ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Lately, Emery et al. developed a bird-specific modification of the "social brain hypothesis", termed "relationship intelligence hypothesis". Although the idea may be valuable, we doubt that it is supported by sufficient evidence and critically discuss some of the arguments raised by the authors in favour of their new idea.
A preview of the PDF is not available
... For example, certain affiliative tactile behaviours, such as feeding or grooming others, are often used as indicators of close bonds between individuals and are expressed similarly in altricial and precocial mammals [75], but are, in contrast to altricial birds, uncommon or absent in many precocial birds [76]. However, both altricial and precocial species express social bonds in a variety of other ways, including vocal and visual displays ( [76][77][78][79][80][81] for a mammalian review) and chemical [82] cues, increased tolerance and spatial proximity [83][84][85]. In particular, the spatial association between individuals is often used as a proxy for determining social relationships ( [86][87][88], but see [89]). ...
... In sum, both altricial and precocial birds and mammals resort to a large variety of displaying affiliative bonds. The lack of any one of these above indicators of social bonds, however, does not necessarily infer weak and/or low quality affiliative relationships between precocial or altricial mammals or birds, since other forms of expressing relationships may be in place [85]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Various types of long-term stable relationships that individuals uphold, including cooperation and competition between group members, define social complexity in vertebrates. Numerous life history, physiological and cognitive traits have been shown to affect, or to be affected by, such social relationships. As such, differences in developmental modes, i.e. the 'altricial-precocial' spectrum, may play an important role in understanding the interspecific variation in occurrence of social interactions, but to what extent this is the case is unclear because the role of the developmental mode has not been studied directly in across-species studies of sociality. In other words, although there are studies on the effects of developmental mode on brain size, on the effects of brain size on cognition, and on the effects of cognition on social complexity, there are no studies directly investigating the link between developmental mode and social complexity. This is surprising because developmental differences play a significant role in the evolution of, for example, brain size, which is in turn considered an essential building block with respect to social complexity. Here, we compiled an overview of studies on various aspects of the complexity of social systems in altricial and precocial mammals and birds. Although systematic studies are scarce and do not allow for a quantitative comparison, we show that several forms of social relationships and cognitive abilities occur in species along the entire developmental spectrum. Based on the existing evidence it seems that differences in developmental modes play a minor role in whether or not individuals or species are able to meet the cognitive capabilities and requirements for maintaining complex social relationships. Given the scarcity of comparative studies and potential subtle differences, however, we suggest that future studies should consider developmental differences to determine whether our finding is general or whether some of the vast variation in social complexity across species can be explained by developmental mode. This would allow a more detailed assessment of the relative importance of developmental mode in the evolution of vertebrate social systems.
... Hence, it has been argued that the social cognition of corvids is driven by challenges associated with long-term monogamous partnership rather than with conspecifics per se (relationship intelligence, Emery et al. 2007). Such an argument can be put forward not only for corvids but for monogamous species in general (Scheiber et al. 2008) and is supported by measures of relative brain sizes (Dunbar and Shultz 2007); it has received limited empirical testing on the behavioral side, though. While I acknowledge the idea of pair partners being key in understanding corvid cognition, possibly in contrast to neighbors/out-group members (compare Ashton et al. 2020), I further argue that the non-breeder state represents an additional source of social complexity. ...
Article
Full-text available
Ravens and other corvids are renowned for their ‘intelligence’. For long, this reputation has been based primarily on anecdotes but in the last decades experimental evidence for impressive cognitive skills has accumulated within and across species. While we begin to understand the building blocks of corvid cognition, the question remains why these birds have evolved such skills. Focusing on Northern Ravens Corvus corax , I here try to tackle this question by relating current hypotheses on brain evolution to recent empirical data on challenges faced in the birds’ daily life. Results show that foraging ravens meet several assumptions for applying social intelligence: (1) they meet repeatedly at foraging sites, albeit individuals have different site preferences and vary in grouping dynamics; (1) foraging groups are structured by dominance rank hierarchies and social bonds; (3) individual ravens memorize former group members and their relationship valence over years, deduce third-party relationships and use their social knowledge in daily life by supporting others in conflicts and intervening in others’ affiliations. Hence, ravens’ socio-cognitive skills may be strongly shaped by the ‘complex’ social environment experienced as non-breeders.
