ArticlePDF Available

Meditation promotes insightful problem-solving by keeping people in a mindful and alert conscious state

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Although previous studies have shown that sleep can inspire insight, it is still unclear whether meditation can promote insight. Meditation differs from other types of passive rest such as relaxation and sleep because it requires full consciousness and mindfulness of targets such as one's breathing. Forty-eight university students without meditation experience were recruited to learn a simple meditation technique. They were given a list of 10 insight problems to solve (the pre-test session). In this study, we focused on the unsolved problems and examined if they could be successfully solved after a 20 min rest interval with or without meditation. Results showed that relative to the control group that listened to Chinese or English words and made a language judgment, the groups who learned meditation successfully solved significantly more failed problems from the pre-test session, providing direct evidence for the role of meditation in promoting insight. Further analysis showed that maintaining a mindful and alert state during meditation (raising a hand to report every 10 deep breaths compared to every 100 deep breaths) resulted in more insight regarding the failed items from the pre-test session. This implies that it was watchfulness in meditation, rather than relaxation, that actually contributed to insight. Consistently, in the meditation session or control task, the percentage of alpha waves-a brain index of mental relaxation-was negatively correlated with insight. These results suggest a meditation-based insight-promoting mechanism different from that involved in passive rest such as relaxation and sleep.
Content may be subject to copyright.
SCIENCE CHINA
Life Sciences
© The Author(s) 2011. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com life.scichina.com www.springer.com/scp
*Corresponding author (email: renj@zjnu.cn; luoj@psycy.ac.cn)
RESEARCH PAPERS October 2011 Vol.54 No.10: 961–965
doi: 10.1007/s11427-011-4233-3
Meditation promotes insightful problem-solving by keeping people
in a mindful and alert conscious state
REN Jun1*, HUANG ZhiHui1, LUO Jing2*, WEI GaoXia2, YING XiaoPing3, DING ZhiGuang4,
WU YiBin5 & LUO Fei2
1College of Education, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua 321004, China;
2Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China;
3Institute of Sociology, Chinese Academy of Social Sciences, Beijing 100026, China;
4Department of Social Sciences, Hebei Medical University, Shijiazhuang 050015, China;
5Beijing Easy Monitor Technology Co., Ltd., Beijing 100101, China
Received December 23, 2010; accepted June 27, 2011
Although previous studies have shown that sleep can inspire insight, it is still unclear whether meditation can promote insight.
Meditation differs from other types of passive rest such as relaxation and sleep because it requires full consciousness and
mindfulness of targets such as one’s breathing. Forty-eight university students without meditation experience were recruited to
learn a simple meditation technique. They were given a list of 10 insight problems to solve (the pre-test session). In this study,
we focused on the unsolved problems and examined if they could be successfully solved after a 20 min rest interval with or
without meditation. Results showed that relative to the control group that listened to Chinese or English words and made a
language judgment, the groups who learned meditation successfully solved significantly more failed problems from the pre-test
session, providing direct evidence for the role of meditation in promoting insight. Further analysis showed that maintaining a
mindful and alert state during meditation (raising a hand to report every 10 deep breaths compared to every 100 deep breaths)
resulted in more insight regarding the failed items from the pre-test session. This implies that it was watchfulness in meditation,
rather than relaxation, that actually contributed to insight. Consistently, in the meditation session or control task, the percentage
of alpha waves—a brain index of mental relaxation—was negatively correlated with insight. These results suggest a medita-
tion-based insight-promoting mechanism different from that involved in passive rest such as relaxation and sleep.
meditation, insight, problem solving
Citation: Ren J, Huang Z H, Luo J, et al. Meditation promotes insightful problem-solving by keeping people in a mindful and alert conscious state. Sci China
Life Sci, 2011, 54: 961–965, doi: 10.1007/s11427-011-4233-3
It has been shown that sleep can significantly increase in-
sightful awareness, for example, discerning a hidden princi-
ple needed to solve the Number Reduction Task (NRT) [1].
However, it is still unclear whether meditation can promote
insight. This question deserves to be explored because med-
itation differs from routine forms of rest such as sleep and
relaxation in that one’s attention is actively focused on the
present state of consciousness [27]. It is widely believed in
Asian cultural traditions such as Buddhism and Taoism that
meditation can lead to insight.
This study recruited undergraduate students without any
previous meditation practice as participants. They were
taught a simple method of meditation and were then tested
to determine if application of meditation could promote
insight. Different from Wagner and colleagues’ experiment
that adopted the NRT [1], we used 10 typical insight prob-
lems [8]. We first instructed participants to try to solve the-
se problems one by one. We selected the unsolved problems
962 Ren J, et al. Sci China Life Sci October (2011) Vol.54 No.10
for each participant for later testing. Then, we required par-
ticipants to engage in either 20 min of meditation or a control
cognitive task. Finally, participants were unexpectedly
re-administered their unsolved problems from the first ses-
sion and asked to re-think and solve these problems. The key
hypothesis of this study was that participants who had learned
meditation would be more capable of insightfully solving the
unsolved problems.
To specify how meditation promotes insight, the effect of
meditation with different levels of alertness and awareness
was examined. These levels were manipulated through
changing the frequency of self-reported (by hand-raising)
number of deep breaths. During meditation, participants
were required to clear their minds, focus on deep breathing,
and count number of breaths. In the M10 condition, partici-
pants were required to raise their hand to report every 10
breaths. In the M100 condition, participants reported every
100 breaths by raising their hand. Both M10 and M100 met
the criterion of meditation, but relative to M100, M10 par-
ticipants stayed in a more alert and awake state because they
were required to frequently report their number of breaths.
