ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Stress involves real or perceived changes within an organism in the environment that activate an organism's attempts to cope by means of evolutionarily ancient neural and endocrine mechanisms. Responses to acute stressors involve catecholamines released in varying proportion at different sites in the sympathetic and central nervous systems. These responses may interact with and be complemented by intrinsic rythms and responses to chronic or intermittent stressors involving the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Varying patterns of responses to stressors are also affected by an animal's assessment of their prospects for successful coping. Subsequent central and systemic consequences of the stress response include apparent changes in affect, motivation, and cognition that can result in an altered relationship to environmental and social stimuli. This review will summarize recent developments in our understanding of the causes and consequences of stress. Special problems that need to be explored involve the manner in which ensembles of adaptive responses are assembled, how autonomic and neurohormonal reflexes of the stress response come under the influence of environmental stimuli, and how some specific aspects of the stress response may be integrated into the life history of a species.
University of Tennessee, Knoxville
Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative
Exchange
Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Publications
and Other Works Ecology and Evolutionary Biology
1-1-2002
Ethological Causes and Consequences of the Stress
Response
Neil Greenberg
University of Tennessee - Knoxville, ngreenbe@utk.edu
James A. Carr
Texas Tech University
Cliff H. Summers
University of South Dakota
This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Ecology and Evolutionary Biology at Trace: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange.
It has been accepted for inclusion in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Publications and Other Works by an authorized administrator of Trace:
Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact trace@utk.edu.
Recommended Citation
Greenberg, Neil; Carr, James A.; and Summers, Cliff H., "Ethological Causes and Consequences of the Stress Response" (2002).
Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Publications and Other Works.
http://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_ecolpubs/2
Integrative & Comparative Biology, Volume 42, Issue 3 pp. 508-516 (2002)
Ethological Causes and Consequences of
the Stress Response
Neil Greenberg, James A. Carr, Cliff H. Summers
.
Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville
Department of Biological Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock
Department of Biology, University of South Dakota, Vermillion
Presented at the SICB Symposium, "Stress --Is it More Than a Disease? A Comparative Look at Stress and
Adaptation,"
2001 SICB Annual Meeting, Chicago Hilton and Towers, January 3-7, 2001
SYNOPSIS. Stress involves real or perceived changes within an organism or in
the environment that activate an organisms attempts to cope by means of
evolutionarily ancient neural and endocrine mechanisms. Responses to acute
stressors involve catecholamines released in varying proportion at different
sites in the sympathetic and central nervous systems. These responses may
interact with and be complemented by intrinsic rhythms and responses to
chronic or intermittent stressors involving the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
axis. Varying patterns of responses to stressors are also affected by an animal=s
assessment of their prospects for successful coping. Subsequent central and
systemic consequences of the stress response include apparent changes in
affect, motivation, and cognition that can result in an altered relationship to
environmental and social stimuli. This review will summarize recent
developments in our understanding of the causes and consequences of stress.
Special problems that need to be explored involve the manner in which
ensembles of adaptive responses are assembled, how autonomic and
neurohormonal reflexes of the stress response come under the influence of
environmental stimuli, and how some specific aspects of the stress response
may be integrated into the life history of a species.
INTRODUCTION
Stressors are real or perceived challenges to an organisms=s ability to meet its real or
perceived needs. In most vertebrates the responses that have evolved to cope with
such challenges are constrained by a threshold for detection of challenge, for attention
based on real or perceived relevance, and for capacity to respond at any particular
level once the challenge is detected. Depending on the intensity and timing of the
stressor, each of these can vary independently.
Stressors that challenge homeostasis, often regarded as the most urgent of needs, are
the best known. When an organism=s competence to maintain homeostasis within a
specific range is exceeded, responses are evoked that enable the organism to cope by
either removing the stressor or facilitating coexistence with it (Antelman and
Caggiula, 1990). While many stressors can evoke dramatic neural and endocrine
responses, a more modest or subclinical response may be exhibited in response to
milder stimuli. These responses may build on or extend homeostatic mechanisms or
they may be more or less tightly linked to homeostatic responses in a hierarchical
manner creating a functional continuum. For example, such a hierarchical system was
described for thermoregulation in mammals by Satinoff (1978) in which more
recently evolved regulatory mechanisms are invoked when more conservative ones
are unable to restore balance. This continuum is expressed in numerous physiological
responses, often measured as an inverted U(Sapolsky, 1997). Although the
inverted-U physiology of stress hormones such as corticosteroids presents the
conundrum of opposing actions at different dosages, progress over the last twenty
years has elucidated some of the physiology involved. For example, both membrane-
bound (Orchinik et al., 1991) and two types of intracellular receptors (Ruel et al.,
1987) help explain how acute glucocorticoid responses may differ from long-term
responses. Membrane receptors acting by means of G proteins stimulate more rapid
cellular response than classic receptors that act as transcription factors: type I (
mineralocorticoid or MR) and type II (glucocorticoid or GR) receptors. Based on the
differences in affinity and capacity, type II receptors only become bound during
circadian or stress-evoked peaks in plasma glucocorticoids. We can now envision how
a gradual succession from low levels of corticosteroids binding to type I receptors
turns the corner on the U as progressively higher titers bind more effectively to
type II receptors (Ruel et al., 1987). Basal levels of glucocorticoid (involving type I
receptors) are proactively permissive for defense mechanisms at tonic and
circadian levels, while reactively suppressive actions are invoked at higher, stress-
induced levels of glucocorticoid (involving type II receptors) and help protect the
organism from a damaging overcompensation that elevated levels of hormone
might induce (Meijer et al 2000). Many if not all of the hormones involved in stress
responses possess, in addition to their direct effects, pleiotropic or collateral
consequences that may or may not reinforce the direct or primary effect. It is likely
that many of these other effects can provide the basis of mechanisms that might serve
other, unrelated adaptive needs.
At one level, coping with challenge is what life is all about. Stress is inevitable, and as
Hans Selye emphasized, a necessary part of life (1976). There is, however, a problem
defining stress as coping with a challenge. The implication that stress is something to
be avoided is a legacy from the clinical perspective dominated by the mandate to
remediate dysfunction, including the stress-evoked diseases of adaptation. Selye
(1976) himself tried to correct this one-sided over-generalization by distinguishing
eustress from distress, but the legacy lives on. The perception that stress
responses, by reallocating resources from growth to coping with a challenge, are not
good, suggests that stress facilitated coping is forced upon organisms by occasional
unfortunate circumstances. However, organisms exist in continually changing
environments and their very existence can be construed as an expression of that
coping. The mechanisms that have evolved to cope with change are organized within
a nested hierarchy. The most conservative functions deal with homeostasis, the most
ancient and urgent of needs. But homeostasis is not an organisms only need, and
Bruce McEwens succinct definition is an excellent beginning to a fuller appreciation
of that fact: Stress may be defined as a threat, real or implied, to the psychological
or physiological integrity of an individual (McEwen,1999a:1).
Survival in the changing external environment depends on the flexible stability of an
organisms internal environment, which is itself always in flux. The flexibility is
attributable to the fact that physiological functions can proceed effectively within a
range of more or less tolerance for deviation from their respective setpoints. The
necessity for organisms to attain some measure of independence from the vagaries of
the external environment was described by Claude Bernard in the 19th century.
Bernard recognized that the stability of the milieu interieur depended on ensembles of
compensating mechanisms (Bernard, 1878). Fifty years later, Walter Cannon (1929)
introduced the term homeostasis to describe the dynamic, interactive nature of
these mechanisms in maintaining the stability of the internal environment. He further
identified the autonomic nervous system (ANS) as an orchestrator of responses when
an organism is suddenly challenged. The famous fight or flight response is one
such ensemble of responses. Within a decade, Hans Selye shifted attention from the
ANS to the adrenal glands by identifying a General Adaptation Syndrome (GAS) in
which the initial sympatho-adrenomedullary system (SAMS) response to an
emergency was augmented by an hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) response as
the body mobilized resources to cope with a sustained stressor (Selye, 1936; 1937).
This was of particular interest to medical science as the coping mechanisms of the
stress response became seen, over time, as potentially deadly diseases of
adaptation. Here, organs fail as their resources become reallocated to deal with a
continuing stressor, possibly leading to exhaustion and death (Selye 1946). By the
early 1970's, stress was no longer viewed mainly as a threat to survival, and Seymour
Levine (1971) was able to state that the normal expression of adaptive behavior
depends upon some optimal level of stress.