... Our findings add to considerable previous evidence of relatively sophisticated cognitive abilities in Greylag Geese. Cognitive traits that facilitate recognition of relative social category and dominance rank of group members would be advantageous to maximize the benefits of targeted alliance formation (Frigerio et al. 2003;Scheiber et al. 2005Scheiber et al. , 2008Scheiber et al. , 2009a and reduce the costs of conflict (Weiß and Kotrschal 2004;Weiß et al. 2008). For example, Greylag Geese can make judgments about relationships using transitive inference . ...
Article
Full-text available
Cues to individuality, and the corresponding capacity for individual-level discrimination, can allow individually specific investment by conspecifics into offspring, partners, neighbors or competitors. Here we investigated possible cues to individuality via faces in an ancient avian lineage, the Greylag Goose ( Anser anser ). Konrad Lorenz could famously name each Greylag Goose in ‘his’ flock from a photograph. Confirming this anecdotal observation, we developed facial recognition software that can reliably (~ 97% accuracy) assign a goose face to a goose ID within a database, using bill morphology normalized during photo preparation. To explore conspecific detection of individuality cues, we erected life-size photos of geese and measured subjects’ responses to photos of themselves (unfamiliar goose), their partner, and another flock mate. Geese displayed significantly greater affiliative response to photos of their partners, providing evidence that geese can use two-dimensional images as cues to determine social category (partner/non-partner) and/or individual-level recognition. Our methods provide novel approaches to automatically detect and monitor geese and to test avian cognition. Our approach may also create new opportunities for species monitoring approaches more generally using photographic images and citizen-science engagement.
... In addition, the nature of social relationships seems to highly impact upon brain size, irrespective of group stability and social dynamics. For instance, species forming long-term relationships and/or partnerships tend to have bigger brains than species engaging in short-term or seasonal relationships only ( 17 , but see 29 ). Hence, interacting with particular individuals over time-rather than interacting with many individuals and/or over limited periods only-seems to be a highly cognitively challenging enterprise 30 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Human children show unique cognitive skills for dealing with the social world but their cognitive performance is paralleled by great apes in many tasks dealing with the physical world. Recent studies suggested that members of a songbird family—corvids—also evolved complex cognitive skills but a detailed understanding of the full scope of their cognition was, until now, not existent. Furthermore, relatively little is known about their cognitive development. Here, we conducted the first systematic, quantitative large-scale assessment of physical and social cognitive performance of common ravens with a special focus on development. To do so, we fine-tuned one of the most comprehensive experimental test-batteries, the Primate Cognition Test Battery (PCTB), to raven features enabling also a direct, quantitative comparison with the cognitive performance of two great ape species. Full-blown cognitive skills were already present at the age of four months with subadult ravens’ cognitive performance appearing very similar to that of adult apes in tasks of physical (quantities, and causality) and social cognition (social learning, communication, and theory of mind). These unprecedented findings strengthen recent assessments of ravens’ general intelligence, and aid to the growing evidence that the lack of a specific cortical architecture does not hinder advanced cognitive skills. Difficulties in certain cognitive scales further emphasize the quest to develop comparative test batteries that tap into true species rather than human specific cognitive skills, and suggest that socialization of test individuals may play a crucial role. We conclude to pay more attention to the impact of personality on cognitive output, and a currently neglected topic in Animal Cognition—the linkage between ontogeny and cognitive performance.
... By examining the relationship quality of some of these species, notably rooks and jackdaws, they suggested that some birds face an adaptive pressure to cooperate and coordinate their behaviour with a partner to raise altricial young, and that this may be underpinned by flexible cognitive abilities. Scheiber et al. (2008) critique many elements of this theory -including the assertion that corvids display more complex pair-bond relationships than other birds -noting that many 'primate-like' features of relationship complexity are also found in greylag geese, which have far smaller brains. However, similar links between brain size and pairbonds have been demonstrated by Shultz and Dunbar and colleagues across a range of species. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
New Caledonian crows are an exceptional species of bird, known to manufacture and use complex tools in the wild. In captivity, they appear to possess a causal understanding of many elements of tool use and to demonstrate a number of advanced cognitive abilities. As a member of the large-brained corvid family, these birds have been collectively referred to as 'feathered apes'. However, to what extent are the cognitive abilities of New Caledonian crows similar to those of the great apes? Is it the case that these crows possess a suite of generalised cognitive abilities, comparable in breadth and flexibility to those of our closest primate relatives? Or, alternatively, have these birds evolved more specialised cognitive adaptions, perhaps in response to the evolutionary pressures of tool manufacture and use? In this thesis, I investigate the cognitive abilities of New Caledonian crows across a broad range of domains. I report the results of five experimental studies designed to tap different aspects of New Caledonian crow cognition, spanning the fields of causal reasoning, reasoning by exclusion, cooperation, self-control, and the cultural transmission of tool designs. From the results of this work I argue that New Caledonian crows could be characterised as possessing a portfolio of advanced physical cognition and reasoning abilities, but more limited social cognition. This body of work therefore highlights a number of similarities, but also a number of differences, between the cognitive abilities of New Caledonian crows and the great apes, and provides a window into the ways in which intelligence evolves.
