ArticlePDF Available

Effects of Perceived Fitness Level of Exercise Partner on Intensity of Exertion

Authors:

Abstract

Problem statement: Social comparison theory was used to examine if exercising with a research confederate posing as either high fit or low fit would increase the exertion in exercising. Approach: 91 college students were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: Biking alone, biking with a high fit confederate, or biking with a low fit confederate. All participants were instructed to complete 20 min of exercise at 60-70% of their maximum target heart rate. Results: Results indicated that participants in the high fit condition exercised harder than those in the low fit condition. However, no mood differences emerged between conditions. Conclusion: Social comparison theory predicts exercise outcome such that participants gravitate towards the behavior (high fit or low fit) of those around them.
Journal of Social Sciences 6 (1): 50-54, 2010
ISSN 1549-3652
© 2010 Science Publications
Corresponding Author: Thomas G. Plante, Department of Psychology, Alumni Science Hall, Room 203,
Santa Clara University, Santa Clara, CA 95053-0333
50
Effects of Perceived Fitness Level of Exercise Partner on
Intensity of Exertion
Thomas G. Plante, Meghan Madden, Sonia Mann, Grace Lee,
Allison Hardesty, Nick Gable, Allison Terry and Greg Kaplow
Department of Psychology, Alumni Science Hall, Room 203,
Santa Clara University, Santa Clara, CA 95053-0333
Abstract: Problem statement: Social comparison theory was used to examine if exercising with a
research confederate posing as either high fit or low fit would increase the exertion in exercising.
Approach: 91 college students were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: Biking alone,
biking with a high fit confederate, or biking with a low fit confederate. All participants were instructed
to complete 20 min of exercise at 60-70% of their maximum target heart rate. Results: Results
indicated that participants in the high fit condition exercised harder than those in the low fit condition.
However, no mood differences emerged between conditions. Conclusion: Social comparison theory
predicts exercise outcome such that participants gravitate towards the behavior (high fit or low fit) of
those around them.
Key words: Exercise, social comparison, perceived fitness, mood, exertion
INTRODUCTION
Research has demonstrated many physical and
psychological benefits of exercise including reduced
risks of cardiovascular disease, hypertension, diabetes,
cancer and obesity (Center for Disease Control and
Prevention, 2006; Blair et al., 1989; Brukner and
Brown, 2005; Byers et al., 2002; Pate et al., 1995;
Bryan et al., 2007; Morgan, 1985) as well as
psychological disturbances such as depression, anxiety
and stress disorders (Plante and Rodin, 1990; Plante,
1999; Kennedy and Newton, 1997). Researchers
generally agree that exercise provides many benefits for
both physical and mental health (Center for Disease
Control and Prevention, 2006; Blair et al., 1989;
Brukner and Brown, 2005; Byers et al., 2002; Pate et al.,
1995; Bryan et al., 2007; Morgan, 1985; Plante and
Rodin, 1990; Plante, 1999; Kennedy and Newton,
1997). Although research on the many benefits of
exercise is abundant, there is surprisingly little research
on the psychological and behavioral effects of
exercising with others. The limited available research
examining social exercise has demonstrated health and
mood advantages and disadvantages of exercising with
a partner (Plante et al., 2001; 2003).
Social comparison theory may offer a helpful
framework for understanding the effects of exercising
with others. The theory states that “humans have a drive
to assess how they are doing and in order to assess
how they are doing, they seek standards against which
to compare themselves. When objective standards are
not available, people look to their social environments
and engage in comparison with available others”
(Corning et al., 2006). Social comparison theory has
been applied to various research areas that may explain
why people are motivated to engage in health-
promoting or health damaging behaviors (Festinger,
1954).
For example, in an investigation on how social
influences encourage healthy behaviors such as
exercise, perceived behaviors of peers influenced the
behavior of others such that individuals were likely to
mimic the behavior of those around them (Festinger,
1954; Luszczyska et al., 2004). Individuals feel the
need to engage in socially acceptable behaviors, such as
exercising, when observing others doing the same
(Luszczyska et al., 2004).
Additional research demonstrates that mood and
energy levels are altered when exercising in the
presence of others or in front of a mirror. For example,
women who exercised with a partner or in front of a
mirror experienced an increased level of exhaustion and
decreases in feelings of revitalization while exercising
compared to women who exercised either alone or
without mirrors (Ginis et al., 2006). Although some
people may feel self-conscious exercising in the
J. Social Sci., 6 (1): 50-54, 2010
51
presence of others, research demonstrates that
individuals often engage in exercise for social
interaction. The likelihood of joining and staying
motivated during exercise increases when friends or
peers engage in those same activities (Faulkner et al.,
2008; Laverie, 1998).