Stressors may be acute, sequential, episodic, chronically intermittent, sustained, or
anticipated (Sapolsky et al. 2000). Alternative strategies may be evoked by the
organisms perception or experience of their effectiveness in coping. The clinical
view of the stress response was that it was largely nonspecific, but it has become clear
that many stressors evoke specific combinations of physiological and behavioral
responses depending in part on their respective potentials for effective coping in a
given context. Stressors perceived as uncontrollable will evoke different ensembles of
responses than those believed to be controllable (see for example, Cabib and Puglisi-
Allegra 1996, Huether 1996). Stressors also can be additive, creating the appearance
of a trivial stressor having a disproportionate effect. The endocrine environment may
also be a significant variable for the action of stress hormones.
The present understanding of stress and behavior has been nicely summarized in
several reviews. In the early 1970's, Mason (1971) called attention to the potency of
psychological stimuli in the stress response. A large literature has emerged since
Christian=s original insights in the 1950's and 1960's that at high densities, mammals
manifest enlarged adrenals indicative of increased stress and also showed increased
mortality and reduced reproduction. These psychoendocrine effects reinforced
perceptions of stress in terms of Selyes (1946, 1976) General Adaptation
Syndrome (see Christian & Davis 1964, Christian 1980). Lee and McDonald (1985)
reviewed this and related literature and appealed for additional research and more
direct evidence for the effects of stress in natural populations. Axelrod and Reisine
(1984) summarized the multiple regulatory mechanisms and interactions of stress
hormones, identifying corticotropin (ACTH) as a principal nexus; Goldstein (1987)
provided a helpful collation of stress-induced actions of the sympathetic nervous
system; Johnson and colleagues (1992) reviewed mechanisms with an emphasis on
the dynamic nature of endocrine and behavioral mechanisms. Neural pathways were
recently reviewed by Van de Kar and Blair (1999) who pointed that prolactin,
oxytocin, and renin have been neglected as stress-sensitive endocrine systems because
they are coordinated by slightly different neural pathways. Saavedra (1999) recently
reviewed evidence of a widespread role for angiotensin in modulating stress and
cortocotrophic releasing hormone (CRH), which has significant stress response
coordinating functions aside from its triggering a corticosterone response (e.g.
Leshner, 1978; Koob, 1991)
A review of the diversity of glucocorticoid actions in the stress response by Sapolsky,
Romero, and Munck (2000) provides a valuable synthesis of the seeming
contradictory functions of glucocorticoids. The permissive actions of glucocorticoids
that are based on tonic levels associated with homeostasis are seen to be distinct from
the suppressive and stimulatory actions that result from stress-induced elevation of
circulating levels. Sapolsky et al. also express an appreciation for the ethological
perspective in developing what they have termed the preparative functions of
glucocorticoids, and caution laboratory researchers to be sensitive to the organism
s
perspective of what constitutes a stressor.
TABLE 1 SEE BELOW
Understanding the causes and consequences of stress in nonhumans has taken on an
urgency of its own as a result of a growing concern for animal welfare as well as a
search for more robust and relevant animal models. This interest in the role of stress in
life history has proven a valuable counterbalance to well-intentioned perceptions by
scientists and citizens who too frequently view stress in a stereotypical way as
necessarily deleterious. Ignorance of the real needs of the animals (echoing
Sapolskys appeal for appreciation for the unique needs of the subject) is most
obvious when well cared for animals fail to thrive or reproduce. Further, freedom
from stress attainable in the laboratory is as serious as inadvertently introduced stress
in compromising the external validity of findings. Attempts to bring perspective to
this issue are proliferating (for example, Broom and Johnson 1994, SCAW 2000)
some of which target nonmammalian vertebrates (e.g., Schaeffer et al., 1992;
Warwick et al., 1995, Greenberg 1994). Other efforts try to deal with issues of
definition and clarity. For example, Moberg (1999) has attempted to identify the
boundary between stress and distress at a point where the cost of coping impairs
functions critical to well-being.
COMPLICATIONS IN DEFINING CAUSE AND CONSEQUENCE:
LESSONS FROM ETHOLOGY
Stress researchers and physiological ethologists often emphasize that stress is evoked
by a perceived challenge to the status quo as well as a physical experience. Since we
now more fully understand that not all change is bad and not all stressors are
deleterious there is renewed attention to the relationships between stress and emotion.
The now distant dispute between proponents of the James-Lange theory of emotion
(that the experience of an emotion was secondary to the physiological events) and WB
Cannon=s view (that physiological changes were subsequent to an emotional
experiences) persists because, as Leshner (1978) points out, both positions have some
validity. Leshner=s review of the problem concluded that at least some hormones may
have a general effect on arousal which then feeds back to evoke enhanced
catecholamine and glucocorticoid responses. For example, maternal care-giving is
positively correlated with cortisol levels in humans (Fleming et al., 1987) but may
also be accounted for by enhanced attention to stimuli. Therefore, endocrine and
neuroendocrine events proceed in an interdependent manner to regulate multiple,
variable stress responses, each unique, but influenced by previous events (Summers,
2001). Taking this a step further, arousal can evoke an emotion which will be tested
against experience and cognition and then by means of a positive feedback loop can
lead to progressively more focused expression.
The hormonal mechanisms responsible for behavioral changes during mild stress are
rarely obvious, as many stress hormones have structurally-related and biologically
active counterparts with multiple receptors and receptor subtypes. For example,
corticotrophin releasing hormone (CRH) and the structurally related peptide urocortin
act on multiple receptor types to rapidly inhibit feeding. It has is only recently that
researchers have been able to identify the respective contribution of each peptide and
receptor type to stress-induced alterations in feeding (Cone, 2000). In some respects
the selective facilitation or inhibition of normal behavioral patterns evoked by mild
stress is analogous to subclinical symptoms of a disease.
Indeed, the expression of many behavioral patterns are stress-sensitive in that their
expression may be secondary to neurotransmitter or hormone-induced increases in
non-specific arousal and selective attention (see Mason 1968, Nelson, 2000). The
actions of stress hormones may also be constrained by the activity of other hormones
and by environmental circumstances. For example, the rapid behavioral (perch-
hopping) response to corticosterone in white-crowned sparrows is influenced by
photoperiod. During a long-day (breeding) photoperiod, but not short-day (winter)
photoperiod, corticosterone will increase activity (Breuner & Wingfield, 2000).
Testosterone is subject to seasonal variation and social dynamics in many species, and
its activity appears to facilitate or enhance responsiveness to stressors both directly
and indirectly. Reduced androgen, such as might be seen in subordinate males,
appears to ameliorate the normal stress-evoking effects of certain stimuli (e.g.,
Greenberg et al., 1984 in the lizard, Anolis). As a female counterpart to the fight-or-
flight responses of males, Taylor (et al. 2000) proposed a tend-and-befriend
response to stress in females where (for example) the effects of oxytocin are
moderated by the presence of estrogen and endogenous opioids. In this response,
female mammals under stress will manifest enhances caregiving and attachment
behavior.
Stress-sensitive Behavior
There is a great diversity of adaptive behavioral patterns that appear to have built on
specific elements of the stress axes (Table 1). No tabulation can be exhaustive but the
one we have assembled underscores the diversity of effects at different levels of
specificity. It is a continuing challenge to distinguish primary from secondary
behaviors: Are the effects of stress on behavior a consequence of hormones acting
directly on specific neural structures mediating actions? Might they be collateral
actions on secondary targets? Or might the manifest behavioral pattern be secondary
to enhanced attention, arousal, cognitive activity, or even sensitivity of sensory
receptors? Collateral effects are particularly rich sources of alternative behavioral
patterns. For example, releasing factors such as corticotropic releasing hormone
(CRH) (Koob et al., 1993) and pituitary hormones frequently have multiple target
tissues. CRH has many behavioral effects mediated through CNS receptors in addition
to its central role in simulating pituitary ACTH secretion and indeed, CRH may
arguably be the principle coordinating regulator of central stress responsiveness,
influencing central serotonergic (Price et al., 1998; Lowry et al., 2000) and
catecholaminergic activities (Dunn and Berridge, 1987; Curtis et al., 1997). CRH is
also believed to have direct effects on behavioral patterns such as locomotion (Lowry
and Moore, 1991), startle responses (Pelton et al., 1997), and learning (Radulovic et
al., 1999; Wang et al., 2000). Significant direct central (extra-adrenal) effects of
ACTH are also well known (Leshner, 1971). Thus, simply administering exogenous
corticosterone as a way of determining its effects on behavior is complicated by the
fact that while it may act directly on a target tissue, it may also be acting indirectly by
means of feedback suppression of CRF or ACTH (Brain, 1972). In addition, as
mentioned above, the same hormone can have opposite effects when present at
different absolute amounts or temporal regimens. Opposing actions of adrenal axis
hormones and central stress peptides stem from an inverted-U dose physiology that is
a part of a framework of optimal stress response mechanisms (Sapolsky, 1997). Also,
different receptor types have alternative effects when stimulated by the same hormone
and different stressors can evoke different patterns of endocrine response. For
example, the stimuli involved in an aggressive exchange between two males
competing for social dominance will elicit comparable corticosteroid release in both
animals, but following such an encounter, the winner will also experience a
testosterone surge (e.g., Coe et al., 1982 for squirrel monkeys; Greenberg and Crews,
1990 for lizards). Further, if they continue in a long-term dominant-subordinate
relationship, the subsequent responsiveness to stress-evoking stimuli will be different
in the two animals.
