ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The embodied view of language comprehension proposes that the meaning of words is grounded in perception and action rather than represented in abstract amodal symbols. Support for embodied theories of language processing comes from behavioral studies showing that understanding a sentence about an action can modulate congruent and incongruent physical responses, suggesting motor involvement during comprehension of sentences referring to bodily movement. Additionally, several neuroimaging studies have provided evidence that comprehending single words denoting manipulable objects elicits specific responses in the neural motor system. An interesting question that remains is whether action semantic knowledge is directly activated as motor simulations in the brain, or rather modulated by the semantic context in which action words are encountered. In the current paper we investigated the nature of conceptual representations using a go/no-go lexical decision task. Specifically, target words were either presented in a semantic context that emphasized dominant action features (features related to the functional use of an object) or non-dominant action features. The response latencies in a lexical decision task reveal that participants were faster to respond to words denoting objects for which the functional use was congruent with the prepared movement. This facilitation effect, however, was only apparent when the semantic context emphasized corresponding motor properties. These findings suggest that conceptual processing is a context-dependent process that incorporates motor-related knowledge in a flexible manner.
This content is subject to copyright.
www.frontiersin.org October 2010 | Volume 1 | Article 150 | 1
Original research article
published: 04 October 2010
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00150
Context effects in embodied lexical-semantic processing
Wessel O. van Dam*, Shirley-Ann Rueschemeyer, Oliver Lindemann and Harold Bekkering
Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Netherlands
The embodied view of language comprehension proposes that the meaning of words is
grounded in perception and action rather than represented in abstract amodal symbols. Support
for embodied theories of language processing comes from behavioral studies showing that
understanding a sentence about an action can modulate congruent and incongruent physical
responses, suggesting motor involvement during comprehension of sentences referring to
bodily movement. Additionally, several neuroimaging studies have provided evidence that
comprehending single words denoting manipulable objects elicits specific responses in
the neural motor system. An interesting question that remains is whether action semantic
knowledge is directly activated as motor simulations in the brain, or rather modulated by the
semantic context in which action words are encountered. In the current paper we investigated
the nature of conceptual representations using a go/no-go lexical decision task. Specifically,
target words were either presented in a semantic context that emphasized dominant action
features (features related to the functional use of an object) or non-dominant action features. The
response latencies in a lexical decision task reveal that participants were faster to respond to
words denoting objects for which the functional use was congruent with the prepared movement.
This facilitation effect, however, was only apparent when the semantic context emphasized
corresponding motor properties. These findings suggest that conceptual processing is a context-
dependent process that incorporates motor-related knowledge in a flexible manner.
Keywords: embodiment, semantics, action, conceptual flexibility
Edited by:
Anna M Borghi, University of Bologna,
Italy
Reviewed by:
Veronique Boulenger, Centre National
de la Recherche Scientifique, France
Rutvik Desai, Medical College of
Wisconsin, USA
*Correspondence:
Wessel O. van Dam, P.O. Box 9104,
6500 HE Nijmegen, Netherlands.
e-mail: w.vandam@donders.ru.nl
Taylor (2006) have shown that motor resonance is activated on-line
during sentence comprehension. For example, while reading the
sentence The marathon runner opened the water bottle, evidence
for motor resonance is seen in conjunction with presentation of
the verb opened. Furthermore motor resonance is elicited by words
which unambiguously specify kinematic properties of actions, even
if these words are not verbs (Taylor et al., 2008). For example, in the
text He looked at the pie and turned the oven dial. The baking time
needed to be shorter/longer evidence for motor resonance is seen
in conjunction with the sentence final adjective (which specifies
how the oven dial should be turned) rather than in conjunction
with the verb turned.
In addition to words and sentences describing active events,
isolated words denoting manipulable objects have also been shown
to interact with motor behaviors and to activate neural motor
areas. For example, Glover et al. (2004) show that words denot-
ing objects that afford particular actions are sufficient to activate
motor representations. Participants in their study were primed
with the names of objects of different sizes (e.g., apple, grape) and
required to reach out and grasp a wooden block. Interestingly, the
maximum grip aperture during the reach (i.e., maximum dis-
tance between forefinger and thumb) was influenced by the size
of the object denoted by the prime word, despite the fact that
this information was irrelevant for executing the grasping task.
Similarly, Rueschemeyer et al. (2010a) had participants perform
a lexical decision task to words denoting objects typically brought
toward or away from the body for functional use (e.g., cup or key,
respectively). The authors found that responses to words were
facilitated if the required response action was congruent to the
IntroductIon
According to an embodied view of language comprehension, lan-
guage concepts are grounded in motor and perceptual systems
(Glenberg, 1997; Barsalou, 1999; Pulvermueller, 1999; Barsalou
et al., 2003). Lexical-semantic representations are postulated to
rely on sensori-motor brain areas and to reflect real-world experi-
ence with words’ referents. For example an object such as a ham-
mer is experienced by most people visually (i.e., we know what
hammers look like) and motorically (i.e., we know what it feels
like to wield a hammer). Thus, embodied language theories postu-
late that upon encountering the word hammer, experiential traces
stored in modality specific (e.g., visual and motor) brain areas are
activated. Importantly, these activations are seen to contribute to
the lexical-semantic meaning of the word form hammer. This view
differs fundamentally from symbolic (disembodied) accounts, in
which conceptual representations are symbolic and amodal, and
lexical-semantic meaning independent of real-world experience
(Kintsch, 2008).
Converging evidence for the idea that sensory-motor brain areas
are involved in language comprehension comes from behavioral,
electrophysiological, and neuroimaging studies. Glenberg and
Kaschak (2002) present the action-sentence compatibility effect
(ACE), in which reading a sentence that implies an action toward
or away from the body (e.g., open the drawer/close the drawer)
facilitates a congruent action (i.e., moving a hand toward or away
from the body). The authors argue that responses are facilitated
because comprehending language about action recruits the same
neural resources as required for action execution; thus compre-
hending the sentence primes a congruent motor act. Zwaan and
Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition October 2010 | Volume 1 | Article 150 | 2
van Dam et al. Conceptual flexibility
studies provide evidence that motor responses rather than being
automatic and invariable, depend on the context (i.e., sentence
context in the case of Raposo et al., 2009, and morphological con-
text in the case of Rueschemeyer et al., 2007) in which action words
are encountered. That is, a crucial factor for observing activity in
motor and premotor regions during action word processing seems
to be that the context emphasizes motor properties (suggesting
that representations are flexible).