In the present study, social comparison theory was
used to determine if exercising with someone perceived
to be either high or low in fitness would alter the
exercise experience and behavior of research
participants. In particular, we examined mood and the
level of exertion in subjects when exercising with
someone who the participant believed to possess either
a high or low level of fitness. We hypothesized that the
level of effort exerted by participants would match the
perceived fitness of their partner such that research
participants would mimic the exercise behavior of those
around them during exercise.
Exercise in this study was defined as biking either
alone or with a partner in one of three experimental
conditions. In two experimental conditions, the
participant exercised with a research confederate as
their partner. One of these conditions used a “high fit”
confederate, while the other condition used a “low fit”
confederate. In a third and control condition, the
participant exercised on a stationary bicycle alone. All
of the participants in the experiment completed the
same intensity and length of time of physical activity
required to meet the daily recommended criteria
suggested by the Center for Disease Control and
Prevention (2009).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants: The sample consisted of 91
undergraduate students at a West coast private
Catholic university (43 females, 48 males, M = 18.84
years, SD = 1.07). All subjects were enrolled in a
general psychology course and received research
participation credit. The project received approval from
the human subjects committee at the university where it
was conducted.
Measures:
Activation-Deactivation Adjective Check List (AD-
ACL) (Thayer, 1978; Thayer, 1986): The AD-ACL
is a brief, frequently used self-report checklist
designed to measure momentary mood states
associated with exercise with reported adequate
reliability and validity used in a number of
investigations involving exercise.
Perceived Exertion Scale (Borg scale, PES) (Borg,
1982): The PES was used to evaluate the participants’
perceived level of exertion where 6 = very light
exertion and 20 = very hard exertion. The PES is often
used in exercise research and has adequate reliability
and validity.
Paces Activity Enjoyment Scale (PACES)
(Kendzierski and DeCarlo, 1991): The PACES scale
includes18 bipolar items on which individuals rate
themselves on a 7 point Likert scale. The scale
measures the amount of enjoyment individuals perceive
themselves to have experienced during an exercise
activity. Sample scale items include “I find it
energizing/I find it tiring “and” I enjoy it/I hate it.” The
authors report that PACES has excellent internal
consistency, stability and validity.
Several researchers developed Likert scales: Several
10 point Likert scales developed by the authors
measuring each participant’s current level of perceived
stress, how participants felt while exercising next to
someone who was either posing as high fit or low fit.
The value of 1 indicated high stress, low enjoyment,
low level of comfort and feeling low fit while the value
of 10 indicated very relaxed, very enjoyable, very
comfortable and feeling very fit.
Procedure: Participants enrolled in the study to
complete a requirement for a general psychology
undergraduate class. On the day prior to their scheduled
laboratory session, participants received an email to
remind them of the experiment and confirmed their
appointment. The participants were told to wear
comfortable and exercise appropriate clothing.
Prior to beginning of the experiment, participants
reviewed and signed consent forms agreeing to
participate. Then, they were administered the pre-
exercise questionnaires and their height and weight was
recorded. The lab assistant then placed heart rate
monitors on the participants’ upper torsos and gave
them a wristwatch that displayed their heart rates.
Participants were randomly assigned to one of three
experimental conditions. The length and intensity of
exercise were the same and included biking for 20 min
on a stationary bike. Participants were instructed to
keep their heart rates at a moderate level of 60-70%
maximum heart rate (i.e., about 130 bpm for college
students). The conditions included: biking alone in a
control condition, biking with a same gender “high-fit”
confederate and biking with a same gender “low-fit”
confederate.
J. Social Sci., 6 (1): 50-54, 2010
52
For the purposes of this experiment we
operationally defined “high fit” as someone who wore
athletic clothing, exercised intensely and stated to the
experimenter in the presence of the research subject, “I
am so glad you had a fitness study, I love exercising”
while mounting the bike. A “low-fit” confederate wore
non-athletic gear (e.g., jeans, slippers), barely exerted
themselves and stated to the experimenter in the
presence of the research subject, “I don’t know why I
signed up for this experiment, I hate exercising” while
mounting the bike.
In all conditions, confederates entered the room
after the participant had arrived and asked if they were
in the right place for the fitness study. This was done to
minimize suspicions that confederates were research
assistants.
After exercising, the participant and the
confederate were asked to rate their perceived level of
exertion according to the PES/Borg Scale (Borg, 1982).
After completing the experiment, the experimenter
debriefed the participants and informed them about the
purpose of the study. The confederates’ identity was
revealed (i.e., that they were part of the experiment) and
participants were asked not to discuss the experiment
with others to avoid future possible participants from
learning about the purpose of the study. The
participants were thanked for their time and provided
with course credit.
RESULTS
A 2×3 Analysis Of Variance (ANOVA) was used
to analyze the data. The only exception was for the
measure of mood post-exercise scores where a 2×3
Analysis Of Covariance (ANCOVA) was used with pre
laboratory exercise mood scores used as covariates.