THE EVOLUTION OF STRESS AND BEHAVIORAL COPING
MECHANISMS
Autonomic responses are among the richest sources of adaptive behavioral patterns.
Tightly yoked somatic and autonomic effects involving sympathetic activation and
occasionally, parasympathetic rebound, have been identified in situations that involve
frustration or conflict (Morris, 1956). It is reasonable to imagine that in sophisticated
decision-making organisms such as humans, there is competence to reflect, at least in
part, on processes that guide the selection of alternative ensembles of adaptive
pathways such as Afight or flight@ (Cannon, 1929), Aflee or freeze@ (Rand, 1964, for a
lizard), or active versus passive coping (Bandler et al., 2000). Interestingly, in
humans, there is evidence that at a critical level of acute stress, cognitive mechanisms
of the prefrontal cortex are suppressed and more rapid, conservative responses are
invoked (Arnsten 1997, 2000); chronic stress may also work through several other
long-term mechanisms to impair cognitive function (McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995).
AIn animals, almost invariably, a change in behavior is the crucial factor initiating
evolutionary innovation,@ Mayr tells us (1988:408). Also, selection pressures can be
altered by behavior that modifies the environment in which an animal must survive
and thrive (see Deacon=s 1997 review of Mark Baldwin). The role of stress in guiding
the evolution of coping mechanisms cannot be overestimated. It is likely that stress
responses are a first means of dealing with altered selection pressures caused by the
inevitable environmental changes organisms are subjected to. A further link is likely
between the stress-evoked changes in behavior when confronted with novel selection
pressures and the ultimate changes identifiable as evolutionary innovations which
seem more abundant in rapidly changing environments (Jablonski & Bottjer 1990,
Hoffmann & Hercus 2000). As brains change in response to specific selection
pressures, the larger contexts in which resolutions to act are made involve the systems
that subsume motivation, affect, and cognition. More conservative coping strategies
are reasonably mediated by more ancient parts of the brain (Paradiso et al., 1999).
Candidate mechanisms for the developmental influence of stress on brain and
behavior, including the effects of corticosteroids impairing the growth of specific
neural areas (see Thomas and Devenport 1988) and the integrity of the highly plastic
hippocampus, are now well established (for example, Fuchs & Flugge 1998 and
McEwen 1999b for recent reviews). Indeed, a major influence of stress on the
evolution of brain structures that selectively respond to stressful stimuli or are
activated by stress hormones was suggested by Huether (1996) in his
conceptualization of a Acentral adaptation syndrome.
CONCLUSIONS AND NEED FOR FUTURE STUDY
One of the more striking effects of confronting such a diverse assortment of stress-
sensitive phenomena is vivid sense of the versatility and flexibility of the system. The
stress response is orchestrated by a deeply embedded, highly conservative sense of
biological priorities and an impressive economy. By assembling and reassembling a
relatively small numbere of possible responses into a diversity of new combinations,
natural selection deals with an almost infinite array of possible challenges. Clearly,
physiological stress responses need not be manifest as conveniently conspicuous
behavioral patterns or pathologies to have adaptive significance. As David Goldstein
(1990) put it, they can be evoked whenever an organism experiences Aexpectations ---
whether genetically programmed, established by prior learning, or deduced from
circumstances[that] do not match the current or anticipated perceptions of the
internal or external environment (p. 243). In addition, the modulation of stress
responses by perceived control or helplessness (see Cabib and Puglisi_Allegra [1996],
and see Seligman [1975], Seligman et al., [1975]) allows us to envision how an
animals perception of the prospects for future remediation of a mismatch can
influence the expression of an appropriate compensating response (Bandler et al.,
2000).
Stress research is compelling not only as a fascinating puzzle that helps make sense of
many previously scattered observations, but is also compelling medically and socially.
Medical researchers (and then the rest of us) began with Selye=s insights about
diseases of adaptation, the clinical expressions of chronic stress. However, we have
learned much about developmental neuroplasticity. Early nurturing experiences (Liu
et al 1997), prenatal stress syndrome, and brief but intense episodes of stress (such as
childhood abuse), have all been implicated in causing enduring neurological changes.
In this regard it is significant that a significant number of violent criminals have
atypical autonomic responses (Raine et al., 2000), often associated with early
experiences of intense stress. The principal function of stress is protective and many
elements of the stress response can also be viewed as a kind of cure chemotherapy
without drugs, in Antelman and Caggiulas terms-- but sometimes the cure can be
worse than the disease. This was Walter Cannon=s insight when he wrote that the
development of pathological functions in a system is quite consistent with its usual
performance of normal functions.
The adaptive value of responsiveness to stressors in animals in nature may provide
invaluable information regarding the dynamics and flexibility of neuroendocrine
stress responses. Absolute levels of transmitters or hormones may not matter in the
production of significant and adaptive results. Relative elevation or inhibition
accruing from previous experience may adjust specific neural centers to produce
relevant output specifically related to the appropriate environmental context. The
neural mechanisms for transduction of relevant information are of necessity very
plastic, with many transmitter, neuromodulator and peripheral hormone systems
interacting. These systems influence behavioral and physiological stress responses,
but are also influenced by that output.
Our goal in this brief review has been more to be provoke than postulate. The
references selected from the vast literature of overlapping behavioral, neurological,
and endocrine reports applicable to stress were exemplary, not exhaustive. Many of
the findings about the reciprocity of behavioral patterns and stress physiology
underscores the fact that systems usually expressed as an ensemble are often cobbled
together by multiple selection pressures. A sense of this opens researchers to creative
hypotheses and the value of the comparative method. By training and disposition,
researchers apply Occam=s Razor to available evidence no matter how fragmentary,
but if the prevailing views of mechanisms cannot assimilate or accommodate new
data, new views must be sought.
The lesson beyond the obvious one of humility in the face of nature=s imagination, has
been one of openness to the myriad possibilities for the organization and
reorganization of the relatively small numbers of ways that hormones, brains, and
behavior can interact. We have often heard that research has become a more
collaborative affair. This is the only solution to the problem of the isolation that
attends explorations of great disciplinary depth. A continuing challenge must be to
enhance the reciprocal influences of the laboratory and the real world in which traits
of interest have evolved. This will require renewed efforts at mutual understanding for
researchers specializing in the unique questions and methodologies of each research
approach. Efforts must be taken to place the limited validity of highly controlled
laboratory studies at the service of less exact field research, and to present the insights
of observers in the real world to bench scientists. In a small way this resembles the
tension between ideologies of freedom and control that plague all would-be
collaborative social groups, but the richness of the reward justifies all possible efforts.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The symposium was made possible by funding from the NIMH (R13 MH62670), NSF
(IBN 0100532), the Center for Biomedical Research Excellence (CoBRE, P20
RR15567) at the University of South Dakota on Neural Mechanisms of Adaptive
Behavior, South Dakota EPSCoR, and the USD Office of Research.
REFERENCES
.
Antelman, S.M. and A.R. Caggiula.1980. Stress-induced behavior: chemotherapy without drugs. In J.M. Davidson
and R.J. Davidson (eds.), The psychobiology of consciousness, pp. 65-104. Plenum Press, New York.
Arnsten, A.F. 1997. Catecholamine regulation of the prefrontal cortex. J. Psychopharmacol. 11:151-162.
