ArticlePDF Available

Engaging in Cognitive Activities, Aging, and Mild Cognitive Impairment: A Population-Based Study

Authors:

Abstract

The authors investigated whether engaging in cognitive activities is associated with aging and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in a cross-sectional study derived from an ongoing population-based study of normal cognitive aging and MCI in Olmsted County, MN. A random sample of 1,321 study participants ages 70 to 89 (N=1,124 cognitively normal persons, and N=197 subjects with MCI) were interviewed about the frequency of cognitive activities carried out in late life (within 1 year of the date of interview). Computer activities; craft activities, such as knitting, quilting, etc.; playing games; and reading books were associated with decreased odds of having MCI. Social activities, such as traveling, were marginally significant. Even though the point-estimates for reading magazines, playing music, artistic activities, and group activities were associated with reduced odds of having MCI, none of these reached statistical significance. The equally high prevalence of reading newspapers in both groups yielded no significant between-group difference.
Engaging in Cognitive Activities, Aging and Mild Cognitive
Impairment: A Population-Based Study
Yonas E. Geda, MD, MSc, Hillary M. Topazian, Lewis A. Roberts, Rosebud O. Roberts, MB
ChB, MS, David S. Knopman, MD, V. Shane Pankratz, PhD, Teresa J.H. Christianson, BSc,
Bradley F. Boeve, MD, Eric G. Tangalos, MD, Robert J. Ivnik, PhD, and Ronald C. Petersen,
MD, PhD
Department of Health Sciences Research, Divisions of Epidemiology (Drs. Geda, Petersen, and
Roberts), Biomedical Statistics and Informatics (Ms. Christianson and Dr. Pankratz). Departments
of Psychiatry and Psychology (Drs. Geda, Ivnik), Neurology (Drs. Boeve, Knopman, and
Petersen), and Primary Care Internal Medicine (Dr. Tangalos), College of Medicine, Mayo Clinic,
Rochester MN, USA. Wheaton University (Topazian), Chicago IL, Rice University, Houston, TX
(LA Roberts) USA
Abstract
We investigated whether engaging in cognitive activities is associated with mild cognitive
impairment (MCI) in a cross-sectional study derived from an ongoing population-based study of
normal cognitive aging and MCI in Olmsted County, Minnesota. A random sample of 1321 non-
demented study participants ages 70 to 89 (n = 1124 cognitively normal persons and n = 197
subjects with MCI) was interviewed about the frequency of cognitive activities carried out in late
life (within one year of the date of interview). Computer activities [OR (95% CI) = 0.50 (0.36,
0.71); p < .0001)], craft activities such as knitting, quilting, etc. [0.66 (0.47, 0.93); p = 0.019)],
playing games [0.65 (0.47, 0.90); p = 0.010)], and reading books [0.67 (0.49, 0.94); p = 0.019)]
were associated with decreased odds of having MCI. Social activities such as traveling were
marginally significant [0.71 (0.51, 1.00); p = 0.050)]. Even though the point estimates for reading
magazines, playing music, artistic activities, and group activities were associated with reduced
odds of having MCI, none reached statistical significance. We could not expect to observe any
difference between the two groups on the variable of reading newspapers since almost identical
proportions of the two groups (97.4% of normals and 97.5% of the MCI group) were engaged in
reading newspapers on a regular basis.
Keywords
cognitive activities; aging; mild cognitive impairment
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is the intermediate stage between the cognitive changes of
normal aging and dementia 1. The reader is referred elsewhere for a detailed discussion of
Corresponding Author: Yonas E. Geda, MD, MSc. Associate Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry. College of Medicine, Mayo
Clinic. 200 First Street SW, Rochester, MN 55905, USA. geda.yonas@mayo.edu.
Author Contributions: Dr. Geda had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and
the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: Geda, Roberts, Knopman, Petersen. Acquisition of data: Geda, Knopman,
Boeve, Tangalos, Petersen. Analysis and interpretation of data: Geda, Christianson, and Pankratz. Drafting of the manuscript: Geda.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Geda, Roberts, Knopman, Christianson, Pankratz, Topazian,
Boeve, Tangalos, and Petersen. Statistical analysis: Christianson, Pankratz. Obtained funding: Geda, Petersen. Administrative,
technical, and material support: Geda, Petersen. Study supervision: Roberts, Petersen.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
NIH Public Access
Author Manuscript
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 April 1.
Published in final edited form as:
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci
. 2011 ; 23(2): 149–154. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.23.2.149.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
MCI 2, 3. Subjects with MCI constitute a high risk group because they develop dementia at a
rate of 10% to 15% per year compared with 1% to 2% per year in the general population 4.
In view of this, it is critical to identify potential protective factors against MCI. Previous
studies have reported an association between cognitive activities and reduced risk of
incident dementia 5-7. However, little is known about the association between cognitive
activities and the odds of having MCI. A convenience sample of a prospective cohort study
involving community-dwelling elderly participants reported that baseline cognitive activities
were associated with decreased risk of amnestic MCI8. There is a need to examine this
question in a population-based setting using a larger sample.
We examined whether engaging in cognitive activities is associated with MCI in a cross-
sectional study derived from an ongoing population-based study of normal cognitive aging
and MCI in Olmsted County, Minnesota. Throughout this manuscript, one can
interchangeably think of the phrase “cognitive activity” to be equivalent to “mental activity”
or “intellectual activity”.
