ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Although health disparity research has investigated social structural, cultural, or psychological factors, the interrelations among these factors deserve greater attention. This study aims to examine cancer screening emotions and their relations to screening fatalism as determinants of breast cancer screening among women from diverse socioeconomic and ethnic backgrounds. An integrative conceptual framework was used to test the multivariate relations among socioeconomic status, age, screening fatalism, screening emotions, and clinical breast exam compliance among 281 Latino and Anglo women, using multi-group structural equation causal modeling. Screening emotions and screening fatalism had a negative, direct influence on clinical breast exam compliance for both ethnic groups. Still, ethnicity moderated the indirect effect of screening fatalism on clinical breast exam compliance through screening emotions. Integrative conceptual frameworks and multivariate methods may shed light on the complex relations among factors influencing health behaviors relevant to disparities. Future research and intervention must recognize this complexity when working with diverse populations.
Content may be subject to copyright.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Culture, Emotion, and Cancer Screening: an Integrative
Framework for Investigating Health Behavior
Patricia M. Flynn, Ph.D., M.P.H. &
Hector Betancourt, Ph.D. &Sarah R. Ormseth, M.A.
#The Society of Behavioral Medicine 2011
Abstract
Background Although health disparity research has inves-
tigated social structural, cultural, or psychological factors,
the interrelations among these factors deserve greater
attention.
Purpose This study aims to examine cancer screening
emotions and their relations to screening fatalism as
determinants of breast cancer screening among women
from diverse socioeconomic and ethnic backgrounds.
Methods An integrative conceptual framework was used to
test the multivariate relations among socioeconomic status, age,
screening fatalism, screening emotions, and clinical breast
exam compliance among 281 Latino and Anglo women, using
multi-group structural equation causal modeling.
Results Screening emotions and screening fatalism had a
negative, direct influence on clinical breast exam compli-
ance for both ethnic groups. Still, ethnicity moderated the
indirect effect of screening fatalism on clinical breast exam
compliance through screening emotions.
Conclusions Integrative conceptual frameworks and multi-
variate methods may shed light on the complex relations
among factors influencing health behaviors relevant to
disparities. Future research and intervention must recognize
this complexity when working with diverse populations.
Keywords Emotions .Culture .Fatalism .Breast cancer
screening .Health disparities
Introduction
In response to the US Preventive Services Task Force
changes to mammography screening guidelines, the
National Cancer Institute [1] recently suggested that
women should consult with their healthcare professionals
to discuss their individual benefits and risks prior to
deciding when and how often they should have mammo-
grams. These changes suggest that some patients may
choose to forgo mammography and instead elect to have a
clinical breast exam after consulting with their health
professional. Therefore, understanding the factors that
encourage or discourage women from consulting with
their healthcare professionals and having a clinical breast
exam may become more important than in the past. This
may be particularly so in the case of Latin American
(Latino)
1
women in the USA, who already report lower
ratesofscreeningcomparedtonon-LatinoWhite(Anglo)
2
women. In fact, disparities in breast cancer screening
between Latino and Anglo women have increased during
the last decade [3,4]. Latino women were also more likely
to present with breast cancer at stages III and IV [5,6],
and consistent with findings regarding socioeconomic
status (SES)-related disparities, Latinas from lower as
compared to higher income levels were more likely to be
diagnosed at later stages [7].
1
The term Latino refers to the individuals or populations of the USA
who came originally from Latin America or a region of the USA that
was once part of Latin America.
2
Anglo American refers to non-Latino White individuals or popula-
tions of the USA who came originally from the UK or other European
backgrounds, who share the English language and Anglo American
cultural heritage [2].
P. M. Flynn (*):H. Betancourt :S. R. Ormseth
Department of Psychology, Loma Linda University,
Loma Linda, CA 92354, USA
e-mail: pflynn@llu.edu
H. Betancourt
Universidad de La Frontera,
Temuco, Chile
ann. behav. med.
DOI 10.1007/s12160-011-9267-z
Understanding health behaviors such as cancer screening
within the context of a culturally diverse population
requires that the interrelations among psychological, cul-
tural, and social structural factors be considered [8]. Still,
research in this area often focuses on either social
structural, cultural, or psychological factors as determinants
of breast cancer screening. Specifically, while some studies
have focused on the role of emotions as motivators and/or
deterrents of health behavior [9,10], others have focused
on the effects of access to care [11] or cultural beliefs [3,
12]. This represents an important limitation for understand-
ing complex phenomena in this area, such as the potential
role of culture in psychological processes (e.g., emotions),
which may in turn impact health behavior. Such phenomena
may actually explain why findings from research regarding
the role of culture in cancer screening are mixed [3]. For
instance, some studies have found that fatalistic beliefs
predict lowered usage of self-breast exams, clinical breast
exams, and Pap tests among Latino women [13,14],
whereas others have not found a direct relationship between
fatalism and cancer screening [15]. Those that do not find
direct relationships between aspects of culture and health
behavior may conclude that culture does not influence
behavior. However, this view may change when research
considers the possibility that culture influences psychological
processes, such as emotions, which may in turn influence
health behavior. In fact, research suggests that variations in
emotional response among Anglo and Latino women may
account for differences in cancer screening [16].
Research has found that some ethnic minority populations
such as Latinos [3] and African Americans [17] experience
higher levels of negative cancer emotions compared to
Anglo Americans, suggesting that the experience of emo-
tions may differ as a function of ethnicity [18,19]. However,
research concerning the influence of emotions on cancer
screening often times controls for ethnicity. Such research
does not take into consideration the potential moderating role
of ethnicity on the relations between cancer emotions and
screening behavior. Furthermore, since cultural values and
beliefs have been found to influence emotions [20], it is
possible that ethnic variations in screening emotions may be
in part a function of cultural factors.
The Structure of Relations Among Social, Cultural,
and Psychological Factors Influencing Health Behavior:
an Integrative Theoretical Framework
Gallo, Smith, and Cox [8] argue that in order to better
understand the role of psychological and social determi-
nants of health behaviors, an integrative theoretical frame-
work is necessary. In fact, previous research in this area has
suggested that theoretical models and multivariate methods
are needed in order to account for the complexity of
relations among psychological, social structural, and cul-
tural determinants of health behaviors [3]. The present
research is guided by a theoretical model (see Fig. 1) for the
study of culture, psychological processes, and health
From distal... to more proximal determinants of behavior
Population Cultural Psychological Health
Categories Factors Processes Behavior
AB C D
Professionals’
Race, Ethnicity,
Gender, SES, and
Religion
---------------
Patients’
Race, Ethnicity,
Gender, SES, and
Religion
Professionals’
Socially Shared
Values, Beliefs,
and Expectations
about Patients
and Health-Care
Practices
--------------
Patients’
Socially Shared
Values, Beliefs,
and Expectations
Relevant to
Health Behaviors
and Interactions
with the Health-
Care System
Professionals’
Motivation and
Emoti ons Relevant
to Health-Care
Practices an d
Interactions with
Patients
-----------
Patients’
Motivation and
Emoti ons Relevant
to Health Behaviors
and Interactions
with the Health-
Care System
Professionals’
Health-Care Practices
and Interactions with
Patients
----------------
Patients’
Health Behaviors and
Interactions with the
Health-Care System
Fig. 1 Betancourtsmodelof
culture, psychological process-
es, and behavior adapted for the
study of health behavior [21]
ann. behav. med.
behavior that provides a theoretical framework for under-
standing ethnic and socioeconomic health disparities [21,
22]. The model can be used to better understand the health
behaviors of culturally diverse patients as well as their
health professionals. In the present study, only the aspects
of the model corresponding to the patients were examined.
An important underlying principle of the model is that
relations among the variables conceived as determinants of
health behavior are structured from most distal to more
proximal (moving from A to D), with proximity to behavior
determining a greater impact. According to the model, health
behavior (D) is a function of psychological processes such as
emotions (C), which are experienced at the individual level.
These psychological processes are the most proximal determi-
nants and therefore have the greatest influence on behavior.
Health behavior (D) is also associated with culture, which is
defined here in terms of aspects such as value orientations,
beliefs, and norms that are socially shared among individuals
from a particular population or society (B). These aspects of
culture (e.g., fatalistic beliefs) may be directly or indirectly
associated with health behavior through psychological pro-
cesses (C). Moving further away from behavior are social
categories such as race, ethnicity, age, and SES (A), which
represent sources of cultural variation. However, these social
categories are more distal determinants and may not neces-
sarily be directly associated with a particular health behavior.
A number of factors such as race/ethnicity, access to care,
education, income, age [11], and language barriers [23] have
been identified as relevant to cancer screening disparities.