Arnsten, A.F. 2000. Through the looking glass: differential noradrenergic modulation of prefrontal cortical function.
Neural Plast. 7:133-146.
Axelrod, J. and T.D. Reisine. 1984. Stress hormones: their interaction and regulation. Science 224:452-459.
Bandler R, K.A. Keay, N. Floyd, and J. Price. 2000. Central circuits mediating patterned autonomic activity during
active vs. passive emotional coping. Brain Res. Bull. 53:95_104
Bernard, C. 1878. Les phenomenes de la vie. Vol 1, p. 879. Libraire J-B Bailliere et Fils. Cited by Johnson et al.,
1992.
Blanchard, D.C., R.R. Sakai, B. McEwen, S.M. Weiss, and R.J. Blanchard. 1993. Subordination stress: behavioral,
brain, and neuroendocrine correlates. Behav. Brain Res. 58:113-121.
Bohus, B. and D. de Wied. 1980. Pituitary-adrenal system hormones and adaptive behavior. In I. Chester-Jones and
I.W. Henderson (eds.), General, comparative and clinical endocrinology of the adrenal cortex, Vol 3, pp. 265-347.
Academic Press, New York.
Brain, P.F. 1972. Mammalian behavior and the adrenal cortex BA review. Behav. Biol. 7:453-477.
Brain, P.F., N.W. Nowell, and A. Wouters. 1971. Some relationships between adrenal function and the effectiveness
of a period of isolation in inducing intermale aggression in albino mice. Physiol. Behav. 6:27-29
Breuner, C.W. and J.C. Wingfield. 2000. Rapid behavioral response to corticosterone varies with photoperiod and
dose. Horm. Behav. 37:23-30.
Britton, D.R., G.F. Koob, J. Rivier, and W. Vale.1982. Intraventricular corticotropin-releasing factor enhances
behavioral effects of novelty. Life Sci. 31:363-367.
Broom, D.M. and K.G. Johnson. 1994. Stress and animal welfare. Chapman and Hall, London.
Broverman, D.M., E. L. Klaiber, W. Vogal. 1974. Short_term versus long_term effects of adrenal hormones on
behaviors. Psychol. Bull. 81:672_694.
Cabib, S. and S. Puglisi_Allegra. 1996. Stress, depression and the mesolimbic dopamine system.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 128:331_342
Cannon, W.B. 1929. The wisdom of the body. Physiol. Rev. 9:399-431. Cited by Johnson et al. 1992.
Carpenter, A.M. and J.A. Carr. 1996. The effects of melanocortin peptides and corticosterone on habituation in the
great plains toad, Bufo cognatus. Horm. Behav. 30:236_243.
Carter, C.S. and M. Altemus. 1997. Integrative functions of lactational hormones in social behavior and stress
management. Annals N.Y. Acad. Sci. 807:164-174.
Christian, J.J. 1980. Endocrine factors in population regulation. In M.N. Cohen, R.S. Malpass, and H.G. Klein
(eds.), Biosocial mechanisms of population regulation, pp. 55-115. Yale, New Haven.
Christian, J.J. and D.E. Davis. 1964 . Endocrines, behavior, and population. Science 146:1550-1560.
Coe, C.L., D. Franklin, E.R. Smith, and S. Levine. 1982. Hormonal responses accompanying fear and agitation in
the squirrel monkey. Physiol. Behav. 29:1051-1057.
Cone, R.D. 2000. The corticotropin-releasing hormone system and feeding behavior-a complex web begins to
unravel. Endocrinology 141: 2713-2714.
Cooper, J. J. and C. Nicol. 1991. Stereotypic behaviour affects environmental preference in bank voles,
Clethrionomys glareolus. Anim. Behav. 41:971-977.
Cooper, J.J. and C.J. Nicol. 1993. The 'coping' hypothesis of stereotypic behaviour:a reply to Rushen. Anim. Behav.
45:616-618.
Curtis,A.L., S.M.Lechner, L.A.Pavcovich, and R.J.Valentino 1997. Activation of the locus coeruleus noradrenergic
system by intracoerulear microinfusion of corticotropin-releasing factor: effects on discharge rate, cortical
norepinephrine levels and cortical electroencephalographic activity. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 281:163-172.
Deacon, T.W. 1997. The symbolic species. W.W. Norton, New York.
Dunn, A.J. and C.W.Berridge. 1987. Corticotropin-releasing factor administration elicits a stress-like activation of
cerebral catecholaminergic systems. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 27: 685-691.
Fanselow, M.S. and R.A. Sigmundi. 1986. Species-specific danger signals, endogenous opioid analgesia, and
defensive behavior. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 12:301-309.
File, S.E. 1978. ACTH, but not corticosterone impairs habituation and reduces exploration. Pharmacol. Biochem.
Behav. 9:161_166.
Fleming, A.S., M. Steiner, and V. Anderson. 1987. Hormonal and attitudinal correlates of maternal behavior during
the early postpartum period in first-time mothers. J. Reprod. Infant. Psychol. 5:193-205.
Fuchs, E.and G. Flugge. 1998. Stress, glucocorticoids and structural plasticity of the hippocampus. Neurosci.
Biobehav. Rev. 23:295-300.
Gispen, W.H. 1982. Neuropeptides and behavior: ACTH. Scan. J. Psychol. suppl 1:16-25.
Goldstein, D.S. 1987. Stress-induced activation of the sympathetic nervous system. Baillieres Clin. Endocrinol.
Metab. 1:253-278.
Goldstein, D.S. 1990. Neurotransmitters and stress. Biofeedback Self Regul. 3:243_271.
Greenberg, N. and D. Crews 1990. Endocrine and behavioral responses to aggression and social dominance in the
green anole lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Gen. Compar. Endocrinol. 77:1-10.
Greenberg, N. 1990. The behavioral endocrinology of physiological stress in a lizard. J. Exp. Zool. Suppl.
4:170_173.
Greenberg, N. 1994. Ethologically informed design in reptile research. In C. Warwick, F.L. Frye, J.B. Murphy (eds.)
Health and Welfare of Captive Reptiles, pp. 239-262. Chapman & Hall, London.
Greenberg, N., T. Chen, and D. Crews. 1984. Social Status, gonadal state, and the adrenal stress response in the
lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Horm. Behav. 18:1-11.
Halliday, MS. 1966. Exploration and fear in the rat. Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 18:45-59.
Hayden Hixson, D.M. and C.F. Ferris. 1991. Steroid-specific regulation of agonistic responding in the anterior
hypothalamus of male hamsters. Physiol. Behav. 50:793-799.
Hiebert, S.M., K.G. Salvante, M. Ramenofsky, and J.C. Wingfield. 2000. Corticosterone and nocturnal torpor in the
rufous hummingbird (Selasphorus rufus). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 120:220_234.
Hoffmann, A.A. and M.J. Hercus. 2000. Environmental stress as an evolutionary force. Bioscience 50:217-226.
Huether, G. 1996. The central adaptation syndrome: psychosocial stress as a trigger for adaptive modifications of
brain structure and brain function. Prog. Neurobiol. 48:569-612.
Izquierdo, I. and J.H. Medina. 1991. GABAA receptor modulation of memory: the role of endogenous
benzodiazepines. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 12:260-265.
Jablonski, D. and D.J. Bottjer. 1990. The ecology of evolutionary innovation: The fossil record. In M.H. Nitecki
(ed.), Evolutionary adaptations, pp. 253-288. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Joëls, M. and E.R. de Kloot. 1992. Control of neuronal excitability by corticosteroid hormones. Trends Neurosci.
15:25-30
Johnson, E.O., T.C. Kamilaris, G.P. Chrousos, and P.W. Gold. 1992. Mechanisms of stress: A dynamic overview of
hormonal and behavioral homeostasis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 16:115-130.
Kabbaj, M., D.P. Devine, V.R. Savage, and H. Akil. 2000. Neurobiological correlates of individual differences in
novelty seeking behavior in the rat: Differential expression of stress-related molecules. J. Neurosci. 20:6983-6988.
Koob, GF. 1991. Behavioral responses to stress: focus on corticotropin-releasing factor. In M.R. Brown, G.F. Koob,
and C. Rivier (eds.) Stress: neurobiology and neuroendocrinology, pp. 255-271. Marcel Dekker, New York.