METHODS
SETTING
The detail of the design and conduct of the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging was reported
elsewhere 9. Briefly, it is an on-going population-based study of normal aging and MCI in
Olmsted County, Minnesota. Elderly persons ages 70 to 89 on the prevalence date of
October 1, 2004, were recruited by using a stratified random sampling from the target
population of nearly 10,000 elderly individuals in Olmsted County, Minnesota. The
sampling involved equal allocation of men and women in two age strata: 70 to 79 and 80 to
89 years old. During the first follow-up phase of the study, which took place between 2006
through 2008, we introduced a structured interview format to collect data on cognitive
activities. 1,321 non-demented study participants completed the interview. At the time of the
interview, neither the study participant nor the research personnel knew the case-control
status of a participant. The classification of a study participant as having MCI or not was a
downstream event to the collection of data on cognitive activities. The details of the
classification process of MCI are discussed elsewhere in this paper. The study was approved
by the institutional review boards of Mayo Clinic and the Olmsted Medical Center.
STUDY DESIGN
A population-based cross-sectional study involving 1,321 non-demented study participants
(n = 197 subjects with MCI and n = 1,124 cognitively normal persons).
MEASUREMENT OF MCI
The association of interest in this study is between cognitive activities and the odds of
having MCI. Each participant in the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging underwent the following
three face-to-face evaluations: (1) neurological evaluation by a physician; (2) risk factor
assessment by a nurse or study coordinator; and (3) neuropsychological testing that was
interpreted by a neuropsychologist. The interview by the nurse or study coordinator included
administration of the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale 10 to the participant and to an
informant. The neurological evaluation was performed by a physician and included
administration of the Short Test of Mental Status 11, medical history review, and a complete
neurological examination.
Neuropsychological testing was performed to assess four cognitive domains: (1) memory
(Logical Memory-II [delayed recall] and Visual Reproduction-II [delayed recall] from
Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, and delayed recall from the Auditory Verbal Learning
Geda et al. Page 2
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 April 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Test) 12-15; (2) executive function (Trail Making Test B 16, and Digit Symbol Substitution
from Wechsler Adult Intelligent Scale-Revised); (3) language (Boston Naming Test 17, and
category fluency) 18; and (4) visuospatial skills (Picture Completion and Block Design from
WAIS-R).
We considered as cases all the participants who met the revised Mayo Clinic criteria for
MCI: (1) cognitive concern expressed by a physician, informant, participant, or nurse; (2)
cognitive impairment in one or more domains (executive function, memory, language, or
visuospatial); (3) normal functional activities; and (4) not demented 2, 3. Subjects with MCI
could have a Clinical Dementia Rating Scale score of 0 or 0.5; however, the final diagnosis
of MCI was not based exclusively on the clinical dementia rating, but rather on all available
data. The diagnosis of normal cognition, MCI, dementia, or Alzheimer’s disease was made
by an expert consensus panel of physicians, psychologists, and nurses based on published
criteria 12, 13, 19-21. The panel meets once per week and reviews three independent sources of
data, i.e., the clinical data collected by behavioral neurologists and physicians of other
specialties with expertise in dementia and MCI, neuropsychological data collected by
psychometrists who are supervised by neuropsychologists, and nursing data gathered by
research nurses 9.
MEASUREMENT OF COGNITIVE ACTIVITIES
We defined the exposure of interest to be reading, craft activities, computer activities,
playing games, playing music, group activities (e.g., book club), social activities (e.g., going
out to movies and theaters), artistic activities, and watching television. We modified
previously validated instruments to measure these activities 6, 22, 23. A research nurse or
psychometrist interviewed each participant by using a structured survey with ordinal
responses (e.g., reading books at a frequency of once per week, twice per week, etc.) The
participants were asked to provide information about these activities within a year of the
date of interview (late life cognitive activity). The measurement of cognitive activities was
conducted along with neurological evaluation, neuropsychological assessment and risk
factor ascertainments. Once these data were collected, then a consensus panel of experts
classified the study participant to be cognitively normal or to have MCI.
MEASUREMENT OF COVARIATES
In addition to traditional confounders (age, sex, and education), we also defined medical
comorbidity and depression to be covariates for the purpose of this study. We measured
medical comorbidity by using the Charlson index, which is a widely used weighted index
that takes into account the number and severity of diseases. Thus for each unit increase in
Charlson index, there is a stepwise increase in the cumulative mortality attributable to the
comorbid medical disease 24. We measured depression by using the Beck Depression
Inventory-II 25. Additionally, we adjusted for physical exercise by assigning a numeric score
to frequency of physical exercise and adding the scores across the light, moderate, and
vigorous strata (equal weighting to all strata). The details of the physical exercise
measurement were reported elsewhere 26.
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Multi-variable logistic regression analyses were conducted to examine the strength of
association of cognitive activities with the odds of having MCI by computing odds ratios
and corresponding 95% confidence intervals. The primary analysis was conducted by
adjusting for traditional confounders (age [continuous variable], sex, and education
[continuous variable]). We also conducted secondary analysis by adjusting for medical
comorbidity (weighted Charlson index as a continuous variable), depressive symptoms
(BDI-II score <13 versus 13), and physical exercise (continuous variable).26.
Geda et al. Page 3
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 April 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The frequency of each activity was dichotomized as none (once per month or less) versus
any other frequency. We considered watching television to be hypothetically less beneficial,
therefore watching TV was “reverse” scored, i.e., watching more television (>6 hours/day)
versus watching less ( 6 hours/day).
Analyses were conducted for cognitive activity carried out in late life (within the past one
year). Statistical testing was done at the conventional 2-tailed alpha level of 0.05. All
analyses were performed by using SAS (Cary, NC).
RESULTS
Table 1 summarizes the demographic data. There were 1,321 non-demented study
participants (n = 1,124 cognitively normal persons, n = 197 subjects with MCI). Among the
cognitively normal group (normals), there were an equal number of males and females,
whereas among the MCI group there were more males than females. On average, the MCI
group was older than the normal group. The two groups also significantly differed in
education, medical comorbidity, and depressive symptoms. Therefore, in the primary
analysis the comparison of engaging in cognitive activities between the two groups was
made after adjusting for age (continuous variable), sex, and education (continuous variable).
In a secondary analysis, we also adjusted for depressive symptoms, medical comorbidity and
physical exercise.
