ArticlePDF Available

Application of gnawing sticks in rabbit housing

Authors:
  • Szent István University Kaposvár Campus

Abstract and Figures

Four experiments are described relating to gnawing sticks application in rabbit housing. In experiment 1 and 2, Pannon White rabbits, weaned at the age of five weeks, were placed into pens with wire net floor. In experiment 1, every pen (180 animals in 12 pens, surface per head: 571 cm2) was provided with three gnawing sticks randomly chosen from White locust, Black elder, White willow, Little-leaf linden, European larch, Black poplar, European white birch, White buckeye and White mulberry species. In experiment 2 (150 animals in 10 pens, surface per head: 571 cm2), only those tree species ingested by rabbits in the first experiment were used (White locust, White willow, Little-leaf linden, Black poplar and White buckeye). In the second experiment, rabbits showed the highest preference towards gnawing sticks of Little-leaf linden, while similar White willow and White buckeye consumption was observed. In the experiment 3, rabbits¿ preference to different types of wood and the influence on rabbits¿ behaviour of added wooden sticks as environmental enrichment was studied. At the age of 38 days, 48 male SIKA rabbits were housed individually in wire cages (surface per head: 1500 cm2). According to the type of wood placed in the cage, rabbits were equally allotted to four groups: control, Common oak, Little-leaf linden and Norway spruce. Rabbits¿ preference to type of wood was studied on all the animals with wooden stick, while rabbits¿ behaviour was studied on 16 focus animals at the age of 5 and 13 weeks using continuous 24 hours video recordings. Rabbits preferred gnawing Little-leaf linden and Norway spruce compared to Common oak. Addition of gnawing sticks had no significant influence on duration of rabbits¿ behaviour, except for eating feed and gnawing wooden stick time. In experiment 4, the effect of housing and environmental enrichment on the performance and behaviour of growing rabbits was tested. 72 hybrid rabbits were housed after the weaning period in standard fattening cages at 2, 3, 4 animals per cage (surface per head: 1045 cm2, 697 cm2, 522 cm2, respectively). Half cages were enriched using a wood stick (Robinia pseudoacacia) dangling from the ceiling of the cage. The environmental enrichment decreased the stereotypies (gnawing the bars of the cage). A tendency to lower aggressive behaviours in enriched caged rabbits was also found compared to the conventional caged rabbits. This might indicate a better satisfaction of the behavioural needs for the enriched caged rabbits.
Content may be subject to copyright.
28
DARZI et al.
As the coccidial oocysts are shed with the faeces contaminating the environment, food and water
the rabbitʼs cages, food dishes and water containers need to be routinely disinfected. Avoidance of
stress and strict sanitary measures are thought to reduce or prevent clinical disease (Fox et al., 1984).
REFERENCES
Barriga O.O. 1997. The class Apicomplexa or Sporozoa. In:
Veterinary Parasitology for Practitioners, 2nd edition, Edina,
Minn: Burgess International Group, 76.1-26.4.
Benjamin M.M. 1981. Outline of Veterinary Clinical Pathology,
the Iowa State University Press Ames, Iowa, USA, 30.
Bhatia B.B. 2000. Textbook of Veterinary Protozoology. 1st edition.
Directorate of Information and Publication of Agriculture,
Indian Council of Agricultural Research, Krishi Anusandhan
Bhavan, Pusa, New Delhi, 178-180.
Chen C.M.C., Tsai L.C., Chung C.F., Han, S.H. 1972. Basement
membrane in hepatic coccidiosis of rabbits. 1. Camp. Pathol.
82, 59-63.
Flynn R.J. 1973. Protozoans and neosporans. In: Parasites of
Laboratory Animals, Ames, Iowa, Iowa State University Press,
54-56.
Fox J.G., Cohen B.J., Loew F.M. 1984. Laboratory Animal Medicine.
Academic Press Inc. (London) Ltd., 222-224.
Frebourg T., Delpech B., Bercoff E., Senant J., Bertrand P., Deugnier
Y., Bourreille J. 1986. Serum hyaluronate in liver diseases:
study by enzymoimmunological assay. Hepatology 6, 392-405.
Gardiner G.H., Fayer R., Dubey J.P. 1998. Apicomplexa. In: An Atlas
of Protozoan Parasites in Animal Tissues, 2nd edition, Armed
Forces Institute of Pathology, Washington, DC., 20-30.
Georgi J.R., Georgi M.E. 1990. Protozoans. In: Parasitology for
Veterinarians, 5th edition, W.B. Saunders, Philadelphia, 34-
87.
George J., Tsutsumi M., Takase S. 2004 Expression of hyaluronic
acid in N-nitrosodimethylamine induced hepatic fibrosis in
rats. Int. J Biochem. Cell Biol. 36, 307-319.
Giacometti L., Parakkal P.F. 1969. Skin transplantation:
Orientation of epithelial cells by the basement membrane.
Nature, 223, 514-515.
Gupta V.K. 2005 Pathology of naturally occurring rabbit diseases
in Himachal Pradesh. In: Souvenir-XXII Annual Convention
of Indian Association of Veterinary Pathologists and
National Symposium on Newer Concepts in Animal and Avian
Disease Diagnosis-A Farmer, Industry and Institutional
Dialogue and a satellite Seminar on Principles of
Histological Basis of Gross Pathology of CL Davis DVM
Foundation, USA. November. 193-204.
John N.M., Zea M.E., Kawano T., Omata Y., Saito A., Toyoda Y.,
Milkano T. 1999. Identification of carbohydrates on Eimeria
stiedae sporozoites and their role in the invasion of cultured
cells in vitro. Vet. Parasitol, 81, 99-105.
Levine N.D. 1985. Apicomplexa- The Coccidia Proper. In:
Veterinary Protozoology, Iowa State University Press, Ames,
Iowa, 130-138, 178, 221-222.
Luna L.G. 1968. Manual of Histologic Staining Methods of the
Armed Forces Institute of Pathology, 3rd edition. McGraw-Hill
Book Company, New York, USA, 38-39, 94-95, 168-169.
Mc Manus J.F.A. 1946. Histopathological demonstration of mucin
after periodic acid. Nature (London), 158, 202, in: BANCRAFT,
JD. and GAMBLE, M (ed), 2002.Theory and practice of
histopathological techniques, 5th edn, Churchill Livingstone,
London, Edinburgh, New York, Philadelphian, St. Louis,
Sydney, Toronto, 175.
Murawaki Y., Ikuta Y., Koda M., Nishimura Y., Kawasaki H. 1996.
Clinical significance of serum hyaluronan in patients with
chronic viral liver disease. J Gastroenterol. Hepatol. 11, 459-
465.
Peres A., Deulofeu R., Gimenez A., Caballeria L., Bruguera M.,
Caballeria J., Ballesta A.M., Rodes J. 1996. Serum hyaluronate
reflects hepatic fibrogenesis in alcoholic liver disease and is
useful as a marker of fibrosis. Hepatology 24, 1399-1403.
Urquhart G.M., Armour J., Duncan J.L., Dunn A.M., Jennings F.W.
1987. Phylum Protozoa. In: Veterinary Parasitology,
Churchill Livingstone, New York, 218-220.
Varga I. 1982. Large scale management systems and parasite
populations: coccidian in rabbits. Vet. Parasitol. 11, 69-84.
29
World Rabbit Sci. 2007, 15: 29 - 36
© WRSA, UPV, 2007
Received July 2006 - Accepted December 2006.
ABSTRACT:Four experiments are described relating to gnawing sticks application in rabbit housing. In experiment 1
and 2, Pannon White rabbits, weaned at the age of five weeks, were placed into pens with wire net floor. In
experiment 1, every pen (180 animals in 12 pens, surface per head: 571 cm2) was provided with three gnawing
sticks randomly chosen from White locust, Black elder, White willow, Little-leaf linden, European larch, Black poplar,
European white birch, White buckeye and White mulberry species. In experiment 2 (150 animals in 10 pens, surface
per head: 571 cm2), only those tree species ingested by rabbits in the first experiment were used (White locust,
White willow, Little-leaf linden, Black poplar and White buckeye). In the second experiment, rabbits showed the
highest preference towards gnawing sticks of Little-leaf linden, while similar White willow and White buckeye
consumption was observed. In the experiment 3, rabbits’ preference to different types of wood and the influence
on rabbits’ behaviour of added wooden sticks as environmental enrichment was studied. At the age of 38 days,
48 male SIKA rabbits were housed individually in wire cages (surface per head: 1500 cm2). According to the type
of wood placed in the cage, rabbits were equally allotted to four groups: control, Common oak, Little-leaf linden and
Norway spruce. Rabbits’ preference to type of wood was studied on all the animals with wooden stick, while
rabbits’ behaviour was studied on 16 focus animals at the age of 5 and 13 weeks using continuous 24 hours video
recordings. Rabbits preferred gnawing Little-leaf linden and Norway spruce compared to Common oak. Addition of
gnawing sticks had no significant influence on duration of rabbits’ behaviour, except for eating feed and gnawing
wooden stick time. In experiment 4, the effect of housing and environmental enrichment on the performance and
behaviour of growing rabbits was tested. 72 hybrid rabbits were housed after the weaning period in standard
fattening cages at 2, 3, 4 animals per cage (surface per head: 1045 cm 2, 697 cm2, 522 cm2
,respectively). Half cages
were enriched using a wood stick (Robinia pseudoacacia) dangling from the ceiling of the cage. The environmental
enrichment decreased the stereotypies (gnawing the bars of the cage). A tendency to lower aggressive behaviours
in enriched caged rabbits was also found compared to the conventional caged rabbits. This might indicate a better
satisfaction of the behavioural needs for the enriched caged rabbits.