The manipulation check was successful in that
participants who exercised with high fit confederates
perceived them as high fit while participants who
exercised with low fit confederates perceived them as
low fit. Participants ranked confederates on a 10-point
scale where 10 was defined as being high fit and 1
was defined as being low fit. The average rating for
high fit confederate was 7.8 (SD = 1.2) while the
average rating of low fit confederates were 5.2
(SD = 1.3; F(2, 63) = 24.04, p<0.001).
Pulse rate and exertion results indicated that
participants in the high fit group had higher pulse rates
and worked harder than participants in the low fit and
control groups (F(2, 90) = 9.05, p<0.001). For example,
females who exercised with high fit confederates had an
average pulse rate of 133 beats min
1
(bpm) (SD = 24.35)
while females who exercised with low fit confederates
had an average pulse rate of 119 bpm (SD = 13.54).
Males who exercised with a high fit confederate had an
average pulse rate of 124 bpm (SD = 18.39) while
males who exercised with a low fit confederate had an
average pulse rate of 99 bmp (SD = 8.46). Additionally,
females in experimental groups worked harder and had
higher pulse rates than males, yielding a significant
gender interaction (F(1, 90) = 20.38, p<0.001).
Participants in experimental groups felt they
exerted themselves more than participants in control
groups (F(2, 91) = 3.42, p<0.05) and females ranked
their exertion levels higher than males (F (1, 91) = 4.38,
p<0.05).
A non significant trend demonstrated that
participants in the high fit conditions were the least
calm while those in the control group were the most
calm (F(2, 91) = 2.87, p = 0.063). Females in the high
fitness condition were the most uncomfortable and the
least calm (F(2, 91) = 3.90, p<0.05).
Interestingly, participants in the control group
enjoyed the exercise activity the most (F(2, 93) =
p<0.05) and reported being the most relaxed (F(2, 93) =
p<0.05).
Surprisingly, there were no significant main effects
or interactions for the measures of mood, tiredness,
tension and energy with all p’s>0.05.
DISCUSSION
Social comparison theory has been applied to a
variety of research areas such as perceived physical
appearance, wealth, success and failure (Buunk and
Gibbons, 1997). The goal of this study was to examine
how social comparisons might impact an individual’s
level of exertion and mood during an exercise routine
with a perceived high fit or low fit exercise partner. The
results were consistent with social comparison theory
predictions when applied to exercise outcome such that
participants gravitate towards the exercise behaviors of
those around them. Even when all participants,
regardless of experimental conditions, were instructed
to exercise at a moderate level and keep their pulse
rates within a particular range, they mimic the exercise
behavior of their exercise partner. Additionally,
individuals who exercised alone reported feeling calmer
and more relaxed in comparison to individuals who
exercised with a partner.
Implications of this research might suggest that
individuals attempting to exercise more intensely could
benefit by exercising next to someone they perceive to
be high fit. However, high fit individuals would likely
not receive those same benefits when working next to
someone lower in fitness. A less fit individual might
J. Social Sci., 6 (1): 50-54, 2010
53
influence a higher fit individual to exert themselves
less. Thus, a high fit individual might benefit more
from either exercising alone or exercising with another
high fit individual. Exercising with someone more fit
than oneself could promote a higher intensity workout
(Daley and Huffen, 2005). However, exercising alone
may prove to be more beneficial than exercising with
either a high or low fit individual when trying to secure
a relaxing exercise experience.
Results from the current study must be considered
cautiously. The sample consisted of a generally
homogeneous population of generally high fit and
healthy undergraduate students at a private university.
Furthermore, the sample size was small (n = 91) and the
findings may have occurred due to unknown factors.
The lab setting of this experiment may not generalize to
the real world such as a fitness club or exercise gym
experience. Health clubs often have a variety of fitness
levels represented, have music, mirrors and other types
of cues that differ from a university laboratory setting.
Curiously, no group differences were found while
measuring mood. One might expect that mood would
be impacted by the experience but results showed that it
was not in this study. Finally, in examining our
manipulation check regarding the difference in
perceived fitness level of the confederate subjects, the
findings were modest suggesting that perhaps
participants experienced the high fit participant as high
fit but the low fit participant as being moderately fit.
CONCLUSION
Future research should further investigate the
effects of social comparison theory on exercise
behaviors. It would be useful to use a heterogeneous
sample with individuals ranging in fitness levels. It
would also be useful to repeat this study in a more real
world setting, such as an exercise gym or health club.
REFERENCES
Blair,
S.N., H.W. Kohl, R.S.
Paffenbarger, D.G. Clarkand
and K.H. Cooper et al., 1989. Physical fitness and
all-cause mortality. A prospective study of healthy
men and women.
J. Am. Med. Assoc., 262: 2394-2401
.
DOI:10.1001/JAMA.262.17.2395
Borg, G.V., 1982. Perceived exertion scale. Med. Sci.