Table 2 displays the data comparing the two groups as measured by OR (95% CI). Reading
books [0.67 (0.49, 0.94)], playing games [0.65 (0.47, 0.90)], craft activities (quilting,
pottery, etc.) [0.66 (0.47, 0.93)], and computer activities [0.50 (0.36, 0.71)] were
significantly associated with decreased odds of having MCI. The point estimate for social
activity (e.g., going out with friends) was also associated with decreased odds of having
MCI, but this association was marginally significant [0.71 (0.51, 1.00)].
The point estimates for reading magazines [0.81 (0.49, 1.32)], playing music [0.79 (0.50,
1.25)], artistic activities [0.81 (0.49, 1.32)], and group activities [0.88 (0.64, 1.22)] were
associated with reduced odds of MCI; however, none reached statistical significance. The
only exception to the overall trend was the cognitive activity of reading newspapers. The OR
for reading newspapers [1.13 (0.43, 2.99)] was suggestive of increased odds of having MCI;
however, close examination of the data indicates that almost identical proportions of the two
groups engaged in regular newspaper reading (97.4% of the cognitively normal group vs
97.5% of the MCI group were reading newspapers on a regular basis).
We considered watching television to be a hypothetically less beneficial activity, therefore
watching TV was “reverse” scored, i.e., watching more television (>6 hours/day) versus
watching less ( 6 hours per day). We observed that watching less TV was associated with
decreased odds of MCI [OR (95% CI) = 0.48 (0.27, 0.86); p= 0.013].
In the secondary analysis, additional adjustment for depressive symptoms, medical
comorbidity and physical exercise did not affect the significance level observed in the
primary analysis (data not shown).
DISCUSSION
In this population-based cross-sectional study we observed that cognitive activities such as
computer use, playing games, reading books, craft activities (quilting, knitting, etc.) and
watching less TV were associated with 30% to 50% reduced odds of having MCI. Social
activities such as traveling were marginally significant. Even though the point estimates for
reading magazines, playing music, artistic activities, and group activities were associated
Geda et al. Page 4
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 April 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
with reduced odds of having MCI, none reached statistical significance. Almost identical
proportions of the two groups were engaged in reading newspapers on a regular basis,
therefore we could not observe a significant difference between the two groups.
Several studies have reported the association of cognitive/intellectual or ‘mental’ activities
with decreased risk of incident dementia5-7. However, little is known about the association
of cognitive activities with MCI. The Bronx Aging Study prospectively followed a
convenience sample of 437 community-dwelling cognitively normal elderly persons ages 75
and older to the outcome of incident amnestic MCI 8. During the median follow-up duration
of 5.7 years, there were 58 subjects who developed incident amnestic MCI. The
investigators noted that a unit increase in cognitive activity was associated with a 5%
decreased risk of incident amnestic MCI. Even though the Bronx study was a convenience
sample, the prospective study design would enable one to make some degree of etiologic
inferences. The investigators retrofitted the MCI criteria; hence this might have potentially
led to misclassification errors. Although our study is population-based, the cross-sectional
design does not allow one to make etiologic inferences. Therefore, the observations made in
our current study need to be tested on a larger sample in a prospective cohort design.
The findings of our study should be interpreted within the context of the following
limitations. The first limitation pertains to study design. Since this was a cross-sectional
study, we cannot determine the direction of causality between the hypothesized exposure of
interest (i.e., cognitive activity) and the hypothesized outcome of interest (i.e., MCI).
Second, like any survey based study, recall bias is a potential limitation. This is even more
relevant to participants with MCI; however, at our center the data on cognitive activities are
collected prior to determination of whether a person has MCI or not. Therefore, neither the
participant nor the research personnel knew the case control status of the participant at the
time of administration of the cognitive activities questionnaire. This likely minimized bias,
but could not eliminate it. Additionally, in the past we had reported that the test-retest
correlations were similar among subjects with normal cognition and MCI 26.
Our study did not address mechanism of action. However, the possible beneficial impact of
cognitive activities as discussed in the literature is worth mentioning. Engaging in cognitive
activities may be a marker for an overall healthy lifestyle, e.g., a person who likes to read
books on a regular basis may also engage in an overall healthy lifestyle that includes
exercise, diet, and stress management. Another possible explanation is related to the brain/
cognitive reserve hypothesis 27, 28. Engaging in cognitive activity is more likely to reinforce
and perhaps stimulate the formation of various neuronal networks in the brain 28 that can
buffer against dementia and Alzheimer’s disease 29. This argument is supported by both
basic science and clinical research. For instance, animals with enriched environments are
protected against cognitive impairment 28, 30. Additionally in clinical settings it is also
observed that clinical manifestations may not correlate with the neuropathological burden on
postmortem examination 6, 31-33, implying that the cognitive reserve may serve as a buffer
against the Alzheimer’s disease neuropathological burden. Since MCI is considered to be a
prodromal state to Alzheimer’s disease, one can invoke the cognitive reserve theory to
explain the inverse association between cognitive activities and the odds of having MCI.
Yet, another potential mechanism pertains to the classic stress model proposed by Sapolsky
and colleagues 34. According to this model, the hippocampus, which is the epicenter of the
memory network 35, has a number of glucocorticoid receptors. These receptors are down
regulated in excessively stressful situations. Thus, cognitive activities may serve as stress
modifying agents, leading to decreased “neurotoxic” insult to the hippocampus and related
structures pertinent to cognition and emotion.
Geda et al. Page 5
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 April 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
In summary, our findings contribute to the growing body of literature that indicates that
cognitive activities are associated with decreased odds of having MCI. A future prospective
population-based cohort study needs to confirm whether cognitive activity is associated with
a decreased risk of incident MCI. We are following a large cohort of cognitively normal
persons to the outcome of incident MCI; thus we will be able to test the observation made
from the current cross-sectional study. The population based setting will improve
generalizability, and the prospective cohort will strengthen etiologic inferences.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to express their appreciation to Stephanie K. Cheung, a summer research student from
Columbia University for their help in the final editing of the manuscript.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (K01 MH68351;
AG06786, Mayo CTSA (RR024150 [Career Transition Award]), the RWJ Foundation (Harold Amos Scholar), and
from the Robert H. and Clarice Smith and Abigail Van Buren Alzheimer’s Disease Research Program.