Koob, G.F., S.C. Heinrichs, E.M. Pich, F. Menzaghi, H. Baldwin, K. Miczek, and K.T. Britton. 1993. The role of
corticotropin-releasing factor in behavioral responses to stress. CIBA Found. Symp. 172: 277-295.
Krahn, D.D., B.A. Gosnell, M. Grace, and A.S. Levine. 1986. CRF antagonist partially reverses CRF- and stress-
induced effects on feeding. Brain Res. Bull. 17: 285-289.
Lee, A.J.K. and I.R. McDonald. 1985. Stress and population regulation in small mammals. In J.R. Clarke (ed.),
Oxford reviews of reproductive biology, pp. 261-304. Clarendon Press, Oxford.
Leibowitz, S.F., ,C.R. Roland, L. Hor, and V. Squillari. 1984. Noradrenergic feeding elicited via the paraventricular
nucleus is dependent upon circulating corticosterone. Physiol. Behav. 32:857_864.
Leshner, A. 1983. Pituitary-adrenocortical effects on intermale agonistic behavior. In B. Svare (ed.), Hormones and
aggressive behavior, pp. 27-38. Plenum Press, New York.
Leshner, A. 1978. An introduction to behavioral endocrinology. Oxford University Press, New York.
Leshner, A.I. and J.A. Politch. 1979. Hormonal control of submissiveness in mice: Irrelevance of the androgens and
relevance of the pituitary-adrenal hormones. Physiol. Behav. 22:531-534.
Levine, S. 1971. Stress and behavior. Sci. Amer. 224:26-31.
Liu, D., J. Diorio, B. Tannenbaum, C. Caldji, D. Francis, A. Freedman, S. Sharma, D. Pearson, P.M. Plotsky, and M.
J. Meaney. 1997. Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
responses to stress. Science 277:1659-1662.
Lowry, C.A., J.D. Rose, and F.L. Moore. 1996. Corticotropin_releasing factor enhances locomotion and medullary
neuronal firing in an amphibian. .Horm. Behav. 30:50_59.
Lowry, C.A. and F.L. Moore 1991. Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) antagonist suppresses stress- induced
locomotor activity in an amphibian. Horm. Behav. 25: 84-96.
Lowry, C.A., J.E. Rodda, S.L.Lightman, and C.D.Ingram 2000. Corticotropin-releasing factor increases In vitro
firing rates of serotonergic neurons in the rat dorsal raphe nucleus: evidence for activation of a topographically
organized mesolimbocortical serotonergic system. J. Neurosci. 20:7728-7736.
Martin, J.T. 1978. Imprinting behavior: pituitary-adrenocortical modulation of the approach response. Science
200:565-567.
Mason, JW. 1971. A reevaluation of the concept of "non-specificity" in stress specificity in stress theory. J.
Psychiatr. Res. 8:323-333.
Mason, G. J. 1991. Stereotypies: a critical review. Anim. Behav. 41:1015-1037.
Mason, J. 1968. A review of psychoendocrine research on the pituitary adrenal cortical system. Psychosomatic
Medicine. 30:576-583.
Mayr, E. 1988. Toward a new philosophy of biology. Harvard University Press, Cambridge Mass.
McEwen, B.S. 1999a. Stress. In R.A. Wilson and F. Keil (eds,), The MIT Encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences.
MIT Press, Cambridge.
McEwen, B.S. 1999b. Stress and hippocampal plasticity. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 22:105-122.
McEwen, B.S. and R.M. Sapolsky. 1995. Stress and cognitive function. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5:205-216.
McEwen, B.S., E.R. De Kloet, and W. Rostene. 1986. Adrenal steroid receptors and actions in the nervous system.
Psychol. Rev. 66:1121_1188.
Meijer, O.C., E.R. deKloet, B.S. McEwen. 2000. Corticosteroid receptors. In G. Fink (ed.), Encyclopedia of Stress,
pp. 557-569. Academic Press, New York.
Miczek, K.A., M.L. Thompson, and L. Shuster. 1982. Opioid-like analgesia in defeated mice. Science. 215:1520-
1522.
Moberg, G.P. 1985. Influence of stress on reproduction: Measure of well-being. In G.P. Moberg (ed.), Animal stress,
pp. 245-267. American Physiological Society, Bethesda, MD.
Moberg, G.P. 1999. When does stress become distress? Lab animal 28:22-26.
Morris, D. 1956. The feather postures of birds and the problem of the origin of social signals. Behaviour 9:75_113.
Nelson, R.J. 2000. An introduction to behavioral endocrinology, 2nd edition. Sinauer, Sunderland Mass.
Oades, RD. 1979. Search and attention: Interactions of the hippocampal-septal axis, adrenocortical and gonadal
hormones. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 3:31-48.
O'Donohue, T.L., G.E. Handelmann, Y.P. Loh, D.S. Olton, J. Leibowitz, and D.M. Jacobowitz. 1981. Comparison
of biological and behavioral activities of alpha_ and gamma_melanocyte stimulating hormones. Peptides 2:101_104.
Orchinik, M.,T.F. Murray, and F.L. Moore. 1991. A corticosteroid receptor in neuronal membranes. Science
252:1848-1851.
Panskepp, J. 1975. Hormonal control of feeding behavior and energy balance. In B.E. Eleftheriou and R.L. Sprott
(eds.), Hormonal Correlates of Behavior, Vol. 2., pp. 657-695. Plenum Press, New York.
Paradiso, S., D.L. Johnson, N.C. Andreasen, D.S. OLeary, G.L.Watkins, L.L. Ponto, and R.D. Hichwa. 1999.
Cerebral blood flow changes associated with attribution of emotional valence to pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral
visual stimuli in a PET study of normal subjects Amer. J. Psychiatry 156:1618-1629.
Pelton, G.H., Y. Lee, and M. Davis. 1997. Repeated stress, like vasopressin, sensitizes the excitatory effects of
corticotropin releasing factor on the acoustic startle reflex. Brain Res. 778: 381-387.
Price, M.L., A.L. Curtis, L.G. Kirby, R.J. Valentino, and I. Lucki. 1998. Effects of corticotropin-releasing factor on
brain serotonergic activity. Neuropsychopharmacology 18:492-502.
Radulovic, J., A. Ruhmann, T. Liepold, and J.Spiess. 1999. Modulation of learning and anxiety by corticotropin-
releasing factor (CRF) and stress: Differential roles of CRF receptors 1 and 2. J. Neurosci. 19: 5016-5025.
Rand, A.S. 1964. Inverse relationship between temperature and shyness in the lizard, Anolis lineotopus. Ecology
45:863-864.
Raine, A., T. Lencz, S. Bihrle, L. LaCasse, and P. Colletti. 2000. Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and
reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57:119_127.
Reul, J.M.H.M., F.R. van den Bosch, and E.R. de Kloet 1987. Relative occupation of type-I and type-II
corticosteroid receptors in rat brain following stress and dexamethasone treatment: functional implications. J.
Endocr. 115:459-467 .
Rosen, J.B. and J. Schulkin. 1998. From normal fear to pathological anxiety. Psychol. Rev. 105:325-350.
Saavedra, J.M. 1999. Emerging features of brain angiotensin receptors. Regul. Peptides 85:31_45.
Saldanha, C.J., B.A. Schlinger, and N.S. Clayton. 2000. Rapid effects of corticosterone on cache recovery in
mountain chickadees (Parus gambeli). Horm. Behav. 37:109-115.
Sandi, C. C. Venero, and C. Guaza. 1996. Novelty-related rapid locomotor effect of corticosterone in rats. Eur. J.
Neurosci. 8:794-800.
Sapolsky, R.M. 1994. Why zebras don
t get ulcers. Freeman, New York.
Sapolsky,R. M. 1997. McEwen-induced modulation of endocrine history: A partial review. Stress, 2: 1-11.
Sapolsky, R.M., L.M. Romero, and A.U. Munck. 2000. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses?
Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocr. Rev. 21(1):55-89.
Satinoff, E. 1978. Neural organization and evolution of thermal regulation in mammals. Science, 201:16-22.
SCAW. 2000. Scientists Center for Animal Welfare, Washington, D.C. http://www.scaw.com/ [downloaded
November 20, 2000]
Schaeffer, D.O., K.M. Kleinow, L. Krulish (eds). 1992. The Care and Use of Amphibians, Reptiles, and Fish in
Research. Scientists Center for Animal Welfare, Bethesda, MD.
Seligman, M, R. Rosellini, and M. Kozak. 1975. Learned helplessness in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 88:542-
547.