Key words: Growing rabbits, gnawing sticks, tree species, performance, behavioural traits.
WW
WW
WO R L DO R L D
O R L DO R L D
O R L D
RR
RR
RABBITABBIT
ABBITABBIT
ABBIT
SS
SS
SCIENCECIENCE
CIENCECIENCE
CIENCE
APPLICATION OF GNAWING STICKS IN RABBIT HOUSING
Princz Z.*, Orova Z.*, Nagy I.*, Jordan D., Štuhec I, Luzi F., Verga M., SzendrőZs.*
*Univ. of Kaposvar, Faculty of Animal Science, KAPOSVÁR, Hungary.
Univ. of Ljubljana, Biotechnical Faculty, Dep. of Animal Science, DOMžALE, Slovenia.
Univ. of Milan, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Animal Husbandry Inst., MILANO, Italy.
INTRODUCTION
During the recent years increasing importance was attached to the analysis of the rabbitsʼ welfare.
Several forms of environmental enrichment were tested in the conventional low level of stimulus
cage system. Gnawing sticks were frequently placed into the rabbit cages (Hansen et a l., 2000; Luzi
et al., 2003; Princz et al., 2005) and their effect on the performance and behaviour was recorded.
Several authors (Lidfors, 1997; Jordan and Štuhec, 2003; Verga et al., 2004) defined the species of the
applied gnawing sticks, but the exact tree species are not always given. Some suggestions were
published about the recommended tree species by Ernte (www.ernte.at): apple, ash, willow, spruce,
oak, however, they suggest the placement of leafy branches. In the present study some consequences
when placing gnawing sticks in rabbit housing are shown.
30
PRINCZ et al.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Experiment 1 and 2 were carried out at the University of Kaposvár using Pannon White rabbits.
Animals were placed into pens (1.71×0.50 m2; 15 rabbits per pen; surface per head: 571 cm2) with wire
net floor in closed air-conditioned rabbitry. Experiments lasted between the age of 5-11 weeks. During
the experiments the room temperature was 18 °C and the daily lighting was 16 hours. Rabbits were fed
a commercial pellet ad libitum between the age of 5-9 (10.3 MJ digetible energy (DE)/kg, 14.5% crude
protein, 2.0% fat, 17.5% crude fibre, 50 ppm Tiamulin, 500 ppm Oxitetracycline, 1 ppm Diclazuril) and
9-11 (10.6 MJ DE/kg, 16.0% crude protein, 3.0% fat, 16.0% crude fibre) weeks. Water was available ad
libitum from nipple drinkers. The feeder was placed on the wall of the cage; the two drinkers were put
on the opposite side (1.71 m from the feeder). After stripping the bark off, the gnawing sticks were
placed horizontally 20 cm high on the wall of the adjacent pens (Figure 1).
In experiment 1 (n=180 rabbits in 12 pens) every pen was provided with three gnawing sticks randomly
chosen from White locust (Robinia pseudoacacia), Black elder (Sambucus nigra), White willow
(Salix alba), Little-leaf linden (Tilia cordata), European larch (Picea abies), Black poplar (Populus
nigra), European white birch (Betula pendula), White buckeye (Aesculus hippocastanum) and
White mulberry (Morus alba) species (with a diameter of 3 cm and length of 20 cm). During the trial
the preference of the rabbits (ingested vs. rejected species) was monitored to choice the main accepted
species for examing their ingestion.
In experiment 2 (n=150 rabbits in 10 pens) only those gnawing sticks (tree species) were used that
were ingested by the rabbits in the first experiment (White locust, White willow, Little-leaf linden,
Black poplar and White buckeye). Every pen was applied with 3 gnawing sticks of different species
(using all possible combinations). Size of the stick was the same as in the preceding trial. Prior to
their placement into the cages the volumes of the gnawing sticks were determined by inserting them
into a glass cylinder filled with water. Gnawing sticks fully ingested during the experiment were
replaced. Gnawing stick consumption was determined (in cm3) from the volume differences measured
at the beginning and at the end of the experiment. The data of gnawing stick ingestion per pen
(experiment 2) were evaluated by one way analysis of variance using SPSS 10.0 software package
(SPSS, 1999).
Experiment 3 was carried out at the University of Ljubljana and lasted nine weeks, namely between
the rabbitsʼ age of 38 to 94 days. It included 48 male New Zealand White rabbits of Slovenian male
line SIKA. Animals were housed individually in wire cages sized 37.5×40×30 cm, equipped with a
feeder and nipple drinker. The rabbits were fed ad libitum with the complete feed mixture for fattening
rabbits (10.4 MJ DE/kg, 17% crude protein, 14% crude fibre, 2% fat). 12 hours light:12 hours dark
Figure 1. Placement of gnawing stick on the wall of pen (experiment 1 and 2).
31
GNAWING STIC K FOR RABBITS
schedule was used, with the lights on from 6:00 am to 6:00 pm. During the experiment ambient
temperature and relative humidity varied from 21 to 25°C and 35 to 57%, respectively. Right after the
individual housing animals were allotted to four groups, each containing 12 rabbits, according to the
type of wooden stick (dimensions: 2.1×4.4×50.3 cm) fixed horizontally right under the upper side of
the rabbitʼs cage. For gnawing material the first group received Common oak (Qercus robur), the
second Little-leaf linden (Tilia cordata), and the third Norway spruce (Picea abies) sticks. The
control group was left without wooden sticks.
To establish the amount of gnawed wood, wooden sticks were weighed once a week. During the
experiment it was observed that the relative humidity of the wood was changing with regard to
relative humidity of the air in the rabbitry, which caused oscillation in the weight of wooden sticks.
For this reason it was impossible to determine the amount of gnawed wood from the weight of the
wooden stick, therefore it was determined on the basis of 5 grades of visual evaluation (1 - visible
marks of teeth or completely intact, 2 – slightly gnawed, 3 – moderately gnawed, 4 – severely gnawed,
5 – extremely gnawed).
To determine the influence of different type of wooden stick on rabbitsʼ behaviour, 16 focus animals
(4 from each group) were recorded for 24 hours by infrared video camera (WV-BP330/Panasonic) in
the first and the last experimental week. Recordings were analysed by two observers with the
“Observer 4.1” program (Noldus), during which the duration of the behavioural elements was
continually registered. Activities of rearing up (sitting with forepaws lifted from the floor), eating
feed, drinking, caecotrophy, biting wire and feeder, wood gnawing, grooming, sniffing, hopping,
stretching, inactivity, and contact with the neighbour rabbit were observed, but in this study only
the results for behaviours important from the rabbitsʼ welfare point of view (biting wire and feeder)
and those, where there was a trend or significant difference between groups (inactivity, eating feed,
wood gnawing) are presented in detail. Statistical data analysis was conducted using statistical
program package SAS (2001). Data were not normally distributed and were therefore analysed with
nonparametric Wilcoxon rank-sum test with Bonferroni-Holms correction for multiple comparison of
groups.
Experiment 4 was carried out in a commercial farm located in North West of Italy. Over the trial, the
room temperature was about 15 °C and the daily lighting was 16 hours. 72 hybrids rabbits were
housed in standard cages with the same dimension during the gnawing period from 35 to 75 days of
age. The animals were housed in 2, 3 or 4 per cage (surface per head: 1045 cm2, 697 cm2 and 522 cm2,
respectively). Half cage were enriched using a Black locust stick (Robinia pseudoacacia) 24 cm
length and 8 cm of diameter, hanging from the ceiling of the cage, to study the effect of the
environmental enrichment. Animals were fed ad libitum a commercial feed (9.62 MJ DE/kg, 15.8%
crude protein, 3.0% fat, 16.5% crude fibre from 35 till 50 days of age; 10.25 MJ DE/kg, 16.5% crude
protein, 3.5% fat, 16.0% crude fibre from 50 days of age till slaughtering). Weight and daily weight
gain were recorded at weaning (35 days of age), at 53 days of age and at slaughter (75 days of age).
Behaviours were video recorded at the beginning (1st period) and at the end of the fattening period
(2nd period), for 24 hours each period, using 3 cameras connected to a Multiplexer and VCR tape
recorder. Thus each cage was observed for 48 hours in total. Behaviour was recorded using a scan
sampling method (Martin and Bateson, 1993) every 10 min during the whole observation time (288
scans per image). The following behaviours were recorded: lying, lying stretched, sniffing another
rabbit, sniffing the object, self and allo-grooming, movement, drinking, eating feed, caecotrophy,
hopping, standing, gnawing the bars of the cage, gnawing the wood stick, motor stereotypies,
aggressive behaviours. The following behaviours were performed very seldom, so they were excluded
form the analysis: lying, lying stretched, sniffing the object, self-grooming, movement, drinking,
30
PRINCZ et al.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Experiment 1 and 2 were carried out at the University of Kaposvár using Pannon White rabbits.
Animals were placed into pens (1.71×0.50 m2; 15 rabbits per pen; surface per head: 571 cm2) with wire
net floor in closed air-conditioned rabbitry. Experiments lasted between the age of 5-11 weeks. During
the experiments the room temperature was 18 °C and the daily lighting was 16 hours. Rabbits were fed
a commercial pellet ad libitum between the age of 5-9 (10.3 MJ digetible energy (DE)/kg, 14.5% crude
protein, 2.0% fat, 17.5% crude fibre, 50 ppm Tiamulin, 500 ppm Oxitetracycline, 1 ppm Diclazuril) and
9-11 (10.6 MJ DE/kg, 16.0% crude protein, 3.0% fat, 16.0% crude fibre) weeks. Water was available ad
libitum from nipple drinkers. The feeder was placed on the wall of the cage; the two drinkers were put
on the opposite side (1.71 m from the feeder). After stripping the bark off, the gnawing sticks were
placed horizontally 20 cm high on the wall of the adjacent pens (Figure 1).