References
1. Petersen RC, Thomas RG, Grundman M, et al. Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild
cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med. 2005; 352(23):2379–2388. [PubMed: 15829527]
2. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004; 256(3):183–194.
[PubMed: 15324362]
3. Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment--beyond controversies,
towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J
Intern Med. 2004; 256(3):240–246. [PubMed: 15324367]
4. Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol.
2001; 58(12):1985–1992. [PubMed: 11735772]
5. Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B. An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life
might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2004; 3(6):343–353. [PubMed: 15157849]
6. Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N
Engl J Med. 2003; 348(25):2508–2516. [PubMed: 12815136]
7. Wilson RS, Mendes De Leon CF, Barnes LL, et al. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities
and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA. 2002; 287(6):742–748. [PubMed: 11851541]
8. Verghese J, LeValley A, Derby C, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of amnestic mild cognitive
impairment in the elderly. Neurology. 2006; 66(6):821–827. [PubMed: 16467493]
9. Roberts RO, Geda YE, Knopman DS, et al. The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging: design and sampling,
participation, baseline measures and sample characteristics. Neuroepidemiology. 2008; 30(1):58–
69. [PubMed: 18259084]
10. Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology.
1993; 43(11):2412–2414. [PubMed: 8232972]
11. Kokmen E, Smith GE, Petersen RC, Tangalos E, Ivnik RC. The short test of mental status.
Correlations with standardized psychometric testing. Arch Neurol. 1991; 48(7):725–728.
[PubMed: 1859300]
12. Ivnik RJ, Malec J, Smith GE. Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies: WAIS--R norms for
ages 56 to 97. Clin Neuropsychol. 1992; 6(Suppl):1–30.
13. Ivnik RJ, Malec J, Smith GE, et al. Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies: WMS--R norms
for ages 56 to 94. Clin Neuropsychol. 1992; 6(Suppl):49–82.
14. Ivnik RJ, Malec J, Smith GE, et al. Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies: updated AVLT
norms for ages 56 through 97. Clin Neuropsychol. 1992; 6(Suppl):83–104.
15. Malec JF, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, et al. Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies: Utility of
corrections for age and education for the WAIS--R. Clin Neuropsychol. 1992; 6(Suppl):31–47.
16. Reitan R. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percep Motor
Skills. 1958; 8:271–276.
Geda et al. Page 6
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 April 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
17. Kaplan, E.; Goodglass, H.; Weintraub, S. The Boston Naming Test. 2. Philadelphia, PA: Lea &
Febiger; 1982.
18. Lucas J, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, et al. Mayo older American studies: Category fluency norms. Journal
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1998; 20:194–200. [PubMed: 9777473]
19. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders - IV.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1994.
20. Ivnik RJ, Malec J, Smith GE, et al. Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies: WAIS-R, WMS-
R, and AVLT norms for ages 56 through 97. Clin Neuropsychol. 1992; 6(Supplement):1–104.
21. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive
impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999; 56(3):303–308. [PubMed:
10190820]
22. Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Beckett LA, et al. Cognitive activity in older persons from a
geographically defined population. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1999; 54(3):155–160.
23. Friedland RP, Fritsch T, Smyth KA, et al. Patients with Alzheimer’s disease have reduced
activities in midlife compared with healthy control-group members. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
2001; 98(6):3440–3445. [PubMed: 11248097]
24. Charlson ME, Pompei P, Ales KL, MacKenzie CR. A new method of classifying prognostic
comorbidity in longitudinal studies: development and validation. J Chronic Dis. 1987; 40(5):373–
383. [PubMed: 3558716]
25. Beck, AT.; Steer, RA.; Brown, GK. Manual for Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). San
Antonio: Psychology Corporation; 2001.
26. Geda YE, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, et al. Physical exercise, aging, and mild cognitive
impairment: a population-based study. Arch Neurol. 2010; 67(1):80–86. [PubMed: 20065133]
27. Katzman R. Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1993;
43(1):13–20. [PubMed: 8423876]
28. Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J Int
Neuropsychol Soc. 2002; 8(3):448–460. [PubMed: 11939702]
29. Snowdon DA, Kemper SJ, Mortimer JA, Greiner LH, Wekstein DR, Markesbery WR. Linguistic
ability in early life and cognitive function and Alzheimer’s disease in late life. Findings from the
Nun Study. JAMA. 1996; 275(7):528–532. [PubMed: 8606473]
30. van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Nat
Rev Neurosci. 2000; 1(3):191–198. [PubMed: 11257907]
31. Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, et al. Neuropathology of older persons without
cognitive impairment from two community-based studies. Neurology. 2006; 66(12):1837–1844.
[PubMed: 16801647]
32. Davis DG, Schmitt FA, Wekstein DR, Markesbery WR. Alzheimer neuropathologic alterations in
aged cognitively normal subjects. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1999; 58(4):376–388. [PubMed:
10218633]
33. Haroutunian V, Schnaider-Beeri M, Schmeidler J, et al. Role of the neuropathology of Alzheimer
disease in dementia in the oldest-old. Arch Neurol. 2008; 65(9):1211–1217. [PubMed: 18779425]
34. Sapolsky RM. Glucocorticoids, stress, and their adverse neurological effects: relevance to aging.
Exp Gerontol. 1999; 34(6):721–732. [PubMed: 10579633]
35. Mesulam MM. From sensation to cognition. Brain. 1998; 121:1013–1052. [PubMed: 9648540]
Geda et al. Page 7
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 April 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Geda et al. Page 8
Table 1
Demographic Characteristics of Study Participants
Variable Normal (N = 1,124) MCI (N = 197) P-value
Men, n (%) 564 (50.2) 116 (58.9) 0.024
Age, yearsa80 (72-93) 83 (72-93) <0.001
Education, yearsa13 (6-20) 12 (6-20) 0.001
>12 years, n (%) 651 (57.9) 91 (46.2)
BDI-II Depression ( 13)b62 (5.5) 29 (14.8) <0.001
Charlson Indexc2 (1-5) 3 (2-6) <0.001
aMedian (range).
b1 patient missing BDI (1 MCI).
cMedian (interquartile range).