During the meditation and control cognitive task period,
electroencephalograms of participants were recorded to
monitor and evaluate mind-brain states. Some EEG indices,
such as percentage of alpha waves, which indicate level of
mental relaxation, as well as the score of i22 related to at-
tention focus [9], were considered in obtaining cross-
domain evidence for the efficiency of our experimental ma-
nipulation and for suggesting possible mental mechanisms
related to how meditation promotes insight.
1 Materials and methods
1.1 Participants
Forty-eight healthy undergraduate students (23 males, mean
age 23.3 years) recruited from a university in Beijing par-
ticipated in the study. They were randomly assigned to three
equal groups with balanced male/female ratios: meditation
group M10 (reported every 10 deep breaths), meditation
group M100 (reported every 100 deep breaths), and the
control group. There were 16 participants in each group.
1.2 Materials
Fifteen insight problems that met the criterion of cognitive
reconstruction were used [8]. Five were used as examples in
the instruction and practice session, and 10 were used for
the formal experiment.
1.3 EEG recording and analysis
EEG data were collected by HXD-I EEG equipment
(HUAXIANG Technology Company, Harbin, Heilongjiang,
China). The collecting electrodes were located in the bilat-
eral prefrontal cortex, reference electrodes were A1 and A2,
and leading electrodes were located in the center of frontal
sites. The frequency was 200 Hz, the time window for
analysis was 1.25, the arithmetic stacking coefficient was 6,
the input impedance was 9 , the resolution of EEG was 3
MV, and the frequency band was 0.5–100 Hz. Wavelet
analysis, spectrum analysis, and pattern recognition were
applied in EEG analysis to extract the wavelet index. The
main indices included alpha waves, i-35 related to relaxa-
tion and i-22 related to alertness. The latter two indices
were developed based on our clinical and experimental tri-
als and can be used as a reference index for common EEG
data [9]. The EEG data recording for the last 15 min was
analyzed.
1.4 Procedure
1.4.1 Meditation and problem-solving training
Before the formal experimental session, participants were
taught Susoku meditation, which requires participants to
mindfully control and focus on their deep abdominal
breathing [1012]. In our pilot study, we found a cartoon
computer animation very helpful for beginners learning the
skill of deep abdominal breathing. Participants were asked
to attentively and slowly take a breath in or out as the “in-
spiration fish” or “expiration fish” gradually and smoothly
went up or down on the computer screen. Each round of
inspiration and expiration lasted 7 s. After participants
grasped the method of deep abdominal breathing, they were
further instructed to attentively take inspiration and expira-
tion breaths without the external aid of the computer anima-
tion (with eyes slightly closed). They were also instructed to
do their best to be mindfully aware and disengage from any
irrelevant thinking, free association, or feelings that might
incidentally occur. During meditation, participants were
asked to count the number of their breaths from 1 to 10 and
then restart (the M10 condition), or from 1 to 100 and then
restart (the M100 condition). They were also asked to raise
their hand to report finishing one round of breaths (i.e., 10
breaths in M10 or 100 breaths in M100). The above-
mentioned training procedures as well as subsequent prob-
lem solving and meditation practice were conducted with
participants individually. Participants in the control group
did not learn meditation. They rested in the waiting room
for the same period of time.
After meditation (the M10 and M100 group) or rest (the
control group), all three groups were given five examples of
insight problems for practice. Every problem was presented
on the screen for a maximum of 3 min. If participants
thought the problem was successfully solved, they were
instructed to press the “2” button as soon as possible and
then write down the solution. If participants could not solve
the problem, they could wait until the problem disappeared
from the screen at the end of the 3 min maximum, or press
the “3” key to stop the presentation and go to the next item.
Ren J, et al. Sci China Life Sci October (2011) Vol.54 No.10 963
It was emphasized that participants should not continue to
think about the unsolved problem once it disappeared from
the screen. Participants did not know they would be given
the unsolved problems later and would have to try to solve
them again.
1.4.2 Formal experimental session
The experimental session included the pre-test, the interval,
and the post-test.
(i) Pre-test. In the pre-test phase, participants were in-
structed to solve 10 insight problems appearing randomly
one by one on the screen using the same procedure as in the
practice session.
(ii) Meditation or control cognitive task interval. During
this interval, the M10 and M100 groups were instructed to
count number of breaths for 20 min, while the control group
was instructed to complete an auditory judgment task com-
prising English and Chinese words. The ratio of Chinese
words to English words was 1 to 6. Words appeared ran-
domly with an ITI of 5 s. Participants were instructed to
raise their hand when they heard the Chinese words and not
make any response when they heard the English words.
EEGs were recorded during meditation and the control cog-
nitive task. In view of the possible unstable state of partici-
pants, the EEG recording for the last 15 min was analyzed.
(iii) Post-test. After the meditation or control cognitive
task interval, participants were given the problems they had
failed in the pre-test session and were required to try to
solve these problems again. The procedure was the same as
in the pre-test session.