The failure of some studies to report motor activation for action-
related words embedded in various contexts already speaks against
a strictly automatic interpretation, and suggests that motor activa-
tions may be called on in a flexible manner during word processing.
In the two studies cited above, motor information is not useful
in comprehending the given language utterances (i.e., idiomatic
phrases or morphologically complex words), and indeed the results
indicate that in these cases the motor system is not reliably activated.
However, even in cases in which motor information is helpful in
processing semantic content, a certain degree of flexibility on the
level of specific motor programs might be expected. For example,
the motor programs associated with the word cup are quite differ-
ent in the sentences She filled her cup at the tap/She drank from the
cup. In the first case, the actor in the sentence brings the cup away
from her body and toward the tap, in the second she brings the
cup toward her mouth. Previous research has shown that words
referring to manipulable objects activate motor areas (Chao and
Martin, 2000; Saccuman et al., 2006; Rueschemeyer et al., 2010a),
but since most objects can be used in multiple ways, does the motor
contribution to lexical-semantic comprehension also vary depend-
ing on the language context in which a word is presented?
In the current study, we investigated precisely this issue: within a
given modality (i.e., action) we investigated whether a word always
activates a specific motor program, or whether the motor program
activated depends on the context in which a word is presented.
Previously we (Rueschemeyer et al., 2010a) showed that compre-
hension of words denoting manipulable objects (e.g., cup, ham-
mer) are facilitated by the prior planning of an action congruent
to the prototypical use of the object. In other words, participants
were faster to respond to the word cup when they had planned an
action toward their body (the most common use of the cup being
a vessel to drink from) than when they had planned an action away
from their body; the opposite pattern was true for words denoting
objects typically brought away from the body (e.g., hammer). We
thus show a strong congruency effect between the typical action
associated with a word’s referent and processing of the individual
word. The target words in this previous study were presented in
isolation, i.e., without any explicit language context. In the current
study we investigated whether embedding the same words in a
lexical context suggesting a non-prototypical (but not unfamil-
iar) use of the objects would influence the observed congruency
effects. To this end object words were always preceded by a prime
word providing a context that emphasized either the dominant
action feature (thirst cup) or a non-dominant action feature
(sinkcup). As previous studies have shown that the retrieval of
a particular conceptual feature depends on the context in which
a word is encountered, we hypothesized that the effect of move-
ment preparation on word processing would interact with the
action typically performed on the word’s referent (i.e., responses
to cup were faster if a movement was made toward the body rather
than away from the body). This indicates that very specific infor-
mation about how an object is manipulated is activated during
lexical retrieval.
Evidence in favor of embodied theories of language also comes
from recent neuroimaging studies. Specifically, the comprehen-
sion of action verbs (Kemmerer et al., 2008), action sentences
(Desai et al., 2010), and words denoting manipulable objects
(Chao and Martin, 2000; Saccuman et al., 2006; Rueschemeyer
et al., 2010b) all reliably activate the cerebral motor system.
Furthermore, embodied lexical-semantic representations activate
the neural motor system in a somatotopic manner (Hauk et al.,
2004; Tettamanti et al., 2005; Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2006; but see also
de Zubicaray et al., 2010). Findings from the studies reviewed
above provide strong evidence for functional links between the
neural motor system and lexical-semantic processing of words
that entail a motor component.
It has been suggested that embodied lexical-semantic repre-
sentations are fast, automatic and invariant. Pulvermueller et al.
(2000) demonstrated that category-specific activation can be
observed as early as about 200 ms after word onset (see also Hauk
and Pulvermueller, 2004), and occurs irrespective of attention to
action words (Shtyrov et al., 2004; Pulvermueller et al., 2005).
Pulvermueller proposes that strong functional links between lan-
guage and motor systems have developed as a consequence of the
fact that actions and their referents often co-occur near-simulta-
neously. Specifically, the action and the word co-occur frequently,
and thereby, neural populations recruited for processing a word
form and those involved in processing the referent body movement
frequently fire together and become strongly linked (Pulvermueller,
1999, 2001). Due to the strong within-assembly connections that
link language and action representations, action word recognition
will thereby automatically and invariably trigger activation in spe-
cific action-related networks.
However, in contrast to what would be expected if embodied
representations are indeed automatic and invariant, several stud-
ies have failed to find motor-related activity for words with an
action-semantic component (Rueschemeyer et al., 2007; Raposo
et al., 2009). Raposo et al. (2009) showed in a recent fMRI study
that action verbs in isolation (e.g., kick) or in literal sentences
(e.g., kick the ball) elicit a response in motor/premotor cortices.
Action verbs in an idiomatic context (e.g., kick the bucket), how-
ever, did not elicit such activations. These findings strongly chal-
lenge the automaticity of motor-related activity for action words
and rather suggests that the activation of meaning attributes of
words is a flexible and contextually dependent process (but see
also Boulenger et al., 2009). In a similar vein, Rueschemeyer et al.
(2007) showed that processing of morphologically complex verbs
built on motor stems showed no differences in involvement of
the motor system when compared with processing complex verbs
with abstract stems. For example, the difference between the verb
begreifen (to comprehend) vs. bedenken (to think). In the first case
the morphologically complex verb is a prefixed form of the sim-
ple motor verb greifen (to grasp), whereas in the latter case it is
a prefixed form of the abstract verb denken (to think). All these
www.frontiersin.org October 2010 | Volume 1 | Article 150 | 3
van Dam et al. Conceptual flexibility
semantic context in which a word is encountered. Specifically, in
trials in which the semantic context emphasizes the dominant
motor properties of a concept, we expect faster onset latencies to
words denoting objects for which the functional use is congruent
with the prepared movement in directionality. Furthermore, we
expect that this facilitation effect is not present if the semantic
context emphasizes motor properties of a concept that are not
related to the object’s functional use.
ExpErImEnt
mEthod
Participants
Thirty-five subjects participated; the average age was 21.3 years.
All subjects were students at the Radboud University Nijmegen
and participated either for money or course credit. No subject was
aware of the purpose of the experiment.
Stimuli
Words were presented in white Arial fonts on a black background.