According to the conceptual model represented in Fig. 1,
these social structural factors are more likely to be associated
with variations in aspects of culture rather than directly with
health behavior. In fact, research indicates that ethnicity,
income, age [24], and education [25] are all factors associated
with fatalistic beliefs about cancer and cancer prevention.
Cultural variables such as fatalism have been examined in
relation to health behavior in several studies [1315,25,26].
Although the definitions vary [27], fatalism has been generally
described as a cultural value orientation characterized by a set
of beliefs around the view that life events are inevitable and
that ones destiny is not within ones own hands [28]. Much
of the research identifies this cultural belief with ethnicity.
However, based on our definition of culture, beliefs, values,
expectations, norms, and practices are considered cultural
when they are socially shared. In fact, the category defining
the population or group could be race, ethnicity, age, gender,
or any other group for that matter. Moreover, many of these
social structural categories often times overlap with one
another (e.g., many ethnic minorities are overrepresented at
lower SES levels). Therefore, fatalistic beliefs may be to a
large extent a function of social structural barriers such as
lower education and income rather than ethnicity per se [27].
Thus, research should test the role of education, income, and
age along with ethnicity as sources of variation in fatalistic
cultural beliefs rather than as direct determinants of screening
behavior. In doing so, the relative impact of these variables as
sources of cultural variation can be delineated and their
relation to emotions and health behavior can be better
understood.
Borrayo and Guarnaccia [29] indicate that a common
problem in research methodology in this area is the failure to
examine confounding relationships between social structural
variables and cultural beliefs. In fact, variables such as SES
and ethnicity are more likely to be controlled for rather than
directly investigated to better understand their influence on
health behaviors. Tov and Diener [30] argue that since these
variables are associated with cultural beliefs, controlling for
them actually results in the removal of cultural effects. As a
result, when research controls for these social structural
variables and predicts health behaviors based on cultural
variables that had some of the cultural effects removed, results
are likely to reveal less significant findings concerning the
role of culture. Still, some studies have found significant
effects for these cultural beliefs after controlling for social
structural factors [24,25], while others have not [15,31].
These nonsignificant findings, however, may have more to
do with research that does not employ methodological and
statistical approaches that specifically investigate the inter-
relations among social structural categories, aspects of
culture, psychological processes, and health behavior.
The Present Study
The purpose of this research was to examine the interrela-
tions among screening emotions, screening fatalism, and
related social structural factors as determinants of clinical
breast exam compliance among culturally diverse women in
Southern California. To ensure that the complexity of
relations among these variables were properly understood,
multi-group structural equation modeling was employed to
test both the direct and indirect effects of the study
variables on health behavior as well as the potential
moderating role of ethnicity.
Consistent with the conceptual model guiding the research,
it was hypothesized that clinical breast exam compliance
would be a function of both screening emotions and screening
fatalism for both Latino and Anglo women. Specifically,
higher levels of screening emotions and screening fatalism
were expected to negatively impact clinical breast exam
compliance. It was also hypothesized that the effect of
screening fatalism on clinical breast exam compliance was,
at least in part indirect, through its effects on screening
emotions. Higher levels of screening fatalism were expected
to positively impact negative screening emotions, which
would in turn negatively impact clinical breast exam
ann. behav. med.
compliance. Finally, consistent with research concerning the
higher level of negative screening emotions among Latino
women [3], it was expected that the role of screening
emotions and screening fatalism as determinants of clinical
breast exam compliance would be moderated by ethnicity.
Method
Participants and Procedures
Multi-stage, stratified sampling was conducted to obtain
nearly equal proportions of Latino and Anglo women from
varying demographic characteristics in Southern California.
Using US Census tract data from the Federal Financial
Institutions Examination Council, projections regarding
ethnicity, SES, and age were anticipated for potential
recruitment sites, including churches, markets, universities,
free/low-cost health clinics, mobile home parks, and
community settings. Once permission from key personnel
at the selected sites was obtained, an English and/or
Spanish recruitment flyer was posted describing the study,
eligibility for participation, and the time and on-site
location where interested women could go to participate.
Institutional Review Board approval for the study was
granted prior to data collection. When interested women
arrived at the noted settings, bilingual research assistants
explained the purpose of the study and restated that women
were eligible to participate if they were Latino or Anglo
American, at least 20 years old, able to read English or
Spanish, and had never been diagnosed with breast cancer.
After participants provided written consent, they were
administered an English or Spanish version of the instru-
ment, which took approximately 30 to 45 min to complete.
All participants were compensated $20 for their time. Once
data were collected from a number of sites, the distributions
of participants across demographics were examined and
additional settings were identified to fulfill the particular
demographic need. As a result, 326 self-identified Latino
(n=173)orAnglo(n=153) women participated in the study.
Measures
Ethnicity
Ethnicity was self-reported by participants and included as a
moderating variable in the structural equation models (SEM).
Social Structural Sources of Cultural Variation
An existing measure used in previous research with
culturally diverse populations [3] was employed to assess
income, education, and age. Participants indicated their age
in years and annual household income based on five
categories (see Table 1). Women also indicated their
number of years of education, which was then coded into
five categories to be consistent with the income categories.
Screening Fatalism
A three-item subscale of the Cultural Cancer Screening
Scale (CCSS; [3]) was used to assess breast cancer
screening fatalism. The CCSS was developed with Latino
and Anglo women based on the bottom-up methodological
approach to the study of culture, which utilizes mixed
methodologies. The approach begins with specific obser-
vations relevant to an area of research (e.g., cancer
screening), which are derived through interviews from the
Table 1 Sample demographics based on ethnicity
Variable Latino
(n=144)
Anglo
(n=137)
M(SD) M(SD)
Education* 11.28 (4.01) 14.49 (2.75)
Age in years* 42.49 (12.03) 47.20 (15.97)
n(%) n(%)
Income*
$14,999 49 (34.03) 28 (20.44)
$1524,999 30 (20.83) 17 (12.41)
$2539,999 19 (13.19) 24 (17.52)
$4059,999 25 (17.36) 21 (15.33)
$60,000 21 (14.58) 47 (34.31)
Marital status
Single 27 (18.75) 25 (18.25)
Married 81 (56.25) 74 (54.02)
Divorced 23 (15.97) 24 (17.52)
Widowed 7 (4.86) 11 (8.03)
Not specified 6 (4.17) 3 (2.19)
Place of birth*
Mexico 64 (44.44) 0 (0.00)
Central America/Caribbean 5 (3.47) 0 (0.00)
South America 3 (2.08) 0 (0.00)
Canada 0 (0.00) 2 (1.46)
Europe 0 (0.00) 3 (2.19)
Not specified 9 (6.25) 3 (2.19)
USA 63 (43.75) 129 (94.16)
Spanish survey language* 63 (43.75) 0 (0.00)
Health insurance coverage* 107 (74.31) 117 (85.40)
Access to healthcare clinic 118 (81.94) 124 (90.51)
Ever diagnosed with cancer 8 (5.56) 16 (11.68)
Know anyone diagnosed with
breast cancer*
90 (62.50) 109 (79.56)
Family history of breast cancer* 23 (16.00) 54 (39.40)
*p<0.05
ann. behav. med.
populations of interest (e.g., Latinos and Anglos), and
evolves from these observations to the development of
quantitative instruments [3]. An advantage of this approach
is that it allows for the identification of aspects of culture
directly from individuals, rather than based on stereotypical
views. Moreover, because efforts are taken to preserve the
participantsinterview responses when developing the scale
items and because the translation process is performed by
SpanishEnglish speaking experts using the double back-
translation [32] and decentering [33] procedures, measure-
ment equivalence is more likely to be achieved.
The CCSS has demonstrated adequate reliability
(Latino α=0.84; Anglo α=0.83), measurement equiva-
lence (Tucker phi= 0.98), and predictive validity with
breast and cervical cancer screening behaviors [3]. The
three items from the screening fatalism subscale include,
It is not necessary to screen regularly for breast cancer
because everyone will eventually die of something
anyway,”“It is not necessary to screen for breast cancer
regularly because it is in Gods hand anyway,and If
nothing is physically wrong, then you do not need to
screen for breast cancer.All items were based on a seven-
point Likert scale from strongly disagreeto strongly
agree.The reliability for this subscale was good for both
ethnic groups (Latino total α=0.782; Latino Spanish α=
0.739; Latino English α=0.781; Anglo α=0.814).
Negative Screening Emotions
Findings from interviews with Latino and Anglo women
revealed that fear, anxiety, and embarrassment were the
most frequently identified emotions associated with clinical
breast exam screening [3]. Therefore, the three screening
emotions included in the questionnaire were, When I think
about having a clinical breast exam I get very scared,
Clinical breast exams are extremely embarrassing,and
Thinking about having a clinical breast exam makes me
terribly anxious.Items were placed on a seven-point Likert
scale from strongly disagreeto strongly agree.The
reliability of this scale was strong (Latino total α= 0.927;
Latino Spanish α=0.922; Latino English α=0.932; Anglo
α=0.836).