2 Results
2.1 Behavioral results
2.1.1 Successful problem-solving rate
Problem-solving rate included problem-solving rates in the
pre- and post-tests. It was defined as the number of suc-
cessfully solved items in the pre- or post-test divided by the
total number of items (total number of items in the post-test
was the 10 total items minus successfully solved items in
the pre-test). This step aimed to compare differences in
problem solving ability among these three groups. An
ANOVA showed that there was no significant difference
among the three groups in problem-solving rate for the
pre-test (P>0.05, Table 1), revealing that participants in the
Table 1 Problem-solving rate in the pre-testa)
Group Number of participants M SD SE
M10 16 0.49 0.15 0.04
M100 16 0.54 0.15 0.04
Ctrl 16 0.56 0.14 0.03
Total 48 0.53 0.15 0.02
a) The M10 group reported every 10 breaths, the M100 group reported
every 100 breaths, and Ctrl represents the control group.
three groups did not differ in their problem-solving ability.
Our study was mainly concerned with the problem-
solving rate in the post-test, which would suggest whether
the intervention (meditation) exerted an effect on problem
solving. Nonparametric statistics (Kruskal-Wallis H test)
were used because the data for problem-solving rate in the
post-test were abnormally distributed. The results showed a
significant difference among problem-solving rates in the
post-test (-square=11.414, df=2, P<0.005). Further analy-
sis with the Mann-Whitney U test showed that the prob-
lem-solving rate of the M10 group was higher than that of
the M100 group (P=0.051) and significantly higher than
that of the control group (P<0.005). The problem-solving
rate of the M100 group was higher than that of the control
group (P<0.05, Table 2). These results revealed that (i)
problem-solving ability of the two meditation groups was
better than that of the control group, and (ii) the perfor-
mance of the M10 group was better than that of the M100
group.
2.1.2 Response times (RTs)
First, we calculated the average response time (RT) for
problem solving (including successful and unsuccessful
tries) in the three conditions. Results showed that in the
pre-test, the RTs in the three conditions were not signifi-
cantly different and there was no interaction effect
(P=0.381). Using RTs in the pre-test as covariates in
the analysis, we found no significant difference or interac-
tion effect among the three groups at post-test (P=0.792,
Table 3).
Then, we compared RTs for successfully solved prob-
lems and abandoned (failed) problems among the three
conditions. Results showed that the three groups did not
differ in RTs and showed no interaction effect in the pre-test
for both kinds of problems (P=0.325 for solved problems,
P=0.830 for abandoned problems). With RTs for success-
fully solved problems or abandoned (failed) problems in the
pre-test as covariates, the analysis revealed no significant
difference or interaction effect at post-test (P=0.378 for
successfully solved problems, P=0.945 for abandoned
problems; Tables 4 and 5) among the three conditions.
2.2 EEG results
In this study, we examined the percentage of alpha waves in
Table 2 Problem-solving rate in the post-testa)
Group comparisons
M10-M100 M10-Ctrl
M100-Ctrl
Mann-Whitney U 77.50 50.00 70.50
Wilcoxon W 213.50 186.00 206.50
Z 1.92 3.01 2.23
a) The M10 group reported every 10 breaths, the M100 group reported
every 100 breaths, and Ctrl represents the control group.
964 Ren J, et al. Sci China Life Sci October (2011) Vol.54 No.10
Table 3 RTs in the pre-test and post-test
Group Number of participants M (ms) SD
Pre-test M10 16 71185.43 16873.03
M100 16 76823.79 17143.16
Ctrl 16 81040.91 24739.61
Total 48 76350.04 19915.76
Post-test M10 16 99166.95 34191.55
M100 16 101111.43 33481.85
Ctrl 16 96973.40 40199.79
Total 48 99083.93 35349.00
Table 4 RTs of successfully solved problems in the pre-test and post-testa)
Group Number of participants M (ms) SD
Pre-test M10 16 56369.91 18393.58
M100 16 61182.66 15213.48
Ctrl 16 67644.36 31260.17
Total 48 61732.31 22704.73
Post-test M10 15 78005.15 36924.43
M100 15 67062.57 41478.93
Ctrl 6 59237.92 38748.66
Total 36 70317.87 38716.58
a) Participants who did not solve any problems correctly were elimi-
nated from the post-test and were regarded as missing values.
Table 5 RTs of the abandoned (failed) problemsa)
Group Number of participants M (ms) SD
Pre-test M10 16 90526.28 24606.78
M100 13 94610.68 25597.84
Ctrl 13 96884.51 34869.44
Total 42 93758.52 27867.09
Post-test M10 9 102690.22 41354.98
M100 10 104566.24 44002.92
Ctrl 13 109053.26 43440.90
Total 32 105861.46 41733.89
a) Participants who solved all problems correctly were eliminated from
the pre- and post-tests and were regarded as missing values.
the three conditions. A higher percentage of alpha waves
have been associated with a more relaxed mental state. A
one-way within-groups ANOVA showed a significant dif-
ference in the percentage of alpha waves among the three
groups (F(2, 45)=4.144, P<0.05). Post-hoc testing showed
that the percentage of alpha waves in the M10 group was
significantly lower than in the control group (P<0.05) and
showed a decrease in tendency relative to that of the M100
group (P=0.098). There was no difference in the percentage
of alpha waves between the M10 group and control group
(Table 6). This indicated that the M10 group stayed more
alert during the meditation interval session. Product-
moment correlation analysis across all participants in the
three groups revealed that percentage of alpha waves was
negatively correlated with problem-solving rate in the
post-test (r=0.471, P=0.001).