The average word length was 10 letters. For a viewing distance
of 100 cm, the stimuli subtended on average a visual angle of
0.57° × 2.86°. A total of 120 letter string stimuli were created for
the experiment (stimuli with English translations can be seen in
Table 1). Eighty were real Dutch words denoting familiar objects
and comprised the critical experimental stimuli. The remaining 40
stimuli were Dutch pseudowords (i.e., phonotactically and ortho-
graphically legal letter strings with no meaning in Dutch). Target
stimuli belonged to one of two experimental conditions: (1) words
denoting objects for which the functional use is associated with a
movement toward the body (e.g., telephone, photo camera), and
(2) words denoting objects for which the functional use is associ-
ated with a movement away from the body (e.g., hammer, pencil).
Target stimuli were presented in two contexts: (1) target words were
preceded by a word that emphasized the action feature related to
the functional use (e.g., conversation-telephone, nail-hammer), and
(2) target words were preceded by a word that emphasized an action
feature not related to the functional use (e.g., adapter-telephone,
tool belt-hammer).
To test that stimuli were truly matched with regards to important
psycholinguistic variables, a questionnaire was administered to 15
native Dutch speakers who did not participate in the behavioral
experiment (see Table 2 for results). In this questionnaire, par-
ticipants were asked to rate words on a 7-point scale with respect
to (1) the imageability of the noun (1 = very difficult to imagine
the referent noun, 7 = very easy to imagine the referent noun),
(2) whether the noun denoted an object that you typically bring
toward or away from the body (3 = toward the body, +3 = away
from the body).
The results of the questionnaire showed that nouns were matched
across the two conditions with respect to imageability (body:
M = 6.82, SE = 0.024; world: M = 6.76, SE = 0.029), t(1,78) = 1.46,
p > 0.1. For object nouns, participants agreed that body words
referred to objects that you typically bring toward the body, world
words referred to objects you typically bring away from the body
(body: M = 1.13, SE = 0.049; world: M = 2.33, SE = 0.057), both
means significantly differed from 0 as indicated by one-sample
Table 1 | Dutch words associated with a movement toward the body
(body words) and a movement away from the body (world words).
English translations are printed in italics.
Body words World words
Haarband Hair ribbon/Hairband Zwabber Swab/Mob
La Drawer Spaarpot Money box
Sjaal Scarf Spade Spade/Shovel
Microfoon Microphone Zaag Saw
Loep Magnifying glass Vaas Vase
Hoed Hat Naald Needle
Nagelvijl Nail file Kaars Candle
Pleister Band-Aid Plant Plant
Fluit Flute Flesopener Bottle opener
Bril (pair of) Spectacles Deegroller Rolling pin
Wijnglas Wine glass Koekenpan Frying pin
Mok Mug Voetbal Football
Make-up Make up Theepot Teapot
Zakdoek Handkerchief Speld Pin
Lolly Lollipop Stempel Stamp
Halssnoer Necklace Sleutel Key
Helm Helmet Lamp Lamp
Telefoon Telephone Schep Scoop/Shovel
Shampoo Shampoo Knikker Marble
Armband Bracelet Bijl Axe
Tondeuse (pair of) Clippers Baksteen Brick
Mondharmonica Mouth organ Fakkel Torch
Want Toothbrush Bel Bell
Tandenborstel Mitten Hamer Hammer
Handdoek Towel Computer Computer
Ring Ring Hengel Fishing rod
Trompet Trumpet Pen Pen
Schoen Shoe Boor Drill
Lippenstift Lipstick Gloeilamp Lightbulb
Lepel Spoon Trommel Drum
Oorbel Earring Kapstok Coat rack
Borstel Brush Verfpot Pot/Tin of paint
Verrekijker Binoculars Vergiet Stainer/Colander
Stropdas Tie/Necktie Karaf Decanter/Carafe
Gordel Belt Garde Whisk
Vork Fork Paraplu Umbrella
Parfum Perfume Mes Knife
Horloge Watch Potlood Pencil
Jas Jacket Ventilator Fan
Fototoestel Photo camera Dobbelsteen Dice
t-tests (all p-values <0.001). In order to obtain an objective measure
of word frequency, we calculated the mean lemma frequency per
million for each condition using the lexical database (Baayen et al.,
1993). This gave a mean of 567 (SE = 128.3) for the body words
and 487 (SE = 119.3) for the world words. An independent sample
t-test indicated words were matched on frequency, t(1,78) = 1.48,
p > 0.1. Additionally, independent sample t-tests indicated that
nouns were matched with regard to length (body: M = 6.8, world:
M = 6.3), t(1,78) = 0.90, p > 0.2.
Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition October 2010 | Volume 1 | Article 150 | 4
van Dam et al. Conceptual flexibility
Data analysis
We measured the latencies to recognize that a presented Dutch
word was lexically valid defined as the time difference between
word onset and release of the start button. Average RTs can be seen
in Figure 2. Additionally, we recorded movement times (MTs; i.e.,
the time from releasing the start button until depressing the target
button). Trials with extreme RTs or MTs (2 × STD ± mean) were
treated as outliers and excluded from further analysis. This led to
an exclusion of 9.12% of the data. The significance criterion for all
analyses was set to α = 0.05.
rEsults
The error rates in the lexical decision task were on average lower than
2% and therefore not further analyzed. RTs were averaged for each
participant in each condition (see Table 3 for means) and submitted
to a two-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with
factors Action Congruency (congruent vs. incongruent) and Context
(focus on dominant vs. non-dominant action feature). The main effect
for Action Congruency and Context did not reach significance, both
p-values >0.2. Importantly, in line with our hypothesis, the ANOVA
revealed a significant interaction F(1,34) = 5.76, p < 0.05,
ηp
20145=.,
indicating that the RT differences between action congruent and
action incongruent words were modulated by the different context
conditions. To further explore this interaction, we calculated post hoc
paired sample t-tests. When the context emphasized the dominant
action feature, words congruent with the prepared action were proc-
essed faster than incongruent words, t(34) = 2.21, p < 0.05. However,
if the context focused on non-dominated action features, the mean
RTs to action congruent and action incongruent words did not differ
statistically, t(34) < 1, and the RT pattern reversed descriptively.
Movement times were averaged for each participant in each
condition and submitted to a two-way repeated measures ANOVA.