... Studies on the understanding of social relationships in birds are underrepresented (Emery et al., 2007), which is surprising since some groups such as corvids show striking cognitive abilities both in the physical and social domain (review in Emery, 2006). A possible reason could be that most species do not live in stable groups, but are characterised by a relatively high degree of fission-fusion -182 -dynamics, with the pair-bond being the main stable unit (Emery et al., 2007; but see Scheiber et al., 2008). However, many species show seasonal differences in sociality (they are more solitary during the breeding season) or may vary social tendencies across developmental periods. ...
Article
Full-text available
The social intelligence hypothesis, originally developed for primates to explain their high intelligence and large relative brain size, assumes that challenges posed by social life in complex societies with many group members lead to the evolution of advanced cognitive abilities. In birds, pair-bonded species have larger brains than non-pair bonded species, indicating that the quality of social relationships better predicts social complexity than group size. Ravens are a long-term monogamous and territorial species, renowned for their sophisticated socio-cognitive skills and complex social relationships. Notably, during their early years they live in fission-fusion-like non-breeder societies in which social relationships could be of particular importance. Here we observed the development of dominance and affiliative relationships in 12 hand-raised captive ravens, examining the influence of age, sex and kinship on social interactions. Furthermore, we investigated at which developmental step a stable hierarchy emerged, whether third-party interventions played a role and how selectively birds intervened in others' conflicts. At 4-5 months post-fledging, we found an increase in socio-positive behaviour and a decrease in aggression, along with the establishment of a linear dominance rank hierarchy. In line with kin selection theory, siblings exhibited a greater degree of tolerance and engaged in more socio-positive behaviour. In their first few months, ravens frequently intervened in others' conflicts but supported mainly the aggressor; later on, their support became more selective towards kin and close social partners. These findings indicate that ravens engage in sophisticated social behaviours and form stable relationships already in their first year of life.
Article
Comparative analysis of higher cognitive abilities in animals provides for assessment of the evolutionary underpinnings of the formation of human thought and language. This review will address the main approaches to studies of thought in animals and analyze the data obtained using these approaches. The results of a diversity of tests indicate that animals with high levels of brain development have a wide spectrum of cognitive abilities. As expected, the widest spectrum is found in the great apes. A quite similar spectrum is found in higher members of the class Aves (corvids and psittacines) despite their different brain structure. Convergent similarity in the level of development of cognitive abilities in higher mammals and birds reflects the operation of common factors determining their evolution. Comparison of several corvid and psittacine species indicates that the high levels of development of their cognitive abilities are due to the high levels of organization of the brains of these species rather than ecological characteristics.
Article
Full-text available
Despite decades of research, much uncertainty remains regarding the selection pressures responsible for brain size variation. Whilst the influential social brain hypothesis once garnered extensive support, more recent studies have failed to find support for a link between brain size and sociality. Instead, it appears there is now substantial evidence suggesting ecology better predicts brain size in both primates and carnivores. Here, different models of brain evolution were tested, and the relative importance of social, ecological, and life-history traits were assessed on both overall encephalisation and specific brain regions. In primates, evidence is found for consistent associations between brain size and ecological factors, particularly diet; however, evidence was also found advocating sociality as a selection pressure driving brain size. In carnivores, evidence suggests ecological variables, most notably home range size, are influencing brain size; whereas, no support is found for the social brain hypothesis, perhaps reflecting the fact sociality appears to be limited to a select few taxa. Life-history associations reveal complex selection mechanisms to be counterbalancing the costs associated with expensive brain tissue through extended developmental periods, reduced fertility, and extended maximum lifespan. Future studies should give careful consideration of the methods chosen for measuring brain size, investigate both whole brain and specific brain regions where possible, and look to integrate multiple variables, thus fully capturing all of the potential factors influencing brain size.