In experiment 1 (n=180 rabbits in 12 pens) every pen was provided with three gnawing sticks randomly
chosen from White locust (Robinia pseudoacacia), Black elder (Sambucus nigra), White willow
(Salix alba), Little-leaf linden (Tilia cordata), European larch (Picea abies), Black poplar (Populus
nigra), European white birch (Betula pendula), White buckeye (Aesculus hippocastanum) and
White mulberry (Morus alba) species (with a diameter of 3 cm and length of 20 cm). During the trial
the preference of the rabbits (ingested vs. rejected species) was monitored to choice the main accepted
species for examing their ingestion.
In experiment 2 (n=150 rabbits in 10 pens) only those gnawing sticks (tree species) were used that
were ingested by the rabbits in the first experiment (White locust, White willow, Little-leaf linden,
Black poplar and White buckeye). Every pen was applied with 3 gnawing sticks of different species
(using all possible combinations). Size of the stick was the same as in the preceding trial. Prior to
their placement into the cages the volumes of the gnawing sticks were determined by inserting them
into a glass cylinder filled with water. Gnawing sticks fully ingested during the experiment were
replaced. Gnawing stick consumption was determined (in cm3) from the volume differences measured
at the beginning and at the end of the experiment. The data of gnawing stick ingestion per pen
(experiment 2) were evaluated by one way analysis of variance using SPSS 10.0 software package
(SPSS, 1999).
Experiment 3 was carried out at the University of Ljubljana and lasted nine weeks, namely between
the rabbitsʼ age of 38 to 94 days. It included 48 male New Zealand White rabbits of Slovenian male
line SIKA. Animals were housed individually in wire cages sized 37.5×40×30 cm, equipped with a
feeder and nipple drinker. The rabbits were fed ad libitum with the complete feed mixture for fattening
rabbits (10.4 MJ DE/kg, 17% crude protein, 14% crude fibre, 2% fat). 12 hours light:12 hours dark
Figure 1. Placement of gnawing stick on the wall of pen (experiment 1 and 2).
31
GNAWING STIC K FOR RABBITS
schedule was used, with the lights on from 6:00 am to 6:00 pm. During the experiment ambient
temperature and relative humidity varied from 21 to 25°C and 35 to 57%, respectively. Right after the
individual housing animals were allotted to four groups, each containing 12 rabbits, according to the
type of wooden stick (dimensions: 2.1×4.4×50.3 cm) fixed horizontally right under the upper side of
the rabbitʼs cage. For gnawing material the first group received Common oak (Qercus robur), the
second Little-leaf linden (Tilia cordata), and the third Norway spruce (Picea abies) sticks. The
control group was left without wooden sticks.
To establish the amount of gnawed wood, wooden sticks were weighed once a week. During the
experiment it was observed that the relative humidity of the wood was changing with regard to
relative humidity of the air in the rabbitry, which caused oscillation in the weight of wooden sticks.
For this reason it was impossible to determine the amount of gnawed wood from the weight of the
wooden stick, therefore it was determined on the basis of 5 grades of visual evaluation (1 - visible
marks of teeth or completely intact, 2 – slightly gnawed, 3 – moderately gnawed, 4 – severely gnawed,
5 – extremely gnawed).
To determine the influence of different type of wooden stick on rabbitsʼ behaviour, 16 focus animals
(4 from each group) were recorded for 24 hours by infrared video camera (WV-BP330/Panasonic) in
the first and the last experimental week. Recordings were analysed by two observers with the
“Observer 4.1” program (Noldus), during which the duration of the behavioural elements was
continually registered. Activities of rearing up (sitting with forepaws lifted from the floor), eating
feed, drinking, caecotrophy, biting wire and feeder, wood gnawing, grooming, sniffing, hopping,
stretching, inactivity, and contact with the neighbour rabbit were observed, but in this study only
the results for behaviours important from the rabbitsʼ welfare point of view (biting wire and feeder)
and those, where there was a trend or significant difference between groups (inactivity, eating feed,
wood gnawing) are presented in detail. Statistical data analysis was conducted using statistical
program package SAS (2001). Data were not normally distributed and were therefore analysed with
nonparametric Wilcoxon rank-sum test with Bonferroni-Holms correction for multiple comparison of
groups.
Experiment 4 was carried out in a commercial farm located in North West of Italy. Over the trial, the
room temperature was about 15 °C and the daily lighting was 16 hours. 72 hybrids rabbits were
housed in standard cages with the same dimension during the gnawing period from 35 to 75 days of
age. The animals were housed in 2, 3 or 4 per cage (surface per head: 1045 cm2, 697 cm2 and 522 cm2,
respectively). Half cage were enriched using a Black locust stick (Robinia pseudoacacia) 24 cm
length and 8 cm of diameter, hanging from the ceiling of the cage, to study the effect of the
environmental enrichment. Animals were fed ad libitum a commercial feed (9.62 MJ DE/kg, 15.8%
crude protein, 3.0% fat, 16.5% crude fibre from 35 till 50 days of age; 10.25 MJ DE/kg, 16.5% crude
protein, 3.5% fat, 16.0% crude fibre from 50 days of age till slaughtering). Weight and daily weight
gain were recorded at weaning (35 days of age), at 53 days of age and at slaughter (75 days of age).
Behaviours were video recorded at the beginning (1st period) and at the end of the fattening period
(2nd period), for 24 hours each period, using 3 cameras connected to a Multiplexer and VCR tape
recorder. Thus each cage was observed for 48 hours in total. Behaviour was recorded using a scan
sampling method (Martin and Bateson, 1993) every 10 min during the whole observation time (288
scans per image). The following behaviours were recorded: lying, lying stretched, sniffing another
rabbit, sniffing the object, self and allo-grooming, movement, drinking, eating feed, caecotrophy,
hopping, standing, gnawing the bars of the cage, gnawing the wood stick, motor stereotypies,
aggressive behaviours. The following behaviours were performed very seldom, so they were excluded
form the analysis: lying, lying stretched, sniffing the object, self-grooming, movement, drinking,
32
PRINCZ et al.
eating feed, caecotrophy, hopping, standing, gnawing the wood stick and motor stereotypies. Data
were evaluated by Analysis of Variance using SAS package (2001)placing environmental enrichment
and housing as main effects. Behaviours were also analysed by the Principal Component Analysis
(PCA, Todeschini, 1998), and by their frequencies, according to the considered independent variables.
An overview about the experiments is presented in Table 1.
RESULTS
Based on the results of experiment 1 rabbits showed preference towards White locust, White willow,
Little-leaf linden, Black poplar and White buckeye as they consumed the gnawing sticks of these
species in various amount. Black elder, European larch, European white birch and White mulberry
species were rejected as no ingestion of these species could be observed (regardless of the different
combinations).
Table 1: Overview of the experiments
Experiment
1 2 3 4
Country Hungary Hungary Slovenia Italy
Breed Pannon White Pannon White SIKA Hybrid
Age (days) 35-77 35-77 38-94 35-75
No. of rabbits 180 150 48 72
No. of rabbits per
placing unit 15 12 1 2-3 or 4
Surface per head (cm2) 571 571 1500 522-1045
Temperature (ºC) 18 18 21-25 15
Daily lighting (hours) 16 16 12 16
Applied gnawing stick
species
White locust, Black
elder, White willow,
Little- leaf linde n,
European larch, Black
poplar, Europen white
birch, White buckeye,
White mulberry
White locust, White
willow, Little -lea f
linden, Black poplar,
White buckeye
Co mmon oak, Little-
leaf lind en, Nor wa y
spruce
Black locust
Examined behaviour Preference Consumption
Biting wire and feeder,
Inactivity, Eating feed,
Stick gnawing
Sniffing another rabbit,
A l l o - g r o o m i n g ,
Gna wing th e ca ge ,
Aggressive behaviour
Statistical method Analysis of variance Analysis of variance Wilcoxon rank-sum test Principal component
analysis
Table 2: Consumption of gnawing stick depending on the tree species (Experiment 2)
Consumption Species SE P-value
Little-leaf linden White willow White buckeye Black popular White locust
Per pen, cm3(35-77 d) 278c132b128b7a3a23.9 0.001
Per rabbit, cm3(per day) 0.44 0.21 0.20 0.01 0.005 - -
Means in a row with different superscripts indicate significant differences (P<0.05).
33
GNAWING STIC K FOR RABBITS
According to experiment 2 (Table 2) the rabbits showed the highest preference towards Little-leaf
linden gnawing sticks, while similar amount of White willow and White buckeye ingestion was
experienced. The gnawing stick consumption of White locust and Black poplar was relatively small.
In experiment 3 rabbits showed significantly higher preference to Little-leaf linden and Norway
spruce sticks than to Common oak sticks (Table 3). The average score of gnawed wood was almost
the same for Little-leaf linden and Norway spruce gnawing sticks (2.95 and 2.77) although Little-leaf
linden sticks received more often grade 5 (extremely gnawed) than Norway spruce sticks (Figure 2),
where the amount of gnawed wood was most often evaluated with grade 3 (moderately gnawed).
