ArticlePDF Available

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
http://pss.sagepub.com/
Psychological Science
http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2011/08/09/0956797611417007
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/0956797611417007
published online 12 August 2011Psychological Science
Jonathan D. Leavitt and Nicholas J. S. Christenfeld
Story Spoilers Don't Spoil Stories
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Association for Psychological Science
can be found at:Psychological ScienceAdditional services and information for
http://pss.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://pss.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
by Nicholas Christenfeld on August 31, 2011pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Psychological Science
XX(X) 1 –3
© The Author(s) 2011
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797611417007
http://pss.sagepub.com
Stories are a universal element of human culture, the backbone
of the billion-dollar entertainment industry, and the medium
through which religion and societal values are transmitted.
The enjoyment of fiction through books, television, and mov-
ies may depend, in part, on the psychological experience of
suspense. Spoilers give away endings before stories begin, and
may thereby diminish suspense and impair enjoyment; indeed,
as the term suggests, readers go to considerable lengths to
avoid prematurely discovering endings. Transportation, a dis-
tinct form of story engagement associated with vivid imagery
and enhanced enjoyment (Green, Brock, & Kaufman, 2004),
is highly associated with suspense via close attention to the
unfolding plot and interest in how it will be resolved (Tal-Or
& Cohen, 2010). However, people’s ability to reread stories
with undiminished pleasure, and to read stories in which the
genre strongly implies the ending, suggests that suspense
regarding the outcome may not be critical to enjoyment and
may even impair pleasure by distracting attention from a
story’s relevant details and aesthetic qualities. In complex sto-
ries, developments hazy in their implications on first read are
readily understood when the narrative is revisited, and nervous
stirrings of uncertainty may become warm anticipation of
coming events once the story is laid bare.
Reading a story with foreknowledge of its outcome may be
analogous to perceptual fluency, in which perceived objects are
processed with ease, an experience that is associated with aes-
thetic pleasure (Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004), positive
affect (Winkielman & Cacioppo, 2001), and story engagement
(Vaughn, Childs, Maschinski, Niño, & Ellsworth, 2010).
Schema discrepancy theory suggests that increased predictabil-
ity can result in increased positivity of affective response,
although this effect is dependent on initial level of uncertainty
(MacDowell & Mandler, 1989). Thus, despite intuitive beliefs
about the effects of spoilers, there are plausible theoretical rea-
sons to think they may not ruin the pleasure of reading a story.
Their actual effect remains unknown. We conducted three
experiments, each with stories from a different, distinct genre,
to test the effects of spoilers on enjoyment.
Method
Participants (176 male, 643 female) were recruited from the
psychology subject pool at the University of California, San
Diego. They took part in three experiments in which they read
three different sorts of short stories―ironic-twist stories, mys-
teries, and more evocative literary stories. For each story, we
created a spoiler paragraph that briefly discussed the story and
revealed the outcome in a way that seemed inadvertent. These
paragraphs were designed so that they could work as either
independent text or the openings of the stories (as though the
stories were intrinsically spoiled).
Each experiment included four stories selected from anthol-
ogies. Each subject read three of these stories, one spoiled
(with the spoiler paragraph presented before the story), one
unspoiled (with the story presented without alteration), and
one in which the spoiler paragraph was incorporated as the
opening paragraph. Story, order, and condition were counter-
balanced such that each story was presented with equal fre-
quency across positions and conditions. Each version of each
story was read and rated for enjoyment (on a 10-point scale
ranging from 1, lowest rating, to 10, best rating) by at least 30
subjects. The stories were by such authors as John Updike,
Roald Dahl, Anton Chekhov, Agatha Christie, and Raymond
Carver, and ranged from 1,381 to 4,220 words. Subjects indi-
cated whether they had read any story previously, and if they
had, their data for that story (< 3% of ratings) were excluded
from analyses. Subjects were also provided the opportunity to
respond freely about each story.