Chapter
The cognitive abilities of birds are remarkable: hummingbirds integrate spatial and temporal information about food sources, day-old chicks have a sense of numbers, parrots can make and use tools, and ravens have sophisticated insights in social relationships. This volume describes the full range of avian cognitive abilities, the mechanisms behind such abilities and how they relate to the ecology of the species. Synthesising the latest research in avian cognition, a range of experts in the field provide first-hand insights into experimental procedures, outcomes and theoretical advances, including a discussion of how the findings in birds relate to the cognitive abilities of other species, including humans. The authors cover a range of topics such as spatial cognition, social learning, tool use, perceptual categorization and concept learning, providing the broader context for students and researchers interested in the current state of avian cognition research, its key questions and appropriate experimental approaches.
Chapter
Affects and their conscious representations, emotions, are the central agents of social organization in humans and non-human animals. These are bio-psychological phenomena with morphological and physiological substrates, with evolutionary functions, modulated in ontogeny and conservatively preserved in evolutionary history. Emotions “motivate” social interactions/relationships and emotionality is directly linked with basic physiology, particularly with the stress systems and also, with the most important “anti-stress” complex in mammals, the oxytocin-attachment system, which has also a major role in bonding. Emotional phenotype (“temperament”) affects social connectedness and finally, fitness in complex social systems. And there is also a clear link to social cognition, as affects are involved in virtually any decision made by the relevant brain centers in mammals and birds. Finally, the communication of emotions and satisfying each others emotional need is also at the core of human–animal relationships, which may be considered as ­indirect evidence for common principles of social organization.
Article
Full-text available
The possible relationships between relative brain size and developmental mode and between relative brain size and five measures of the length of the development period were tested across over 1400 species of birds. Using both conventional statistics and phylogenetically based comparative methods, significant differences in relative brain size were detected among modes of development. Across all species, there were significant relationships between relative brain size and each of the following developmental traits: incubation period, age of fledging, duration of postfledging parental care, and total period of parental care. In contrast, the age of first flight was not significantly correlated with relative brain size. The relationships between these five developmental traits and relative brain size varied among developmental modes and orders such that significant relationships were present within some modes and orders but not in others. Thus, developmental differences play a significant role in the evolution of brain-size differences, but the role depends upon the taxonomic level being investigated. This is likely due to the differential lengths of periods of neural and behavioural development in young birds. Our conclusions support the contention of previous studies that developmental differences have played a key role in avian brain evolution.
Article
Full-text available
The mouthbrooding cichlid Tropheus moorii exhibits an exceptional degree of maternal in-vestment. Females produce very large eggs, which are incubated for a period of six weeks, and they feed their young in the mouth while starving themselves. Only very few fish species are known to feed their young. We hypothesized that feeding may either (1) benefit females directly if young develop faster, or (2) provide a size advantage to young. To distinguish be-tween these mutually non-exclusive hypotheses, we measured costs and benefits arising for mouthbrooding females when feeding their young. Field observations revealed that mouth-brooding females reduced bite rates, locomotion and territorial defence compared to non-brooding adults. Feeding rates correlated positively with locomotion, as fish moving around more also spent more time with territory defence and other social interactions. This suggests that buccal feeding is costly when compared to mere incubation without feeding the young. In an experiment, in which we controlled the access of females to food, we showed that these costs are apparently not counterbalanced by a benefit to females through a shorter incubation duration. Rather, fed young were larger, heavier and had higher burst-swimming speeds. The extreme maternal investment of T. moorii appears to yield fitness benefits to females by pro-ducing larger and stronger young, which consequently should have better survival chances in their natural environment.
Article
Full-text available
In monogamous animals, reproductive success may vary considerably among pairs. To study this variation, we focussed on reproductive events and the circannual hormonal co-ordination within pairs during the reproductive cycle. Testosterone was chosen as covariable for both sexes because of its mediator function between behaviour and physiology. In a flock of free-living Greylag Geese Anser anser, individual faecal samples were collected weekly from 23 pairs over a complete annual cycle. From the faeces, equivalents of testosterone, oestrogen and other steroid hormones were analysed by enzyme immunoassay. In contrast to correlations between male and female testosterone, no correlations were found between oestrogen-oestrogen or oestrogen-testosterone. Therefore, only testosterone (T) is considered here. Sex-specific differences in T were in amplitude rather than in the annual timing of maxima and minima. However, the annual timing varied to some degree between individuals within sexes. Therefore, we examined the degree of annual testosterone correlation (TC) within pairs. Pairs that nested had significantly higher TC over the year than pairs that did not. The higher the within-pair TC, the larger the clutches and the heavier the eggs. Also, TC in the year investigated was positively and significantly correlated with the pairs' long-term reproductive output. No correlation was found between TC and the duration of the pair-bond, individual age, or age difference from pair partner. We suggest that TC is a measure of behavioural synchrony and, therefore, pair-bond quality. We consider whether within-pair TC results from mate choice.