We also analyzed i-35 and i-22, which are shown to be
related to states of attention and general relaxation, respec-
tively. Results showed there was a significant difference in
i-35 among the three groups (F(2, 45)=6.516, P<0.005).
Post-hoc testing revealed that the M10 group had higher
i-35 than the M100 group (P<0.05) and control group
(P=0.073), but no significant difference existed between the
M100 group and the control group. This again indicated that
the latter two groups were more relaxed than the M10 group.
As for i-22, the results showed differences among the three
groups (F(2, 45)=3.377, P<0.05). Post-hoc testing showed
that the M10 group showed a lower i-22 tendency than the
M100 group (P=0.084) and control group (P=0.093). How-
ever, there was no difference between the M100 group and
control group. This implied that participants in the M10
group were more mentally focused than the M100 and con-
trol group. The correlation analysis showed that i-35 was
not correlated with problem-solving rate in the post-test,
and i-22 was negatively correlated with problem-solving
rate in the post-test (r=.403, P<0.005). These results in-
dicated that a more alert and focused mind in the meditation
or control cognitive task interval may have been associated
with better problem-solving performance in the post-test.
3 Discussion
Our results showed that after 20 min of meditation or a con-
trol cognitive task, participants were able to solve some of
the problems they had failed in the pre-test. This is general-
ly in line with previous research on the role of incubation.
However, participants who engaged in meditation solved
more previously unsolved problems compared to partici-
pants in the control condition, thereby providing direct evi-
dence for the role of meditation in promoting insight.
The following considerations excluded, or partially ex-
cluded, the possibility that the observed differences between
M10, M100, and Ctrl conditions were caused by other un-
related factors. First, in the initial problem solving session
(pre-test), there were no significant differences in solution
rate and RTs among the three groups, thus excluding the
possibility that the observed key differences were caused by
differences in problem solving ability in the groups. Second,
in the final problem solving session (post-test) after medita-
tion or the control cognitive task, there was no detectable
difference in RTs for problem solving among the three
groups, thus excluding the possibility that the observed dif-
ferences were caused by participants in the M10 and M100
groups having learned to be more patient and insistent on
solving the problems after meditation. Further, we also cal-
culated the RTs for the successfully solved items and failed
items for the three groups. We found that in neither the ini-
tial session nor the final session was there any detectable dif-
ference among the groups. This excluded the possibility that
the better performance of the M10 and M100 groups was
related to the increase of participants’ meta-cognitive ability
in identifying the solvable and unsolvable items, thus ena-
Ren J, et al. Sci China Life Sci October (2011) Vol.54 No.10 965
bling them to wisely spend more time on the solvable items.
Rather, it is reasonable to propose that the increased in-
sight ability brought about by meditation may be related to
the fact that the participants had maintained a more alert and
mindful state of consciousness. First of all, participants in
M10, relative to M100, showed a stronger tendency for in-
sight. Although the difference was not significant, it ap-
proached significance (P=0.051). Secondly, there was a
significant negative correlation between insightful problem
solving in the final session and the percentage of alpha
waves known to be closely related to mental relaxation.
This implies that the more relaxed the participants were in
the meditation or control cognitive task interval, the less
insight they achieved later. Thirdly, the percentage of alpha
waves in M10 was not only significantly lower than in the
Ctrl condition, but also lower than that in M100 (but did not
reach significance, P=0.098). Taken together, these findings
imply that meditation promoted insight by keeping people
in an alert and mindful state. This appears to be different
from the possible mechanism underlying promotion of in-
sight during a passive type of relaxation, like sleep, that
does not require any mental effort.
This work was supported by the Education Program of the National Social
Science Foundation of China (Grant No. BBA100016), the National Basic
Research Program of China (Grant No. 2010CB833904), the National
Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Nos. 30970890 and
30770708), and the National High Technology Research and Development
Program of China (Grant No. 2008AA022604).
1 Wagner U, Gais S, Haider H, et al. Sleep inspires insight. Nature,
2004, 427: 352–355
2 Tang Y Y, Lu Q, Geng X, et al. Short-term meditation induces white
matter changes in the anterior cingulate. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA,
2010, 107: 15649–15652
3 Tang Y Y, Ma Y, Fan Y, et al. Central and autonomic nervous sys-
tem interaction is altered by short-term meditation. Proc Natl Acad
Sci USA, 2009, 106: 8865–8870
4 Tang Y Y, Ma Y, Wang J, et al. Short-term meditation training im-
proves attention and self-regulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 2007,
104: 17152–17156
5 Slagter H A, Lutz A, Greischar L L, et al. Mental training affects dis-
tribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biol, 2007, 5: e138
6 Lutz A, Slagter H A, Rawlings N B, et al. Mental training enhances
attentional stability: neural and behavioral evidence. J Neurosci, 2009,
29: 13418–13427
7 Lutz A, Slagter H A, Dunne J D, et al. Attention regulation and mon-
itoring in meditation. Trends Cogn Sci, 2008, 12: 163–169
8 Weisberg R W. Prolegomena to theories of insight in problem solv-
ing: A taxonomy of problems. In: Sternberg R J, Davidson J E, eds.