The main effect for Action Congruency and Context did not reach
significance, both p-values >0.05. Furthermore, the ANOVA did not
reveal a significant interaction (p > 0.5).
Procedure and design
Participants were seated comfortably in front of a computer moni-
tor, and responded by means of a key press (i.e., by pressing either
a key that was located nearer or further from the body). To start a
trial participants had to press the start button of a response device
(located in the middle of the response device). Subsequently, they
received a cue (i.e., the letters A or B) that signaled them to prepare
a movement (either toward or away from the body) which they
only executed if the second word was lexically valid. Participants
were instructed to read both words carefully. The advent of the
two words was signaled by a fixation cross (appearing centrally
for 500 ms). The first word was presented for 1000 ms. The second
word appeared 1000 ms after the offset of the first word, calling
for a speeded response to the identity of the word (i.e., a response
in the case that the second word was a real word in Dutch). The
second word remained visible until participants responded, or for
a maximum of 2000 ms (see Figure 1 for an illustration of a Go
and NoGo trial). That is, the first presented word was not clearly
associated with a specific direction or action, the differences
we report are relative differences between reaction times (RTs)
acquired in response to the second presented word. The experi-
ment comprised 160 critical trials composed by 20 replications
of the factorial combination of two movement cues, two word
types, and two contexts. That is, the direction of the participant’s
response and the motor program generally associated with the
word’s referent either corresponded or not (action congruent
vs. action incongruent) and the word was either presented in a
language context highlighting the functional use or a less typical
use of the object (focus on dominant vs. non-dominant action
feature). The order in which word pairs were presented was coun-
terbalanced over participants. That is, half of the participants
first saw a target word (e.g., telephone) preceded by a word that
emphasized the dominant action feature (conversation) and then
the same target word preceded by a word that emphasized a non-
dominant action feature (adapter) and vice versa.
FIGURE 1 | Illustration of a Go and NoGo trial.
www.frontiersin.org October 2010 | Volume 1 | Article 150 | 5
van Dam et al. Conceptual flexibility
flexible and contextually dependent process, and not a process
of an automatic and invariant simulation of a specific motor
program. In other words, semantic features are flexibly recruited
with respect to the type of properties emphasized by the semantic
context (see also Hoenig et al., 2008; Masson et al., 2008; Raposo
et al., 2009).
The congruency effect in the present study extends the results of
our previous research on motor involvement in language processing
(Rueschemeyer et al., 2010a) and demonstrates the important role
of contextual information in the embodied processing of language.
We show that preparing an action congruent to the typical, func-
tional use of an object facilitates processing of the word denoting
the object. We suggest that words presented in isolation rely on a
default representation, which highlights the typical functional use
of the object. In our current results we extend this finding to show
(not surprisingly) that words presented in a context highlighting
the denoted object’s functional use are also facilitated by preparing
a congruent action.
Interestingly, the action congruency did not affect the word
processing time if the semantic context emphasized action fea-
tures not belonging to the core of the concept. There are two
alternative explanations for this finding: first, it is possible that
motor semantic features are not activated at all in a non- dominant
action context. Alternatively, it is possible that dominant concep-
tual features are co-activated along with non-dominant features.
In other words, cup in the context of sink activates both motor
programs associated with moving a cup toward the sink and
motor programs associated with bringing the cup toward the
mouth. This activation of two opposite motor programs might
have resulted in a null effect for trials in which properties unre-
lated to the functional use of the object are emphasized. In this
scenario, the priming effect of movement preparation on lexi-
cal access is canceled out, because motor codes underlying two
opposing movements are activated simultaneously. This explana-
tion is in line with the findings of Hoenig et al. (2008) in which
they show that activation in regions coding non-dominant object
features increased if semantic context encouraged participants
to focus on the non-dominant feature. However, brain regions
coding information about dominant features were co-activated,
even when not directly probed by the task. The authors argue that
dominant object features are co-activated with non-dominant
object features through collateral support (see also Farah and
McClelland, 1991).
The reliable congruency effect for trials in which there was a
contextual focus on action properties related to the functional use
of the object, bolsters the claim that in processing of a word with
an action-semantic component we activate information stored in
modality specific sensory-motor systems (Hoenig et al., 2008). The
present study, however, cannot determine whether motor system
involvement is a fundamental necessity or a consequence of word
comprehension. One might argue that the reason for observing
a congruency effect between prepared movement direction and
associated movement direction is because the person voluntarily
images the functional use of the referent object, after the meaning
of the object word is already understood. The design of our current
study prevents us from ruling out this possibility. For a better under-
standing of the exact nature of the interaction between context
dIscussIon
The present study clearly demonstrates contextual effects on
embodied word processing, evidenced by a reliable Action
Congruency × Context interaction effect. Specifically, a congru-
ency effect (i.e., faster word recognition times) is observed for
trials in which the direction of the participant’s response and
the motor program generally associated with the word’s refer-
ent correspond, but only if the word is presented in a language
context highlighting the functional use of the object (e.g., thirst
cup). In a language context highlighting a less typical use of
the object (e.g., sink cup), the congruency effect disappears.
These findings suggest that activation of modality specific (in
this case motor) information during word comprehension is a
FIGURE 2 | Average reaction times (RTs) for words, as a function of the
congruency between the cue and associated movement direction, and
the contextual focus (focus on dominant action feature vs. non-dominant
action feature).
Table 3 | Average performance rates (PR), reaction times (RTs), and
movement times (MTs) with standard errors for congruent and
incongruent trials in both the dominant focus and non-dominant focus
condition.
PR (SE) RT (SE) MT (SE)
DOMINANT FOCUS
Congruent 98.4 (0.26) 543 (14.73) 276 (16.05)
Incongruent 98.3 (0.37) 553 (16.21) 272 (15.55)
NON-DOMINANT FOCUS
Congruent 98.6 (0.35) 551 (15.54) 272 (15.51)
Incongruent 98.3 (0.37) 548 (15.84) 269 (15.69)
Table 2 | Mean ratings of the pre-tests.
Body words World words
Length 6.8 6.3
Lemma frequency 567 487
per million (CELEX)
Imageability 6.82 6.76
Action association 1.33 2.33
Frontiers in Psychology | Cognition October 2010 | Volume 1 | Article 150 | 6
van Dam et al. Conceptual flexibility
rEfErEncEs
Aziz-Zadeh, L., Wilson, S. M., Rizzolatti, G.,
and Iacoboni, M. (2006). Congruent
embodied representations for visu-
ally presented actions and linguistic
phrases describing actions. Curr. Biol.