Results
For all three experiments, analyses of variance revealed a sig-
nificant effect of condition. (In order to control for variability
between stories, we analyzed the data by comparing different
versions of the same story.) Subjects significantly preferred
spoiled over unspoiled stories in the case of both the ironic-
twist stories (6.20 vs. 5.79), p = .013, Cohen’s d = 0.18, and
the mysteries (7.29 vs. 6.60), p = .001, d = 0.34. The evocative
stories were appreciated less overall, likely because of their
more expressly literary aims, but subjects again significantly
preferred spoiled over unspoiled versions (5.50 vs. 5.03),
Corresponding Author:
Jonathan D. Leavitt, Department of Psychology, University of California,
San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr., La Jolla, CA 92093
E-mail: jleavitt@ucsd.edu
Story Spoilers Don’t Spoil Stories
Jonathan D. Leavitt and Nicholas J. S. Christenfeld
University of California, San Diego
Received 4/4/11; Revision accepted 5/26/11
Short Report
Psychological Science OnlineFirst, published on August 12, 2011 as doi:10.1177/0956797611417007
by Nicholas Christenfeld on August 31, 2011pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
2 Leavitt, Christenfeld
p = .019, d = 0.22. In all three story types, incorporating spoiler
texts into stories had no effect on how much they were liked,
ps > .4. Subjects also did not indicate in their free responses
that they found these altered beginnings out of place or jarring.
Figure 1 shows the ratings for the spoiled and unspoiled ver-
sions of each story.
Conclusions
Writers use their artistry to make stories interesting, to engage
readers, and to surprise them, but we found that giving away
these surprises makes readers like stories better. This was true
whether the spoiler revealed a twist at the end (e.g., that the
condemned man’s daring escape is just a fantasy as the rope
snaps taut around his neck) or solved the crime (e.g., Poirot
discovers that the apparent target of attempted murder was in
fact the perpetrator). It was also true when the spoiler was
more poetic, as when frisky adolescents watching a couple
struggle with a baby are revealed to be previewing their own
futures, and the couple glimpsing their own pasts. In all these
types of stories, spoilers may allow readers to organize devel-
opments, anticipate the implications of events, and resolve
ambiguities that occur in the course of reading.
It is possible that spoilers enhance enjoyment by actually
increasing tension. Knowing the ending of Oedipus Rex may
heighten the pleasurable tension caused by the disparity in
knowledge between the omniscient reader and the character
marching to his doom. This notion is consistent with the assertion
that stories can be reread with no diminution of suspense
(Carroll, 1996). Although our results suggest that people
are wasting their time avoiding spoilers, our data do not
suggest that authors err by keeping things hidden. Stories
that open by revealing outcomes may lead readers to antici-
pate additional revelations at the end; in other words, readers
do not expect a story to provide complete premature knowledge
of its ending the way an external source might. Indeed, it
was only spoilers external to the stories that enhanced
readers’ delight; there was no benefit to our editing the stories
themselves.
Erroneous intuitions about the nature of spoilers may per-
sist because individual readers are unable to compare spoiled
and unspoiled experiences of a novel story. Other intuitions
about suspense may be similarly wrong: Perhaps birthday
presents are better when wrapped in cellophane, and engage-
ment rings when not concealed in chocolate mousse.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
References
Carroll, N. (1996). The paradox of suspense. In P. Vorderer, H. J.
Wulff, & M. Friedrichsen (Eds.), Suspense: Conceptualization,
theoretical analysis, and empirical explorations (pp. 71–90).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Green, M. C., Brock, T. C., & Kaufman, G. F. (2004). Understand-
ing media enjoyment: The role of transportation into narrative
worlds. Communication Theory, 14, 311–327.