Clinical Breast Exam Compliance
According to the American Cancer Society (ACS) [34],
clinical breast exams are recommended for women in
their 2030s at least every 3 years and for women 40 and
older every year. To assess clinical breast exam compli-
ance, participants were provided with an illustration of a
woman having a clinical breast exam and a brief
description of the exam. Participants were then asked,
Have you ever had a clinical breast exam?followed by,
If yes, how many have you had in the last 5 years?
Using similar methods employed by Kundadjie-Gyamfi
and Magai [35], a screening compliance proportion was
calculated based on the total number of clinical breast
exam tests reported, divided by the maximum number that
a woman of her age should have if they were fully
compliant with screening guidelines (minimum compliance =
0; maximum compliance =1.0).
Covariates
Based on previous research [3], insurance status, knowledge
about the availability of free/low-cost healthcare, country of
birth, length of residence in the USA, language of the survey,
diagnosis of cancer other than breast cancer, acquaintance
with anyone diagnosed with breast cancer, and family
history of breast cancer were assessed.
Statistical Analyses
All hypotheses were tested using Bentlers structural
equations program (EQS 6.1; [36]) with the ML method
of estimation. In order to maintain a simplified model
without using up model degrees of freedom [37], all
relevant covariates were partitioned from the indicators of
the noted outcomes prior to SEM analyses. Due to
theoretical considerations, age, education, and income
were included in the test of models as social structural
sources of cultural variation. Adequacy of fit was
assessed using the nonsignificant χ
2
goodness-of-fit
statistic, a ratio of less than 2.0 for the χ
2
/df [38], a
Comparative Fit Index (CFI) of 0.95 or greater [36], and a
root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) of
less than 0.05 [39]. Modifications of the hypothesized
model were performed based on results from the
Lagrange multiplier (LM) test and the Wald test in
addition to theoretical considerations.
To test ethnicity-based differences in the magnitude of
the relations among the study variables, multi-group
structural equation modeling for Latino and Anglo
women was also conducted. If the constrained structural
model showed a decrement in fit based on a significant
Δχ
2
or ΔCFI of 0.01 or greater as compared to the
reference model, the LM test of equality constraints was
assessed for evidence of noninvariance [40]. Equality
constraints were considered noninvariant and released in a
sequential manner if doing so dramatically improved the
model fit (LM χ
2
5.0 per df [41]). Since it is necessary in
cross-cultural research to establish that differences observed
between groups are not due to measurement artifacts
[42], measurement equivalence was examined prior to
invariance testing. Establishing measurement equivalence
allows the researcher to more confidently assert that ethnic
ann. behav. med.
group differences are the result of the cultural factors
being tested [43].
Results
Preliminary Analyses
As a result of multi-stage stratified sampling, the sample
was well balanced between Latino and Anglo partici-
pants (n=173 and n=153, respectively). Cases with
missing values on a manifest (e.g., measured) variable or
more than half of the items on multi-item subscales were
excludedfromtheanalyses.Thereweresomedifferences
between the omitted and retained sample in regards to
education (t(317)=1.97, p=0.05) and Spanish/English
version of the survey (χ
2
(1)=14.10, p<0.001). The
retained sample reported higher levels of education (M=
12.86, SD=3.81) compared to the omitted participants (M=
11.56, SD= 4.72). Omission was also more likely among
Latinos that completed the instrument in Spanish (25.88%,
n=22) than English (9.54%, n=23). After imputing values for
26 cases using the expectationmaximization algorithm, data
from 281 (144 Latino; 137 Anglo) women were available for
analyses.
Although multi-stage stratified sampling efforts resulted
in Latino and Anglo women represented across all levels of
income, education, and age, the distribution of women
within these categories was not equal. For instance, the
Latino sample was overall younger, of lower income and
education, and more likely to be uninsured. As expected,
they were more likely to have been born outside the USA
and complete the Spanish version of the survey (see
Table 1).
Analysis of Covariates
Only one covariate was found to be statistically significant.
For Latino participants, the negative screening emotion,
When I think about having a clinical breast exam I get
very scaredwas associated with a shorter period of
residence in the USA (r=0.251, p=0.020). The variance
explained by this covariate was partialed from the indicator
prior to SEM.
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
Table 2includes the means and standard deviations for the
study variables. Approximately 58% of the total sample
(62% Anglos, 55% Latinos) was fully adherent to ACS
screening guidelines. Table 2also reports the correlations
among the study variables after adjustment of the covariate
noted above. Fischersr-to-ztest of difference revealed a
number of significantly different bivariate correlations
based on ethnicity, confirming the necessity for conducting
a test of invariance.
Structural Equation Modeling
Test of the Hypothesized Model
Prior to conducting a test of the model for the Latino and
Anglo samples independently, the data were screened
revealing a violation of multivariate normality for both
ethnic groups. Therefore, the ML robust test statistics,
which corrects for non-normal data, are reported. The
hypothesized model for Anglo women fit the data well
(CFI=0.998, χ
2
(30, n=137)=30.51, p=0.440, χ
2
/df=1.02,
RMSEA= 0.011). However, for Latinos, based on the
Lagrange test and theoretical plausibility, the disturbance
terms for screening fatalism and screening emotions were
covaried (cov=0.457, p=0.031), which resulted in an
improvement in model fit (CFI=1.00, χ
2
(29, n=144)=
25.27, p=0.664, χ
2
/df=0.87, RMSEA= 0.000). The factor
structure, including the direction and significance of factor
loadings, appeared similar for both groups. However, there
were some differences in magnitude and significance of the
associations between factors, which were further examined
in multiple group analyses (Fig. 2).
Test of Configural Invariance (Model 1)
Testing for measurement equivalence began with the
least restrictive model in which only the factor structure
of the baseline model, namely the number of factors and
the factor-loading pattern, was checked for equality
across ethnic groups. The requirement for configural
invariance suggests that the same items must be indictors
of the same factor for Latinos and Anglos, yet differ-
ences in factor loadings are permitted across groups [44].
AsshowninTable3, the fit indices revealed an excellent
fit to the data.
Test of Measurement Invariance (Table 3, Model 2)
In the second level of measurement equivalence, the factor
loadings of the baseline model were constrained to be equal
across ethnic groups, making these coefficients invariant
between Latinos and Anglos. The fit of the constrained
measurement model was also good, indicating that con-
straining factor loadings did not result in a significant
decrement in model fit. Furthermore, a review of the LM
test of equality constraints statistics showed no significant
between-group differences in the paths of the measurement
model. Because the measurement model operated similarly
for both Latinos and Anglos, any group variations observed
ann. behav. med.
Table 2 Intercorrelations, means, and standard deviations as a function of ethnicity
1 234 56 789101112
1. Education
2. Income 0.530***
(0.443***)
3. Age 0.136
(0.316***)
0.235**
(0.031)
4. Screening
fatalism
0.416***
(0.252**)
0.369***
(0.213*)
0.089
(0.302***)
5. Nothing
wrong
0.299***
(0.162*)
0.265**
(0.137)
0.064
(0.195*)
0.718***
(0.644***)
6. In Gods
hands
0.302***
(0.228**)
0.268**
(0.193*)
0.064
(0.274**)
0.727***
(0.906***)
0.522***
(0.584***)
7. Everyone
will die
0.318***
(0.192*)
0.283***
(0.162)
0.068
(0.230**)
0.765***
(0.761***)
0.550***
(0.490***)
0.557***
(0.689***)
8. Screening
emotions
0.252**
(0.031)
0.225**
(0.026)
0.054
(0.026)
0.170*
(0.122)
0.122
(0.079)
0.124
(0.111)
0.130
(0.093)
9. Embarrassment 0.216**
(0.022)
0.192*
(0.018)
0.046
(0.033)
0.145
(0.087)
0.104
(0.056)
0.105
(0.079)
0.111
(0.066)
0.851***
(0.709***)
10. Anxiety 0.248**
(0.027)
0.220**
(0.023)
0.053
(0.030)
0.166*
(0.109)
0.119
(0.070)
0.121
(0.099)
0.127
(0.083)
0.976***
(0.890***)
0.830***
(0.631***)
11. Scared 0.221**
(0.025)
0.196*
(0.021)
0.047
(0.034)
0.148
(0.099)
0.106
(0.064)
0.108
(0.090)
0.113
(0.075)
0.872***
(0.807***)
0.742***
(0.572***)
0.851***
(0.718***)
12. Clinical
breast exam
compliance
0.169*
(0.081)
0.150
(0.070)
0.036
(0.097)
0.275***
(0.322***)
0.197*
(0.208*)
0.200*
(0.292***)
0.210*
(0.245**)
0.340***
(0.178*)
0.289***
(0.126)
0.331***
(0.158)
0.296***
(0.144)
M11.27
(14.49)
2.58
(3.31)
42.49
(47.20)
2.24
(1.54)
2.46
(1.65)
2.23
(1.55)
2.04
(1.41)
3.02
(2.44)
2.98
(2.83)
3.06
(2.45)
3.03
(2.02)
0.65
(0.76)
SD 4.01
(2.75)
1.47
(1.54)
12.03
(15.97)
1.70
(1.15)
2.06
(1.38)
2.01
(1.39)
2.03
(1.28)
2.14
(1.54)
2.30
(1.94)
2.32
(1.86)
2.22
(1.50)
0.37
(0.35)
Intercorrelations, means, and standard deviations for Latino participants (n= 144) are presented in upper portion of cell, and values in parentheses represent Anglo participants (n=137). Boldface
indicates that groups differ significantly at p<0.05
*p<0.05; **p< 0.01; ***p< 0.001
ann. behav. med.
