, particularly Orderliness, appears protective against Baseline Sadness, and Openness to Experience, especially Intellect, is linked to lower sensitivity to Surprise. Potential mechanisms underlying these relationships are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
One of the foundational tenets of evolutionary psychology, the modular view of the mind, offers promising applications for clinical psychiatry. This perspective conceptualizes the mind as a collection of specialized information-processing modules, shaped by natural selection to address adaptive challenges faced by our ancestors. In this paper, we propose several points of integration between the modularity framework and clinical psychiatric practice. First, we argue that the descriptive psychopathology of self-disorders provides evidence supporting the modular view, demonstrating how a dysfunctional minimal self may expose the mind's modular architecture to conscious awareness. Next, we will explore how the modular perspective can illuminate the nature of intrapsychic conflicts. Finally, we will discuss how evidence from neuropsychiatric syndromes supports the modular view of the mind and, in turn, how this perspective can provide a basis for classifying mental disorders.
Article
Full-text available
The behavioral immune system can produce strong perceptual responses to pathogen-prevalent stimuli. This experiment was designed to examine whether the presence of pathogen-prevalent auditory stimuli would provide a greater distraction – and thus, cause decreased retention of information – compared to the presence of neutral (i.e., non-pathogen-related) auditory stimuli. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions – the control condition, the neutral sound condition, or the pathogen sound condition. Participants in the neutral sound and the pathogen sound conditions heard various distracting sounds during a lecture video and while answering quiz questions related to the lecture. The neutral sounds consisted of sounds that were not indicative of pathogen-prevalence – keys jingling, papers shuffling, and a backpack zipper being zipped. The pathogen sounds consisted of coughing and sniffling. Participants in the control condition did not hear any distracting sounds. Participants exposed to pathogen-prevalent distracting sounds performed significantly worse on the statistics quiz compared to the control group, though their performance was only marginally lower than that of participants in the neutral sound condition. While this suggests a potential effect of pathogen-related auditory distractions on cognitive performance, further research is needed to confirm and contextualize these findings.
Article
Full-text available
It is hypothesized that traits that are most likely to be the subject of social discourse (i.e., most communicable) are most likely to persist in ethnic stereotypes over time and that this effect is moderated by the extent to which an ethnic group is the subject of social discourse. Study 1 yielded communicability ratings of 76 traits. Study 2 tested the relation between a trait's communicability and its presence in stereotypes of 4 Canadian ethnic groups. Study 3 tested the relation between a trait's communicability and its persistence over time in stereotypes of 8 American ethnic groups. Results supported the hypotheses. A communication-based analysis of stereotypes appears helpful in predicting persistence and change in the contents of stereotypes of real groups in the real world.
Article
Full-text available
Although the individualism–collectivism dimension is usually examined in a U.S. versus Asian context, there is variation within the United States. The authors created an eight-item index ranking states in terms of collectivist versus individualist tendencies. As predicted, collectivist tendencies were strongest in the Deep South, and individualist tendencies were strongest in the Mountain West and Great Plains. In Part 2, convergent validity for the index was obtained by showing that state collectivism scores predicted variation in individual attitudes, as measured by a national survey. In Part 3, the index was used to explore the relationship between individualism–collectivism and a variety of demographic, economic, cultural, and health-related variables. The index may be used to complement traditional measures of collectivism and individualism and may be of use to scholars seeking a construct to account for unique U.S. regional variation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
This article explores how much memes like urban legends succeed on the basis of informational selection (i.e., truth or a moral lesson) and emotional selection (i.e., the ability to evoke emotions like anger, fear, or disgust). The article focuses on disgust because its elicitors have been precisely described. In Study 1, with controls for informational factors like truth, people were more willing to pass along stories that elicited stronger disgust. Study 2 randomly sampled legends and created versions that varied in disgust; people preferred to pass along versions that produced the highest level of disgust. Study 3 coded legends for specific story motifs that produce disgust (e.g., ingestion of a contaminated substance) and found that legends that contained more disgust motifs were distributed more widely on urban legend Web sites. The conclusion discusses implications of emotional selection for the social marketplace of ideas.
Article
Full-text available
Data analysis methods in psychology still emphasize statistical significance testing, despite numerous articles demonstrating its severe deficiencies. It is now possible to use meta-analysis to show that reliance on significance testing retards the development of cumulative knowledge. But reform of teaching and practice will also require that researchers learn that the benefits that they believe flow from use of significance testing are illusory. Teachers must revamp their courses to bring students to understand that (a) reliance on significance testing retards the growth of cumulative research knowledge; (b) benefits widely believed to flow from significance testing do not in fact exist; and (c) significance testing methods must be replaced with point estimates and confidence intervals in individual studies and with meta-analyses in the integration of multiple studies. This reform is essential to the future progress of cumulative knowledge in psychological research.
Article
Full-text available
The Big Five Inventory (BFI) is a self-report measure designed to assess the high-order personality traits of Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness. As part of the International Sexuality Description Project, the BFI was translated from English into 28 languages and administered to 17,837 individuals from 56 nations. The resulting cross-cultural data set was used to address three main questions: Does the factor structure of the English BFI fully replicate across cultures? How valid are the BFI trait profiles of individual nations? And how are personality traits distributed throughout the world? The five-dimensional structure was robust across major regions of the world. Trait levels were related in predictable ways to self-esteem, sociosexuality, and national personality profiles. People from the geographic regions of South America and East Asia were significantly different in open- ness from those inhabiting other world regions. The discussion focuses on limitations of the current data set and important directions for future research.
Article
Traditionally, much of the discipline of psychology has attempted to comprehend behavior as a function of stimuli impinging upon an individual. In recent years, the approach of ecological psychology has noted that the stimuli usually employed in psychology really represent only a very narrow range of all possible stimuli, and that they are excessively artificial in character; as a result, ecological psychology has emphasized the need to study behavior in more molar and naturalistic contexts. Similarly, an emerging cross-cultural psychology has argued that we should be attending to broad ranges of situations drawn from a cross section of cultures. It soon became clear, though, that sampling from new cultures also meant sampling from the new environmental contexts in which the cultures were situated. Thus, it became essential that the movement cross-culturally be accompanied by increased attention to the environmental settings of the cultures studied, a position similar to that espoused by ecological psychology.
Article
Humans have borrowed plants' chemical 'recipes' for evolutionary survival for use in cuisine to combat foodborne microorganisms and to reduce food poisoning.
Chapter
This chapter discusses the cultural ecology of social behavior. There appears to be a growing interest in relationships between social behavior and its sociocultural context, and in the cross-cultural generality of these relationships. The chapter attempts to illustrate how such studies are done (a methodological emphasis), what does such study reveals (a substantive emphasis), and finally, how such knowledge can be returned to the people (an applied emphasis). The chapter discusses an ecological-cultural-behavioral model. The model views the development of individual behavior as a function of membership in a cultural group; and cultural characteristics are viewed as a function of both the ecological setting of the group and the acculturative influences, which impinge upon the group. The chapter also focuses on social behaviors, charted cross-culturally, and on their group and individual distributions. The chapter attempts to show that both cultural and cross-cultural dimensions of social behavior are investigated. The aim of science is to make general statements, but the aim of social psychology is to make culturally universal statements about human social behavior.