Sports Exerc., 14: 377-381. PMID: 7154893
Brukner, P.D. and W.J. Brown, 2005. Is exercise good
for you? Med. J. Aust., 183: 538-541. PMID:
16296971
Bryan, A., K.E. Hutchison, D.R. Seals and D.L. Allen,
2007. A transdisciplinary model integrating
genetic, physiological and psychological correlates
of voluntary exercise. Health Psychol., 26: 30-39.
DOI: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.30
Buunk, B.P. and F.X. Gibbons, 1997. Health, Coping
and Well-Being: Perspectives from Social
Comparison Theory. Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, New Jersey. ISBN: 0805818588.
Byers, T., M. Nestle, A. Mctiernan, C. Doyke and
A. Currrie-Williams et al., 2002. American cancer
society guidelines on nutrition and physical activity
for cancer prevention: Reducing the risk of cancer
with healthy food choices and physical activity.
CA: Cancer J. Clin., 52: 92-119. DOI:
10.3322/CANJCLIN.52.2.92
Center for Disease Control and Prevention 2006.
Physical Activity for Everyone: Recommendations:
How active do adults need to be to gain some
benefit?
http://www.cdc.gov/nccdphp/dnpaphysical/recom
mendations/adults.htm
Center for Disease Control and Prevention, 2009.
Physical activity for everyone: Recommendations:
How active do adults need to be to gain some
benefit?
http://www.cdc.gov/nccdphp/dnpaphysical/recom
mendations/adults.htm
Corning, A.F., A.J. Krumm, J. Angela and L.A. Smitham
,
2006. Differential social comparison processes in
women with and without eating disorder
symptoms. J. Couns.
Psychol., 53: 338-349
.
DOI:10.1037/0022-0167.53.3.338
Daley, A.J. and C. Huffen, 2005. The effects of low and
moderate intensity exercise on subjective
experiences in a naturalistic health and fitness club
setting. J. Health Psychol., 8: 685-691. DOI:
10.1177/13591053030086003
Faulkner, J., G. Parfitt and R. Eston, 2008. The rating
of perceived exertion during competitive running
scales with time. Psychophysiology, 45: 977-985.
DOI: 10.1111/J.1469-8986.2008.00712.X
Festinger, L., 1954. A theory of social comparison
processes. Hum. Relat., 7: 117-140. DOI:
10.1177/001872675400700202
Ginis, K.A., S.M. Burke and L. Gauvin, 2006.
Exercising with others exacerbates the negative
effects of mirrored environments on sedentary
women’s feeling
states. Psychol. Health, 22: 945-962.
DOI
:
10.1080/14768320601070571
Kendzierski, D. and K.J. DeCarlo, 1991. Physical
activity enjoyment scale: Two validation studies. J.
Sport Exerc. Psychol., 13: 60-64.
J. Social Sci., 6 (1): 50-54, 2010
54
Kennedy, M.N. and M. Newton, 1997. Effect of
exercise intensity on mood in step aerobics. J.
Sports Med. Phys., 37: 200-204. PMID: 9407751
Laverie, D.A., 1998. Motivations for ongoing
participation in a fitness activity. Leisure Sci.,
20: 277-302. DOI: 10.1080/01490409809512287
Luszczyska, A., F.X. Gibbons, B.F. Piko and M. Tekozel,
2004. Social comparison and perceived peers
behaviors as predictors of nutrition and physical
activity: A comparison among adolescents in
Hungary, Turkey, Poland and USA. Psychol.
Health, 19: DOI: 10.1080/0887044042000205844
Morgan, W.P., 1985. Psychogenic factors and exercise
metabolism: A review. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.,
17: 309-316. PMID: 3894867
Pate, R.R., M. Pratt, S.N. Blair, W.L. Haskell and
C.A. Macera et al. 1995. Physical activity and
public health. A recommendation from the centers
for disease control and prevention and the
American college of sports medicine. J. Am. Med.
Assoc., 273: 402-407. DOI:
10.1001/JAMA.273.5.402
Plante, T.G. and J. Rodin, 1990. Physical fitness and
enhanced psychological health. Curr. Psychol. Res.
Rev., 9: 3-24. DOI: 10.1007/BF02686764
Plante, T.G., 1999. Could the perception of fitness
account for many of the mental and physical
health benefits of exercise. Adv. Mind-Body
Med., 15: 291-301. PMID: 10555401
Plante, T.G., L. Coscarelli and M. Ford, 2001. Does
exercising with another enhance the stress-reducing
benefits
of exercise? Int. J. Stress Manage., 8: 201-213
.
DOI: 10.1023/A:1011339025532
Plante, T.G., R. Bogdan, Z. Kanani, M. Babula and
E. Ferlic et al., 2003. Psychological benefits of
exercising with another. J. Hum. Move. Stud.
44: 93-106. DOI: 10.1037/1072-5245.14.1.88
Thayer, R.E., 1978. Factor analytic and reliability
studies on the activation deactivation adjective
check list. Psychol. Rep., 42: 747-756. PMID:
674499
Thayer, R.E., 1986. Activation-deactivation adjective
check list: Current overview and structural
analysis. Psychol. Rep., 58: 607-614.