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 April 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Geda et al. Page 9
Table 2
Cognitive Activities Carried Out Within 1 year of the Date of Interview
Activity Normal (N=1124) N (%) MCI (N=197) N (%) OR (95% CI)aP-value
Reading newspapers 1095 (97.4) 192 (97.5) 1.13 (0.43, 2.99) 0.81
Reading magazines 1033 (91.9) 174 (88.3) 0.81 (0.49, 1.32) 0.39
Reading books 776 (69.1) 111 (56.3) 0.67 (0.49, 0.94) 0.019
Play games 795 (70.7) 118 (59.9) 0.65 (0.47, 0.90) 0.010
Play music 203 (18.1) 25 (12.7) 0.79 (0.50, 1.25) 0.31
Artistic activities 159 (14.1) 21 (10.7) 0.81 (0.49, 1.32) 0.39
Craft activities 455 (40.5) 57 (28.9) 0.66 (0.47, 0.93) 0.019
Group activities 456 (40.6) 71 (36.0) 0.88 (0.64, 1.22) 0.45
Social activities 871 (77.5) 134 (68.0) 0.71 (0.51, 1.00) 0.050
Computer activities 549 (48.8) 58 (29.4) 0.50 (0.36, 0.71) <0.001
aOR, odds ratios and CI, confidence intervals were computed by comparing frequencies of activities carried once a month or less (reference) versus
any other frequency of activity. Findings are Adjusted for age, sex, and education. Secondary analysis also adjusted for, depression, medical
comorbidity (Charlson index) and physical exercise. We did not observe any significant difference from the primary analysis (data not shown).
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 April 1.
... Most studies found that addressing social, emo�onal, and physical well-being plays a crucial role in promo�ng cogni�ve health outcomes among the elderly popula�on. Specifically, interven�ons focusing on social engagement, emo�onal well-being, and physical ac�vity have been found to promote cogni�ve health outcomes among older adults and reduce the risk of cogni�ve decline (Chiao et al., 2011;Geda et al., 2012;Bryant et al., 2014). ...
... Physical ac�vity was also iden�fied as a cri�cal component of cogni�ve health outcomes in older adults (Geda et al., 2012). Studies have shown that physical ac�vity raises an individual's overall physical well-being, reduces the risk of age-related cogni�ve decline, and improves cogni�ve performance (Geda et al., 2012). ...
... Physical ac�vity was also iden�fied as a cri�cal component of cogni�ve health outcomes in older adults (Geda et al., 2012). Studies have shown that physical ac�vity raises an individual's overall physical well-being, reduces the risk of age-related cogni�ve decline, and improves cogni�ve performance (Geda et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
This systematic review examines the impact of social isolation and loneliness on the cognitive and mental health of elderly individuals living in rural areas. It highlights the limited research on effective interventions tailored to the needs of this population and identifies the barriers that hinder social engagement activities in rural settings. The review identifies the importance of social, emotional, and physical well-being in addressing cognitive health outcomes in rural elderly populations and recommends policy improvements that enhance social and community support networks. The findings of this study can inform policymakers and healthcare practitioners of the need for tailored interventions that improve the quality of life for rural elderly adults.
... Several studies have investigated the role of cognitive reserve in MCI to understand whether it can reduce the cognitive impairment characteristic of this condition, delaying or halting its conversion into dementia. Accordingly, many studies have highlighted the role played by years of schooling, leisure time, and work activity as protective factors for cognitive decline [17][18][19][20]. ...
... Ten studies were selected for this systematic review, including five longitudinal studies [25][26][27][28][29] and five cross-sectional studies [20,[30][31][32][33]. ...
... The geographic distribution of the studies was as follows: four were conducted in Europe [30][31][32][33], three in Asia [25,26,29], and three in North America [20,27,28] These studies included 6145 participants, among whom 2180 were participants with MCI and 3965 were healthy control participants. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive reserve (CR) represents the ability to optimize performance and functioning to cope with brain damage or disease. CR reflects the capability to adaptively and flexibly use cognitive processes and brain networks to compensate for the deterioration typical of aging. Several studies have investigated the potential role of CR in aging, especially from the perspective of preventing and protecting against dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). This systematic literature review aimed to investigate the role of CR as a protective factor against MCI and associated cognitive decline. The review process was conducted according to the PRISMA statement. For this purpose, ten studies were analyzed. The results of this review show that high CR is significantly associated with a reduced risk of MCI. In addition, a significant positive relationship between CR and cognitive functioning is observed when comparing subjects with MCI and healthy subjects and within people with MCI. Thus, the results confirm the positive role of cognitive reserve in mitigating cognitive impairment. The evidence from this systematic review is consistent with the theoretical models of CR. Indeed, previous research hypothesized that specific individual experiences (such as leisure activities) allow a person to acquire successful neural resources over the years to cope with cognitive decline.