in the multi-group structural model could be interpreted as
cross-cultural differences rather than the result of measure-
ment artifacts (see [43]).
Test of Structural Invariance (Table 3, Model 3)
To test for differences in the magnitude of the paths among
the study variables across ethnicity, constraints were
imposed on all structural paths. Specifically, invariance
tests for path coefficients were used to test whether the
effect of one variable on another variable differed as a
function of ethnicity. In comparison with the configural
model (model 1, Table 3), the constrained structural model
showed a decrement in fit based on the change in CFI
greater than 0.01 (ΔCFI= 0.035). The chi-square differ-
ence test (ΔS-B χ
2
(10)=15.65, p=0.110) also revealed a
similar trend. A review of the LM test of equality
constraints statistics confirmed significant between-group
differences in the path from screening fatalism to screening
emotions (LM χ
2
(1)=6.13, p=0.013).
Test of Partial Structural Invariance (Table 3, Model 4)
After releasing the path constraint from screening fatalism to
screening emotions (Latino: β=0.61, p=0.004; Anglo: β=
0.12, p=0.311), the fit of the model improved. The fit shown
in the final test of invariance was comparable to the
configural model, indicating that no additional paths should
be released.
Test of Research Hypotheses
The proposed structure of relations among SES, age,
screening fatalism, screening emotions, and clinical breast
exam compliance explained the data well for both Latino
and Anglo women. As expected, ethnicity was found to
significantly impact the magnitudes of some structural
paths resulting in different combinations of significant
direct and indirect effects for the Latino and Anglo models.
The first hypothesis concerning the direct effects of
screening emotions and screening fatalism on clinical breast
Fig. 2 Final model with estimated path coefficients and factor loadings for Latino (Anglo) subgroups. *p< 0.05; **p< 0.01; ***p<0.001
Table 3 Model summary for tests of configural, measurement, and structural invariance across ethnicity
Model S-B
χ
2
df CFI RMSEA (90% CI) ΔS-B
χ
2a
pΔdf ΔCFI
Model 1 configural 56.28 59 1.00 0.000 (0.000, 0.034) ––
No constraints
Model 2 measurement model (factor loadings
constrained across ethnicity)
59.91 63 1.00 0.000 (0.000, 0.033) 3.68 0.451 4 0.00
Model 3 structural model (constrained factor loadings
and 6 structural paths)
72.69 69 0.965 0.014 (0.000, 0.038) 15.65 0.110 10 0.035
Model 4 structural model (constrained factor loadings
and 5 structural paths, released FatalismScreening
emotions)
67.56 68 1.00 0.000 (0.000, 0.035) 10.97 0.278 9 0.00
S-B χ
2
SatorraBentler scaled statistic, CFI robust CFI, RMSEA robust RMSEA, 90% CI 90% confidence interval
a
Corrected value
ann. behav. med.
exam compliance was confirmed for Latino women and
partially confirmed for Anglo women. For both ethnic
groups, screening fatalism exerted a direct and negative impact
on clinical breast exam compliance (Latinos: β=0.22, p=
0.019; Anglos: β=0.31, p=0.006). While the negative effect
of screening emotions on clinical breast exam compliance
was significant for Latino women (β=0.30, p<0.001), it
was not for Anglo women.
The second hypothesis concerning the indirect effect of
screening fatalism on clinical breast exam compliance
through screening emotions was also partially confirmed.
For Latino women, screening fatalism significantly influ-
enced compliance indirectly through screening emotions
(β
indirect
=0.184, p=0.015). Specifically, higher levels of
screening fatalism positively impacted screening emotions
(β=0.61, p=0.004), and as already indicated in the first
hypothesis, screening emotions in turn negatively influ-
enced clinical breast exam compliance (β=0.30, p<
0.001). For Anglo women, however, this indirect effect
was not significant.
The third hypothesis, which predicted that the influence
of screening emotions and screening fatalism on clinical
breast exam compliance would be moderated by ethnicity,
was also confirmed. The test of invariance revealed that the
influence of screening fatalism on screening emotions was
stronger for the Latino sample (β=0.61, p= 0.004; Anglos:
β=0.12, p=0.311). Regarding the influence of screening
emotions on clinical breast exam compliance, though the
effect was only significant for the Latino sample (Latinos:
β=0.30, p<0.001; Anglos: β=0.14, p=0.151), the test of
invariance did not reveal a significant difference in the
effect between Latinos and Anglos. To further examine the
moderating role of ethnicity on the indirect effect of
screening fatalism on clinical breast exam compliance
through screening emotions, MacKinnons[45] procedures
for contrasting indirect effects were employed. The indirect
effects were shown to be statistically different between
Latinos and Anglos (t=2.00, p=0.046), indicating that
ethnicity significantly moderated the indirect effect of
screening fatalism on clinical breast exam compliance
through screening emotions.
In addition to the noted hypotheses, age, education, and
income were found to be social structural sources of
variation in the cultural factor screening fatalism. However,
the relative influence of these social structural categories
varied for Latino and Anglo women. For instance, among
Latino women, lower education and income was associated
with higher levels of screening fatalism (β=0.31, p=
0.003; β=0.21, p=0.008, respectively), but age was not.
On the other hand, among Anglos, greater age and lower
income was associated with higher levels of screening
fatalism (β=0.28, p=0.010; β=0.19, p= 0.028, respectively),
but education was not found to be a strong social structural
source of cultural variation in screening fatalism. Interesting-
ly, the LaGrange test statistic, which provides recommenda-
tions for adding parameters not hypothesized in the causal
model, did not recommend that any direct paths be added
from these social structural factors in relation to clinical breast
exam compliance.
Discussion
Overall, this research reveals that psychological processes,
such as screening emotions, and cultural beliefs, such as
those related to fatalism, are factors relevant to disparities in
health behaviors such as clinical breast exam compliance.
Findings concerning the interrelations among social struc-
tural, cultural, and psychological phenomena demonstrate
the importance of models that can guide the investigation of
complex relations among multiple determinants of health
behavior. Specifically, consistent with the conceptual model
guiding the research, screening emotions and screening
fatalism were found to influence compliance for both ethnic
groups. These findings not only point to the interrelations
among cultural beliefs, emotions, and breast cancer screen-
ing, as proposed by the model, but also point to the
moderating role of ethnicity on these relations, which has
conceptual as well as practical implications.
The fact that the structure of relations specified by the
model is supported by the results strengthens its value as a
foundation for both research and theory. Still, findings
concerning the moderating role of ethnicity represent both a
contribution to a better understanding of the relations
among social structural, cultural, and psychological ante-
cedents of behavior as well as a challenge to the linear
representation of the relations specified in Fig. 1. In general
terms, this finding is not inconsistent with the conceptual
foundations of the model [21,22,46], in that ethnicity is
thought to represent more than socially shared beliefs.
Specifically, it is conceivable that aspects associated with
ethnicity, other than those relevant to the measured cultural
factor, can make a difference in how individuals of different
ethnic backgrounds behave in a particular situation, which
could explain in part a moderating effect like the one
observed. In sum, while it sheds light on the nature of the
culture-behavior link within the context of a multiethnic
society, the observed moderating effect is expected to
stimulate further conceptual work and research.