... According to the social comparison theory, training with a partner is perceived as competition accompanied by supervision [14], which directly and positively impacts the partner since it increases enthusiasm and improves athletic performance [15,16]. Plante et al. (2010) reported that individuals who exercised with a highly fit partner exercised harder than those who exercised alone [17]. Studies have also shown that competitiveness can increase training intensity and that training with peers under the influence of competitiveness can enhance athletic performance [18,19]. ...
... According to the social comparison theory, training with a partner is perceived as competition accompanied by supervision [14], which directly and positively impacts the partner since it increases enthusiasm and improves athletic performance [15,16]. Plante et al. (2010) reported that individuals who exercised with a highly fit partner exercised harder than those who exercised alone [17]. Studies have also shown that competitiveness can increase training intensity and that training with peers under the influence of competitiveness can enhance athletic performance [18,19]. ...
... However, it is important to acknowledge that there is a gap in existing research regarding PT versus EP. Thus, the relative advantages of PT versus EP remain uncertain [7,8,17]. The emphasis of previous studies on PT-guided training has led to discrepancies in training intensity and athletic performance. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to examine the effects of supervised fitness training under the guidance of a personal trainer and those of competitive fitness training with others and reveal the effects of specific differences between them in a detailed manner. The study's participants consisted of 66 healthy male adults (age: 29.2 ± 5.4 years). The participants were divided into three groups: the individual training group (n = 21), which served as the control group; the exercising with a partner group (n = 22); and the group trained by a personal trainer (n = 23). Each participant was subsequently assessed using one repetition maximum bench press, squats, skeletal muscle mass, fat mass, and a questionnaire regarding nutritional plan and injury to compare the effects of training sessions over a period of 12 weeks. Among the three groups, only the group trained by a personal trainer showed an obvious enhancement in fat reduction compared to baseline (-1.61 kg, p = 0.033), which was suggestive of a salient trend that far surpassed those of the individual training group and the exercising with a partner group. Regarding squats, only the group trained by a personal trainer showed a significant change compared to the individual training group (p = 0.003). Regarding the participants' consistent use of a nutritional plan, only the group trained by a personal trainer exhibited a palpable tendency (p < 0.001); furthermore, the effect of preventing injury in the group trained by a personal trainer was more notable than that in the individual training group and the exercising with a partner group. Our results indicate that a fitness personal trainer service is effective in expediting the process of achieving fitness goals in a relatively safe manner, thereby substantiating the diversified values of the fitness personal trainer service.
... Components like gender, age, training age, sporting background, competitiveness and motivation have been offered as potentially influential factors thought to influence the proclivity to train either independently or in a group in the gym environment (Weinberg and Gould, 2023) and thereby formed the basis of our enquiry. Plante et al. (2010) found insufficient evidence in support of motivational levels as a significant factor influencing training modality in terms of solo or group exercise. Thinking of motivation in terms of perseverance and adherence, however, Radhakrishnan et al. (2020) found in their analysis of data logs pertaining to over 6000 gym-users that engaging in group exercise at the gym led to a lower "ceasing" rate than in those exercising individually. ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper presents a study of exercise behaviours among Maltese gym users where it seeks to understand what factors govern choices among gym users to exercise either alone or in a group. It is argued that being able to predict the propensity to exercise alone or in a group presents an opportunity for fitness providers to organise their efforts more efficiently in the broader context of contributing to improving public health and, therefore, possibly providing a more relevant gym product. Exercise and fitness literature highlights various factors likely to influence participants' proclivities to exercise solo or in a group, including motivation, competitiveness, sporting background, age, and gender. To explore the possible influences of such factors in the Maltese context, a cross-sectional survey was carried out via an online questionnaire, which was analysed statistically using logistic regression. This was conducted by first incorporating the above factors with a propensity to exercise alone or in a group as the dependent binary outcome. Finally, a most parsimonious model was applied following a stepwise iterated process of factor elimination. The results show that gender and age were the most significant predictors of training modality, with being male increasing the likelihood of engaging in solo gym exercise behaviour by over five times. Increased age was also a significant predictor of exercising alone. We present a discussion of the findings in terms of exercise and fitness service provision, as well as broader issues related to the interaction between gym culture and gender norms.