Seligman, M. 1975. Helplessness. Freeman & Co., San Francisco.
Selye, Hans. 1936. A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature 138:32.
Selye, H. 1937. Studies on adaptation. Endocrinology 21: 169-188.
Selye, H. 1946. The general adaptation syndrome and the diseases of adaptation. J. Clin. Endocrinol. 6:117-173.
Cited by Johnson et al. 1992.
Selye, H. 1976. The stress of life. (Revised edition) McGraw Hill, New York.
Shibasaki, T., N. Yamauchi, Y. Kato, A. Masuda, T. Imaki, M. Hotta, H. Demura, H. Oono, N. Ling, and K.
Shizume. 1988. Involvement of corticotropin-releasing factor in restraint stress-induced anorexia and reversion of
the anorexia by somatostatin in the rat. Life Sci. 43: 1103-1110.
Shiraishi, I., K.-I. Honma, S. Honma, and T. Hiroshige. 1984. Ethosecretogram: relation of behavior to plasma
corticosterone in freely moving rats. Am. J. Physiol. 247:R40-R45.
Shors, T.J. and R.J. Servatius.1997. The contribution of stressor intensity, duration, and context to the
stress_induced facilitation of associative learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 68:92_96.
Shors, T.J., C. Weiss, and R.F. Thompson. 1992. Stress-induced facilitation of classical conditioning. Science.
257:537-539.
Shors, T.J, A.V. Beylin, G.E. Wood, and E. Gould. 2000. The modulation of Pavlovian memory. Behav. Brain Res.
110:39-52.
Silverin, B. 1997. The stress response and autumn dispersal behavior in willow tits. Anim. Behav. 53:541-549.
Sirinathsinghji, D.J.S., L.H. Rees, J. Rivier, and W. Vale 1983. Corticotropin-releasing factor is a potent inhibitor of
sexual receptivity in the female rat. Nature 305:230-235.
Smith, G.P. 1973. Adrenal hormones and emotional behavior. Prog. Physiol. Psychol. 5:299-351.
Summers, C.H. 2001. Mechanisms for quick and variable responses. Brain Behav.Evol. 58: In Press.
Sutton, R.E., G.F. Koob, M.. Le Moal, J. Rivier, and W. Vale. 1982. Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) produces
behavioral activation in rats. Nature 297:331-332.
Taylor, S.E., L.C. Klein, B.P. Lewis, T.L. Gruenwald, R.A.R. Gurung, and J.A. Updegraff. 2000. Biobehavioral
responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychol. Rev. 107:411-429.
Tazi, A., R. Dantzer, M. Le Moal, J. Rivier, W.Vale, and G.F. Koob. 1987. Corticotropin-releasing factor antagonist
blocks stress-induced fighting in rats. Regul. Peptides 18:37-42.
Teichner, W.H. 1968. Interaction of behavioral and physiological stress reactions. Psychol. Rev. 75:271-291.
Thomas, T.L. and L.D. Devenport. 1988. Site specificity of adrenalectomy-induced brain growth. Exper. Neurol.
102:340-345.
Van de Kar, L.D. and M.L. Blair. 1999. Forebrain pathways mediating stress-induced hormone secretion. Front.
Neuroendocrinol. 20:1-48.
Vergoni, A.V., H.B. Schiöth, and A. Bertolini. 2000. Melanocortins and feeding behavior. Biomed. Pharmacother.
54: 129-134.
Veldhuis, H.D., E.R. De Kloet, I. Van Zoest, and B. Bohus 1982. Adrenalectomy reduces exploratory activity in the
rat: a specific role of corticosterone. Horm. Behav. 16:191-198.
Wang J, Akirav I, Richter-Levin G. 2000. Short-term behavioral and electrophysiological consequences of
underwater trauma. Physiol. Behav. 70:327-332.
Ward, I.L. and J. Weisz.1980. Maternal stress alters plasma testosterone in fetal males. Science. 207:328-329.
Warwick, C., F.L. Frye, and J.B. Murphy (eds.), 1995. Health and welfare of captive reptiles. Chapman & Hall,
London.
Wingfield, J.C., C. Breuner, and J. Jacobs. 1997. Corticosterone and behavioral responses to unpredictable events. In
S. Harvey and R.J. Etches (eds.), Perspectives in avian endocrinology, pp. 267-278. Journal of Endocrinology Ltd.,
Bristol.
Wood, G.E., and T.J. Shors.1998. Stress facilitates classical conditioning in males, but impairs classical conditioning
in females through activational effects of ovarian hormones. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 95:4066-4071.
TABLE 1. REPRESENTATIVE BEHAVIORAL
RESPONSES TO STRESS
.
the original TABLE 1 is below; for an annotated and
updated document
(revised as appropriate) click here
.
Parameter
Response
Reference
AROUSAL/ATTENTION
Epi intensified but does not evoke affect
Teichner, 1968
Stress narrows attention onto specific stimuli
Teichner, 1968
GC (acute) enhances salience of stimuli
Hayden-Hixson, pers
comm
Stress impairs sustained attention
Arnsten, 2000
Stress impairs selective attention
McEwen et al., 1986
Stress modulates hippocampus and septum to
effect selective perception (attention)
Oades, 1979
CRH activates behavior and intensifies response
to stress independently of HPA axis
Koob et al., 1993
Excitatory or inhibitory effects of GCs on neurons
may depend on their state of activation
Joëls and de Kloot, 1992
AVP associated with "defensive" arousal,
attention, or vigilance
Carter and Altemus, 1997
Sensory
Thresholds
CRH facilitates acoustic startle
Koob, 1991
Social defeat diminishes nocioception in mice
Miczek et al., 1982
Handling and species-specific stress-evoking
odors cause analgesia in rats
Fanselow and Sigmundi,
1986
ACTIVITY
Locomotor
CRH-induced in familiar habitat
Sutton et al., 1982
CRH-inhibited in unfamiliar habitat
Britton et al 1982
CRH_induced walking and swimming (newt)
Lowry et al., 1996
Feeding and grooming correlated with subsequent
increase in GC
Shiraishi et al., 1984
Dispersal
GC/stress involved in seasonal population
dispersal in birds
Lee and McDonald, 1985;
Silverin, 1997; Wingfield
et al., , 1997
Exploratory
GC restores exploratory activity eliminated by
adrenalectomy
Veldhuis et al., 1982
Exploratory
CRH enhances effects of novelty
Koob, 1991
ACTH excitatory with novel stimulus and
inhibitory with prolonged stimulus
Oades, 1979
ACTH but not GC impairs habituation to an
acoustic stimulus and reduces exploration
File, 1978
Some elements enhanced, others impaired in
lizards; stress affects ameliorated in castrates
Greenberg, 1985
GC evokes rapid (nongenomic) locomotor
response in rats in novel but not familiar cage
Sandi et al., 1996
THERMOGREGULATION
Hippocampal GC receptors mediate stress
responsiveness to novel habitats
Hiebert et al., 2000
Diurnal torpor in hummingbird
COGNITIVE
Memory and
learning
Epi facilitates acquisition
Smith, 1973
CRH facilitates acquisition of visual
discrimination
Koob, 1991
ACTH facilitates and corticosterone impairs
imprinting
Martin, 1978
Melanocortins facilitate habituation (toad)
Carpenter and Carr, 1996
Melanocortin enhance learned avoidance and
approach behavior was contrasted with
Attenuated learning
Bohus and de Wied, 1980
Attenuated acquisition and performance of
learned behavior
McEwen et al., 1986
Stress facilitates classical conditioning in males
but not females
Shors et al., 1992, 2000;
Wood & Shors, 1998
Stress-facilitated learning depends on stressor
intensity, duration and context
Shors and Servatius, 1997
Stress-induced rise in natural benzodiazepine
levels rise and apparently enhance the inhibitory
neurotransmitter GABA, preventing retention of
irrelevant information
Levine, 1971; Izquierdo
and Medina, 1991
SOCIAL DOMINANCE
GC increases submissiveness
Leshner and Politch, 1979
Losers of territorial fights become subordinate in
the lab (lizard)
Greenberg et al., 1984
REPRODUCTION
Endorphins block gonadotropin releasing factor
and CS impairs gonadal responsiveness to
gonadotropins
Sapolsky, 1994
CRF inhibits sexual behavior in female rats
Sirinathsinghji et al., 1983
ACTH can induce transient increase in
testosterone while sustained CS suppresses
testosterone
Moberg, 1985
Can be facilitated by presumed stress of
aggressive activity
Antelman and Caggiula,
1980
Prenatal stress syndrome: stressed pregnant rats
deliver feminized male pups
Ward & Weisz, 1980;
Greenberg and Wingfield,
1987
DYSFUNCTIONAL COMPENSATIONS
Stereotypies: precipitated by stress
Broverman et al., 1974,
Cooper & Nicol, 1991,
1993, but see Mason 1991
ACTH-induced stretch-yawn syndrome and
grooming.