Article
Background Lifestyle behavior change and mindfulness have direct and synergistic effects on cognitive functioning and may prevent Alzheimer disease and Alzheimer disease–related dementias (AD/ADRD). We are iteratively developing and testing My Healthy Brain (MHB), the first mindfulness-based lifestyle group program targeting AD/ADRD risk factors in older adults with subjective cognitive decline. Our pilot studies (National Institutes of Health [NIH] stage 1A) have shown that MHB is feasible, acceptable, and associated with improvement in lifestyle behavior and cognitive outcomes. Objective We will compare the feasibility of MHB versus an education control (health enhancement program [HEP]) in 50 older adults (aged ≥60 y) with subjective cognitive decline and AD/ADRD risk factors. In an NIH stage 1B randomized controlled trial (RCT), we will evaluate feasibility benchmarks, improvements in cognitive and lifestyle outcomes, and engagement of hypothesized mechanisms. Methods We are recruiting through clinics, flyers, web-based research platforms, and community partnerships. Participants are randomized to MHB or the HEP, both delivered in telehealth groups over 8 weeks. MHB participants learn behavior modification and mindfulness skills to achieve individualized lifestyle goals. HEP participants receive lifestyle education and group support. Assessments are repeated after the intervention and at a 6-month follow-up. Our primary outcomes are feasibility, acceptability, appropriateness, credibility, satisfaction, and fidelity benchmarks. The secondary outcomes are cognitive function and lifestyle (physical activity, sleep, nutrition, alcohol and tobacco use, and mental and social activity) behaviors. Data analyses will include the proportion of MHB and HEP participants who meet each benchmark (primary outcome) and paired samples 2-tailed t tests, Cohen d effect sizes, and the minimal clinically important difference for each measure (secondary outcomes). Results Recruitment began in January 2024. We received 225 inquiries. Of these 225 individuals, 40 (17.8%) were eligible. Of the 40 eligible participants, 21 (52.5%) were enrolled in 2 group cohorts, 17 (42.5%) were on hold for future group cohorts, and 2 (5%) withdrew before enrollment. All participants have completed before the intervention assessments. All cohort 1 participants (9/21, 43%) have completed either MHB or the HEP (≥6 of 8 sessions) and after the intervention assessments. The intervention for cohort 2 (12/21, 57%) is ongoing. Adherence rates for the Garmin Vivosmart 5 (128/147, 87.1% weeks) and daily surveys (105/122, 86.1% weeks) are high. No enrolled participants have dropped out. Enrollment is projected to be completed by December 2024. Conclusions The RCT will inform the development of a larger efficacy RCT (NIH stage 2) of MHB versus the HEP in a more diverse sample of older adults, testing mechanisms of improvements through theoretically driven mediators and moderators. The integration of mindfulness with lifestyle behavior change in MHB has the potential to be an effective and sustainable approach for increasing the uptake of AD/ADRD risk reduction strategies among older adults. Trial Registration ClinicalTrials.gov NCT05934136; https://www.clinicaltrials.gov/study/NCT05934136 International Registered Report Identifier (IRRID) DERR1-10.2196/64149
Article
Full-text available
One dementia prevention strategy that is effective for older adults is frequent participation in leisure time physical activity. However, research gaps exist in our understanding of the longitudinal relationship between different levels of leisure time physical activity participation and the cognitive function of older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Thus, we investigated this relationship using Health and Retirement Study data from 2012 to 2020. Results from Repeated Measures Multivariate Analysis of Covariance indicated that both the mid (Mean Difference = −0.45) and high level (Mean Difference = −0.82) leisure time physical activity groups showed higher levels of memory function compared to the low-level leisure time physical activity group. Additionally, the mid (Mean Difference = −0.21) and high level (Mean Difference = −0.37) leisure time physical activity groups reported better working memory function, and the mid (Mean Difference = −0.02) and high level (Mean Difference = −0.02) leisure time physical activity groups showed higher levels of attention and processing speed than the low-level leisure time physical activity group. These findings suggest that older adults with mild cognitive impairment who engage either vigorously or moderately in leisure time physical activity (more than three times a week) experience improvements in cognitive function compared to those who do not. This study provides valuable insights for clinicians on the optimal level of leisure time physical activity required to mitigate cognitive decline in older adults with mild cognitive impairment.
Article
Full-text available
Background and aims Amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) is the most common subtype of MCI, which carries a significantly high risk of transitioning to Alzheimer’s disease. Recently, increasing attention has been given to remnant cholesterol (RC), a non-traditional and previously overlooked risk factor. The aim of this study was to explore the association between plasma RC levels and aMCI. Methods Data were obtained from Brain Health Cognitive Management Team in Wuhan (https://hbtcm.66nao.com/admin/). A total of 1,007 community-dwelling elders were recruited for this project. Based on ten tools including general demographic data, cognitive screening and some exclusion scales, these participants were divided into the aMCI (n = 401) and normal cognitive groups (n = 606). Physical examinations were conducted on all participants, with clinical indicators such as blood pressure, blood sugar, and blood lipids collected. Results The aMCI group had significantly higher RC levels compared to the normal cognitive group (0.64 ± 0.431 vs. 0.52 ± 0.447 mmol/L, p < 0.05). Binary logistics regression revealed that occupation (P<0.001, OR = 0.533, 95%CI: 0.423–0.673) and RC (p = 0.014, OR = 1.477, 95% CI:1.081–2.018) were associated factors for aMCI. Partial correlation analysis, after controlling for occupation, showed a significant negative correlation between RC levels and MoCA scores (r = 0.059, p = 0.046), as well as Naming scores (r = 0.070, p = 0.026). ROC curve analysis demonstrated that RC levels had an independent predictive efficacy in predicting aMCI (AUC = 0.580, 95%CI: 0.544 ~ 0.615, P < 0.001). Conclusion Higher RC levels were identified as an independent indicator for aMCI, particularly in the naming cognitive domain among older individuals. Further longitudinal studies are necessary to validate the predictive efficacy of RC.