... Çoğu kişi (popülasyonun %96'sı) normal dağılım eğrisinin sağa ve sola doğru ikişer standart sapma sınırı içinde yer alır, aptal ya da çıkıntı ya da anormal görünmek istemez. Plante (2010) bu bilişsel yanlılıktan faydalanarak bir deney tasarlamıştır: 91 üniversite öğrencisi rastgele üç koşuldan birine atanmıştır (a) tek başına bisiklete binme, (b) çok fit bir kişiyle bisiklete binme (c) az fit bir kişiyle bisiklete binme. Tüm katılımcılara maksimum hedef kalp atış hızlarının %60-70'inde 20 dakikalık egzersizi tamamlamaları talimatı verilmiştir. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Gelecekte, kaçınılmaz olarak tüm dünyada hükümetlerin en üst düzeylerinde davranış biliminin çok daha fazla kullanıldığına şahit olunacaktır. Peki bu alanda öncü kimdir? 2002’de Daniel Kahneman zihinsel kısa yolları anlatarak davranışsal ekonomi alanında Nobel İktisat ödülü almıştır. 2017 yılında ise Richard Thaler ve Cass Sunstein bu kısa yolları baz alıp “insanları dürterek davranışlarını şekillendirmenin” kitabını yazmış, hatta Thaler bu buluşla Nobel ödülü kazanmıştır. Dürtmenin bu kadar önemsenmesinin nedeni tüm toplum davranışlarını belirleyebilecek deneyci bir yaklaşım olmasına dayalıdır. “Seçim mimarisi” terimi nudge ile eşleşmiş durumdadır. Anlamı, insanları önceden hedeflenen seçimleri yapmaya dürtecek şekilde ortamı tasarlamaktır. Liberal yönetimler bireylere yapacakları seçimleri dayatıyormuş gibi görünmeden düşük maliyetle politika hedeflerine ulaşabilmek için seçim mimarisinden yararlanmayı etkili bulmakta ve bunu geniş çapta uygulamaktadır. Bu akımın temelinde politikaların hümanist ve kanıta dayalı evrilmesi yatmaktadır.
... Exercising in a group can be motivating as supportive peer relationships can encourage adherence [49]. From a social psychology perspective, people are drawn to the exercise habits of those around them [50]. Research shows that social support can motivate individuals to adopt healthier habits, such as better medication compliance and a higher propensity to seek out medical care [51]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Exergames have the potential to reduce sedentary behavior and motivate physical activity. However, they suffer from retention problems mainly because the level of interest declines over time. In this paper, we report on the results of a social exergame prototype called Garden Quest. The game implements gamification elements based on the Hexad player type framework that have been shown to motivate players. Before the development of a full game, we designed wireframes of the game interface, followed by an interactive prototype, and conducted a usability test and heuristic evaluation with six experts. We present initial results showing that the user interface is usable and simple, and the overall system is persuasive. Based on the collected results, we plan to revise the prototype and perform a second round of evaluation before moving on to the development and field evaluation of the game with a large sample.
... When it comes to well-being in the context of the workplace, however, organizations and managers have a moral responsibility to make this more achievable. Fortunately, individuals tend to be more motivated to engage in PA when it occurs in a group context (Plante et al., 2010). Including team PA in the team charter as a criterion for productive teamwork (Mathieu & Rapp, 2009) might be pragmatic. ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this article is to conceptualize a novel theoretical occurrence—team physical activity (PA)—and its relevance for researchers and organizations. By building a testable model of the consequences and contingencies of team PA, we integrate the science of teamwork with the scholarly domain of employee health and well-being. Hence, we clarify the construct of team PA, present a three-dimensional typology, and outline a model drawing on neuroscience, positive organizational behavior, and teams research. Our propositions and subsequent discussion proffer an outline of potential benefits for organizations when they increase the utility and frequency of team PA. We also suggest ways in which researchers can advance scholarship in this area.
... The social environment refers to social aspects of the environment, such as whether or not the person is engaging in PA with someone else. It has been shown that engaging in PA with a partner is positively associated with PA behavior (Dunton et al., 2009;Gellert et al., 2011;Giles-Corti & Donovan, 2002;Plante et al., 2010), intentions to engage in PA (Rackow et al., 2014;Fox et al., 2000), and motivation for PA (Granner et al., 2007;Burke et al., 2006). In addition to the nature of the physical and social environment, one may perceive physical and social aspects of the environment to be more or less positive versus negative. ...
Article
Full-text available
Perceptions of the physical and social environment have been shown to be predictive of physical activity (PA) behavior. However, the mechanisms of this association have not been examined. Affective response to PA was examined as a putative mediator of the association between perceptions of the PA environment and subsequent PA behavior. As part of a PA promotion study, 59 low-active overweight or obese but otherwise healthy adults completed real-time assessments of the perceived physical and social PA environment, affective response to PA, and PA behavior over a 6-month period. As hypothesized, decreased latency to and greater duration of subsequent PA was predicted by engaging in PA with a partner (b = 17.24, SE = .45, p < .01), engaging in PA outdoors versus indoors (b = 3.70, SE = 0.67, p < .01), and perceived pleasantness of the physical (b = 0.59, SE = .17, p < .01) and social settings (b = 0.68, SE = .16, p < .01). Affective response to PA (a shift toward feeling good versus bad during PA) mediated the association between engaging in PA with a partner (a path: 0.53(.11), p < .01, b path: 0.42(.12), p < .01, ab path: 0.22(.08), 95% CI .09–.41) and perceived pleasantness of the physical (a path: .38(.02), p < .01; b path: .65(.23), p = .01; ab path: .25(.09), 95% CI .08–.43) and social setting (a path: .35(.02), p < .01; b path: .57(.23), p = .01; ab path: .20(.08), 95% CI .03–.37) and PA behavior, but not the association between engaging in PA outdoors versus indoors and PA behavior. These findings suggest that perceived environmental variables may have their effects on PA through the process of psychological hedonism.