From a methodological perspective, these results con-
firm the importance of employing statistical techniques that
take into account the role of ethnicity such as multi-group
structural equation modeling. For instance, in the present
study, Latino women reported significantly higher levels of
anxiety and fear about clinical breast exam screening as
compared to Anglo women, which in turn impacted clinical
ann. behav. med.
breast exam compliance to a greater degree for the Latino
as compared to the Anglo sample. If these data were not
analyzed separately for Latino and Anglo women, but
rather controlled for ethnicity, the complexity of these
relations might not have been recognized. These results are
consistent with Consedine and associates[47] suggestion
that the lower screening behaviors of African American
women may be more influenced by fear, due to finding that
this population is more afraid of cancer [48]. In fact, similar
results have been reported for Latino and Anglo women
regarding the influence of emotions on health behavior as a
result of perceptions of healthcare mistreatment [16].
Concerning the proposed influence of socially shared
fatalistic beliefs on screening emotions and clinical breast
exam compliance, the role of ethnicity is interesting,
particularly when considering the indirect influence of
those cultural beliefs. Consistent with the model for the
study of culture, screening fatalism was strongly and
directly related to clinical breast exam compliance for both
Latino and Anglo women. For Latinas, screening fatalism
also exerted a significant indirect effect through screening
emotions. These findings suggest that for Latino women,
both screening fatalism as well as screening emotions are
important determinants of cancer screening. However, for
Anglo women, screening fatalism mainly exerted a direct
effect on clinical breast exam compliance as the indirect
effect through emotions was not significant. Future research
should perhaps examine the role of screening fatalism in
relation to cognitive processes, such as perception of
control and causal attributions. It is possible that among
Anglo women, these cognitive variables may be more
directly influenced by culture than are emotions.
In addition to supporting the hypothesized structure of
relations among cultural and psychological variables asso-
ciated with clinical breast exam compliance, results also
shed light on the role of SES and age as social structural
sources of variation in cultural beliefs. While a significant
amount of research in the health disparities literature has
examined factors such as ethnicity, SES, age, and access to
care, most research in this area typically controls for such
social structural variables or test their direct effects on
health behavior. Consistent with the model, these social
structural factors are postulated to be sources of cultural
variation and are believed to influence health behavior
indirectly through psychological processes. Although in
some cases income and access to care may be directly
related to health behavior due to the instrumental impor-
tance of resources, this direct relation may not have been
observed because California provides free breast and
cervical cancer screening exams to underserved women.
In fact, the present research revealed that age, education,
and income did influence clinical breast exam compliance
but only through their effects on screening fatalism. Similar
results have been reported by Murguia and Zea [49]
revealing that Latino cultural health beliefs were better
predictors of healthcare utilization as compared to SES and
acculturation.
Despite the significance of the study findings, some
limitations of the research should be considered. For
instance, the Latino sample of this study reflected the
demographic reality of Southern California, which is
predominantly of a Mexican cultural background. There-
fore, it is unclear whether the results would be the same
with Latinos from regions of the country that represent
other national origins. The sample also included both
immigrant and US born Latinos, which was expected to
contribute to more variance in the cultural factor. Future
research could include samples of Latinos that would make
it possible to examine generation status and other potential
sources of cultural variation, which may provide more
targeted information. In addition, while the theoretical
model on which the hypothesized relations were based
provided a meaningful conceptual foundation for SEM
models, the cross-sectional design of this study limits the
test of temporal relations. Future work could employ
longitudinal data in order to examine such relations in a
more definitive manner.
The findings of the research have important implications
for interventions with culturally diverse patients. First,
results concerning the indirect effects of culture on clinical
breast exam compliance for the two ethnic groups suggest
that efforts designed to improve clinical breast exam
compliance among culturally diverse populations may have
to emphasize different variables highlighted in the model.
For instance, on the one hand, in order to be effective with
Anglo patients, health professionals may have to recognize
cultural beliefs. On the other hand, when working with
Latino patients, health professionals may be better off
paying attention to not only their cultural beliefs but also
emotions such as those related to the screening process. In
fact, research with women in Korea has indicated that
interventions that focus on both cognitions and emotions,
such as shame, embarrassment, and worry related to Pap
smear screening, are particularly successful [50]. All things
considered, intervention efforts that address both screening
fatalism as well as emotions may prove effective at
enhancing screening and reducing the noted health disparities
between Latino and Anglo women.
Acknowledgments This research was supported by a NIH grant
1R21CA101867-01A2 to H. Betancourt, PI, through the National
Cancer Institute and the Office of Research on Womens Health and
by grant PFT-08-014-01-CPPB to P. Flynn, PI, through the American
Cancer Society.
Conflict of Interest The authors have no conflict of interest to
disclose.
ann. behav. med.
References
1. National Cancer Institute. NCI statement on breast cancer
screening. Available at http://www.cancer.gov/newscenter/
pressreleases/BreastScreen2009. Accessibility verified Decem-
ber 16, 2009.
2. Betancourt H, Fuentes JL. Culture and Latino issues in health
psychology. In: Kazarian S, Evans D (eds). Handbook of Cultural
Health Psychology. San Diego: Academic, 2001, 205221.
3. Betancourt H, Flynn PM, Riggs M, Garberoglio C. A cultural
research approach to instrument development: The case of breast
and cervical cancer screening among Latino and Anglo women.
Health Educ Res. 2010; 25:9911007.
4. Ries LA, Melbert D, Krapcho M, et al. SEER cancer statistics
review, 19752005. Bethesda: National Cancer Institute, 2008.
5. Li IC, Malone EK, Dailing RJ. Differences in breast cancer stage,
treatment, and survival by race and ethnicity. Arch Intern Med.
2003; 163: 4956.
6. Biffl WL, Myers A, Franciose RJ, Gonzalez RJ, Darnell D. Is
breast cancer in young Latinas a different disease? Am J Surg.
2001; 182: 596600.
7. Howe HL, Wu X, Ries LAG, et al. Annual report to the nation on
the status of cancer, 19752003, featuring cancer among U.S.
Hispanic/Latino populations. Cancer. 2006; 107: 17111742.
8. Gallo LC, Smith TW, Cox CM. Socioeconomic status, psychoso-
cial processes, and perceived health: An interpersonal perspective.
Ann Behav Med 2006; 31: 109119.
9. Diefenbach MA, Miller SM, Daly MB. Specific worry about
breast cancer predicts mammography use in women at risk for
breast and ovarian cancer. Health Psychol. 1999; 18: 532536.
10. Lindberg NM, Wellisch D. Anxiety and compliance among
women at risk for breast cancer. Ann Behav Med. 2001; 23:
298303.
11. Strzelczky J, Dignan M. Disparities in adherence to recommended
follow-up on screening mammography: Interaction of sociodemo-
graphic factors. Ethnic Dis. 2002; 12: 7786.
12. Borrayo EA, Gonzalez P, Swaim R, Marcus AL, Flores E,
Espinoza P. The Latina Breast Cancer Screening Scale: Beliefs
about breast cancer and breast cancer screening. J Health Psychol.
2009; 14: 944955.
13. Suarez L, Roche L, Nichols D, Simpson D. Knowledge, behavior,
and fears concerning breast and cervical cancer among older low-
income Mexican-American women. Am J Prev Med. 1997; 13:
137424.
14. Lobell M, Bay RC, Rhoads KVL, Keske B. Barriers to cancer
screening in Mexican-American women. Mayo Clin Proc. 1998;
73: 301308.
15. Dettenborn L, DuHamel K, Butts G, Thompson H, Jandorf L.
Cancer fatalism and its demographic correlates among African
American and Hispanic women: Effects on adherence to cancer
screening. J Psychosoc Oncol. 2004; 22: 4760.
16. Betancourt H, Flynn PM, Ormseth SR. Healthcare mistreatment
and continuity of cancer screening among Latino and Anglo
American women in Southern California. Women & Health. 2011;
51(1): 124.
17. Friedman LC, Webb JA, Weinberg AD, Lane M, Cooper HP,
Woodruff A. Breast cancer screening: Racial/ethnic differences in
behaviors and beliefs. J Cancer Educ. 1995; 10: 213216.
18. Consedine NS, Magai C. The uncharted waters of emotion:
Ethnicity, trait emotion and emotion expression in older adults.
Cross-Cult Geront. 2002; 17: 71100.
19. Matsumoto D. Ethnic differences in affect intensity, emotion
judgments, display rule attitudes, and self-reported emotional
expression in an American sample. Motiv Emotion. 1993; 17:
107123.
20. Wong S, Bond MH, Mosquera PM. The influence of cultural
value orientations on self-reported emotional expression across
cultures. Cross-Cult Psychol. 2008; 39: 224229.
21. Betancourt H, Flynn PM. The psychology of health: Physical
health and the role of culture in behavior. In: Villarruel FA, Carlo
G, Contreras Grau JM et al (eds). Handbook of US Latino
psychology. Thousand Oaks: Sage, 2009, 34761.