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Older people are at high risk of accidental injuries, sudden diseases and other adverse events. First aid skills for older people can enable them to rescue themselves in times of crisis and avoid disability and death. This study aimed to explore Chinese older people's participation intentions and needs towards first aid skills training. Methods This study used phenomenological methods in qualitative research. A total of 28 older people were interviewed from November 2021 to February 2022. The data were analyzed by using Colaizzi content analysis methods. Results Three themes were extracted from the interview data: First aid training participation intention; First aid training participation barriers; First aid training recommendations. Conclusion Older people have a strong intention to participate in first aid training. First aid training is feasible for older people, but the first aid training content and plans should be formulated according to their physical and mental characteristics, as well as their needs.
Article
Full-text available
Questions about cognitive aging are inherent questions about why individuals of different ages or from different subpopulations differ from each other and how these differences change over time. One of the main goals of cognitive gerontology is to describe how cognitive abilities change over time and to link these changes with brain aging. When we talk about the cognitive deterioration in the elderly, we have to consider great individual differences in cognitive functioning, that are greater than in the earlier period of life. In neurophysiology, significant progress has been made in mapping the brain areas responsible for changes in cognitive functioning; whether biological weakening will manifest in our behavior is greatly determined by life experiences and habits. Emotional stability, openness to experience, higher level of education, higher socioeconomic status, enjoyment in intellectual activities through the lifespan, and better physical and mental health are positively correlated with preserved cognitive abilities among older adults.
Article
Full-text available
Although physical activity has been recognized as a key determinant of successful aging, a sedentary lifestyle has been prevalent in modern societies. Sedentary behaviors are defined as any waking behavior characterized by an energy expenditure equal or greater than 1.5 metabolic equivalents (METs) while in a sitting or reclining posture, such as TV watching, reading, computer and internet use, eating, working and social chatting etc. Research evidence showed that too much time in sedentary behaviors could increase the risks of morbidity and mortality. These behaviors have been increasingly regarded as a new risk factor for chronic diseases and a significant health research agenda. Although older adults spend most of their awake time in sedentary activities (ranging in hours per day from approximately 8-12), the associations of sedentary behaviors with cognitive aging has not been comprehensively examined. The study aimed to provide a narrative review by scrutinizing the recent epidemiologic evidence for prospective cohort studies of late-life sedentary behaviors and cognitive function in older adults over 65 years old. To fulfill the objective, the following issues were raised and analyzed in an effort to disentangle the complex relationships between late-life sedentary behaviors and cognition. First, what are the effects of sedentary behaviors on various health outcomes? The findings of the recent reviews on this topic in adults (including older adults) were synthesized and compared. Among fourteen reviews, only one was specifically for older adults. Overall, greater sedentary time was observed to be closely related to increased risks of all-cause mortality, cardiovascular diseases, metabolic syndromes, overweight/obesity, diabetes, some kinds of cancer, and depression. Additionally, most studies tend to adopt questionnaires to assess sedentary behaviors. In contrast, few studies utilized objective instruments, such as accelerometers, to measure sedentary behaviors. To date, less attention, however, has been paid to assess the relation of sedentary behaviors with cognitive function among older adults. Second, the review of prospective cohort studies investigating the relationships between diverse sedentary behaviors and various cognitive outcomes among older adults was conducted. Based on the evidence of eight prospective cohort studies, it revealed that it may be not the total time spent in sedentary behavior that is important. Instead, specific sedentary behaviors are related to cognitive performance among older adults in different ways. For example, in contrast to TV viewing, computer and internet use may be beneficial for maintaining cognitive performance during the aging process. It is possible that computer use is more likely to have the potential to stimulate individuals’ cognitive functioning in an active way rather than a passive way. Third, the research issues regarding measurements of sedentary behaviors and cognitive function, research design, statistical data analysis and potential mechanisms to explain the relations of sedentary behaviors with cognition aging were identified and discussed. Finally, conclusions and suggestions for future research on this topic were proposed. Based on a nationally representative sample, a large-scale prospective cohort study with multiple waves of data collection employs a combination of subjective and objective measures of sedentary behaviors, such as a questionnaire and accelerometry, is recommended.
Article
Full-text available
Background There is a growing interest in the significance of adopting a variety of lifestyle habits for maintaining cognitive function among older adults. A lifestyle that is easy to modify, simple, and less burdensome for older people is ideal. We investigated the longitudinal association between global cognitive decline and cognitive leisure activities (CLAs) combined with long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFAs) intake. Methods The National Institute for Longevity Sciences-Longitudinal Study of Aging (NILS-LSA) enrolled community-dwelling middle-aged and older men and women who were randomly selected from Obu-City and Higashiura Town, Aichi, Japan. Baseline data (2006–2008), including CLAs and dietary intake, were obtained from 517 participants (aged 60–84 years) with normal cognition. Global cognitive decline, defined as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score ≤ 27, was assessed at baseline and four years later. Interaction between CLAs and LCPUFAs on cognitive decline was investigated using a multiple logistic analysis with adjustment for confounders. CLA engagement and LCPUFA intake were divided into high and low groups according to the frequency at which each participant engaged in the activity and the median intake level according to sex, respectively. Results A significant interaction was detected for the combination of CLA engagement and LCPUFA intake. Logistic regression coefficients revealed significant interactions when participants engaged in more than five CLA varieties. One of the CLAs, art appreciation, produced a significant main effect against cognitive decline and a significant interaction in combination with LCPUFA intake. The major LCPUFAs—docosahexaenoic acid and arachidonic acid—also exhibited a significant interaction. The combination of high LCPUFA intake and high art appreciation frequency yielded a lower adjusted odds ratio for cognitive decline than the combination of low LCPUFA and low art appreciation [0.25 (95 % confidence intervals, 0.11–0.56)]. Conclusions Preserving cognitive function might be associated with a combination of varied and high-frequency engagement in CLAs combined with high LCPUFA intake.