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
“A Dark Brown Dog”
“Owl Creek Bridge”
“Lamb to the Slaughter”
“The Bet”
“Blitzed”
“A Chess Problem”
“McHenry's Gift”
“Rhyme Never Pays”
“The Calm”
“Good Dog”
“Plumbing”
“Up at a Villa”
Mean Hedonic Rating
Spoiled Unspoiled
Ironic-
Twist Stories
Literary Stories
Mysteries
Fig. 1. Hedonic ratings of the individual spoiled and unspoiled stories. Error bars represent standard
errors.
by Nicholas Christenfeld on August 31, 2011pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Story Spoilers Don’t Spoil Stories 3
MacDowell, K., & Mandler, G. (1989). Constructions of emotion: Dis-
crepancy, arousal, and mood. Motivation and Emotion, 13, 105–124.
Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency
and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing expe-
rience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 364–382.
Tal-Or, N., & Cohen, J. (2010). Understanding audience involve-
ment: Conceptualizing and manipulating identification and trans-
portation. Poetics, 38, 402–418.
Vaughn, L. A., Childs, K. E., Maschinski, C., Niño, N. P., &
Ellsworth, R. (2010). Regulatory fit, processing fluency, and nar-
rative persuasion. Social & Personality Psychology Compass, 4,
1181–1192.
Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile
on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facili-
tation elicits positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 81, 989–1000.
by Nicholas Christenfeld on August 31, 2011pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... Spoilers: Spoilers are seen as a high-stakes issue when they are about the death of a major character, and consumers can take precautions to avoid learning about the endings of their media narratives prematurely. However, recent research suggests that knowing the outcome of a narrative does not change the pleasure of being exposed to it (Leavitt & Christenfeld, 2011), and in some cases, it might actually enhance enjoyment (Johnson & Rosenbaum, 2015). ...
... Although the mentioned research (Johnson & Rosenbaum, 2015;Leavitt & Christenfeld, 2011) suggests otherwise, in the study data the commenters were generally angry at being exposed to spoilers, and the general consensus was that spoilers undermined the effects of character deaths: "not sure if I want to keep going now"; "Unfortunately I knew about it happening so when it happened it didn't affect me all"; "Wait a minute…Aeris dies???..Thanks for the spoiler alert asshats."; "maybe because I knew she was going to die, I didn't allow myself to get emotionally invested in her character"; I was at the first season and i was checking who was Marissa's actress and google search results gave me the spoiler that she died […] It ruined the show for me." ...
... Spoilers: Before being exposed to death, consumers will either interact with or try to avoid spoilers. Giving or avoiding spoilers on social media has been a major discussion point for audiences; however, research suggests that spoilers do not diminish the effects of narratives (Leavitt & Christenfeld, 2011), and in fact, in certain cases, they may actually enhance enjoyment (Johnson & Rosenbaum, 2015). While experiencing the death sequence, viewers will usually be moved by and remember aural cues, specifically the music that is used. ...
Article
Full-text available
We analyze the audience response to the death of narrative-driven fictitious characters with predetermined fates, whether part of a virtual or cinematic story, and specifically from video games and TV series. Our aim is to contribute to the studies of identification and empathy with fictitious characters in media, as well as to close the research gap around these studies by specifically focusing on the death of the characters. We collected 3000 online comments on the deaths of 16 characters from video games and TV series. We coded each comment according to the five stages of grief by Kübler-Ross and Kessler and performed quantitative (using LIWC2015 psycholinguistic analysis software) and qualitative analysis (using thematic analysis). Overall, we found a strong resemblance between the processes of grief for real and fictitious characters and uncovered differences of language when discussing the death of a character based on (a) their gender; (b) their role in the story; (c) their interactivity mode; and (d) the form of media. Finally, qualitative analysis revealed unique and novel themes for on-screen deaths, such as (a) the effects of aural cues; (b) nostalgia and beauty; (c) resurrection and transmedia; (d) spoilers; (e) comparisons and real-life connotations; (f) the effects on the franchise; and (g) the effects of the gender of the viewer on these discussions. We discuss our findings in detail, along with implications for future character development.