22. Betancourt H, Lopez SR. The study of culture, ethnicity, and race
in American psychology. Am Psychol 1993; 48: 62937.
23. Jacobs EA, Karavolos K, Rathouz PJ, Ferris TG, Powell LH.
Limited English proficiency and breast and cervical cancer
screening in a multiethnic population. Am J Public Health.
2005; 95: 14101416.
24. Powe BD. Cancer fatalism among elderly African American
women: Predictors of the intensity of the perceptions. J Psychosoc
Oncol. 2001; 19: 8595.
25. Niederdeppe J, Levy AG. Fatalistic beliefs about cancer preven-
tion and three prevention behaviors. Cancer Epidem Biomar.
2007; 16: 9981003.
26. Chavez L, Hubbell F, Mishra S, Valdez R. The influence of
fatalism on self-reported use of Papanicolaou smears. Am J Prev
Med. 1997; 31: 418424.
27. Abraido-Lanza AF, Viladrich A, Florez KR, Cespedes A, Aguirre
AN, De La Cruz AA. Commentary: Fatalismo reconsidered: A
cautionary note for health-related research and practice with
Latino populations. Ethnic Dis. 2007; 17: 153158.
28. Perez-Stable E, Sabogal F, Otero-Sabogal R, Hiatt R, McPhee S.
Misconceptions about cancer among Latino and Anglos. J Amer
Med Assoc. 1992; 268: 32193223.
29. Borrayo EA, Guarnaccia CA. Differences in Mexican-born and U.S.
born women of Mexican descent regarding factors related to breast
cancer screening behaviors. Health Care Women I. 2000; 21: 599613.
30. Tov W, Diener E. Culture and subjective well-being. In Kitayama
S, Cohen D, Handbook of cultural psychology. New York:
Guilford; 2007: 691713.
31. Mayo RM, Ureda JR, Parker VG. Importance of fatalism in
understanding mammography in rural elderly women. J Women
Aging. 2001; 13: 5772.
32. Knight GP, Roosa MW, Umana-Taylor AJ. Studying ethnic
minority and economically disadvantaged populations. Washing-
ton, DC: American Psychological Association, 2009.
33. Prieto AJ. A method for translation of instruments to other
languages. Adult Educ Quart. 1992; 43: 114.
34. Smith RA, Cokkinides V, Eyre HJ. American Cancer Society
guidelines for the early detection of cancer, 2006. CA Cancer J
Clin. 2006; 56: 1125.
35. Kudadjie-Gyamfi E, Magai C. The influence of coping styles on
mammography screening in a multiethnic sample. Cult Diver and
Ethn Min. 2008; 14: 183192.
36. Bentler PM. EQS 6 structural equations program. Encino:
Multivariate Software; 2005.
37. Kammeyer-Mueller JD, Wanberg CR. Unwrapping the organiza-
tional process: Disentangling multiple antecedents and their
pathways to adjustment. J Appl Psychol. 2003; 88: 779794.
38. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS: Using multivariate statistics. New
York: Harper Collins, 2001.
39. Browne M, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In
K. A. Bollen and J. S. Long (eds), Testing structural equation
models. Thousand Oaks: Sage, 1993.
40. Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes
for testing measurement invariance. Struct Equ Modeling. 2002;
9: 233255.
41. Scott-Lennox JA, Lennox RD. Sex-race differences in social
support and depression among older low-income adults. In Hoyle
RH eds. Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and
applications. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 1995: 199216.
ann. behav. med.
42. van de Vijver FJR, Leung K. Methods and data analysis for cross-
cultural research. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 1997.
43. Chen FF. What happens if we compare chopsticks with forks? The
impact of inappropriate comparisons in cross-cultural research. J
Pers Soc Psycho. 2008; 95:10051018.
44. Byrne BM. Structural equation modeling with EQS. Mahwah:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2006.
45. Mackinnon DP. Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. New
York: Taylor and Francis; 2008.
46. Betancourt H, Hardin C, Manzi J. Beliefs, value orientation, and
culture in attribution processes and healing behavior. J Cross
Cultur Psychol 1992; 23:179195
47. Consedine NS, Magai C, Neugut AI. The contribution of
emotional characteristics to breast cancer screening among
women from six ethnic groups. Prev Med. 2004; 38: 6477.
48. Miller LY, Hailey BJ. Cancer anxiety and breast cancer screening
in African-American women: A preliminary study. Womens
Health Iss. 1994; 4: 170174.
49. Murguia A, Zea MC. The development of the Cultural Health
Attributions Questionnaire (CHAQ). Cult Diver and Ethn Min.
2000; 6: 268283.
50. Park S, Chang S, Chung C. Effects of a cognition-emotion
focused program to increase public participation in Papanicolaou
smear screening. Public Health Nurs. 2005; 22: 289298.
ann. behav. med.
... Several health models in social and health psychology have studied and identified some critical variables associated with the adoption of different health behaviors, such as alcohol consumption, drug and condom use, and vaccination, among others (Conner & Sparks, 2005). In the current study, we are interested in testing Betancourt's integrative model for studying health behaviors (Betancourt et al., 2010;Flynn et al., 2011). This theoretical model suggests that health behaviors are influenced by variables of a different nature, differentiating between population categories (sociodemographic variables), cultural variables, and psychological processes. ...
... This sequence could imply that risk perception could mediate the relationship between injunctive social norms and protective behaviors. Furthermore, according to Betancourt's model (Betancourt & Flynn, 2009;Flynn et al., 2011), an indirect effect through the perception of risk and severity on the association between injunctive norms and protective behaviors should be expected. This mechanism would allow us to hypothesize that the concern and pressure of significant others to perform protective behaviors (i.e., social norms) could be explained by the increased perception of the risk and severity of COVID-19 (Savadori & Lauriola, 2021). ...
... Our results highlight that the integrated model of health developed by Betancourt and Flynn (Betancourt et al., 2010;Flynn et al., 2011) provides an appropriate theoretical framework that allows the understanding of sociodemographic, cultural, and psychological variables in protective health behaviors in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Moreover, the model was invariant, or equivalent, in the countries assessed in our study: Chile, Mexico, and Colombia. ...
Article
Full-text available
The literature has suggested that social norms and perceptions of risk and severity are critical variables in predicting protective health behaviors. Using an integrative conceptual framework, this study evaluates educational level, socioeconomic status, injunctive pro-care norms, perception of risk, and severity of COVID-19 as predictors of hygiene behaviors, mask use, and physical distancing. Participants included 2,075 adults from Chile, Mexico, and Colombia, three countries with the worst pandemic management. A structural equation model including the hypothesized structure of relations among the study variables fitted the data well and was invariant among Chilean, Mexican, and Colombian data. As proposed, injunctive pro-care norms against COVID-19 predicted protective health behaviors directly and indirectly through the perception of the risk and severity of COVID-19, and educational level predicted pro-care norms against COVID-19. These findings are essential in light of the development of prevention campaigns and the promotion of COVID-19 care strategies.
... The bottom-up cultural research approach [23] is a methodology that has reliably identified socially shared (i.e. cultural) beliefs in a particular population or cultural group (see [20,24,25]). As previously discussed, prior research employing this cultural research approach identified beliefs about diabetes-related social exclusion that were socially shared (e.g. ...
... Experts argue that in order to better understand the role of social and psychological determinants of health behaviors, integrative theoretical frameworks are needed [34]. As such, the use of integrative models and multivariate statistical approaches could significantly advance knowledge in this area and lead to the development of more effective, culturally responsive interventions [24,34]. ...
... Betancourt's Integrative Model of Culture, Psychological Processes, and Health Behavior [23,24,35] is one such model that specifies the structure of relations between social structural, cultural, and psychological factors as determinants of health behaviors such as those involved in the management of T2DM (see Fig. 1). According to the Integrative Model (see Fig. 1), aspects of culture (B), such as the socially shared beliefs, values, norms, and practices relevant to T2DM, can directly impact health behavior (D), including self-care behaviors involved in the management of T2DM (e.g., adhering to the diabetes diet, exercise, and medication regimens). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM) disproportionally impacts Latin Americans (Latinos) in the U.S. compared to non-Latino Whites, as reflected by an increased risk for disease complications and higher mortality rates. Guided by an Integrative Model of Culture, Psychological Processes, and Health Behavior, the purpose of the present study was to examine the role of cultural beliefs and diabetes distress as determinants of self-care behaviors and HbA1c among Latino patients with T2DM. Methods Participants included 109 Latino patients with T2DM recruited from a diabetes treatment center located in a region of Southern California with high diabetes mortality rates. Structural equation modeling was employed to examine the extent to which cultural beliefs about diabetes-related social exclusion and diabetes distress impact self-care behaviors and self-reported HbA1c. Results Consistent with the study hypotheses, cultural beliefs about diabetes-related social exclusion predicted diabetes distress, which in turn predicted poor diabetes self-care. Conclusions Findings suggest an important need for intervention efforts that address both cultural and psychological factors in order to improve diabetes self-care behaviors and associated disease outcomes among Latino patients with T2DM. Future research could benefit from investigating protective aspects of culture that could help counter the negative implications of cultural beliefs about social exclusion and diabetes distress associated with poor self-care.