Common oak sticks were almost all intact. Enrichment of cages with different types of wooden sticks
had no significant influence on duration of observed behaviours, except eating feed and wood
gnawing (Table 3). Rabbits with Norway spruce sticks ate feed significantly less time than rabbits in
Common oak group. Rabbits spent significantly greater percentage of time gnawing Little-leaf linden
and Norway spruce sticks in comparison with Common oak sticks. Difference in duration of wood
gnawing between Norway spruce and Little-leaf linden group was not significant, although mean
Frequency (%)
Figure 2: Distribution of the grade
of gnawed wood (Experiment 3)
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5
Grade
Common oak
Little - leaf linden
N
o
Table 3: Grade of gnawed wood and the influence of environmental enrichment on mean duration
(percentage per hour) of rabbits' behaviours (Experiment 3)
Group P-value
Control Common oak Little-leaf linden Norway spruce
Grade of gnawed wood1- 1.0a2.95b2.77b0.0021
Behaviour, %
Inactivity 60.15 56.37 55.69 59.79 0.0580
Eating feed 7.99ab 8.49a7.86ab 6.91b0.0342
Biting wire and feeder 1.62 2.16 1.15 1.21 0.8759
Wood gnawing - 0.00a0.01b0.21b0.0005
Means in a row with different superscripts indicate significant differences (P<0.05). 1Five grades of visual evaluation from
1=visible marks of teeth or completely intact to 5=extremely gnawed.
32
PRINCZ et al.
eating feed, caecotrophy, hopping, standing, gnawing the wood stick and motor stereotypies. Data
were evaluated by Analysis of Variance using SAS package (2001)placing environmental enrichment
and housing as main effects. Behaviours were also analysed by the Principal Component Analysis
(PCA, Todeschini, 1998), and by their frequencies, according to the considered independent variables.
An overview about the experiments is presented in Table 1.
RESULTS
Based on the results of experiment 1 rabbits showed preference towards White locust, White willow,
Little-leaf linden, Black poplar and White buckeye as they consumed the gnawing sticks of these
species in various amount. Black elder, European larch, European white birch and White mulberry
species were rejected as no ingestion of these species could be observed (regardless of the different
combinations).
Table 1: Overview of the experiments
Experiment
1 2 3 4
Country Hungary Hungary Slovenia Italy
Breed Pannon White Pannon White SIKA Hybrid
Age (days) 35-77 35-77 38-94 35-75
No. of rabbits 180 150 48 72
No. of rabbits per
placing unit 15 12 1 2-3 or 4
Surface per head (cm2) 571 571 1500 522-1045
Temperature (ºC) 18 18 21-25 15
Daily lighting (hours) 16 16 12 16
Applied gnawing stick
species
White locust, Black
elder, White willow,
Little- leaf linde n,
European larch, Black
poplar, Europen white
birch, White buckeye,
White mulberry
White locust, White
willow, Little -lea f
linden, Black poplar,
White buckeye
Co mmon oak, Little-
leaf lind en, Nor wa y
spruce
Black locust
Examined behaviour Preference Consumption
Biting wire and feeder,
Inactivity, Eating feed,
Stick gnawing
Sniffing another rabbit,
A l l o - g r o o m i n g ,
Gna wing th e ca ge ,
Aggressive behaviour
Statistical method Analysis of variance Analysis of variance Wilcoxon rank-sum test Principal component
analysis
Table 2: Consumption of gnawing stick depending on the tree species (Experiment 2)
Consumption Species SE P-value
Little-leaf linden White willow White buckeye Black popular White locust
Per pen, cm3(35-77 d) 278c132b128b7a3a23.9 0.001
Per rabbit, cm3(per day) 0.44 0.21 0.20 0.01 0.005 - -
Means in a row with different superscripts indicate significant differences (P<0.05).
33
GNAWING STIC K FOR RABBITS
According to experiment 2 (Table 2) the rabbits showed the highest preference towards Little-leaf
linden gnawing sticks, while similar amount of White willow and White buckeye ingestion was
experienced. The gnawing stick consumption of White locust and Black poplar was relatively small.
In experiment 3 rabbits showed significantly higher preference to Little-leaf linden and Norway
spruce sticks than to Common oak sticks (Table 3). The average score of gnawed wood was almost
the same for Little-leaf linden and Norway spruce gnawing sticks (2.95 and 2.77) although Little-leaf
linden sticks received more often grade 5 (extremely gnawed) than Norway spruce sticks (Figure 2),
where the amount of gnawed wood was most often evaluated with grade 3 (moderately gnawed).
Common oak sticks were almost all intact. Enrichment of cages with different types of wooden sticks
had no significant influence on duration of observed behaviours, except eating feed and wood
gnawing (Table 3). Rabbits with Norway spruce sticks ate feed significantly less time than rabbits in
Common oak group. Rabbits spent significantly greater percentage of time gnawing Little-leaf linden
and Norway spruce sticks in comparison with Common oak sticks. Difference in duration of wood
gnawing between Norway spruce and Little-leaf linden group was not significant, although mean
Frequency (%)
Figure 2: Distribution of the grade
of gnawed wood (Experiment 3)
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 4 4.5 5
Grade
Common oak
Little - leaf linden
N
o
Table 3: Grade of gnawed wood and the influence of environmental enrichment on mean duration
(percentage per hour) of rabbits' behaviours (Experiment 3)
Group P-value
Control Common oak Little-leaf linden Norway spruce
Grade of gnawed wood1- 1.0a2.95b2.77b0.0021
Behaviour, %
Inactivity 60.15 56.37 55.69 59.79 0.0580
Eating feed 7.99ab 8.49a7.86ab 6.91b0.0342
Biting wire and feeder 1.62 2.16 1.15 1.21 0.8759
Wood gnawing - 0.00a0.01b0.21b0.0005
Means in a row with different superscripts indicate significant differences (P<0.05). 1Five grades of visual evaluation from
1=visible marks of teeth or completely intact to 5=extremely gnawed.
3 4
PRINCZ et al.
values differed substantially. In the enriched cages a trend of decrement of inactivity (P=0.058) was
noticed, but in wire and feeder biting no significant decrement was observed.
In experiment 4, the effects of presence of a wood stick inside the cage and of the housing systems
(2, 3, 4 animal per cage) were not statistically significant on the rabbit performance. Weight gain
between 35 and 75 days and body weight at 75 days of age were not affected by treatment. The
means of these traits were between 38.0 and 39.5 g/day, and 2540 and 2626 g, respectively. As far as
the behaviour is concerned, the enriched caged rabbits performed significantly more allo-grooming
and sniffing the other rabbits, while the control animals performed significantly more gnawing the
bars of the cage. A trend was also found in these latter rabbits to show higher alert and aggressive
behaviours (Figure 3).
DISCUSSION
The highest level of gnawing stick consumption was observed for Little-leaf linden. Considerable
amount of consumption was found for White buckeye and White willow. All these species are soft
(hardness) (Kovács, 1979). The mechanical and chemical characteristics of the tree species (hardness,
smell and taste) may determine which of the investigated species is suitable for gnawing stick.
Consumption of White locust and Black poplar was small. The White locust is a tough tree and has
indelicate fibres that may explain its consumption level. Although Black poplar is a soft tree its
consumption was also small. According to rabbitsʼpreference for wood species, the results of the
experiment 3 were quite similar to results of experiment 2. Rabbits preferred sticks made of Little-leaf
linden and Norway spruce wood compared to Common oak. The possible reason of this finding
could be, as in experiment 2, mechanical and chemical characteristics of used three species. Common
oak is very dense and hard in comparison with Norway spruce and Little-leaf linden (Čermak, 1998;
Pipa, 1990) and it also contains a large amount of tannin, about 10% (Pipa, 1990). Occupation of
rabbits with environmental enrichment did not significantly reduced animalsʼinactivity, as it was
reported in several studies investigating environmental enrichment (Metz, 1987; Huls et al., 1991). In
neither group was observed any significant decrement of biting wire and feeder, which is one of the
0%
1%
2%
3%
4%
5%
6%
7%
8%
Sniffing other
rabbits
Allo-
Grooming
Agressiveness Alert Gnawing bars
of the the cage
Environmental
Enrichment
Control
**
**
n.s
*
n.s
Figure 3: Behaviour of rabbits with and without environmental enrichment. (Average values - % of total
scans) (Experiment 4).
35
GNAWING STIC K FOR RABB ITS
most common behavioural abnormalities in cage systems (Gunn and Morton, 1995; Laboratory Animals,
1993; Love, 1994). This last result is surprising as studies using different kind of environmental
enrichment reported significant decrement of these kinds of behaviours (Lidfors, 1997; Jordan et al.,
2003).
Contrary to experiment 3, in experiment 4 the environmental enrichment affected the behaviour (but
not the rabbitsʼ performance). It significantly increased social activities, such as sniffing the other
rabbit and allo-grooming. On the contrary, it decreased the behaviour of gnawing the bars of the
cage, which may be an oral stereotypy, as well a trend was found to decreased alert and aggressive
behaviours. The growth and carcass traits were not statistically significantly affected by environmental
enrichment in agreement with other previous results (Luzi et al., 2002). These results support that
environmental enrichment and housing have no effect on the production of the rabbits.
CONCLUSIONS
Based on our results the following suggestions can be made:
a) Unsuitable for gnawing stick: Black elder, European larch, European white birch tree, White mulberry
and Common oak.
b) Suitable for environment enrichment: Black poplar and White locust tree.
c) Suitable for gnawing stick: White willow, White buckeye, Little-leaf linden and Norway spruce.
d) Our results can be utilised in further experiments connected with the gnawing stick application
and determining the suitable tree species.
Housing and environmental enrichment did not affect significantly the growth rate contrary to some
rabbit behaviours. However in this case the results are not always consistent. In fact in experiment 3
the addition of wooden stick as environmental enrichment did not decrease wire and feeder biting,
which is one of the most common behavioural abnormalities in cage systems. The type of wood
significantly influenced only the duration of feed eating. On the contrary, in experiment 4 environmental
enrichment significantly decreased gnawing the bars of the cage and increased some social behaviours,
such as sniffing the other rabbit and allo-grooming. A trend was shown also to reduced, alert and
aggressive behaviours. Thus further research is needed in order to understand how environmental
enrichement may affect rabbitsʼbehaviour, possibly reducing oral stereotypies and increasing the
behavioural repertoire.