... However, while some research has shown that spoilers that reveal the ending of a story can indeed decrease the enjoyment of a narrative (Rosenbaum & Johnson, 2016), other research has found the opposite (M. E. Ellithorpe & Brookes, 2018;Leavitt & Christenfeld, 2011. Recent scholarship examining how the nature (major, minor) and placement (middle or end) of a spoiler influences enjoyment produced similarly contradictory findings (Johnson et al., 2020;Levine et al., 2016). ...
... The field of research into spoilers has grown exponentially in the past 10 years, including work that examines the relationship between spoilers and processing fluency. However, most of the experiment-based studies conducted to date rely on participants' interactions with written short stories (e.g., Johnson & Rosenbaum, 2015;Leavitt & Christenfeld, 2011Levine et al., 2016;Rosenbaum & Johnson, 2016), movie posters (e.g., Johnson & Rosenbaum, 2018), specific movie scenes (Johnson et al., 2020), or trailers (e.g., Kryston et al., 2022). Studies that do examine participants' experiences with "real" media content have to date taken the shape of surveys where respondents report their fluency and enjoyment after the actual viewing (e.g., M. E. Ellithorpe & Brookes, 2018;Johnson & Rosenbaum, 2018, Study 3). ...
... While some research has failed to find a connection between spoilers and processing fluency (e.g., Johnson et al., 2020;Johnson & Rosenbaum, 2018), advance knowledge of a narrative arc, or spoilers, has also been argued to enhance enjoyment due to its ability to increase this processing fluency (M. E. Ellithorpe & Brookes, 2018;Leavitt & Christenfeld, 2011. This has been attributed to mental model resonance, a process wherein previous information about a narrative is incorporated into people's mental models about that narrative. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although spoilers are commonly believed to ruin the experience of a story, recent research has produced conflicting results about the role spoilers play in enjoyment. In response, scholars have begun to explore factors that impact the relationship between spoilers and the narrative experience. A series of three studies were conducted to investigate the relationship between spoilers and processing fluency by examining how expectations regarding a narrative’s ending can interact with spoilers to affect fluency and the resulting enjoyment. Three online between-subjects experiments exposed participants to a spoiled or unspoiled preview and then assigned them to watch the season finale of a long-running sitcom with either the original ending that defied expectations or an alternate ending that aligned with genre conventions. Results reveal that both spoilers and whether an ending is expected have a complex relationship with fluency, with the three studies producing slightly different interactions between these two variables. In all three studies, fluency was associated with increased enjoyment.
... A recent study suggests that the experience of uncertainty can be exciting [11,12]. Consumers sometimes actively embrace uncertainty, as observed in behaviors such as delaying the discovery of outcomes (e.g., gender revelation of unborn babies), or taking measures to avoid spoilers that would reveal the ending [13][14][15][16]. Moon and Nelson (2020) [11] found that, compared to certain prospects, people consistently rate uncertain prospects higher in terms of anticipated enjoyment and interest. Buechel and Li (2022) [17] found that individuals prefer uncertain items over certain items when outcomes are horizontally differentiated (i.e., outcomes do not differ in objective superiority). ...
Article
Full-text available
Mysterious consumption, characterized by product purchases without knowledge of their exact nature, is gaining popularity in the modern marketplace. In two online experiments, we examined how consumers’ psychological states, particularly their sense of personal control, influence their perception and intent to purchase mysterious products in the context of purchasing a random flight ticket. Study 1 demonstrated that when consumers experience low personal control, as opposed to high personal control, they are less likely to value the uncertainty inherent in random flight tickets, resulting in decreased purchase intentions. Study 2 revealed that introducing even limited choice options to random flight tickets can enhance consumers’ appreciation of uncertainty, thereby increasing their purchase intention. This effect was especially notable among those initially hesitant to embrace uncertainty, ultimately boosting their intent to purchase. These findings deepen our understanding of consumer psychology surrounding mystery consumption and provide practical insights for marketers seeking to tailor their products and marketing appeals to consumers’ psychological states.