16, 1818–1823.
Baayen, R., Piepenbrock, R., and Van
Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX Lexical
Database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia,
PA: Linguistic Data Consortium,
University of Pennsylvania
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual sym-
bol systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 22,
577–660.
Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey,
A. K., and Wilson, C. D. (2003).
Grounding conceptual knowledge
in modality-specific systems. Trends
Cogn. Sci. 7, 84–91.
Boulenger, V., Hauk, O., and Pulvermueller,
F. (2009). Grasping ideas with your
motor system: semantic somatotopy
in idiom comprehension. Cer eb.
Cortex 19, 1905–1914.
Chao, L. L., and Martin, A. (2000).
Representation of manipulable man-
made objects in the dorsal stream.
Neuroimage 12, 478–484.
Desai, R. H., Binder, J. R., Conant, L.
L., and Seidenberg, M. S. (2010).
Activation of sensory-motor areas
in sentence comprehension. Cereb.
Cortex 20, 468–478.
de Zubicaray, G., Postle, N., McMahon, K.,
Meredith, M., and Ashton, R. (2010).
Mirror neurons, the representation
of word meaning, and the foot of the
third left frontal convolution. Brain
Lang. 112, 77–84.
Farah, M. J., and McClelland, J. L. (1991).
A computational model of semantic
memory impairment: modality specif-
icity and emergent category specificity.
J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 120, 339–357.
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is
for. Behav. Brain Sci. 20, 1–55.
Glenberg, A. M., and Kaschak, M. (2002).
Grounding language in action.
Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 558–565.
Glover, S., Rosenbaum, D. A., Graham,
J., and Dixon, P. (2004). Grasping the
meaning of words. Exp. Brain Res. 154,
103–108.
Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., and Pulvermueller,
F. (2004). Somatotopic representation
of action words in human motor
and premotor cortex. Neuron 41,
301–307.
Hauk, O., and Pulvermueller, F. (2004).
Neurophysiological distinction of
action words in the fronto-central cor-
tex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 21, 191–201.
Hoenig, K., Sim, E.-J., Bochev, V.,
Herrnberger, B., and Kiefer, M.
(2008). Conceptual flexibility in the
human brain: dynamic recruitment
of semantic maps from visual, motor,
and motion-related areas. J. Cogn.
Neurosci. 20, 1799–1814.
Kemmerer, D., Castillo, J. G., Talavage,
T., Patterson, S., and Wiley, C. (2008).
Neuroanatomical distribution of five
semantic components of verbs: evi-
dence from fMRI. Brain Lang. 107,
16–43.
Kintsch, W. (2008). “Symbol systems
and perceptual representations,” in
Symbols and Embodiment, eds M. De
Vega, A. Glenberg, and A. Graesser
(Oxford: Oxford University Press),
145–164.
Masson, M., Bub, D., and Warren, C.
(2008). Kicking calculators: contri-
bution of embodied representations
to sentence comprehension. J. Mem.
Lang. 59, 256–265.
Pulvermueller, F. (1999). Words in the
brain’s language. Behav. Brain Sci. 22,
253–336.
Pulvermueller, F. (2001). Brain reflections
of words and their meaning. Trends
Cogn. Sci. 5, 517–524.
Pulvermueller, F., Härle, M., and Hummel,
F. (2000). Neurophysiological distinc-
tion of verb categories. Neuroreport 11,
2789–2793.
Pulvermue ller, F., Shtyrov, Y., an d
Ilmoniemi, R. (2005). Brain signa-
tures of meaning access in action
word recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
17, 884–892.
Raposo, A., Moss, H. E., Stamatakis, E. A.,
and Tyler, L. K. (2009). Modulation
of motor and premotor cortices by
actions, action words and action
sentences. Neuropsychologia 47,
388–396.
Rueschemeyer, S.-A., Brass, M.,
and Friederici, A. D. (2007).
Comprehending prehending: neural
correlates of processing verbs with
motor stems. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19,
855–865.
Rueschemeyer, S.-A., Pfeiffer, C., and
Bekkering, H. (2010a). Body schemat-
ics: on the role of the body schema in
embodied lexical-semantic representa-
tions. Neuropsychologia 48, 774–781.
Rueschemeyer, S.-A., van Rooij, D.,
Lindemann, O., Willems, R., and
Bekkering, H. (2010b). The function
of words: distinct neural correlates for
words denoting differently manipu-
lable objects. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22,
1844–1851.
Saccuman, M. C., Cappa, S. F., Bates, E. A.,
Arevalo, A., Della Rosa, P., Danna, M.,
and Perani, D. (2006). The impact of
semantic reference on word class: an
fMRI study of action and object nam-
ing. Neuroimage 32, 1865–1878.
Shtyrov, Y., Hauk, O., and Pulvermueller,
F. (2004). Distributed neuronal net-
works for encoding category-specific
semantic information: the mismatch
negativity to action words. Eur. J.
Neurosci. 19, 1083–1092.
Taylor, L. J., Lev-Ari, S., and Zwaan, R.
A. (2008). Inferences about action
engage action systems. Brain Lang.
107, 62–67.
Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman,
M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo,
P., Fazio, F., Rizzolatti, G., Cappa, S.
F., and Perani, D. (2005). Listening
to action-related sentences activates
fronto-parietal motor circuits. J. Cogn.
Neurosci. 17, 273–281.
Zwaan, R. A., and Taylor, L. J. (2006).
Seeing, acting, understanding: motor
resonance in language comprehen-
sion. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 135, 1–11.
Conflict of Interest Statement: The
authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any com-
mercial or financial relationships that
could be construed as a potential conflict
of interest.
Received: 16 June 2010; paper pending pub-
lished: 03 July 2010; accepted: 21 August
2010; published online: 04 October 2010.
Citation: van Dam WO, Rueschemeyer
S-A, Lindemann O and Bekkering H
(2010) Context effects in embodied lexical-
semantic processing. Front. Psychology
1:150. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00150
This article was submitted to Frontiers
in Cognition, a specialty of Frontiers in
Psychology.