REFERENCES
Čermak M. 1998. Tehnologija lesa 1. Železniki, Pami. 205.
Gunn D., Morton D.B. 1995. Inventory of the behaviour of New
Zealand White rabbits in laboratory cages. Appl. Animal
Behaviour Science, 45, 277-292.
Hansen L.H., Berthelsen H. 2000. The effect of environmental
enrichment on the behaviour of caged rabbits (Oryctolagus
Cuni culus). Appl. Animal Behaviour Science, 68, 163-178.
Huls W.L., Brooks D.L., Bean-Knudsen D. 1991. Response of adult
New Zealand White rabbits to enrichment objects and paired
housing. Lab. Anim. Sci., 41, 609-612.
Jordan D., Štuhec I., Pečlin G., Gorjanc G. 2003. The influence of
environmental enrichment on the behaviour of fattening rabbits
housed in individual wire cages. 13. Arbeitstagung über
Haltung und Krankheiten der Kaninchen, Peltztiere und
Heim tiere , Celle , 14.-15. Mai, 119-126.
Kovács I. 1979. Wood properties, Mezőgazdasági Kiadó, Budapest
Laborat ory An imals. Refinements in rabbit husbandry. Second
repo rt of the BVAAWF/RSPCA/UFAW Joint working gr oup
on refinement. Lab. Anim., 27(1993), 301-329.
Lidfors L. 1997. Behavioural effects of environmental enrichment
for individually caged rabbits. Appl. Animal Behaviour
Science, 52, 157-169.
Love J.A. 1994. Group housing: Meeting the physical and social
needs of the laboratory rabbit. Lab. Anim. Sci., 44, 5-11.
Luzi F., Ferrante V., Heinzl E., Verga M., 2003. Effect of
environmental enrichment on productive performance and
welfare aspects in fatteni ng rabbits. It alian J . Animal S cienc e,
2: Suppl. 1, 438-440.
Martin P., Bateson P. 1993. Measuring behavior: an introductory
guide. 2° Ed. , Cambr idge.
3 4
PRINCZ et al.
values differed substantially. In the enriched cages a trend of decrement of inactivity (P=0.058) was
noticed, but in wire and feeder biting no significant decrement was observed.
In experiment 4, the effects of presence of a wood stick inside the cage and of the housing systems
(2, 3, 4 animal per cage) were not statistically significant on the rabbit performance. Weight gain
between 35 and 75 days and body weight at 75 days of age were not affected by treatment. The
means of these traits were between 38.0 and 39.5 g/day, and 2540 and 2626 g, respectively. As far as
the behaviour is concerned, the enriched caged rabbits performed significantly more allo-grooming
and sniffing the other rabbits, while the control animals performed significantly more gnawing the
bars of the cage. A trend was also found in these latter rabbits to show higher alert and aggressive
behaviours (Figure 3).
DISCUSSION
The highest level of gnawing stick consumption was observed for Little-leaf linden. Considerable
amount of consumption was found for White buckeye and White willow. All these species are soft
(hardness) (Kovács, 1979). The mechanical and chemical characteristics of the tree species (hardness,
smell and taste) may determine which of the investigated species is suitable for gnawing stick.
Consumption of White locust and Black poplar was small. The White locust is a tough tree and has
indelicate fibres that may explain its consumption level. Although Black poplar is a soft tree its
consumption was also small. According to rabbitsʼpreference for wood species, the results of the
experiment 3 were quite similar to results of experiment 2. Rabbits preferred sticks made of Little-leaf
linden and Norway spruce wood compared to Common oak. The possible reason of this finding
could be, as in experiment 2, mechanical and chemical characteristics of used three species. Common
oak is very dense and hard in comparison with Norway spruce and Little-leaf linden (Čermak, 1998;
Pipa, 1990) and it also contains a large amount of tannin, about 10% (Pipa, 1990). Occupation of
rabbits with environmental enrichment did not significantly reduced animalsʼinactivity, as it was
reported in several studies investigating environmental enrichment (Metz, 1987; Huls et al., 1991). In
neither group was observed any significant decrement of biting wire and feeder, which is one of the
0%
1%
2%
3%
4%
5%
6%
7%
8%
Sniffing other
rabbits
Allo-
Grooming
Agressiveness Alert Gnawing bars
of the the cage
Environmental
Enrichment
Control
**
**
n.s
*
n.s
Figure 3: Behaviour of rabbits with and without environmental enrichment. (Average values - % of total
scans) (Experiment 4).
35
GNAWING STIC K FOR RABB ITS
most common behavioural abnormalities in cage systems (Gunn and Morton, 1995; Laboratory Animals,
1993; Love, 1994). This last result is surprising as studies using different kind of environmental
enrichment reported significant decrement of these kinds of behaviours (Lidfors, 1997; Jordan et al.,
2003).
Contrary to experiment 3, in experiment 4 the environmental enrichment affected the behaviour (but
not the rabbitsʼ performance). It significantly increased social activities, such as sniffing the other
rabbit and allo-grooming. On the contrary, it decreased the behaviour of gnawing the bars of the
cage, which may be an oral stereotypy, as well a trend was found to decreased alert and aggressive
behaviours. The growth and carcass traits were not statistically significantly affected by environmental
enrichment in agreement with other previous results (Luzi et al., 2002). These results support that
environmental enrichment and housing have no effect on the production of the rabbits.
CONCLUSIONS
Based on our results the following suggestions can be made:
a) Unsuitable for gnawing stick: Black elder, European larch, European white birch tree, White mulberry
and Common oak.
b) Suitable for environment enrichment: Black poplar and White locust tree.
c) Suitable for gnawing stick: White willow, White buckeye, Little-leaf linden and Norway spruce.
d) Our results can be utilised in further experiments connected with the gnawing stick application
and determining the suitable tree species.
Housing and environmental enrichment did not affect significantly the growth rate contrary to some
rabbit behaviours. However in this case the results are not always consistent. In fact in experiment 3
the addition of wooden stick as environmental enrichment did not decrease wire and feeder biting,
which is one of the most common behavioural abnormalities in cage systems. The type of wood
significantly influenced only the duration of feed eating. On the contrary, in experiment 4 environmental
enrichment significantly decreased gnawing the bars of the cage and increased some social behaviours,
such as sniffing the other rabbit and allo-grooming. A trend was shown also to reduced, alert and
aggressive behaviours. Thus further research is needed in order to understand how environmental
enrichement may affect rabbitsʼbehaviour, possibly reducing oral stereotypies and increasing the
behavioural repertoire.
REFERENCES
Čermak M. 1998. Tehnologija lesa 1. Železniki, Pami. 205.
Gunn D., Morton D.B. 1995. Inventory of the behaviour of New
Zealand White rabbits in laboratory cages. Appl. Animal
Behaviour Science, 45, 277-292.
Hansen L.H., Berthelsen H. 2000. The effect of environmental
enrichment on the behaviour of caged rabbits (Oryctolagus
Cuni culus). Appl. Animal Behaviour Science, 68, 163-178.
Huls W.L., Brooks D.L., Bean-Knudsen D. 1991. Response of adult
New Zealand White rabbits to enrichment objects and paired
housing. Lab. Anim. Sci., 41, 609-612.
Jordan D., Štuhec I., Pečlin G., Gorjanc G. 2003. The influence of
environmental enrichment on the behaviour of fattening rabbits
housed in individual wire cages. 13. Arbeitstagung über
Haltung und Krankheiten der Kaninchen, Peltztiere und
Heim tiere , Celle , 14.-15. Mai, 119-126.
Kovács I. 1979. Wood properties, Mezőgazdasági Kiadó, Budapest
Laborat ory An imals. Refinements in rabbit husbandry. Second
repo rt of the BVAAWF/RSPCA/UFAW Joint working group
on refinement. Lab. Anim., 27(1993), 301-329.
Lidfors L. 1997. Behavioural effects of environmental enrichment
for individually caged rabbits. Appl. Animal Behaviour
Science, 52, 157-169.
Love J.A. 1994. Group housing: Meeting the physical and social
needs of the laboratory rabbit. Lab. Anim. Sci., 44, 5-11.
Luzi F., Ferrante V., Heinzl E., Verga M., 2003. Effect of
environmental enrichment on productive performance and
welfare aspects in fatteni ng rabbits. It alian J . Animal S cienc e,
2: Suppl. 1, 438-440.
Martin P., Bateson P. 1993. Measuring behavior: an introductory
guide. 2° Ed. , Cambr idge.
3 6
PRINCZ et al.
Metz J.H.M. 1987. Behavioural problems of rabbits in cages. In:
Agriculture: Rabbit production systems including welfare
(Ed. : Auxil ia, T.), Lu xembu rg, Comm issio n of the Eu ropea n
Communities, 221-230.
Pipa R. 1990. Anatomija in tehnologija lesa. Tehnologija tvoriv.
Ljubljana, Zveza inženirjev in tehnikov gozdarstva in
lesarstva, Lesarska Založba, 136 p.
Princz Z., SzendrőZs., Dalle Zotte A., Radnai I., Biró-Németh E.,
Metz ger Sz. , Gyovai M., Orov a Z. 200 5. Effec t of diff erent
housing on productive traits and on some behaviour patterns
of growing rabbits; In Proc.: 17th Hungarian Conference on
Rabbit Production, Kaposvár, 95-102.
SAS. 2 001. SAS /STAT. User ʼs gui de. (Rel ease 8.02). SAS I nstitu te
Inc., Cary, NC, USA.
SPSS for Windows 1999. Version 10.0, Copyright SPSS Inc.
Todes chin i R., 19 98. Int rodu zion e alla C hemiometr ia. Ed iSES ,
Napoli, Italia.