... Thus, those who are discriminated against (i.e., immigrants) experience negative emotions, such as anger, sadness, uncertainty, and frustration, more frequently [40][41][42][43]. For their part, these negative emotions have been connected to various adverse mental health outcomes such as psychosomatic illnesses, distress, depression, anxiety, and stress [44][45][46][47][48][49]. On top of more frequently experiencing negative emotions associated with discrimination, immigrants must adjust the expression and modulation of these emotions to a cultural context different from that of their country of origin. ...
... As for experiencing negative emotions, research on the indigenous population in southern Chile (i.e., the Mapuche people) has associated this type of emotion with adverse physical health outcomes [33,[44][45][46][47]51,53,54]. Although this relationship has not been studied in other types of minority populations, such as migrants, descriptive evidence points to a high prevalence of negative emotions, such as anger, rage, and sadness, in the migrant population of southern Chile. ...
... Although this relationship has not been studied in other types of minority populations, such as migrants, descriptive evidence points to a high prevalence of negative emotions, such as anger, rage, and sadness, in the migrant population of southern Chile. This indicates that negative emotions could explain anxiety (i.e., as a mechanism), depression, and stress-related symptomatology [13,45,47,53,54]. Consequently, this study proposes a possible mediating role of emotions in the association between acculturative stress and mental health symptoms. ...
Article
Full-text available
The role of perceived social support in the acculturation process of immigrants remains unclear. In this study, we jointly evaluated the associations between acculturative stress and negative emotions associated with discrimination as antecedents of anxiety, depression, and stress symptoms in 283 immigrants living in Chile. Three competing models were tested via structural equation modelling to assess (1) the association among these variables and mental health symptoms and (2) to clarify the role of perceived social support. The third model was theoretically more adequate, showed a better fit, and explained 42.7% of the variance of mental health symptoms. In this model, perceived social support was associated with acculturative stress by reducing mental health symptomatology. Moreover, a direct relationship and an indirect relationship were found between acculturative stress (through negative emotions associated with discrimination) and mental health symptomatology. These results contribute to the understanding of the acculturation process experienced by immigrants in Chile and provide empirical evidence to be used to improve migration policies.
... This is supported by the finding that the Value and Emotional consequences variables predicted both past attendance as well as intention to participate in the future. This is in alignment with existing evidence that shows that anxiety about the screening, fear of pain, and embarrassment are well-recognised barriers to screening attendance (Acharya et al., 2021;Aro et al., 2001;Bolarinwa & Holt, 2023;Consedine et al., 2004;Crump et al., 2000;Fayanju et al., 2014;Feldstein et al., 2011;Flynn et al., 2011;Jaafar Sidek et al., 2023;Lee & Schwartz, 2019;Trigoni et al., 2008;Whelehan et al., 2013). A high score in Beliefs about capability predicted both previous non-regular attendance in the past and not intending to attend in the future. ...
... Further work has explained these differences in the behaviour of people from different SES groups through a psychosocial model that suggests that low SES groups experience higher levels of life-stress and lower levels of social support (Wardle et al., 2000). However, research has also highlighted the importance of physical, cognitive, and emotional factors, such as the psychological domains examined in this survey, as mediators to preventative health behaviours (Acharya et al., 2021;Crump et al., 2000;Fawns-Ritchie et al., 2022;Feig et al., 2022;Flynn et al., 2011;Glanz et al., 1992;Marvan et al., 2013;Prowse et al., 2024;Waller et al., 2009). Further evidence also suggests that such factors vary significantly by SES (Creavin et al., 2023;Wardle et al., 2004;Wardle & Steptoe, 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: In the UK, breast cancer affects 1 in 8 women, accounting for 31% of cancers and 15% of cancer-related deaths in women. In 2023, London’s breast screening coverage was 56%, falling below the NHS target of 70%. This survey assesses psychological and behavioural factors impacting screening attendance. Methods: Using the Theoretical Domains Framework, an online survey including 15 behavioural factors was distributed through a market research company to women eligible for breast screening (aged 47–73) across London and Southeast and West England. Logistic regression was used to predict the impact of behavioural variables on history of attendance and intention to attend future screening opportunities. Results: Of the 922 respondents who returned the full survey, 88.6% intended to attend future screenings, and 88.1% reported previously attending screenings regularly. ‘Behavioural regulation’ had the strongest influence on past attendance (OR = 1.92, p < 0.001) and future intent (OR = 1.56, p = 0.003). Not intending to attend was linked to emotional consequences (OR = 0.68, p = 0.003) and environmental barriers (OR = 0.66, p < 0.001), where OR—Odds Ratio. Conclusions: This survey identifies behavioural factors influencing breast screening participation and screening intention, providing insights that may help design interventions to increase attendance rates.
... The embarrassment associated with this could cause difficulty when talking about these examinations with a physician and with relatives and friends. Some authors (Theisen, 2004;Betancourt et al., 2010;Flynn et al., 2011) showed that shame, fear and anxiety were the most frequent emotions accompanying cervical cancer and breast cancer screening tests. ...
... Flynn et al. (Flynn et al., 2011) showed that screening emotions and screening fatalism had a negative influence on clinical breast exam compliance. A quarter of the centers analyzed in this study considered that people do not take part in screening because they believe that cancer is incurable. ...
Article
Full-text available
The level of participation in cancer screening is low in the Polish population. The aim of this study was to assess the opinions of centers providing cancer screening as to the reasons for the low frequency of cancer screening in Poland and possible methods to increase participation. In July 2020 433 centers in Poland carried out breast and/or cervical cancer screening. Of these, 136 centers decided to participate in the study. The study was conducted using an original questionnaire. The questions were addressed to opinion of centers about: reasons for the low frequency of cancer screening in Poland, methods to increase the frequency of cancer screening, pricing and motivating factors for providing cancer screening. Among opinions as to possible reasons for the low frequency of cancer screening in Poland related to the care-system, lack of encouragement from general practitioners, lack of invitations for cancer screening and lack of proper social advertising were most prevalent; whereas among reasons related to patients, a low awareness of cancer screening and fear of cancer diagnosis. The main methods that could potentially increase screening participation are considered to be the inclusion of cancer screening in mandatory periodic employee examinations, more activity by general practitioners, better promotion of screening by central institutions, and sending personal invitations. In conclude some interventions should be carried out to motivate people to break down barriers.
... Es por esta razón que los hallazgos del presente estudio son tan relevantes, puesto que entregan evidencia empírica respecto del rol que desempeñan variables posibles de intervenir, como la percepción de riesgo y las normas procuidado contra la COVID-19 en la adopción individual de conductas de autocuidado en la población nacional (Gerber et al., 2021;Gonzalez et al., 2021;Salazar-Fenández et al., en prensa). Específicamente, en la literatura basada en el modelo de creencias en salud (Becker, 1974;Rosenstock, 1974), en la teoría de la conducta planificada y razonada (Conner & Sparks, 2005) y el modelo integrador de Betancourt (Betancourt et al., 2010;Flynn et al., 2011) se señalan lineamientos para abordar el aumento de la percepción de riesgo de la COVID-19 y promover las normas procuidado a partir de estrategias e intervenciones (Albery & Munafò, 2008;Baeza-Rivera et al., 2021a;Baron & Byrne, 2005;Cialdini & Trost, 1998;Conner & Norman, 1996Cori et al., 2020;Joffe, 2003;Kasperson et al., 1988;Loewenstein et al., 2001;Roos et al., 2015;Slovic, 2010;Tyler, 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
La pandemia por la COVID-19 se ha transformado en la principal crisis epidemiológica del siglo, por lo que debieron adoptarse diversas conductas de autocuidado. La eficacia de estas estrategias preventivas depende de la adherencia que mantenga la población. El objetivo de este estudio fue establecer los predictores de la adherencia a las conductas de autocuidado para la prevención del contagio de la COVID-19 en población chilena mayor a 18 años. Una muestra no probabilística de 1.033 personas respondió un cuestionario virtual, bajo un diseño transversal. Se emplearon escalas de autoreporte sobre percepción de riesgo, normas procuidado y conductas de autocuidado contra la COVID-19. Utilizando regresiones múltiples jerárquicas, se encontró que el género, percepción de riesgo y normas procuidado contra la COVID-19 predijeron significativa y directamente el uso de mascarilla, conductas de higiene y distancia social, mientras que la edad solo predijo mayor adhesión a conductas de higiene y el estatus socioeconómico solo predijo el uso de mascarilla y distancia social. En todas las conductas de autocuidado la interacción entre percepción de riesgo y normas procuidado sugiere que a niveles bajos de norma procuidado, la percepción de riesgo potencia la adopción de estas conductas. A partir de estos hallazgos, se sugiere elaborar estrategias que promuevan conductas de autocuidado con base en la percepción de riesgo, considerando variables como el género, edad y estatus socioeconómico, a través de la creación y mantención de normas procuidado que consideren las relaciones entre el individuo y otros significativos en la percepción de la valoración positiva de sus conductas en salud.