Article
Full-text available
p class="ResumoeAbstract">O presente artigo tem por objetivo descrever a estrutura e analisar as contribuições de um curso de formação continuada na inclusão de alunos com deficiência visual, tendo como participantes docentes e apoio técnico-pedagógico de Cristalina/GO. A cada encontro as participantes realizavam discussões e fichamentos, bem como o treinamento para produção de modelos 3D. Além do questionário na inscrição, outros 2 foram entregues, um ao final do curso, e outro após 1 ano. Como resultados, as participantes foram unânimes em afirmar que o curso foi útil e forneceu conhecimentos aplicáveis mesmo após um ano. Para atender as necessidades dos docentes é fundamental: espaço para estudo, atualização, e ensino de tecnologias inclusivas, versáteis e duráveis.</p
Article
Background: Although the risk of Alzheimer disease (AD) has been hypothesized to be lowered by frequent participation in cognitively stimulating activities, there is a lack of prospective data regarding such an association. Method: To determine whether frequent participation in cognitively stimulating activities does indeed reduce the risk for developing AD. this longitudinal cohort study assessed cognitive activity and presence of AD in 801 older Catholic nuns, priests, and brothers without dementia at enrollment who were recruited from 40 groups across the United States, At baseline assessments, which took place between January 1994 and July 2001, study subjects rated frequency of participation in common cognitive activities (e.g., reading a newspaper); a previously validated composite measure of cognitive activity frequency was derived from these ratings. Outcome measures included a clinical diagnosis of AD by a board-certified neurologist (using National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke/Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria) and change at follow-up (mean =4.5 years after baseline) in global and specific measures of cognitive function. Results: At baseline, scores on the composite measure of cognitive activity ranged from 1.57 to 4.71 (mean ±SD = 3.57 ±0.55; higher scores indicate more frequent activity). 111 individuals developed AD between baseline and follow-up. A proportional hazards model that controlled for age, sex, and education found that a 1-point increase in cognitive activity score was associated with a 33% reduction in risk of AD (hazard ratio = 0.67; 95% confidence interval = 0.49 to 0.92). Comparable results were found when individuals with memory impairment at baseline were excluded and when terms for the apolipoprotein E ε4 allele and other medical conditions were added. A 1-point increase in cognitiveactivity was associated with a smaller decline in global cognition (by 47%), working memory (by 60%), and perceptual speed (by 30%) in random-effects models that controlled for age, sex, education, and baseline level of cognitive function. Conclusion: Frequent participation in cognitively stimulating activities may reduce risk of AD,.
Article
The recent availability of longitudinal data on the possible association of different lifestyles with dementia and Alzheimer's disease (AD) allow some preliminary conclusions on this topic. This review systematically analyses the published longitudinal studies exploring the effect of social network, physical leisure, and non-physical activity on cognition and dementia and then summarises the current evidence taking into account the limitations of the studies and the biological plausibility. For all three lifestyle components (social, mental, and physical), a beneficial effect on cognition and a protective effect against dementia are suggested. The three components seem to have common pathways, rather than specific mechanisms, which might converge within three major aetiological hypotheses for dementia and AD: the cognitive reserve hypothesis, the vascular hypothesis, and the stress hypothesis. Taking into account the accumulated evidence and the biological plausibility of these hypotheses, we conclude that an active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life protects against dementia and AD. Further research is necessary to better define the mechanisms of these associations and better delineate preventive and therapeutic strategies.
Article
The objective of this study was to develop a prospectively applicable method for classifying comorbid conditions which might alter the risk of mortality for use in longitudinal studies. A weighted index that takes into account the number and the seriousness of comorbid disease was developed in a cohort of 559 medical patients. The 1-yr mortality rates for the different scores were: "0", 12% (181); "1-2", 26% (225); "3-4", 52% (71); and "greater than or equal to 5", 85% (82). The index was tested for its ability to predict risk of death from comorbid disease in the second cohort of 685 patients during a 10-yr follow-up. The percent of patients who died of comorbid disease for the different scores were: "0", 8% (588); "1", 25% (54); "2", 48% (25); "greater than or equal to 3", 59% (18). With each increased level of the comorbidity index, there were stepwise increases in the cumulative mortality attributable to comorbid disease (log rank chi 2 = 165; p less than 0.0001). In this longer follow-up, age was also a predictor of mortality (p less than 0.001). The new index performed similarly to a previous system devised by Kaplan and Feinstein. The method of classifying comorbidity provides a simple, readily applicable and valid method of estimating risk of death from comorbid disease for use in longitudinal studies. Further work in larger populations is still required to refine the approach because the number of patients with any given condition in this study was relatively small.
Article
Background Subjects with a mild cognitive impairment (MCI) have a memory impairment beyond that expected for age and education yet are not demented. These subjects are becoming the focus of many prediction studies and early intervention trials.Objective To characterize clinically subjects with MCI cross-sectionally and longitudinally.Design A prospective, longitudinal inception cohort.Setting General community clinic.Participants A sample of 76 consecutively evaluated subjects with MCI were compared with 234 healthy control subjects and 106 patients with mild Alzheimer disease (AD), all from a community setting as part of the Mayo Clinic Alzheimer's Disease Center/Alzheimer's Disease Patient Registry, Rochester, Minn.Main Outcome Measures The 3 groups of individuals were compared on demographic factors and measures of cognitive function including the Mini-Mental State Examination, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised, Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised, Dementia Rating Scale, Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test, and Auditory Verbal Learning Test. Clinical classifications of dementia and AD were determined according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Revised Third Edition and the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke–Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria, respectively.Results The primary distinction between control subjects and subjects with MCI was in the area of memory, while other cognitive functions were comparable. However, when the subjects with MCI were compared with the patients with very mild AD, memory performance was similar, but patients with AD were more impaired in other cognitive domains as well. Longitudinal performance demonstrated that the subjects with MCI declined at a rate greater than that of the controls but less rapidly than the patients with mild AD.Conclusions Patients who meet the criteria for MCI can be differentiated from healthy control subjects and those with very mild AD. They appear to constitute a clinical entity that can be characterized for treatment interventions.