Verga M., Zingarelli I., Heinzl E., Ferrante V., Martino P.A., Luzi F.
2004. Effect of housing and environmental enrichment on
perf orman ce and behaviou r in fatt ening r abbits. In Proc .: 8th
World Rab bit Con gress, Puebl a City, 1283-1288.
37
World Rabbit Sci. 2007, 15: 37 - 41
© WRSA, UPV, 2007
Correspondence: A. Finzi, finzi@unitus.it
Received February 2006 - Accepted October 2006.
ABSTRACT:Visual Image Analysis (VIA) has been evaluated to estimate morphological traits and weights of live
rabbits and carcasses to improve the body conformation of the new breed Leprino di Viterbo. The reliability of
VIA was firstly tested on a sample of 30 does. Then, a total of 365 animal (130 additional does and 205 rabbits
at slaughtering weight of kg 2.5) was utilised to calculate some surface traits and correlations between live
and carcass weights that were later validated over a new sample of 112 rabbits (37 does and 75 fattened).
VIA gave very good results to evaluate morphological and weight measurements. Maximum observed individual
difference between data measured by metre and by VIA was only 3.6%. Since standard error (SE) of VIA was
lower than SE of metre (0.06 and 0.33 respectively), VIA was considered more reliable than the instrumental
measurement. A new parameter, Body Side Surface, was made available by the Image Analysis to estimate
Live Weight and Carcass Weight efficiently. Correlations were high (P<0.01) both in does (R2=0.87 for live
weight equation) and at slaughter time (R2=0.82 and 0.76 for live and carcass weight equations, respectively).
It was concluded that VIA is a viable, quick and practical mean to measure and select for weight and
morphological traits as head length, ear length, body length and body side surface.
Key words: Visual image analysis, morphology, body weight, carcass, rabbit.
WW
WW
WORLDORLD
ORLDORLD
ORLD
RR
RR
RABBITABBIT
ABBITABBIT
ABBIT
SS
SS
SCIENCECIENCE
CIENCECIENCE
CIENCE
VISUAL IMAGE ANALYSIS TO ESTIMATE MORPHOLOGICAL
AND WEIGHT MEASUREMENTS IN RABBITS
Negretti P., Bianconi G., Finzi A.
Rabbit Unconventional Rearing Experimental Centre. Dpt. of Animal Production, Univ. of Tuscia. VITERBO, Italy
INTRODUCTION
Visual Image Analysis (VIA) has been demonstrated as a very practical and easy mean to estimate
morphological measurements in different domestic species (Filippi Balestra et al., 1994; Negretti et
al., 1995; Borggard et al., 1996; Kuchida et al., 1996; Sakowski et al., 1996; Tözsér et al., 2000; White
et al., 2004; Negretti and Bianconi, 2005). The system is quicker and more reliable than both, subjective
linearized descriptive type traits and even instrumental measurements, showing a lower standard
error. Moreover it allows easy recording of an electronic permanent archive.
New measurements as angles and surfaces are made possible too, thus increasing the number of
parameters to be utilised with selective purposes. Reliable weight estimations (Negretti and Bianconi,
2001; 2002; 2004) are also possible, both on live and carcass weight of different species.
A trial was planned to test the possibility of introducing VIA body measurements in rabbits,
considering that biased data could be produced by measurement errors due to the small dimensions
of the animals and by the peculiar problem that could rise depending on the presence of the fur.
The purpose of this work was to obtain reliable data to improve the morphological selection of a new
Italian breed named Leprino di Viterbo, that is selected to be raised in the open air without any
pharmacological treatment, any suspected animal being stamped out (Finzi, 2004).
... Among other behaviors, whether it is recognized that rabbits under wild conditions have the opportunity of gnawing besides foraging, the presence of gnawing objects under farming conditions has been claimed to improve their welfare by increasing locomotion [22,23], reducing stress [8], and reducing negative social interactions in collective housing systems [6]. On the other hand, based on available literature, most studies reported that the provision of gnawing materials does not affect the productive performance of individually or park-housed growing rabbits [5,9,23], which was confirmed under the conditions of the present study. ...
... The preference and time spent interacting with different gnawing objects can vary according to their physical and nutritional characteristics [5,9]. Wooden boards [8], cardboard and rubber chewing materials [24,25], straw [22], and gnawing blocks [23,25] have been tested. ...
... Wooden boards [8], cardboard and rubber chewing materials [24,25], straw [22], and gnawing blocks [23,25] have been tested. When comparing wooden sticks, soft-wood sticks were preferred over hard-wood sticks [5,9]. Indeed, the compressed hay blocks used in the present trial were expected to be rather soft to gnaw, and rabbits were found to have an average observation time of 0.4% (sniffing, licking, or gnawing). ...
Article
Full-text available
To improve animal welfare in collective park housing systems, this study assessed the effects of the provision of gnawing hay blocks and the group composition (F: females, M: males, FM: mixed sex) on performance, behavior, and reactivity of 288 growing rabbits reared in 18 parks (16 rabbits/park) from 31 to 73 days of age. The presence of gnawing blocks inside the feeding area of the parks scarcely affected performance and budget time, but decreased the time spent in the resting area compared to parks without blocks (p < 0.001); it increased the time spent moving during the open field test (p < 0.05) and the rate of rabbits that approached the object in the novel-object test (p < 0.05). As for sex-group composition, the feed conversion ratio was lower in parks F and FM than in park M (p < 0.05). During the open-field test, FM rabbits spent more time moving (p < 0.05), whereas M rabbits displayed self-grooming for a longer time (p < 0.01). Results related to production, behavior, and reactivity indicate the provision of gnawing blocks for welfare improvement, but do not support the change from current mixed-sex to single-sex rearing.
... Chewing objects are desirable as rabbit environmental enrichment objects, as chewing is a species-typical behaviour (Bayne, 2003). Different materials have been used as enrichment for growing rabbits, such as wood sticks (Princz et al., 2007), iron chains (Lang et al. 2011), rubber, cardboard and food for laboratory rabbits (Harris et al., 2001;Poggiagliolmi et al., 2011). Some of these objects are not commercially available and are difficult to acquire. ...
... Before starting the experiment, training to recognise rabbit behaviour in the rabbitry was undertaken regarding the studies of Gunn and Morton (1995), Princz et al. (2007), Poggiagliolmi et al. (2011) and Rommers et al. (2014). Rabbit behaviours were classified as self-cleaning, yawning, stretching, eating, drinking, sitting, lying down (sternal and stretched), standing, in movement, biting the cage, biting the nipple, digging the grid, walking in circles, caecotrophy and object-related behaviours: sniffing, gnawing, pulling, eating, licking, and marking via the chin scent glands. ...
... The time of evaluation (14:00 to 16:30 h) could explain the minor feeding behaviour observations (eating feed, eating hay, and drinking) and the higher percentages of grooming as resting-related behaviours (less locomotion and more lying down). The normal behaviours of rabbits are generally not affected by environmental enrichment (Poggiagliolmi et al., 2011;Rommers et al., 2014), although a variation was observed by Princz et al. (2007) and Bozicovich et al. (2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Environmental enrichment improves rabbit welfare in rabbitries. Various toys for cats and dogs are commercially available, which are made of materials that could be safely used for rabbits as well. The objectives of this study were to evaluate whether cardboard and rubber materials could be used for environmental enrichment for rabbits. The study involved 42 adult New Zealand white rabbits (20 females and 22 males), randomly assigned to seven treatment groups: “C”, without object (control); “RB”, a solid rubber ball; “FT”, a fillable teether filled with hay; “CH”, a cardboard hole; “CS”, a piece of a cat scratcher; “CSC”, a piece of a cat scratcher with catnip; “CF”, an articulated cardboard fish. The behaviour of the rabbits and the percentage of destruction of the objects were recorded for 28 d. The normal behaviours of locomotion, rearing, stretching, stereotypies and sitting were not influenced by the treatments. Lying down was observed more frequently than the full stretched out position for resting. The FT-treatment group presented most behaviours of interaction (biting and sniffing) ( P <0.05) as compared to RB, CSC, and CF-treatment groups. All the objects showed some level of destruction; the mean rates of destruction for CH, CS, CSC and CF were up to 40%, whereas those for FT and RB were under 30%. Taken together, the results suggest that cardboard and rubber materials can be used as means of environmental enrichment for rabbits.
... The behaviours such as standing (12 of the total records, average 0.1% of observation time), jumping (7 records) and scratching (5 records) were rarely observed whatever the group (NS). Gnawing of hard wood was not observed at all, which confirmed the preference of rabbits for soft than hard wood as observed by Princz et al. (2007). As expected, gnawing behaviour was more frequent in Gnawing and Combination housing systems than in other groups (4.5% and 4.7% vs <3%; P < 0.05) as confirmed by the consumption of compacted forage blocks (23 ± 14 g and 29 ± 15 g in Gnawing and Combination groups, NS). ...
... However, evaluation of soft wood sticks at the end of the experiment showed that they were fairly used by does from Gnawing and Combination groups (average score: 1.7 ± 1.0 vs. 1.5 ± 1.0, P = 0.54). The decrease of stereotypes by the inclusion of gnawing sticks has been previously reported (Princz et al., 2007). . ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
One way to improve rabbit welfare is to enable rabbits to express their natural behaviours. The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of housing and enrichments on behaviour, performance and mortality of growing rabbits (trial 1, n = 294), does and their kits (trial 2, n = 40 females over 2 cycles). The modifications concerned size of housing (length to allow jumping, height to allow standing up), enrichment (gnawing or scratching materials, platform for jumping, burrow for hiding), floor type (wire or plastic mesh) and/or group size. These modifications were implemented separately each other or together. The productive performance of growing rabbits and reproductive does was not influenced by the housing system. In trial 1, behaviours such as standing up, gnawing, jumping and walking were rarely observed (< 5%), but the burrow was frequently occupied (35%). In trial 2, inclusion of gnawing blocks increased gnawing activity. The platform permitted standing and jumping of does, but the proportion of time spent for this activity remained low (0.1%). On the floor, does were more often on plastic-mesh than on wire-mesh (62 vs. 38 % of observations; P < 0.001). The frequency of does on plastic-mesh decreased from 68% the week of parturition to 60% at weaning (P < 0.05), related to increased area occupation by suckling kits (P < 0.001). We confirmed that the enrichments tested allow rabbits to express or increase the frequency of natural behaviour.