... Similarly, readers have different preferences for spoilers. For some, spoilers are valued because they give readers a more fluent experience while reading the novel, while other readers strongly dislike and avoid spoilers [100][101][102]. ...
Article
Full-text available
What psychological mechanisms underlie aesthetic judgements? An influential account known as the Hedonic Marking of Fluency, later developed into a Processing Fluency Theory of Aesthetic Pleasure, posits that ease of processing elicits positive feelings and thus enhances stimulus evaluations. However, the theory faces empirical and conceptual challenges. In this paper, we extend it by integrating insights from predictive processing frameworks (PPF) and the epistemic motivation model (EMM). We propose four extensions. First, fluency of a stimulus depends on perceivers' expectations—their internal model of the world. Second, perceivers also form expectations about fluency itself and thus can experience surprising fluency. These expectations can come from the individual's history, their current task and their environment. Third, perceivers can value fluency but also disfluency, reflecting their non-directional epistemic goals. Fourth, perceivers also have directional epistemic goals, preferring specific conclusions or belief content. Consequently, affective reactions depend on whether the stimulus satisfies those goals. These directional epistemic goals may override concerns about fluency or change the value of fluency associated with specific content. We review supporting evidence and introduce novel predictions. By integrating insights from PPF and EMM, our framework can better capture established fluency effects and highlights their limitations and extensions. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Art, aesthetics and predictive processing: theoretical and empirical perspectives’.
... Examples 17-19 include notes about what happens to the hero in the end. Although such spoilers may kill the enjoyment of the watching experience, previous research has shown mixed or null effects of this practice on consumers' behaviour (Johnson and Rosenbaum 2015;Leavitt and Christenfeld 2011). According to Johnson and Rosenbaum (2015), this may depend on the genre of the movie; a comedy film may be less enjoyable when spoiled, yet the opposite applies to the fantasy thriller film. ...
Article
Translation notes, as employed in fansubbing, are remarks added to the subtitles without reference to the source text. The current study adopts quantitative and qualitative approaches to investigate the receptivity and classification of translation notes explicitly utilized in fan-subtitled English movies for Arab audiences. In the quantitative part, 90 participants were engaged to assess fan-subbed English films with embedded translation notes in Arabic and subsequently completed an eighteen-item questionnaire. The analysis revealed a favourable disposition towards translation notes, albeit with a caveat of requisite technical refinements in future products. Moreover, the findings indicated a predilection for incorporating such notes in specific film genres, such as comedy and romance, as opposed to horror or documentaries. In the qualitative part, the researchers conducted an exhaustive examination of 120 translation notes, subsequently categorizing them into ten thematic classifications. The investigation further showed that fansubbers might provide commentary on scenes laden with disbelief or blasphemy, distancing the viewers from the content while maintaining a comedic tone. Additionally, fansubbers may exercise discretion in abstaining from translating sequences deemed repetitive or ill-suited for the target audience. The results further posit that vernacular dialects demonstrate a heightened efficacy in conveying humour, as opposed to the utilization of Modern Standard Arabic (MSA), given the former’s ubiquity in quotidian communication and the latter’s association with formal contexts. Finally, the study concludes that more streaming platforms are recommended to add a new feature where viewers can watch the A.V. material with translation notes.
Article
The proactive removal of condensed clips from TikTok decreased demand for corresponding full-length originals by 3%, revealing their positive impact to TV industry.
Article
Full-text available
The affect system, in its position to monitor organismic-environmental transactions, may be sensitive to the internal dynamics of information processing. Hence, the authors predicted that facilitation of stimulus processing should elicit a brief, mild, positive affective response. In 2 studies, participants watched a series of neutral pictures while the processing ease was unobtrusively manipulated. Affective reactions were assessed with facial electromyography (EMG). In both studies, easy-to-process pictures elicited higher activity over the region of zygomaticus major, indicating positive affect. The EMG data were paralleled by self-reports of positive responses to the facilitated stimuli. The findings suggest a close link between processing dynamics and affect and may help understand several preference phenomena, including the mere-exposure effect. The findings also highlight a potential source of affective biases in social judgments.