The Psychology of Problem Solving. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press, 1995. 157–196
9 Luo F, Luo J, Wu Y B, et al. Psychosomatic recovery by relaxing
and rebuilding the brain: Possible application in post traumatic psy-
chological recovery (in Chinese). Adv Psychol Sci, 2009, 17: 594–
601
10 Takahashi T, Murata T, Hamada T, et al. Changes in EEG and auto-
nomic nervous activity during meditation and their association with
personality traits. Int J Psychophysiol, 2005, 55: 199–207
11 Murata T, Takahashi T, Hamada T, et al. Individual trait anxiety lev-
els characterizing the properties of Zen meditation. Neuropsychobi-
ology, 2004, 50: 189–194
12 Kubota Y, Sato W, Toichi M, et al. Frontal midline theta rhythm is
correlated with cardiac autonomic activities during the performance
of an attention demanding meditation procedure. Brain Res Cogn
Brain Res, 2001, 11: 281–287
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction
in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
... Solving problems involves cognitive and behavioral domains encompassing advanced cognitive skills, such as ascertaining, selecting, and formulating efficacious resolutions [8]. Ren et al. [9] found that individuals who participated in meditation practice demonstrated an increased ability to solve previously unresolved problems compared to those in the control condition. ...
... The literature suggests that mindfulness has the potential to enhance problemsolving abilities, particularly in resolving insight problems [9,11]. Additionally, mindfulness has been found to aid in identifying innovative solutions to given problems [12]. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Conflict can lead to lower employee productivity and job satisfaction. By incorporating mindfulness practices into the workplace, organizations create an environment where employees can respond thoughtfully and promote a harmonious and cooperative atmosphere. Mindfulness is the act of paying attention in a particular way: on purpose, in the present moment, and non-judgmentally. It enables us to observe our thoughts, emotions, and sensations without being carried away by them. The benefits of mindfulness in the workplace are enormous. It decreases stress levels and turnover rates, improves productivity, and fosters innovation. Mindfulness improves negotiation, problem-solving, and decision-making. It also promotes a culture of respect, compassion, and empathy. It allows individuals to recognize and regulates harmful behaviors. Mindful leadership enhances feedback loops and communication channels. Studies show a positive correlation between leader mindfulness and employee well-being and performance. Clear roles, expectations, and conflict resolution skills that promote collaboration and discourage competition are some of its benefits. Some companies, such as Google, have successfully implemented mindfulness programs to reduce conflict and improve employee well-being, performance, and creativity. Mindfulness allows for better empathy, understanding, and open communication during conflicts. Open communication and active listening foster trust and prevent misunderstandings during conflict situations.
... During a long run, one should have slow, long and steady breathing to support a healthy mind [34]. Yoga practices can affect the nervous system differently, creating stimulating effects (chest/thoracic breath) or calming effects (abdominal breath) [35][36][37][38]. Proper breathing helps to boost energy levels by oxygenating the blood and thus all organs, tissues and cells [7]. ...
... A growing number of research reveal the benefits of mindfulness intervention in promoting emotional regulation capacity and selfawareness when working with children (D'Alessandro et al., 2022). One significant explanation is that mindfulness practice can improve the ability to pay attention, which facilitates executive functions such as cognitive flexibility and reasoning to promote insightful thinking (Ren et al., 2011). Such capacity is crucial for children and adolescents to process their adverse experiences and manage the associated strong emotions in a tolerable state. ...
Article
Full-text available
Formulating interventions when working with children from a wide age range who have experienced familial adversity with diverse developmental needs present significant challenges that warrant investigation. The MAP framework, which was established by a support service in Hong Kong, encompasses guidelines for both therapeutic goals and strategies to facilitate focus and clarity in the therapeutic work based on literature review, critical reflection, and feedback from participants. As therapeutic goals, M stands for mindful awareness and management of emotions, A denotes attachment building, and P refers to positive self-worth. In working toward intervention strategies, M represents mindfulness practice, and A and P refer to art and play mediums, respectively. Case examples are included to illustrate the application of the model.
... The popularity of meditation in the Western world over the past 40 years, building on centuries and millennia of practice across different cultures, has led to extensive research on its physiological and behavioural effects, including creative performance (Murphy et al., 2004). Previous studies have explored the relationship between creativity and meditation, and research on the link between creative performance and meditation has shown that overall creative performance is improved after meditation training (Ball, 1980;Colzato et al., 2012;Ding et al., 2011;Ding et al., 2014a;Ding et al., 2014b;Ren et al., 2011;Strick et al., 2012), but the reverse has also occurred (O'Haire & Marcia, 1980;Otis, 1974). Another study taking the TTCT-FormB test to measure creative thinking skills found an unsupported relationship between three years of meditation experience and creative performance. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Creativity is long regarded as one of the fundamental traits that indicates design capability. The concept of creativity encompasses the capacity to produce innovative and novel concepts or ideas, to devise or articulate imagination and intellect, and to stimulate the potential of imagination and ingenuity embodying their capacity to conceive, craft, and develop innovative ideas for products. Previous studies have revealed the connection and functionality between meditation and creativity. However, general creativity measurements, which studies to date have mainly focused on, might not be able to demonstrate the performance of designers in a creative process adequately.Therefore, this study applied a design task-based evaluation with traditional TTCT assessment, which might be more suitable to describe the creative performance of novice designers. The study aims to explore: (1) the relationship between short-term meditation and creativity; (2) the effects of short-term meditation on the design qualities in design tasks of novice designers.42 first-year design students were recruited and were divided into meditation group (n=24) and control group (n=18). Participants conducted a demographic survey and the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT, in its figural variant) firstly. The meditation group was then given a 110-minute audio tape-based meditation intervention, and the control group was given a 110-minute audio tape intervention, which is a recording of scientific articles. Both interventions were performed twice a week on the weekends. After that, TTCT was given to the participants again. Then each participant received an interview on the changes in mood state and creativity. After the TTCT on the second day, the participants completed the Design with Morphological Table Task (DwMT) to assess their design qualities in a product design task. Through data analysis, TTCT results indicate that short-term meditation can significantly improve the creativity of novice designers and the meditation group outperformed the control group significantly in fluency and elaboration. DwMT results show that the meditation group significantly provided better design qualities than the control group.