Copyright © 2010 van Dam, Rueschemeyer,
Lindemann and Bekkering. This is an open-
access article subject to an exclusive license
agreement between the authors and the
Frontiers Research Foundation, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the
original authors and source are credited.
and embodied word processing, we need to further investigate the
temporal dynamics of the observed contextual effects on embodied
word processing.
conclusIon
The current study demonstrates that the interaction between
lexical-semantic processing and movement preparation processes
was modulated by the context in which these words were encoun-
tered. Together the data suggest that context plays a fundamental
role in sensory-motor activations during language processing. That
is, the activation of specific motor properties in language compre-
hension is flexible and context-dependent.
AcknowlEdgmEnts
The authors thank P. de Water, and G. van Oijen for technical
support. The study was supported by NWO-Veni grant nr (016-
094-053) to Shirley-Ann Rueschemeyer and NWO-VICI grant nr
(453–05–001) to Harold Bekkering.
... For instance, sensorimotor features are essential for the representation of concrete (i.e., material) concepts (e.g., APPLE), whereas abstract meanings (e.g., LOYALTY) rely more on features derived from linguistic and affective experience, because their referents lack those direct sensorimotor attributes (Dove, 2018;Kousta, Vigliocco, Vinson, Andrews, and Del Campo, 2011). Moreover, during retrieval, context/task-relevant conceptual features are prioritized, leading to observable context effects on both behaviour (e.g., Tousignant and Pexman, 2012;Van Dam, Rueschemeyer, Lindemann, and Bekkering, 2010) and neural activity (e.g., Kuhnke, Kiefer, and Hartwigsen, 2020;Muraki, Doyle, Protzner, and Pexman, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Multiple representation theories posit that concepts are represented via a combination of properties derived from sensorimotor, affective, and linguistic experiences. Recently, it has been proposed that information derived from social experience, or socialness, represents another key aspect of conceptual representation. How these various dimensions interact to form a coherent conceptual space has yet to be fully explored. To address this, we capitalized on openly available word property norms for 6339 words and conducted a large-scale investigation into the relationships between 18 dimensions. An exploratory factor analysis reduced the dimensions to six higher-order factors: sub-lexical, distributional, visuotactile, body action, affective and social interaction. All these factors explained unique variance in performance on lexical and semantic tasks, demonstrating that they make important contributions to the representation of word meaning. An important and novel finding was that the socialness dimension clustered with the auditory modality and with mouth and head actions. We suggest this reflects experiential learning from verbal interpersonal interactions. Moreover, formally modelling the network structure of semantic space revealed pairwise partial correlations between most dimensions and highlighted the centrality of the interoception dimension. Altogether, these findings provide new insights into the architecture of conceptual space, including the importance of inner and social experience, and highlight promising avenues for future research.
... In this view -which, at this point, remains speculative -the DMN itself represents semantic content at a level of representation in which information about various experiential modalities is integrated and compressed, resulting in a lack of experiential detail. This compressed, experientially impoverished content is what allows the DMN to modulate (via reciprocal connections) the appropriate lower-level representations stored in other areas, which, we propose, provide additional experiential information as needed to achieve the intended goal (for evidence of context-dependent recruitment of sensory-motor representations, see Kuhnke et al., 2020;Muraki, Doyle, Protzner & Pexman, 2023;van Dam, Brazil, Bekkering, & Rueschemeyer, 2014;van Dam, Dijk, Bekkering, & Rueschemeyer, 2011;van Dam, Rueschemeyer, Lindemann, & Bekkering, 2010;Wang, Gao, Smallwood, & Jefferies, 2021). The DMN's record of coactivation patterns, by itself, may not be sufficient to support a situation model that is distinct and detailed enough for the task at hand, and the additional information stored in lower-level areas may be essential. ...
... Semantic processing is crucial in reading comprehension. Many previous studies advocate the embodied view of semantic processing and believe that semantic processing involves readers' sensorimotor system (Barsalou, 2008;Dam et al., 2010;García et al., 2019;Phillips et al., 2012;Zwaan, 2016). Along with this view, mental simulation theory further proposes that readers can automatically or subconsciously simulate the textual descriptions during semantic processing in natural reading (Mak, 2022;Mak & Willems, 2021;Zwaan, 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
The embodied view of semantic processing holds that readers achieve reading comprehension through mental simulation of the objects and events described in the narrative. However, it remains unclear whether and how the encoding of linguistic factors in narrative descriptions impacts narrative semantic processing. This study aims to explore this issue under the narrative context with and without perspective shift, which is an important and common linguistic factor in narratives. A sentence-picture verification paradigm combined with eye-tracking measures was used to explore the issue. The results showed that (1) the inter-role perspective shift made the participants’ to evenly allocate their first fixation to different elements in the scene following the new perspective; (2) the internal–external perspective shift increased the participants’ total fixation count when they read the sentence with the perspective shift; (3) the scene detail depicted in the picture did not influence the process of narrative semantic processing. These results suggest that perspective shift can disrupt the coherence of situation model and increase the cognitive load of readers during reading. Moreover, scene detail could not be constructed by readers in natural narrative reading.
... Indeed, textual context prompts specific emotional, empathic, and mentalistic operations that ground these concepts in actual social experience 2,9,24 . Though blind to the individual role of such socio-cognitive variables, text-level paradigms thus enable more naturalistic assessments of social concepts, addressing calls for ecologically valid insights on the construct 9, 24 and on language processing at large [36][37][38][39][40][41][42] . ...
Article
Full-text available
Situated models suggest that social concepts are grounded in interpersonal experience. However, few studies have tested this notion experimentally, and none has targeted individuals with reduced social interaction. Here, we assessed comprehension of text-level social and non-social concepts in persons with and without autistic-like traits. Participants read a social and a non-social text and answered questionnaires targeting social and non-social concepts, respectively. We compared behavioral outcomes, gauged their contribution to subject-level classification, and examined their association with validated measures of autism. Persons with autistic-like traits showed selective deficits in grasping text-level social concepts, even adjusting for intelligence, memory, and vocabulary. Also, social concept comprehension was the only variable that significantly classified between groups. Finally, social concept outcomes correlated negatively with measures of autism, including social interaction. Our results suggest that reduced interpersonal experience selectively compromises text-level social concept processing, offering empirical constraints for situated models of social semantics.