... Nevertheless, the high incidence of breast cancer in Argentina [6], also highlights the impor-tance to better understand the determinants involved in health behavior. It appears that social, cultural, including fatalism, as well as psychological factors have relevance on mammographic screening [7,8]. The goal of this research was to analyze these aspects in advantaged and disadvantaged women in order to clarify their influence on breast cancer screening behaviors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Citation: Luciano Cermignani, Martin E. Rabassa, María Virginia Croce. The relevance of cultural, fatalistic and psychosocial factors in breast cancer screening behavior in middle age and older women. Abstract The aim of this study was to analyze cultural and social factors in advantaged and disadvantaged women in relation to mammographic screening. A cross-sectional study was performed; 850 women aged 45-79 years were interviewed; based on socioeconomic aspects, women were grouped in low economic power (Low group, LG) (379 women) and middle economic power (MG) (471). A questionnaire previously validated was employed and information about cultural, fatalistic, and psychosocial factors as well as breast cancer and mammographic screening was assessed. An extensive statistical analysis was performed including three regression models and a principal component analysis. 98% MG and 49.7% LG had a high level of education. Women who stated having a high level of education, regularly visit a doctor and being communicatively open showed the most positive mammographic screening behaviors. In general, analysis of fatalistic affirmatives in relation to mammogram variables did not show a significant difference in relation to total MG and LG while psychosocial variables showed a very low significant relationship with mammographic screening. Regression analysis showed similar results. This study highlights that communication as well as family and social support constitute important factors which impact on mammographic screening, while fatalism, although present, should not constitute a crucial aspect.
... As illustrative applications of the GBCatL, we employed two data sets encompassing different types of decisions concerning two cancers: colorectal cancer (CRC) screening and prostate cancer (PCa) treatment. The rationale for using these DCE data sets for testing GBCatL is that in daily practice patients receive on the one hand concrete information (numbers, words, pictures) about the different CRC screening and PCa treatment options available (which are mimicked by the DCE tasks), while on the other hand the decision itself is subject to the impacts of information filtered through emotion, culture, knowledge/experience, etc. (see Denberg et al. 2006, Flynn et al. 2011, Clarke et al. 2021). In addition, focusing on the decision of CRC screening and PCa treatment involves different diseases and consequences, which is important for the generalizability of our study findings. ...
Article
The rational microeconomic decision model is hard-coded into usual econometric specifications such as the Multinomial Logit and Probit models, inter alia. There is a very tight link between utility maximization and the apparatus of welfare theory that underlies economic policy analysis, which creates a tension around the possibility of representing other decision rules. We propose a less restrictive model of choice, built on the concept of gist-based categorization judgments that are assumed to precede (thus, condition) the maximization-driven selection process in decision making. This categorization facilitates decision making by allowing adoption of certain simpler decision rules under appropriate conditions, the drivers of which are endogenously determined. We demonstrate that the proposed model provides better fit than traditional choice models, using cancer screening and treatment choice data from two discrete choice experiments. In addition, we show that the model provides a deeper, more nuanced and insightful perspective on (healthcare) decision making.
Article
Objectives: To synthesize current evidence on the association between anticipatory anxiety, defined as apprehension-specific negative affect that may be experienced when exposed to potential threat or uncertainty, and cancer screening to better inform strategies to maximize participation rates. Methods: Searches related to cancer screening and anxiety were conducted in seven electronic databases (APA PsycINFO, Scopus, Web of Science, Embase, Cochrane Library, PubMed, CINAHL), with potentially eligible papers screened in Covidence. Data extraction was conducted independently by multiple authors. Barriers to cancer screening for any type of cancer and relationships tested between anticipatory anxiety and cancer screening and intention were categorized and compared according to the form and target of anxiety and cancer types. Results: A total of 74 articles (nparticipants = 119,990) were included, reporting 103 relationships tested between anticipatory anxiety and cancer screening and 13 in- stances where anticipatory anxiety was reported as a barrier to screening. Antici- patory anxiety related to a possible cancer diagnosis was often associated with increased screening, while general anxiety showed no consistent relationship. Negative relationships were often found between anxiety about the screening procedure and cancer screening. Conclusion: Anticipatory anxiety about a cancer diagnosis may promote screening participation, whereas a fear of the screening procedure could be a barrier. Public health messaging and primary prevention practitioners should acknowledge the appropriate risk of cancer, while engendering screening confidence and highlighting the safety and comfort of screening tests.
Article
Background: In the United States, black and Hispanic white women with breast cancer present with more advanced stages and have poorer survival rates than non-Hispanic whites, whereas Asians and Pacific Islanders do not. However, Asians and Pacific Islanders and Hispanic whites are heterogeneous populations, and few studies have evaluated breast cancer stage, treatments, and mortality rates for subgroups of these populations. Methods: Using data from 11 population-based tumor registries that participate in the Surveillance, Epidemiology, and End Results Program, we conducted a retrospective cohort study to evaluate the relationship between race and ethnicity and breast cancer stage, treatments, and mortality rates. The cohort of 124,934 women diagnosed as having a first primary invasive breast carcinoma between January 1, 1992, and December 31, 1998, included 97,999 non-Hispanic whites, 10,560 blacks, 322 American Indians, 8834 Asians and Pacific Islanders, and 7219 Hispanic whites. Results: Relative to non-Hispanic whites, blacks, American Indians, Hawaiians, Indians and Pakistanis, Mexicans, South and Central Americans, and Puerto Ricans had 1.4- to 3.6-fold greater risks of presenting with stage IV breast cancer. Blacks, Mexicans, and Puerto Ricans were 20% to 50% more likely to receive or elect a first course of surgical and radiation treatment not meeting the 2000 National Comprehensive Cancer Network standards. In addition, blacks, American Indians, Hawaiians, Vietnamese, Mexicans, South and Central Americans, and Puerto Ricans had 20% to 200% greater risks of mortality after a breast cancer diagnosis. Conclusions: Differences in breast cancer stage, treatments, and mortality rates are present by race and ethnicity. Breast cancer survival may be improved by targeting factors, particularly socioeconomic factors, that underlie these differences.
Article
Each January, the American Cancer Society (ACS) publishes a summary of its recommendations for early cancer detection, including guideline updates, emerging issues that are relevant to screening for cancer, and a summary of the most current data on cancer screening rates for US adults. In 2004, there were no updates to ACS guidelines. In this article, we summarize the current guidelines, discuss recent evidence and policy changes that have implications for cancer screening, and provide an update of the most recent data pertaining to participation rates in cancer screening by age, gender, and insurance status from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention's Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System.
The purpose of this study was to develop a reliable and valid measure of Latino health beliefs, the Cultural Health Attributions Questionnaire (CHAQ). 340 Ss (aged 19-82 yrs) were recruited in 2 metropolitan areas with large Latino populations. In Exp1, all Ss completed the CHAQ. In Exp 2, 100 of the Ss were given additional questionnaires (health locus of control, acculturation, barriers to health care utilization) to further assess construct validity and provide initial criterion validity. A Exploratory factor analysis revealed 2 highly interpretable 12-item subscales: the Equity Attribution and the Behavioral–Environmental Attribution scales. Examination of the relationships between the subscales and measures of acculturation provided evidence of construct validity. Moreover, the prediction of health care behavior by the CHAQ also indicated initial criterion validity. English and Spanish versions of the CHAQ are appended.
Chapter
This chapter describes some of the general aspects concerning the study of culture in mainstream psychology that are relevant to the study and practice of health psychology with nondominant ethnic groups in a multicultural society. The chapter discusses the issues relevant to study and practice of health psychology of Latinos: intragroup diversity in cultural background, socioeconomic status, and levels of acculturation. In order to address health concerns of Latinos, it is important to consider the potential changes that Latinos experience. Health data suggests that across most health indicators, low socioeconomic status is associated with higher health risks. The final part of the chapter focuses on specific health psychology issues that concern the various Latino groups: coronary heart disease, HIV, cervical cancer, diabetes, and chronic pain management.