... The behaviours such as standing (12 of the total records, average 0.1% of observation time), jumping (7 records) and scratching (5 records) were rarely observed whatever the group (NS). Gnawing of hard wood was not observed at all, which confirmed the preference of rabbits for soft than hard wood as observed by Princz et al. (2007). As expected, gnawing behaviour was more frequent in Gnawing and Combination housing systems than in other groups (4.5% and 4.7% vs <3%; P < 0.05) as confirmed by the consumption of compacted forage blocks (23 ± 14 g and 29 ± 15 g in Gnawing and Combination groups, NS). ...
... However, evaluation of soft wood sticks at the end of the experiment showed that they were fairly used by does from Gnawing and Combination groups (average score: 1.7 ± 1.0 vs. 1.5 ± 1.0, P = 0.54). The decrease of stereotypes by the inclusion of gnawing sticks has been previously reported (Princz et al., 2007). . ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
One way to improve rabbit welfare is to enable rabbits to express their natural behaviours. The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of housing and enrichments on behaviour, performance and mortality of growing rabbits (trial 1, n = 294), does and their kits (trial 2, n = 40 females over 2 cycles). The modifications concerned size of housing (length to allow jumping, height to allow standing up), enrichment (gnawing or scratching materials, platform for jumping, burrow for hiding), floor type (wire or plastic mesh) and/or group size. These modifications were implemented separately each other or together. The productive performance of growing rabbits and reproductive does was not influenced by the housing system. In trial 1, behaviours such as standing up, gnawing, jumping and walking were rarely observed (< 5%), but the burrow was frequently occupied (35%). In trial 2, inclusion of gnawing blocks increased gnawing activity. The platform permitted standing and jumping of does, but the proportion of time spent for this activity remained low (0.1%). On the floor, does were more often on plastic-mesh than on wire-mesh (62 vs. 38 % of observations; P < 0.001). The frequency of does on plastic-mesh decreased from 68% the week of parturition to 60% at weaning (P < 0.05), related to increased area occupation by suckling kits (P < 0.001). We confirmed that the enrichments tested allow rabbits to express or increase the frequency of natural behaviour.
... Although rarely recorded during direct observations, when provided, the soft wood sticks were used by the females. Additionally, in line with the results of Princz et al. (2007), the present study confirmed that the rabbits preferred soft wood to hard wood, as hard wood sticks were not used at all. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the effect of housing enrichments (scratching card, gnawing material and a platform), of a change in height and in the type of flooring on the live weight, reproductive performance and behaviour of female rabbits, as well as on the feed intake and spatial distribution of females and their kits. A total of 40 multiparous female rabbits were monitored in three consecutive reproductive cycles (48-d intervals). Four days before parturition in each reproductive cycle, the females were randomly assigned to one of the five types of housing: Control (CNT: 102×47×30 cm, L×W× H); Scratching card (SCT: containing a scratching card); Gnawing materials (GNW: CNT dimensions plus a compressed lucerne hay block and a wooden stick); Platform (PLT: 102×47×60 cm, including a platform with a plastic floor) and Combination (CBN: PLT dimensions with the scratching card, the gnawing materials and a platform). Data were only recorded during the first and third reproductive cycles. The living conditions did not significantly alter the females’ live weight (4889 g at housing; 4890 g at mid-lactation; 4867 g at weaning), reproductive performance (9.0 kits born alive), survival of the kits (90%), nor the feed intake of females and their litters (542 g/day). Providing animals with a gnawing block stimulated Gnawing behaviour (median frequency per group: CTL=0.00, SCT=0.00, GNW=4.69, PLT=0.00, and CBN=2.34; PRearing up behaviour (median frequencies per group: CTL=0.00, SCT=0.00, GNW=0.00, PLT=2.08, and CBN=3.12; P=0.06), and when a platform was present, the rabbits used it (mean values per group: CTL=0.00, SCT=0.00, GNW=0.00, PLT=1.79, and CBN=4.91; P=0.003). Regarding the type of floor, females appeared to prefer the plastic mesh flooring (31.2%) to the wire mesh flooring (18.8%). To sum up, providing female rabbits with simple enrichments appears to stimulate specific behaviours like Gnawing and Rearing up and may contribute to their wellbeing.
... These elements satisfy one of the most important needs of the rabbits, and it is performed independently of the housing system in which the animals are bred (see Figure 3). They also help to avoid the excessive growing of incisors [28], which is helpful especially in group systems, in order to decrease the intensity of the injuries when aggressive episodes take place. Moreover, this type of enrichment also leads to hygienic problems, especially when wooden sticks are used. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Animal welfare is a rising concern in livestock production and to assess the welfare state of an animal, it is needed to consider health, behaviour and emotions. Housing conditions and management normally impair animal welfare at different levels, so developing housing systems and management practices that imply a high level of animal welfare by preserving productive results is a need. Rabbit cages have to be improved in terms of space, enrichment and materials used to promote better conditions, and several alternatives are under evaluation such as increasing available space, providing animals with elevated platforms and hiding spaces, making available gnawing materials or changing cage materials. Moving from individual to collective housing systems to enhance social interaction is also being tested. Most of these alternatives have provided some steps towards better housing and management conditions for rabbits, while some of them have demonstrated to create more problems and have been abandoned. There is still a long way to go on this research topic.
Article
Full-text available
Many studies have shown that stress is associated with gut microbiota. Environmental enrichment (EE) could reduce stress in farm animals; however, limited information is available on the microbial community composition in rabbits raised with or without EE. This study aimed to identify EE influences on the behavior, serum hormonal levels, and cecal microbiota of rabbits. Two hundred Rex rabbits were segregated randomly within four cohorts ( n = 50); reared for 76 d within standardized enclosures (non-enriched) or within cages containing a willow-stick (WS), rubber-duck (RD), or a can of beans (CB). The rabbits’ ingestive, rest, locomotion, exploratory, grooming, and abnormal behavior were observed. The serum hormone levels for rabbits were measured, and cecal specimens were sequencedfrom the V3–V4 region using 16S rRNA amplicons. Environmental enrichment increased feeding and drinking time, promoted exploratory behavior, and reduced abnormal behavior in rabbits. Insulin-like growth factor 1(IGF-1) levels of the enriched cohorts were elevated in comparison to the control cohort. Serum cortisol level for CB cohort was markedly reduced in comparison to the control cohort ( p < 0.05), while dopamine levels for CB cohort peaked. Further, we found that EE mainly affected the dominant microbiota. Several families, such as Erysipelotrichaceae, Tannerellaceae, Enterobacteriaceae, Burkholderiaceae, and Prevotellaceae were markedly reduced within the CB cohort. Bacteria such as Alloprevotella, Bifidobacterium, Enterobacteriaceae, Parabacteroides , and Erysipelatoclostridium were identified as having negative associations with the presence of serum cortisol. EE influenced rabbit behavior and serum hormonal levels, and CB enrichment was the most suitable for rabbits. Further, cecal microbiota composition and diversity were affected by CB enrichment. These findings suggested that CB could be considered for use in rabbit husbandry.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: This study aimed to investigate the effect of different environmental enrichment materials on the growth performance, carcass traits, meat quality, and hair follicle development of growing Rex rabbits. Methods: One hundred and twenty Rex rabbits were randomly divided into four groups; reared in conventional cages (not enriched) and in enriched cages with either willow stick (WS), rubber duck (RD), or a can containing beans (CB), for 44 days. Results: The average daily gain of the CB group was the highest and had a significant difference from that of the other groups (P < 0.05). The spleen and cecum weight of the CB group was greater than those of the WS and control groups (P < 0.05). The redness (CIE a*) of the meat sample of the control group was lower than those of the enriched cage groups (P < 0.05). Moreover, the hue value of the CB group was significantly lower than that of the other groups (P < 0.05). The tenderest meat belonged to the CB group. In addition, more secondary (P < 0.05) and primary follicles were found in the CB group than in the control group. Conclusion: Environmental enrichment increased the average daily gain and improved some carcass traits, meat quality, and hair follicle density. Among the three environmental enrichment materials, CB could be recommended for rabbit husbandry.