... The quantitative results revealed a statistically significant improvement in verbal insight problem-solving performance for the experimental group after undergoing the mindfulness-based training program. This finding is consistent with a growing body of research that suggests a positive relationship between mindfulness and insight problem-solving (e.g., Ostafin & Kassman, 2012;Ren et al., 2011) The enhanced ability to solve verbal insight problems among participants who received mindfulness training may be attributed to several potential mechanisms. Mindfulness cultivates a state of open, non-judgmental awareness and attentional control, which can facilitate cognitive flexibility and the ability to disengage from habitual thought patterns (Brown & Ryan, 2003;Chambers et al., 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
The ability to effectively solve problems is a critical skill for success in academic and professional domains. This study investigated the impact of mindfulness training on problem-solving abilities among university students, with a focus on verbal insight problems and social problems. Sixty male students from the Information Technology Department at Al-Azhar University participated in the study, divided into an experimental group (n=30) that received an 8-week mindfulness-based training program and a control group (n=30). Quantitative measures assessed performance on a researcher-developed verbal insight problem test and the Social Problem-Solving Inventory-Revised (SPSI-R) before and after the intervention. Qualitative data was collected through interviews and focus groups exploring participants' subjective experiences. Results showed a statistically significant improvement in verbal insight problem-solving for the experimental group compared to controls after mindfulness training (p<0.01). However, no significant differences emerged on social problem-solving measured by the SPSI-R. Qualitative findings suggested participants perceived benefits of mindfulness for emotional awareness, cognitive flexibility, and non-judgmental attitudes, which may facilitate insight problem-solving. Originality/value: This mixed-methods study provides empirical support for integrating mindfulness-based interventions in academic programs to enhance cognitive functioning and creative problem-solving capacities among students.
... It is a secular experience that enhances the senses and generates an added appreciation of landscape, culture, and the richness of human achievement, where people start to escape from their daily routine and stress to look for a journey with the goal of preserving or promoting their well-being, staying in a location specifically created to facilitate and improve body, mind, and spirit (Smith and Puczko, 2013). Ren et al. (2011) stated that Meditation-based tourism services are special products in the wellness sub segment of tourism. According to Chen et al. (2013), mediation tourism is a travelling process intended or aimed towards promoting body, mind and well-being. ...
... Previous studies have described the impact of mindfulness on psychological or EEG-related outcomes in students (Galante et al. 2018;Kuyken et al. 2013;Morais et al. 2021;Ren et al. 2011;Sanger et al. 2018). However, none of neuroscientific studies have explored whether and how brief mindfulness training influences certain psychological features especially social support in college students during the COVID-19 pandemic. ...
Article
Full-text available
The effects of short-term mindfulness are associated with the different patterns (autonomic, audio guided, or experienced and certified mindfulness instructor guided mindfulness). However, robust evidence for reported the impacts of different patterns of mindfulness on mental health and EEG biomarkers of undergraduates is currently lacking. Therefore, we aimed to test the hypotheses that mindfulness training for undergraduates would improve mental health, and increase alpha power over frontal region and theta power over midline region at the single electrode level. We also describe the distinction among frequency bands patterns in different sites of frontal and midline regions. 70 participants were enrolled and assigned to either 5-day mindfulness or a waiting list group. Subjective questionnaires measured mental health and other psychological indicators, and brain activity was recorded during various EEG tasks before and after the intervention. The 5-day mindful-ness training improved trait mindfulness, especially observing (p = 0.001, d = 0.96) and nonreactivity (p = 0.03, d = 0.56), sleep quality (p = 0.001, d = 0.91), and social support (p = 0.001, d = 0.95) while not in affect. Meanwhile, the expected increase in the alpha power of frontal sites (p < 0.017, d > 0.84) at the single electrode level was confirmed by the current data rather than the theta. Interestingly, the alteration of low-beta power over the single electrode of the midline (p < 0.05, d > 0.71) was difference between groups. Short-term mindfulness improves practitioners' mental health, and the potentially electrophysiological biomarkers of mindfulness on neuron oscillations were alpha activity over frontal sites and low-beta activity over midline sites.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to compare the effectiveness of social-emotional cooperative learning and mindfulness training on improving social problem solving skills of students in Zanjan who were studying in the 96-95. The research method was a quasi-experimental design with a pre-test and post-test with an experimental group and control group and a one-month follow-up. The statistical population included all female students in the first grade of high school in the academic year 1395-96. 339 students were selected by multi-stage cluster sampling and completed a short form of social problem solving questionnaire and among them 45 subjects were selected and randomly assigned to the two experimental groups and one control group. The first experimental group received 12 sessions of social-emotional cooperative learning and Group II received 8 sessions of mindfulness training. The statistical method of Two-way Mixed Design (ANOVA) was used in order to analyze the data. The results indicate that implementation of social-emotional cooperative learning and mindfulness training increased the positive problem attitude scores and reduced the negative problem attitude, the avoidance style, and impulsivity-carelessness style scores but they did not significantly change in rational problem solving style. However, there was no significant difference between the two independent variables. The social-emotional cooperative learning compared with the control group, resulted in a significant decrease in mean, but mindfulness training did not affect it. The follow-up results indicate that their effect had remained over time.