Article
Full-text available
VR exergaming is a promising motivational tool to incentivise exercise. We present a novel VR exergaming method called social interactive feedforward. The player competes with an 'enhanced model' of one of their friends in a real-time VR environment, showing improved performance levels in a way the player can relate to. Social interactive feedforward was tested in a cycling-based VR exergame and players competed with enhanced models of themselves, their friend, and a stranger moving at the same enhanced pace. Results show that friend-modelled social interactive feedforward improves performance and intrinsic motivation the most. This indicates that the mere association of the enhanced model with their friend results in a rapid improvement in performance and motivation which implies that social feedforward was successfully elicited by using an enhanced friend's model. This widens the application of self-modelled feedforward to a wide range of social options which enables players to also reap the benefits of socialising in addition to feedforward benefits.
Article
Full-text available
Engaging in regular moderate-to-vigorous intensity physical activity (MVPA) is crucial to reduce future health risk for individuals living with the effects of a stroke and their partners. Although numerous studies point to the importance of social factors in physical activity engagement, little is known about with whom individuals after stroke and their partners engage in physical activity with and whether different physical activity companions are uniquely associated with MVPA. Eighty-nine community-dwelling individuals after stroke (Mage = 68.64, SD = 10.44; 74% male) and 83 partners (Mage = 66.04, SD = 9.91; 24% male) completed 14 consecutive days of daily life assessments that included wearing physical activity monitors (accelerometers) and self-reporting physical activity companions (n = 1,961 days). Results show that average levels of MVPA were correlated between partners (r = 0.38), as were day-to-day MVPA fluctuations (r = 0.34). Importantly, for individuals after stroke, being active with their partner, but not with any other physical activity companion, was linked with elevated daily MVPA. In contrast, for partners of individuals after stroke, engaging in physical activity with a variety of different companions (partner, other family member, friend, colleague) was each associated with higher MVPA in daily life. For both individuals after stroke and their partners being active by oneself (without a companion) on a given day was not associated with elevated MVPA. Findings suggest that interventions that promote physical activity engagement should consider the role of meaningful others, with the partner being particularly key for individuals living with chronic health conditions.
Article
Background Bike share has been hypothesized to be a phenomenon of social contagion. The current study describes the features of the social network of annual subscribers of the Citi Bike share system in NYC and the characteristics of those who ever took a trip with another member. Methods Associations comparing members who rode with others (social) to those who rode alone (non-social) on selected study variables were examined. Social network analysis was performed to examine characteristics of the network of Citi Bike members. A social trip was determined based on start and end trip time and station locations. Results There were 241,340 active Citi Bike members with at least 365 days of membership between May 13, 2013 and December 31, 2018. Those who had taken at least one social trip (n = 139,302, 57.7%) were more likely to be older; have higher household income; have ridden a bike in NYC 12 months before membership; not own a car; self-report as physically active; self-report good health; live in a ZCTA with a Citi Bike station; and list primary reasons for membership as a faster way to get around town or as friends and family joining. Those who never took a social trip were more likely to have requested safety tips from Citi Bike; and to report their primary reason for membership as an alternative to public transportation, personal health and fitness, or support for the program. The social network of Citi Bike members was composed of 139,302 members with 164,735 unique connections to other members. The average number of connections per social member was 2.4 and the maximum was 66. The network was not very dense or well connected with 19,809 subnetworks. Conclusion Social members differed from non-social members in several ways. Understanding the characteristics of key network members could play a part in promoting and encouraging bike share usage. In particular, organized social trips could increase bike share usage among women.