Gispen, 1982
MSH induces grooming behavior in rats
O'Donohue et al., 1981
Subordinate rats increase alcohol consumption
Blanchard et al., 1993
Addictions, neuroses and psychoses precipitated
by stress
Arnsten, 1997
Atress catalyzes hyperexcitability in fear-
mediating circuits leading to anxiety disorders
Rosen and Shulkin, 1998
ACTH, adrenocorticotrophic hormone; EPI, epinephrine; GC, glucocorticoid; MSH, melanocyte-
stimulating hormone3)
... The objectives of the study are to: (Greenberg, Carr & Summers, 2002). ...
... However, prolonged periods of stress, where the cost of coping harms the individual's functions necessary for well-being, may cause subclinical symptoms of a disease, leading to serious ill-health including mental illness (Greenberg et al., 2002;Griffin, 2010;Moberg, 2000). Chronic stress may impair cognition, volition, decision-making and affect (Greenberg et al., 2002;McEwen, 2017), and may hamper individuals' social and occupational well-being. ...
... However, prolonged periods of stress, where the cost of coping harms the individual's functions necessary for well-being, may cause subclinical symptoms of a disease, leading to serious ill-health including mental illness (Greenberg et al., 2002;Griffin, 2010;Moberg, 2000). Chronic stress may impair cognition, volition, decision-making and affect (Greenberg et al., 2002;McEwen, 2017), and may hamper individuals' social and occupational well-being. Impaired social and occupational well-being is cause for concern especially in terms of mental health. ...
Full-text available
Thesis
Distinctly African health-promoting human occupations are under-researched in occupational therapy. Many occupational therapy interventions used in South Africa have been developed elsewhere and may be inaccessible to many. African dance that produces neurogenic tremors (ADNT) is an occupation that may already be accessible to many, and a potential resource for health and could be used in occupational therapy. Research Question: What is the nature and enactment of ADNT? Aim: The study aimed to explore, describe and explain the nature and enactment of ADNT among professional dancers in Cape Town, South Africa. Research objectives: To explore the perceived temporal, spatial and sociocultural conditions conducive for ADNT. To describe and explain the enactment of ADNT in terms of format, pace, and the sequence of steps involved in performing African dance repertoires that are known to produce neurogenic tremors. To describe the experience of those who participate in ADNT by exploring the subjective effect it has had on stress levels or during stressful periods. Methodology: Case study methodology was used. Semi-structured interviews, focus groups, and participant observation were the data collection methods used. Thematic analysis was employed to analyse the data. Findings: Four themes emerged from the study (1) Triggers: Improvisation, Energy and pushing beyond limits. (2) Essence of self: Embodying Africa through dance. (3) Leaving and returning to the body and (4) Creatures of the soil: Connected to the ground and beyond…for health. Discussion: ADNT is healing, relational, transcendent, and contextually situated. It facilitates self-acceptance (ubuwena) through embodying Africa (KwaNtu) and holds potential to promote social cohesion (ubuntu). It is mainly enacted through improvisation (on and off-stage), through which socio-historical-cultural intergenerational resources (isintu) embedded within, are tapped into. ADNT offers opportunities for connectedness with the self, other people, as well as with transcendent beings. Conclusion: This study has illuminated the nature of ADNT as an extraordinary human occupation that offers participants instances of personal and collective meaning-making, healing, and transcendence. Transcendence is proposed as a source of personal and shared meaning.
... The expression of these two interdependent life history traits (e.g. in fish; Crossin et al., 2014;Gislason et al., 2010) is optimal within a range of environmental conditions (Stearns, 1992), and deviations from this range are perceived by individuals as threats to their homeostasis (i.e. equilibrium state) triggering a stress response (Greenberg et al., 2002;McEwen and Wingfield, 2003). While acute stress responses to short perceived challenges enable individuals to cope with environmental perturbations (Bonier et al., 2009), chronic stress responses to long-lasting stress factors can have detrimental effects on individuals' life history traits. ...
... Indeed, chronic stress affects multiple physiological processes and the energy allocated to the stress response can lead to reduced growth and reproductive investment and ultimately individuals' death (Barton, 2002;Bonier et al., 2009). Therefore, individuals' stress response can increase their fitness in the short-term but will become detrimental if sustained; as such intermediate stress responses (due to intermediate perception or intermediate reactivity) are expected to be optimal (Greenberg et al., 2002). Consequently, the increasing variation in environmental factors exposes juvenile fish to more and more frequent stressful environmental conditions that may affect their life history traits and hence alter nurseries' functioning. ...
Article
Environmental changes can alter the nursery function of coastal areas through their impact on juveniles' growth and survival rates, an effect mediated by individuals’ chronic stress response. Fish chronic stress can be quantified using scale cortisol but no study has yet been quantified the spatio-temporal variations in scale cortisol and its relationship with growth in wild nurseries. We collected wild sea bass juveniles (Dicentrarchus labrax, four years, three nurseries) and found that scale cortisol levels increased consistently with age and across cohorts in 2019 and 2020 probably due to greater stress history in older fish and/or heatwaves that occurred in summers of 2018 and 2019. Growth was impaired in fish with high scale cortisol in 2019 and 2020, confirming the usefulness of scale cortisol as a biomarker of broad and local constraints in wild fish; longer time series will enable us to identify environmental factors underpinning these temporal variations.
... The lesser scaup is a diving duck species that has experienced population declines in North America due to hunting, habitat loss, climate change, and contaminants [27]. Such prolonged, mostly anthropogenic stressors cause low survival and poor reproductive success [28] and are detrimental to individual and population health [29,30]. As the lesser scaup has experienced population declines due to multiple long-term stressors, it is an ideal model species in which to study stress responses. ...
Full-text available
Article
Human-induced environmental changes that act as long-term stressors pose significant impacts on wildlife health. Energy required for maintenance or other functions may be re-routed towards coping with stressors, ultimately resulting in fluctuations in metabolite levels associated with energy metabolism. While metabolomics approaches are used increasingly to study environmental stressors, its use in studying stress in birds is in its infancy. We implanted captive lesser scaup (Aythya affinis) with either a biodegradable corticosterone (CORT) pellet to mimic the effects of a prolonged stressor or a placebo pellet. 1D 1H nuclear magnetic resonance (NMR) spectroscopy was performed on serum samples collected over 20 days after implant surgery. We hypothesized that CORT pellet-induced physiological stress would alter energy metabolism and result in distinct metabolite profiles in ducks compared with placebo (control). Quantitative targeted metabolite analysis revealed that metabolites related to energy metabolism: glucose, formate, lactate, glutamine, 3-hydroxybutyrate, ethanolamine, indole-3- acetate, and threonine differentiated ducks with higher circulatory CORT from controls on day 2. These metabolites function as substrates or intermediates in metabolic pathways related to energy production affected by elevated serum CORT. The use of metabolomics shows promise as a novel tool to identify and characterize physiological responses to stressors in wild birds.
... Doch die psychische Belastung durch Stress kann nicht nur zu körperlichen, son dern auch zu psychischen Beschwerden führen. So findet man einen erhöhten Stresshormonspiegel ebenfalls bei Menschen, die sich depressiv fühlen [46,47]. ...
Article
Viele somatische Erkrankungen, zum Beispiel Diabetes mellitus oder Herz-Kreislauf- Erkrankungen, gehen mit einem erhöhten Risiko für depressive Symptome einher. Sportliche Aktivität kann sowohl die psychischen als auch die körperlichen Beschwerden positiv beeinflussen. Der folgende Artikel bietet einen Überblick zu pathophysiologischen Wechselwirkungen und den Effekten von Sporttherapie auf depressive Symptome bei körperlichen Grunderkrankungen.© underdogstudios / stock.adobe.com © underdogstudios / stock.adobe.com
... Stress has not only been shown to have a negative impact on mental health, but also on behavioural variables, such as drinking, smoking, and violent behaviour [10][11][12][13]-variables that in turn adversely influence health and wellbeing. Greenberg et al. [14] further collected literature describing the emotional and cognitive changes that resulted of stress, giving more context to the dangers of stressful life events for mental health. Besides that, plenty of studies have reported an inverse relationship between stress and subjective life satisfaction, providing evidence of the unfavourable influences that stress has on psychological wellbeing [15][16][17][18]. ...