... The grounding theory of cognition argues that humans process abstract concepts by creating a perceptual representation that is inherently concrete as it is generated through exposure to real world situations using our five senses (Van Dam et al., 2010;Brysbaert et al., 2014). However, more recent work brings forth evidence suggesting that such representations incorporate both perceptual and nonperceptual features (Dove, 2009;Naumann et al., 2018;Frassinelli and Schulte im Walde, 2019). ...
... Tomasino and Rumiati (2013) mentioned that sensorimotor activation was not automatically elicited by linguistic stimuli (words or sentences). Instead, the involvement of sensorimotor activation can depend on the context of language processing (van Dam et al., 2010;van Dam, van Dijk et al., 2012;van Dam, van Dongen et al., 2012) ...
Article
The purpose of this paper is to suggest a strategy to enhance vocabulary learning through a sensory-motor experience. A case study was conducted involving four college students who were all non-English majors and had intermediate TOEIC scores. The study emphasized the role of the brain. The material used was from season 1, episode 1 of the American television series Community (Russo & Russo, 2009). During the study, the four students participated in one instruction session, did three test activities assessing attention and saliency, collocation, memorization, and had an oral interview. In the attention and saliency test, the results revealed that three of the four participants were successful. In the collocation test, three participants were excellent, and one participant was fair. In memorization of an entire dialogue, Participant A was perfect and the other three participants were very good, despite some minor errors. In the oral interview, three participants were positive about using this new method, and one was critical. This participant preferred the traditional vocabulary learning method of matching form-meanings with vocabulary lists. The participants all agreed that this method was innovative and fun, but they all indicated that the instruction was too complicated and suggested that it should be adjusted to make it more understandable.
... It is also possible that the richness or amount of detail present in language simulations varies across individuals and reading conditions. Indeed, in expert domains or in the case of richer linguistic context, we might expect language simulations to be more precise, construed, and robust [68][69][70][71][72][73][74][75]. In this regard, quantifying and comparing the richness of simulations generated by action reading and motor imagery remains an important goal for future research. ...
Article
Full-text available
While action language and motor imagery both engage the motor system, determining whether these two processes indeed share the same motor representations would contribute to better understanding their underlying mechanisms. We conducted two experiments probing the mutual influence of these two processes. In Exp.1, hand-action verbs were presented subliminally, and participants (n = 36) selected the verb they thought they perceived from two alternatives. When congruent actions were imagined prior to this task, accuracy significantly increased, i.e. participants were better able to “see” the subliminal verbs. In Exp.2, participants (n = 19) imagined hand flexion or extension, while corticospinal excitability was measured via transcranial magnetic stimulation. Corticospinal excitability was modulated by action verbs subliminally presented prior to imagery. Specifically, the typical increase observed during imagery was suppressed after presentation of incongruent action verbs. This mutual influence of action language and motor imagery, both at behavioral and neurophysiological levels, suggests overlapping motor representations.
Article
Full-text available
Controversy persists regarding the representation of linguistically negated actions, specifically concerning activation and inhibitory mechanisms in the motor system, and whether negated action sentences evoke an initial motor simulation of the action to be negated. We conducted two experiments probing corticospinal excitability (CSE) and short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI) in the primary motor cortex at different latencies while reading affirmative and negative action sentences. In experiment one, twenty-six participants read action and non-action sentences in affirmative or negative forms. Using transcranial magnetic stimulation, we probed CSE in hand muscles at rest and at several latencies after verb presentation. We observed a greater CSE for action sentences compared to non-action sentences, regardless of verb form. In experiment two, nineteen participants read affirmative and negative action sentences. We measured CSE and SICI at short and long latencies after verb presentation. CSE was greater for affirmative and negative action sentences at both latencies compared to rest. SICI did not change at the short latency but increased at longer latencies, regardless of verb form. Negated action sentences showed the same motor excitability as affirmed action sentences with no additional inhibition at early latencies. These results lend support for the idea that actions to be negated are initially simulated within the motor system. Neural differences between affirmative and negative action sentences may occur outside the primary motor cortex.
Chapter
Hierarchical visual attention refers to the global and local visual pathways that process information that gets integrated to support where one places their attention visually. These Pathways have been found to integrate slower than is typical causing overwhelm and misinformation. This chapter explores this hidden dimension of vision by discussing eye movements, technologies that employ eye trackers to study eye movements, and technology that projects the filter images. By low level characteristics of the image–specifically the low level sensory processing that is manipulated is special frequency and luminance. This is done to alleviate some of the processing burden of the global and local integration disturbance, also known as local interference. Discussion of local interference in other neurodivergent conditions is also discussed specifically in ADHD and dyslexia. Design implications around filtering out distraction visually are provided.
Article
Full-text available
Action affordances can be activated by non-target objects in the visual field as well as by word labels attached to target objects. These activations have been manifested in interference effects of distractors and words on actions. We examined whether affordances could be activated implicitly by words representing graspable objects that were either large (e.g., APPLE) or small (e.g., GRAPE) relative to the target. Subjects first read a word and then grasped a wooden block. Interference effects of the words arose in the early portions of the grasping movements. Specifically, early in the movement, reading a word representing a large object led to a larger grip aperture than reading a word representing a small object. This difference diminished as the hand approached the target, suggesting on-line correction of the semantic effect. The semantic effect and its on-line correction are discussed in the context of ecological theories of visual perception, the distinction between movement planning and control, and the proximity of language and motor planning systems in the human brain.
Article
Full-text available
Evocation of motor representations during sentence comprehension was examined by training subjects to make a hand action in response to a visual cue while listening to a sentence. Sentences referred to manipulable objects that were either related or unrelated to the cued action. Related actions pertained either to the function of the object or to its volumetric properties (e.g., shape). The results demonstrate priming of hand actions even though the sentences referred to non-manual interactions with manipulable objects. When sentences described an attentional interaction (looking at the calculator), only functional actions were primed. Sentences describing a non-manual physical interaction (kicking the calculator) primed volumetric as well as functional actions. We describe how seemingly irrelevant motor representations can play a role in constructing sentence meaning.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research indicates that language processing relies on brain areas dedicated to perception and action. For example, processing words denoting manipulable objects has been shown to activate a fronto-parietal network involved in actual tool use. This is suggested to reflect the knowledge the subject has about how objects are moved and used. However, information about how to use an object may be much more central to the conceptual representation of an object than information about how to move an object. Therefore, there may be much more fine-grained distinctions between objects on the neural level, especially related to the usability of manipulable objects. In the current study, we investigated whether a distinction can be made between words denoting (1) objects that can be picked up to move (e.g., volumetrically manipulable objects: bookend, clock) and (2) objects that must be picked up to use (e.g., functionally manipulable objects: cup, pen). The results show that functionally manipulable words elicit greater levels of activation in the fronto-parietal sensorimotor areas than volumetrically manipulable words. This suggests that indeed a distinction can be made between different types of manipulable objects. Specifically, how an object is used functionally rather than whether an object can be displaced with the hand is reflected in semantic representations in the brain.