Article
Full-text available
For millennia, people have used narratives to inform and persuade. However, little social psychological research addresses how and when narrative persuasion occurs, perhaps because narratives are complex stimuli that are difficult to vary without significantly changing the plot or characters. Existing research suggests that regulatory fit and/or processing fluency can be varied easily and in ways completely exterior to narrative content but that nonetheless affect how much narratives engage, transport, and persuade. We review research on narrative transportation and persuasion and then discuss regulatory fit and its relationship to processing fluency. Afterward, we discuss how regulatory fit and processing fluency may affect psychological engagement, transportation, and persuasion via narratives.
Article
Full-text available
The affect system, in its position to monitor organismic-environmental transactions, may be sensitive to the internal dynamics of information processing. Hence, the authors predicted that facilitation of stimulus processing should elicit a brief, mild, positive affective response. In 2 studies, participants watched a series of neutral pictures while the processing ease was unobtrusively manipulated. Affective reactions were assessed with facial electromyography (EMG). In both studies, easy-to-process pictures elicited higher activity over the region of zygomaticus major, indicating positive affect. The EMG data were paralleled by self-reports of positive responses to the facilitated stimuli. The findings suggest a close link between processing dynamics and affect and may help understand several preference phenomena, including the mere-exposure effect. The findings also highlight a potential source of affective biases in social judgments.
Article
Full-text available
We propose that aesthetic pleasure is a function of the perceiver's processing dynamics: The more fluently perceivers can process an object, the more positive their aesthetic response. We review variables known to influence aesthetic judgments, such as figural goodness, figure-ground contrast, stimulus repetition, symmetry, and prototypicality, and trace their effects to changes in processing fluency. Other variables that influence processing fluency, like visual or semantic priming, similarly increase judgments of aesthetic pleasure. Our proposal provides an integrative framework for the study of aesthetic pleasure and sheds light on the interplay between early preferences versus cultural influences on taste, preferences for both prototypical and abstracted forms, and the relation between beauty and truth. In contrast to theories that trace aesthetic pleasure to objective stimulus features per se, we propose that beauty is grounded in the processing experiences of the perceiver, which are in part a function of stimulus properties.
Article
Two central avenues for audience involvement in entertainment are identification and transportation. This study conceptually and empirically differentiated between these processes by manipulating information about the hero of a film and about the plot in order to affect the ways viewers respond to the film and character. The valence of information about the hero affected the level of identification (but not the level of transportation), and the time of deeds affected the level of transportation (but not the level of identification). These results provide evidence that identification and transportation are distinct processes and an analysis of how each of them relates to enjoyment supports this conclusion. Results are discussed in terms of their theoretical and methodological contribution to the study of audience involvement.
Article
“Transportation into a narrative world” is an experience of cognitive, emotional, and imagery involvement in a narrative. Transportation theory (Green & Brock, 2000, 2002) provides a lens for understanding the concept of media enjoyment. The theory suggests that enjoyment can benefit from the experience of being immersed in a narrative world, as well as from the consequences of that immersion. Consequences implied by transportation theory include connections with characters and self-transformations.
Article
An interactive adventure-type computer game was used to explore the constructive components of emotional experience — sympathetic nervous system arousal and evaluative cognitions — postulated by discrepancy/evaluation theory. Manipulations of expectations produced significant and large heart rate changes for both positive and negative discrepancies. Autonomic response also reflected the degree of interruption/discrepancy produced by an event. Negatively toned (discrepant) events tended to produce significant intensity and higher overall autonomic arousal. In addition, subjects'' intensity of experience was related to their autonomic response.