Full-text available
Article
The COVID-19 pandemic has been a major source of stress for a majority of people that might have negative long-term effects on mental health and well-being. In recent years, video games and their potential positive effects on stress relief have been researched and “relaxation” has been an important keyword in marketing a certain kind of video game. In a quasi-experimental design, this study investigated the increase of average daily player peak (ADPPs) for the COVID period compared to the pre-COVID period and if this increase was significantly larger for relaxing games in contrast to non-relaxing games. Results showed a medium-sized increase of ADPPs over all types of games but no difference between relaxing games and non-relaxing games. These results are discussed in regards to their potential of presenting gaps between the current theoretical models of the influence of video games on mental health and actual observed player behaviour.
Article
In aquatic ecosystems, filter feeders like mussels are particularly vulnerable to microplastics (MP). However, little is known about how the polymer type and the associated properties (like additives or remaining monomers) of MP impact organisms, as the predominant type of MP used for effect studies on the organismic level are micron grade polystyrene spheres, without considering their chemical composition. Therefore, we exposed the freshwater mussel Dreissena bugensis (D. bugensis) to in-depth characterized fragments in the same concentration and size range (20-120µm): recycled polyethylene terephthalate from drinking bottles, polyamide, polystyrene, polylactic acid, and mussel shell fragments as natural particle control. Real-time valvometry, used to study behavioral responses via the movement of the mus-sels' valves, showed that mussels cannot distinguish between natural and MP particles., and therefore do not cease their filtration, as when exposed to dissolved pollutants. This unintentional ingestion led to polymer type-dependent adverse effects (activity of antioxidant enzymes and proteomic alterations), related to chemicals and residual monomers found in MP. Overall, recycled PET elicited the strongest negative effects, likely caused by anthranilamide, anthranilonitrile and butylated hydroxytoluene, contained in the fragments, which are toxic to aquatic organisms. As PET is among the most abundant MP in the environment, sublethal effects may gradually manifest at the population level, leading to irreversible ecosystem changes.
Article
The Largemouth Bass Micropterus salmoides, a popular sport fish, is subjected to multiple sublethal stressors during angling, including high water temperature, exercise, handling, live‐well retention, and weigh‐in procedures. Combined effects of ambient and live‐well temperatures on the stress response and recovery from angling‐induced exercise have not been tested in conditions similar to those encountered in tournaments. Therefore, we assessed the effects of ambient temperature (17, 25, and 33°C) and live‐well temperature differential (−4, 0, and +4°C) on the physiological stress response of Largemouth Bass (mean length = 331 mm) at rest, following a simulated angling stressor, and throughout 8 h of recovery in live wells. Stress variables were measured in whole blood (hematocrit, hemoglobin, pH, partial pressure of oxygen [pO2], partial pressure of carbon dioxide [pCO2], Na+, K+, Ca2+, Cl−, and leukocytes) and plasma (cortisol, glucose, lactate, and osmolality). Fish acclimated to 17°C showed the greatest cortisol response after the chasing stressor; however, higher levels of glucose, lactate, pCO2, K+, and monocyte percentage were found at 33°C, and blood pH, Cl−, and lymphocyte percentage were lower at 33°C than at 17°C. When live‐well temperature was manipulated, cortisol levels were highest in fish subjected to the coldest conditions (acclimated to 17°C and retained in 13°C and 17°C live wells) and the warmest condition (acclimated to 33°C and retained in 37°C live wells). However, all fish subjected to the colder extremes survived, whereas 100% mortality occurred in the warmest condition. Besides cortisol, indicators of stress were less pronounced at colder temperatures. Glucose, lactate, and notably K+ concentrations were highest in 37°C live wells, and blood pH, Ca2+, Na+, and Cl− were lowest. Low blood lymphocytes and high monocytes at the warmest conditions indicate reduced immunocompetence or inflammation. Mortality at high temperature may result from exhaustion of aerobic and anaerobic energy sources, failure to recover from metabolic acidosis, and an inability to regain ionic balance.
Article
The paper provides a comprehensive and foundational mechanistic framework of hormesis that establishes its centrality in medicine and public health. This hormetic framework is applied to the assessment of olive polyphenols with respect to their capacity to slow the onset and reduce the magnitude of a wide range of age-related disorders and neurodegenerative diseases, including Alzheimer’s Disease and Parkinson’s Disease. It is proposed that olive polyphenol-induced anti-inflammatory protective effects are mediated in large part via the activation of AMPK and the upregulation of Nrf2 pathway. Consistently, herein we also review the importance of the modulation of Nrf2-related stress responsive vitagenes by olive polyphenols, which at low concentration according to the hormesis theory activates this neuroprotective cascade to preserve brain health and its potential use in the prevention and therapy against aging and age-related cognitive disorders in humans.
Chapter
Mental Stress can be defined as a normal physiological and biological reaction to an incident or a situation that makes a person feel challenged, troubled, or helpless. While dealing with stress, some changes occur in the biological function of a person, which results in a considerable change in some bio-signals such as, Electrocardiogram (ECG), Electromyography (EMG), Electrodermal Activity (EDA), respiratory rate. This paper aims to review the effect of mental stress on mental condition and health, the changes in biosignals as an indicator of the stress response and train a model to detect stressed states using the biosignals. This paper delivers a brief review of mental stress and biosignals correlation. It represents four Support Vector Machine (SVM) models trained with ECG and EMG features from an open access database based on task related stress. After performing comparative analysis on the four types of trained SVM models with chosen features, Gaussian Kernel SVM is selected as the best SVM model to detect mental stress which can predict the mental condition of a subject for a stressed and relaxed condition having an accuracy of 93.7%. These models can be investigated further with more biosignals and applied in practice, which will be beneficial for the physician.
Full-text available
Book
This is a book. The Preface describing contents is uploaded.
Chapter
Life ultimately depends on an organism’s capacity to sustain the flame of intracellular metabolism. Hence all living animals store fuel within their bodies, and they possess many behavioral and physiological processes to insure the relative stability of these stores. While eating and related instrumental behaviors oversee the maintenance of adequate energy intake, hormones and ancillary patterns of enzyme induction establish a balance between the storage and usage of the ingested nutrients.
Article
Corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF), the recently characterized and synthesized 41-amino acid polypeptide isolated from ovine hypothalami, has been shown to be a potent stimulator of adenohypophyseal beta-endorphin and corticotropin (ACTH) secretion both in vitro and in vivo. In common with other regulatory peptides, CRF has also been demonstrated to possess extra-hypophysiotropic roles. Indeed, intracerebroventricularly (i.c.v.) administered CRF elicits several endocrine and behavioural responses compatible with the concept that this peptide could be a key signal in coordinating the organism's endocrine and behavioural responses to stressful and other adaptive stimuli. We now provide the first evidence for neurally placed CRF in the control of a specific hormone-dependent behavioural response and unequivocally demonstrate an extremely potent suppressive effect of CRF on sexual behaviour in the female rat when microinfused into the arcuate-ventromedial area of the hypothalamus (ARC-VMH) and the mesencephalic central grey (MCG).
Chapter
There is a necessary relationship between research design, the welfare of research animals, and the validity of research data. This paper explores several dimensions of this relationship along with comments on the importance of ethologically informed design. Design, in the sense of a coherent programme that guides a specific scientific undertaking, involves defining and selecting research variables and the methods that govern their manipulation and/or observation, measurement and subsequent interpretation. Design both guides and is guided by the questions or problems an investigator wishes to address.
Chapter
Until the mid-1960s, it was widely believed that the gonadal hormones, particularly the androgens, were the only hormones involved in the control of aggressive behavior. This belief was based on the rather full literature establishing that these hormones are important to aggression, and the effects of no other subsystem had received experimental attention. In the past 15 years, however, it has been established that there are indeed many other kinds of hormonal effects on aggressive behavior, and many of those are discussed in other chapters of this book. This chapter is concerned with the effects of the hormones of the pituitary-adrenocortical axis, ACTH and corticosterone.