Article
The relationship between verbal mental representations and action, perception and emotion is discussed. It is argued that symbols are not directly grounded in the real world but are made meaningful because of their relationship with other symbols. Symbols are not defined by the real world, but symbols mirror the real world. An isomorphism exists between symbolic representations and the world of action and perception, but the meaning of symbols cannot be reduced to perception and action. This argument is illustrated with some examples from LSA and related models of verbal meaning.
Article
If the cortex is an associative memory, strongly connected cell assemblies will form when neurons in different cortical areas are frequently active at the same time. The cortical distributions of these assemblies must be a consequence of where in the cortex correlated neuronal activity occurred during learning. An assembly can be considered a functional unit exhibiting activity states such as full activation (“ignition”) after appropriate sensory stimulation (possibly related to perception) and continuous reverberation of excitation within the assembly (a putative memory process). This has implications for cortical topographies and activity dynamics of cell assemblies forming during language acquisition, in particular for those representing words. Cortical topographies of assemblies should be related to aspects of the meaning of the words they represent, and physiological signs of cell assembly ignition should be followed by possible indicators of reverberation. The following postulates are discussed in detail: (1) assemblies representing phonological word forms are strongly lateralized and distributed over perisylvian cortices; (2) assemblies representing highly abstract words such as grammatical function words are also strongly lateralized and restricted to these perisylvian regions; (3) assemblies representing concrete content words include additional neurons in both hemispheres; (4) assemblies representing words referring to visual stimuli include neurons in visual cortices; and (5) assemblies representing words referring to actions include neurons in motor cortices. Two main sources of evidence are used to evaluate these proposals: (a) imaging studies focusing on localizing word processing in the brain, based on stimulus-triggered event-related potentials (ERPs), positron emission tomography (PET), and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and (b) studies of the temporal dynamics of fast activity changes in the brain, as revealed by high-frequency responses recorded in the electroencephalogram (EEG) and magnetoencephalogram (MEG). These data provide evidence for processing differences between words and matched meaningless pseudowords, and between word classes, such as concrete content and abstract function words, and words evoking visual or motor associations. There is evidence for early word class-specific spreading of neuronal activity and for equally specific high-frequency responses occurring later. These results support a neurobiological model of language in the Hebbian tradition. Competing large-scale neuronal theories of language are discussed in light of the data summarized. Neurobiological perspectives on the problem of serial order of words in syntactic strings are considered in closing.
Article
I address the commentators' calls for clarification of theoretical terms, discussion of similarities to other proposals, and extension of the ideas. In doing so, I keep the focus on the purpose of memory: enabling the organism to make sense of its environment so that it can take action appropriate to constraints resulting from the physical, personal, social, and cultural situations.
Raw Data
Describes 124,136 lemmas and 381,292 wordforms in Dutch, 52,446 lemmas and 160,594 wordforms in English, and 50,708 lemmas and 359,611 wordforms in German. The information is presented in a series of plain ASCII files that can be queried with tools such as AWK and ICON. Unique identity numbers allow the linking of information from different files.
Article
The Simulation Framework, also known as the Embodied Cognition Framework, maintains that conceptual knowledge is grounded in sensorimotor systems. To test several predictions that this theory makes about the neural substrates of verb meanings, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to scan subjects’ brains while they made semantic judgments involving five classes of verbs—specifically, Running verbs (e.g., run, jog, walk), Speaking verbs (e.g., shout, mumble, whisper), Hitting verbs (e.g., hit, poke, jab), Cutting verbs (e.g., cut, slice, hack), and Change of State verbs (e.g., shatter, smash, crack). These classes were selected because they vary with respect to the presence or absence of five distinct semantic components—specifically, ACTION, MOTION, CONTACT, CHANGE OF STATE, and TOOL USE. Based on the Simulation Framework, we hypothesized that the ACTION component depends on the primary motor and premotor cortices, that the MOTION component depends on the posterolateral temporal cortex, that the CONTACT component depends on the intraparietal sulcus and inferior parietal lobule, that the CHANGE OF STATE component depends on the ventral temporal cortex, and that the TOOL USE component depends on a distributed network of temporal, parietal, and frontal regions. Virtually all of the predictions were confirmed. Taken together, these findings support the Simulation Framework and extend our understanding of the neuroanatomical distribution of different aspects of verb meaning.
Article
Words denoting manipulable objects activate sensorimotor brain areas, likely reflecting action experience with the denoted objects. In particular, these sensorimotor lexical representations have been found to reflect the way in which an object is used. In the current paper we present data from two experiments (one behavioral and one neuroimaging) in which we investigate whether body schema information, putatively necessary for interacting with functional objects, is also recruited during lexical processing. To this end, we presented participants with words denoting objects that are typically brought towards or away from the body (e.g., cup or key, respectively). We hypothesized that objects typically brought to a location on the body (e.g., cup) are relatively more reliant on body schema representations, since the final goal location of the cup (i.e., the mouth) is represented primarily through posture and body co-ordinates. In contrast, objects typically brought to a location away from the body (e.g., key) are relatively more dependent on visuo-spatial representations, since the final goal location of the key (i.e., a keyhole) is perceived visually. The behavioral study showed that prior planning of a movement along an axis towards and away from the body facilitates processing of words with a congruent action semantic feature (i.e., preparation of movement towards the body facilitates processing of cup.). In an fMRI study we showed that words denoting objects brought towards the body engage the resources of brain areas involved in the processing information about human bodies (i.e., the extra-striate body area, middle occipital gyrus and inferior parietal lobe) relatively more than words denoting objects typically brought away from the body. The results provide converging evidence that body schema are implicitly activated in processing lexical information.