Article
Full-text available
The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is part of a network implicated in the development of self-regulation and whose connectivity changes dramatically in development. In previous studies we showed that 3 h of mental training, based on traditional Chinese medicine (integrative body-mind training, IBMT), increases ACC activity and improves self-regulation. However, it is not known whether changes in white matter connectivity can result from small amounts of mental training. We here report that 11 h of IBMT increases fractional anisotropy (FA), an index indicating the integrity and efficiency of white matter in the corona radiata, an important white-matter tract connecting the ACC to other structures. Thus IBMT could provide a means for improving self-regulation and perhaps reducing or preventing various mental disorders.
Article
Full-text available
The capacity to stabilize the content of attention over time varies among individuals, and its impairment is a hallmark of several mental illnesses. Impairments in sustained attention in patients with attention disorders have been associated with increased trial-to-trial variability in reaction time and event-related potential deficits during attention tasks. At present, it is unclear whether the ability to sustain attention and its underlying brain circuitry are transformable through training. Here, we show, with dichotic listening task performance and electroencephalography, that training attention, as cultivated by meditation, can improve the ability to sustain attention. Three months of intensive meditation training reduced variability in attentional processing of target tones, as indicated by both enhanced theta-band phase consistency of oscillatory neural responses over anterior brain areas and reduced reaction time variability. Furthermore, those individuals who showed the greatest increase in neural response consistency showed the largest decrease in behavioral response variability. Notably, we also observed reduced variability in neural processing, in particular in low-frequency bands, regardless of whether the deviant tone was attended or unattended. Focused attention meditation may thus affect both distracter and target processing, perhaps by enhancing entrainment of neuronal oscillations to sensory input rhythms, a mechanism important for controlling the content of attention. These novel findings highlight the mechanisms underlying focused attention meditation and support the notion that mental training can significantly affect attention and brain function.
Article
Full-text available
Five days of integrative body-mind training (IBMT) improves attention and self-regulation in comparison with the same amount of relaxation training. This paper explores the underlying mechanisms of this finding. We measured the physiological and brain changes at rest before, during, and after 5 days of IBMT and relaxation training. During and after training, the IBMT group showed significantly better physiological reactions in heart rate, respiratory amplitude and rate, and skin conductance response (SCR) than the relaxation control. Differences in heart rate variability (HRV) and EEG power suggested greater involvement of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) in the IBMT group during and after training. Imaging data demonstrated stronger subgenual and adjacent ventral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) activity in the IBMT group. Frontal midline ACC theta was correlated with high-frequency HRV, suggesting control by the ACC over parasympathetic activity. These results indicate that after 5 days of training, the IBMT group shows better regulation of the ANS by a ventral midfrontal brain system than does the relaxation group. This changed state probably reflects training in the coordination of body and mind given in the IBMT but not in the control group. These results could be useful in the design of further specific interventions.
Article
Full-text available
The information processing capacity of the human mind is limited, as is evidenced by the so-called "attentional-blink" deficit: When two targets (T1 and T2) embedded in a rapid stream of events are presented in close temporal proximity, the second target is often not seen. This deficit is believed to result from competition between the two targets for limited attentional resources. Here we show, using performance in an attentional-blink task and scalp-recorded brain potentials, that meditation, or mental training, affects the distribution of limited brain resources. Three months of intensive mental training resulted in a smaller attentional blink and reduced brain-resource allocation to the first target, as reflected by a smaller T1-elicited P3b, a brain-potential index of resource allocation. Furthermore, those individuals that showed the largest decrease in brain-resource allocation to T1 generally showed the greatest reduction in attentional-blink size. These observations provide novel support for the view that the ability to accurately identify T2 depends upon the efficient deployment of resources to T1. The results also demonstrate that mental training can result in increased control over the distribution of limited brain resources. Our study supports the idea that plasticity in brain and mental function exists throughout life and illustrates the usefulness of systematic mental training in the study of the human mind.