Article
Full-text available
Az európai fogyasztók egyre nagyobb hangsúlyt helyeznek az állatok jóllétére. Az elvárások megfogalmazásában jelentős szerepe van az állatvédő mozgalmaknak, amelyek egy része szakmai ismeretek, mások viszont érzelmi benyomások alapján fogalmazzák meg véleményüket. Sajnos ritkán jutnak el a kutatási eredmények a kereskedőkhöz és fogyasztókhoz, az állatvédők viszont nagyon eredményesen gyakorolnak nyomást a kereskedelemre, a politikusokra, és végül a fogyasztók választását is hatékonyan befolyásolják. Ugyanakkor az állatjólléti előírásoknak kutatási eredményeken kellene alapulniuk, valóban az állatok jóllétét kellene szolgálniuk, figyelemmel a termelőre és a fogyasztói árakra is. A nyulak védelmére vonatkozó minimális követelményekről szóló, az Európai Parlament 2017. március 14-i állásfoglalásban (2016/2077 (INI)) rámutattak arra, hogy „számos szempont mérlegelésével egyensúlyt kell tartani, figyelemmel az állatok jóllétére és egészségére, a gazdálkodók pénzügyi helyzetére és munkakörülményekre, a termelés fenntarthatóságára, a környezeti hatásra és a fogyasztóvédelmre, továbbá a fogyasztók megfizethető, jó minőségű nyúlhús iránti igényére”. A házinyulak tartásával és jóllétével kapcsolatban számos tudományos cikk, irodalmi összefoglaló és tanulmány jelent meg (EFSA, 2005; Hoy és mtsai, 2006; Trocino és Xiccato, 2006; Verga és mtsai, 2009; Szendrő és Dalle Zotte, 2011; Szendrő és McNitt, 2012; Szendrő és mtsai, 2016; Hoy és Matics, 2016; González-Mariscal és mtsai, 2017; Turner és mtsai, 2017; Szendrő és mtsai, 2019). A fogyasztók igényeinek kielégítése érdekében az Európai Unió Tanácsa (EU Tanácsa) irányelveket adott ki a tenyésztés céljából tartott állatok védelméről (Council Directive 98/58/EC of 20 July 1998), a tojótyúkok, (Council Directive 1999/74/EC of 19 July 199), a hústermelés céljából tartott csirkék (Council Directive 2007/43/EC of 28 June 2007), a borjak (Council Directive 2008/119/EC of 18 December 2008), valamint a sertések védelmére vonatkozó minimális követelményekről (Council Directive 2008/120/EC of 18 December 2008). Az EU Tanácsa azonban még nem tett közzé semmilyen előírást a tenyésztett nyulak védelméről. Jelen cikk célja, hogy tudományos eredményekre alapozva foglalja össze a házinyulak nagyüzemi tartásának minimális követelményeit. Nemcsak a követelményeket írjuk le, hanem ahol van, röviden összefoglaljuk az adott terület irodalmi eredményeit, vagyis a javaslatok magyarázatát is. A minimális követelmények „A mezőgazdasági haszonállatok tartásának állatvédelmi szabályairól” szóló 32/1999. (III. 31.) FVM rendeletre, illetve annak módosított változataira (20/2002. (III. 14.) FVM rendelet, 72/2004. (IV. 29.) FVM rendelet, 178/2009. (XII. 29.) FVM rendelet) épülnek. Ugynakkor független a sokkal szigorúbb állatkísérletekről szóló 40/2013. (II. 14.) Korm. rendelettől.
Article
The impact of behavioural disorders on animal welfare in modern animal husbandry has been much debated. While other abnormal behaviours have been explored at length, there are a paucity of studies on tail-biting in rabbits ( Oryctolagus cuniculus ). In the present study, severe tail lesions were observed in group-reared rabbit does on a commercial rabbit farm. In the subsequent investigations, the occurrence of tail lesions in 219 rabbit does from nine batches was compared between group- or single-housing and a scoring system recording the severity of tail lesions was developed and verified. This five-grade scoring system was applied to evaluate the progression of prevalence and severity of tail lesions in 21 groups during rearing in two batches. The results revealed a significant difference in the score level between housing types with a higher prevalence of injured tails in group- (60.4%) compared to single-reared (4.0%) does. An increase in severity and frequency of tail lesions was observed in groups during the course of a rearing period. Furthermore, the established scoring system was characterised by adequate observer reliability. Overall, tail injuries occurred on a regular basis in the investigated rearing groups, indicating tail-biting to be a prevalent problem. This could be considered relevant in terms of animal welfare, both for the animal doing the biting and the individual being bitten. The findings draw attention to an inadequately described problem in rabbit husbandry. However, the search for preventive measures needs to scrutinise the role of single-housing, without failing to consider the gregarious nature of rabbits.
Article
Fattening rabbits are conventionally housed in 2 animals per cages, according to the local tradition. Presently in Europe no EU Directive exists on cage dimensions for these animals, while in some Countries local Codes (i.e.Ministry of Agriculture, U.K., 1987) indicate the housing dimensions for fattening rabbits (rabbits between 5 and 12 weeks of age: 0.07m2 pro capite; rabbits above 12 weeks of age: 0.18m2 pro capite in individual cages and rabbit between 5 and 12 weeks of age: 0.09m2 in colony cages). The conventional housing system may negatively affect animals' welfare, both for breeding and for fattening rabbits (Verga, 2000). No abnormal behaviours have been shown in group-housed females (Gunn, 1994) while they have been found in males, because they may become more aggressive when reaching sexual maturity (Hackness and Wagner, 1989)...
Article
The behavioural effects of providing male laboratory rabbits with one of four objects in their cage as environmental enrichment were investigated. A total of 60 New Zealand White rabbits housed individually in cages were used. The rabbits were assigned to one of 5 treatments by a random procedure; hay in a water bottle, grass-cubes, two gnawing sticks, a box, or nothing (controls). One week after purchase they were given their object and behavioural observations began. These were made by instantaneous recordings at 2 min intervals for one h/day and totaled 16 days. Rabbits given hay interacted more with their object than those given grass-cubes, gnawing sticks or a box (143, 58, 13 and 19 mean no. of rec. resp., p < 0.0001 for each treatment), they showed less abnormal behaviour, such as excessive fur-licking, sham chewing and bar-biting, than the control rabbits (50 vs. 97 mean no. of rec., p < 0.01), and they lay still less often than the control rabbits (179 vs. 256 mean no. of rec., p < 0.05). Rabbits given grass-cubes interacted more often with their object than those given gnawing sticks (p < 0.001) or a box (p < 0.01), and they showed less abnormal behaviour than the control rabbits (p < 0.001). Rabbits given gnawing sticks or a box interacted only rarely with their object, and they showed the same amount of abnormal behaviour as the control rabbits. The weight gain was higher for rabbits given grass-cubes (906 g, p < 0.001), but not for rabbits given hay (650 g), gnawing sticks (611 g) or a box (570 g) compared to the control rabbits (617 g). It is concluded that hay was the most effective of the tested objects in reducing abnormal behaviour and giving the individually housed rabbits some alternative occupation.
Article
In view of recent concerns regarding the current method of housing laboratory rabbits in the UK, the 24 h behaviour of 18 New Zealand White rabbits kept individually in standard laboratory cages (49 cm × 61 cm × 48 cm) was observed. In 24 h each rabbit was observed 288 times for 10 s and behaviour(s) was recorded using an ethogram. Results were analysed by calculating the percentage frequency of each behaviour for individual rabbits, adding these values together, and dividing by 18 to give a mean and standard error for the whole group. The most common behaviours were lie alert (23%), doze (20%), groom (10%), sleep (9%) and eat (8%). Overall, 56% of the rabbits' time was spent inactive. The level of mobility (1.2%) reflected their inability to hop normally and, in addition, they were unable to sit up, rear fully or stretch out because of spatial restriction. All rabbits showed stereotyped activities (11%) such as repetitive hair-chewing (4%), bar-chewing, head-swaying and pawing which indicated psychological problems, particularly in rabbits that were innately more active. Stereotypies and body maintenance activities were also performed at the expense of behavioural diversity. Male rabbits were significantly more likely to chin mark, while females showed a significantly higher level of hair-chewing and licking which could indicate social deprivation. In addition, this study introduces the cyclical pattern of activities throughout the day and night showing that rabbits are nocturnal. It also introduces the extent of boredom behaviours and cage frustration (stereotypies) over 24 h, providing a baseline for further research into improving the welfare of laboratory rabbits.
Article
Enhancing the psychological well-being of laboratory animals has received much attention recently. Although many studies have been undertaken to determine the effects of cage enrichment techniques on dogs and nonhuman primates, other than scant empirical observations, little has been done to measure these events objectively in lagomorphs. We studied adult female New Zealand White (NZW) rabbits to learn if, when given the opportunity, individual rabbits would use different enrichment objects placed in their cages, and to determine if rabbits preferred to be in proximity to one another, or apart. Three different objects were evaluated with eight rabbits individually housed in conventional cages. Each object introduced into individual rabbit cages stimulated substantial interaction, especially chewing behavior. Eight other rabbits were pair-housed in a modified caging system with a special access port between two separate cages. When given a choice, rabbits preferred to be in the same cage with other rabbits. In both studies, individual behaviors were monitored, as well as either the type of interaction and percentage of observations spent with each object or, in the housing study, percentage of observations involved with different types of activity, and relative location of the paired rabbits.
Article
Behaviour and use of the cage area were studied in 96 rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) kept in an enriched cage system - with access to shelter and raised height at the back of the cage - and in a conventional cage system to estimate the effects of the environmental enrichment on the rabbits' welfare. The rabbits' behaviour and placement in the cage were observed, using continuous video recording through 24 h and direct scan sampling during the daytime. In addition, an open-field test was carried out with each rabbit, and after every single test, the rabbits' timidity of being captured was recorded. Rabbits kept in the conventional cage system, especially the females, showed more restlessness, excessive grooming, bar-gnawing and timidity than rabbits kept in the enriched cage system. This indicates increased stress in the rabbits kept in the conventional cage system. All the rabbits performed most of the active behavioural elements in the daytime and were resting mostly at night that shows that the rabbits in both cage systems were adapted to the daily activity in the animal unit; the enrichment had no effect on the daytime activity.Only a few rabbits, particularly the females, used the box as a shelter or resting-place. On the other hand, they more often used the roof of the box as a look-out or resting-place. Furthermore, the rabbits' behaviour showed that they utilised the raised height in the enriched cage system.These results indicate that rabbits kept in an enriched cage system, particularly the females, had better welfare than rabbits kept in a conventional cage system because they had access to shelter and a better chance of interacting with the environment.