Article
In mammals, support by a social partner may reduce stress levels and ease access to resources. We investigated the effects of the passive presence of a nearby social ally on excreted corticosterone immunoreactive metabolites and behaviour in juvenile graylag geese (Anser anser). Two groups of hand-raised juveniles (N1 = 9, N2 = 3) were tested over 1 year by positioning humans of different familiarity (i.e., the human foster parent, a familiar human, a nonfamiliar human, no human) at a standard distance to the focal geese. Their success in agonistic interactions significantly decreased with age and with decreasing familiarity of the accompanying human. The humans present modulated the excretion of corticosterone immunoreactive metabolites, with the strongest effects recorded after fledging when corticosterone metabolites were also positively correlated with agonistic behaviour. This suggests that a human foster parent may provide similar supportive benefits as goose parents do in natural families. We discuss the benefits of social alliances with regard to the integration into the flock, access to resources, and life history.
Article
Conventional wisdom over the past 160 years in the cognitive and neurosciences has assumed that brains evolved to process factual information about the world. Most attention has therefore been focused on such features as pattern recognition, color vision, and speech perception. By extension, it was assumed that brains evolved to deal with essentially ecological problem-solving tasks. © 1998 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Article
A paper presented at the symposium "Human Ethology: Claims and Limits of a New Discipline," held at Bad Homburg, West Germany (October 1977), offers a tentative conceptual scheme for future studies by examining the question "What goals (other than reproductive goals) are served by the group life of nonhuman primates?" The author feels that concepts like dominance, familiarity, and bond have been emphasized to the neglect of other aspects of relationships. This paper discusses a primate dyad, A-B, in terms of the value to A of his/her social relationships with B. According to the suggested scheme, A selects, monitors, and alters 3 of B's attributes—his/her qualities, tendencies, and availability—in such a way as to increase B's value to A. These attributes and actions are described in detail. Interactions with the environment involve a 3rd element, C (a conspecific, an extraspecific resource, a predator, or a parasite). Examples of these interactions are cited from field studies. The greatest area of ignorance about primate relationships has to do with what a primate learns about and through his/her social companions. A valuable insight derived from the scheme is that a single social act, such as a scream, may serve widely differing goals. (40 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
In general, support by social allies may reduce stress, increase an individual's status and facilitate access to resources. In Greylag geese (Anser anser), offspring stay with their parents for an entire year or even longer and thereby enjoy social support in encounters with other flock members. We investigated the influence of spatial distance to one's allies on the outcome of agonistic encounters in a natural flock situation for a total of 12 sibling groups after fledging. In addition, we tested two groups of hand-raised juvenile geese over a time span of 11/2 yr. Passive (i.e. not interfering) human supporters of different familiarity were placed at a standard distance during food provisionings, which produced a tight flock situation. Success in agonistic interactions increased with decreasing distance to members of their social unit. The hand-raised juveniles were more successful in agonistic interactions and showed increased feeding rates when accompanied by a familiar human than when alone or with a non-familiar human. The effect of the presence of familiar humans on success in agonistic encounters significantly decreased with increasing age, while feeding rates remained elevated. The positive effects of social support were particularly evident in females. We conclude that social support has similar effects and functions in the highly social greylag geese as reported for social mammals.
Article
Experiments vary in their ability to distinguish between competing hypotheses. In tests on primate cognition the majority of this variation is due to an experimenter's ability to test primates in valid settings while providing the adequate amount of experimental control. While experimenters studying primate cognition can use methods of control perfected in captivity, it is still very unclear how to design and then objectively evaluate the external validity of new experimental paradigms. I recommend that more effort be allocated to specify how to create relevant test settings for primates. Primate social life is highly competitive. This means that all aspects of primates themselves, including their cognitive abilities, have likely been shaped by the need to out-compete conspecifics. Based on this hypothesis, sophisticated cognitive abilities of primates might best be demonstrated in competitive contexts. Thus, it is suggested that one possible measure of validity is whether investigators integrate a competitive component into their experimental designs. To evaluate this methodological prediction I review the literature on chimpanzee perspective-taking as a case study including several recent studies that include a competitive component in their experimental designs.