Chapter
The Nature of Insight brings together diverse perspectives, including recent theories and discoveries, to examine the nature and origins of insightful thinking, as well as the history of theory and research on the topic and the methods used to study it. There are chapters by the leading experts in this field, including Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi, Ronald Finke, Howard Gruber, Marcel Just, David Meyer, David Perkins, Dean Simonton, and Robert Weisberg, among others. The Nature of Insight is divided into five main parts. Following an introduction that reviews the history and methods of the field, part II looks at how people solve challenging puzzles whose answers cannot be obtained through ordinary means. Part III focuses on how people come up with ideas for new inventions, while part IV explores the thinking of some of the most insightful people in the history of civilization. Part V considers metaphors such as evolution and investment as bases for understanding insight. An epilogue integrates all these approaches. Contributors R.E. Mayer, R.L. Dominowsk, P. Dallob, C.M. Seifert, D.E. Meyer, N. Davidson, A.J. Patalano, I. Yaniv, J.E. Davidson, R.W. Weisberg, M.L. Gick, R.S. Lockhart, S.M. Smith, R.A. Finke, M.I. Isaak, M.A. Just, M. Csikszentmihalyi, K. Sawyer, K. Dunbar, H.E. Gruber, M.F. Ippolito, R.D. Tweney, D.K. Simonton, D.N. Perkins, R.J. Sternberg, T.I. Lubart Bradford Books imprint
Article
Frontal midline theta rhythm (Fm theta), recognized as distinct theta activity on EEG in the frontal midline area, reflects mental concentration as well as meditative state or relief from anxiety. Attentional network in anterior frontal lobes including anterior cingulate cortex is suspected to be the generator of this activity, and the regulative function of the frontal neural network over autonomic nervous system (ANS) during cognitive process is suggested. However no studies have examined peripheral autonomic activities during Fm theta induction, and interaction of central and peripheral mechanism associated with Fm theta remains unclear. In the present study, a standard procedure of Zen meditation requiring sustained attention and breath control was employed as the task to provoke Fm theta, and simultaneous EEG and ECG recordings were performed. For the subjects in which Fm theta activities were provoked (six men, six women, 48% of the total subjects), peripheral autonomic activities were evaluated during the appearance of Fm theta as well as during control periods. Successive inter-beat intervals were measured from the ECG, and a recently developed method of analysis by Toichi et al. (J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 62 (1997) 79-84) based on heart rate variability was used to assess cardiac sympathetic and parasympathetic functions separately. Both sympathetic and parasympathetic indices were increased during the appearance of Fm theta compared with control periods. Theta band activities in the frontal area were correlated negatively with sympathetic activation. The results suggest a close relationship between cardiac autonomic function and activity of medial frontal neural circuitry.
Article
Insight denotes a mental restructuring that leads to a sudden gain of explicit knowledge allowing qualitatively changed behaviour. Anecdotal reports on scientific discovery suggest that pivotal insights can be gained through sleep. Sleep consolidates recent memories and, concomitantly, could allow insight by changing their representational structure. Here we show a facilitating role of sleep in a process of insight. Subjects performed a cognitive task requiring the learning of stimulus-response sequences, in which they improved gradually by increasing response speed across task blocks. However, they could also improve abruptly after gaining insight into a hidden abstract rule underlying all sequences. Initial training establishing a task representation was followed by 8 h of nocturnal sleep, nocturnal wakefulness, or daytime wakefulness. At subsequent retesting, more than twice as many subjects gained insight into the hidden rule after sleep as after wakefulness, regardless of time of day. Sleep did not enhance insight in the absence of initial training. A characteristic antecedent of sleep-related insight was revealed in a slowing of reaction times across sleep. We conclude that sleep, by restructuring new memory representations, facilitates extraction of explicit knowledge and insightful behaviour.
Article
Meditation is a specific consciousness state in which deep relaxation and increased internalized attention coexist. There have been various neurophysiological studies on meditation. However, the personal predispositions/traits that characterize the properties of meditation have not been adequately studied. We analyzed changes in neurophysiological parameters [EEG coherence and autonomic nervous activity using heart rate variability (HRV) as an index] during Zen meditation, and evaluated the results in association with trait anxiety (assessed by Spielberger's State-Trait Anxiety Inventory) in 22 healthy adults who had not previously practiced any form of meditation. During meditation, in terms of mean values in all subjects, an increase in slow alpha interhemispheric EEG coherence in the frontal region, an increase in high-frequency (HF) power (as a parasympathetic index of HRV), and a decrease in the ratio of low-frequency to HF power (as a sympathetic index of HRV) were observed. Further evaluation of these changes in individuals showed a negative correlation between the percent change (with the control condition as the baseline) in slow alpha interhemispheric coherence reflecting internalized attention and the percent change in HF reflecting relaxation. The trait anxiety score was negatively correlated with the percent change in slow alpha interhemispheric coherence in the frontal region and was positively correlated with the percent change in HF. These results suggest that lower trait anxiety more readily induces meditation with a predominance of internalized attention, while higher trait anxiety more readily induces meditation with a predominance of relaxation.
Article
Meditation is the attainment of a restful yet fully alert physical and mental state practiced by many as a self-regulatory approach to emotion management, but the psychophysiological properties and personality traits that characterize this meditative state have not been adequately studied. We quantitatively analyzed changes in psychophysiological parameters during Zen meditation in 20 normal adults, and evaluated the results in association with personality traits assessed by Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI). During meditation, increases were observed in fast theta power and slow alpha power on EEG predominantly in the frontal area, whereas an increase in the normalized unit of high-frequency (nuHF) power (as a parasympathetic index) and decreases in the normalized unit of low-frequency (nuLF) power and LF/HF (as sympathetic indices) were observed through analyses of heart rate variability. We analyzed the possible correlations among these changes in terms of the percent change during meditation using the control condition as the baseline. The percent change in slow alpha EEG power in the frontal area, reflecting enhanced internalized attention, was negatively correlated with that in nuLF as well as in LF/HF and was positively correlated with the novelty seeking score (which has been suggested to be associated with dopaminergic activity). The percent change in fast theta power in the frontal area, reflecting enhanced mindfulness, was positively correlated with that in nuHF and also with the harm avoidance score (which has been suggested to be associated with serotonergic activity). These results suggest that internalized attention and mindfulness as two major core factors of behaviors of mind during meditation are characterized by different combinations of psychophysiological properties and personality traits.