Article
Objectives: The objective of this review was to pool and rank the efficacy of lifestyle intervention strategies targeting weight, body mass index, waist circumference and waist-to-hip ratio in people with psychosis by comparing the effect size of these weight outcomes. Secondary to this, the objective was to stratify the lifestyle interventions according to their inclusion of dietary information that adheres to Australian Dietary Guidelines. Introduction: People living with psychosis have a significantly increased risk of all-cause mortality, with cardiovascular disease a considerable contributor to this risk. Controlling lifestyle risk factors, which include smoking, poor diet and inadequate physical activity, leads to significant weight reduction and decreases cardiovascular disease risk. Previous reviews on this topic have not clearly identified essential components of lifestyle interventions in people with psychosis, mainly due to statistical limitations of analyses. This review employed the network meta-analysis, which compares more than two groups of interventions and ranks them according to efficacy, thus providing a global estimate of effect. Additionally, available reviews have not assessed compliance of dietary information offered in lifestyle interventions to established guidelines. Inclusion criteria: This review considered randomized controlled studies that delivered lifestyle interventions to community-dwelling adults with psychotic disorders. Outcomes of interest were weight, body mass index, waist circumference and waist-to-hip ratio. Methods: The Cochrane Library, MEDLINE/PreMEDLINE, Embase, CINAHL, Scopus and PsycINFO were searched for studies published in English from 1985 to June 2018. Data were qualitatively summarized, during which lifestyle intervention subgroups were created (based on key similarities) and then compared in direct meta-analyses and network meta-analyses. Assessments of study adherence to Australian Dietary Guidelines was conducted in a narrative format. Results: Thirty-two randomized control studies were included, and the overall quality of these studies ranged from what appeared to be low to moderate. Lifestyle intervention studies contained both a dietary and physical activity component, with the exception of two studies that focused solely on physical activity. Delivery of dietary and physical activity information was mainly through education; however, some studies provided additional structure to the intervention by offering tailored advice or helping participants to set goals, and providing regular review of progress for diet, physical activity or both. Results from network-meta-analyses showed that only studies with a structured approach for both diet and physical activity demonstrated significant decreases in weight (effect size = -4.12, 95% confidence interval = -7.772 to -2.760, P = 0.000) and body mass index (effect size = -2.94, 95% confidence interval = -1.78 to -0.357, P = 0.003). Waist circumference subgroup comparisons mainly comprised single studies; therefore, findings are inconclusive. Dietary information provided in studies generally complied with Australian Dietary Guidelines; however, none of the studies complied with all guidelines. Conclusions: Lifestyle interventions incorporating both dietary and physical activity components led to the greatest decreases in weight (4.1 kg) and body mass index (2.9 points) among people with psychosis. Important intervention strategies for both components are the personalization of education through tailored advice or goal setting, and a corresponding progress review. Dietary information in the included studies appeared to comply with the Australian Dietary Guidelines. However, these findings were weakened by an increased risk of bias, complex and multicomponent study designs, and lack of clarity in reporting of study methodology.
Article
Full-text available
This study sought to determine whether exercising with another person improves the psychological benefits of exercise relative to exercising alone. One hundred and fifty-five participants completed a series of questionnaires measuring levels of tension, calmness, energy and tiredness before exercise, immediately following exercise and later that day before bedtime. Participants exercised on a stationary laboratory bicycle for 30min either alone, with another person of the same sex or with a person of the opposite sex. Results suggest that exercising alone or with others did not differentially impact upon mood. All groups equally experienced mood benefits after exercising. Depression was found to be positively correlated with perceived exertion and women were found to report greater exertion when exercising alone than when exercising in mixed pairs.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: To encourage increased participation in physical activity among Americans of all ages by issuing a public health recommendation on the types and amounts of physical activity needed for health promotion and disease prevention. Participants: A planning committee of five scientists was established by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American College of Sports Medicine to organize a workshop. This committee selected 15 other workshop discussants on the basis of their research expertise in issues related to the health implications of physical activity. Several relevant professional or scientific organizations and federal agencies also were represented. Evidence: The panel of experts reviewed the pertinent physiological, epidemiologic, and clinical evidence, including primary research articles and recent review articles. Consensus process: Major issues related to physical activity and health were outlined, and selected members of the expert panel drafted sections of the paper from this outline. A draft manuscript was prepared by the planning committee and circulated to the full panel in advance of the 2-day workshop. During the workshop, each section of the manuscript was reviewed by the expert panel. Primary attention was given to achieving group consensus concerning the recommended types and amounts of physical activity. A concise "public health message" was developed to express the recommendations of the panel. During the ensuing months, the consensus statement was further reviewed and revised and was formally endorsed by both the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American College of Sports Medicine. Conclusion: Every US adult should accumulate 30 minutes or more of moderate-intensity physical activity on most, preferably all, days of the week.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated whether the effects of self-regulatory cognitions and social influence variables on healthy behaviors – nutrition and physical activity – vary across countries. Adolescents (N = 2387) from Hungary, Poland, Turkey, and the USA participated in the study. Measures included self-efficacy, future orientation, social comparison orientation (SCO), perceived behaviors of peers, as well as age and gender. These variables were included in the path model as predictors of healthy behaviors. The role of a country as a moderator was also examined. Results showed that self-efficacy, SCO, and perceived behaviors of peers predicted both health-promoting behaviors in all four countries. Some differences were found regarding the role of future orientation and gender.
Article
Full-text available
On the basis of predictions from social comparison theory (L. Festinger, 1954) and informed by findings from the social comparison and eating disorder literatures, hypotheses were tested regarding the social comparison behaviors of women with eating disorder symptoms and their asymptomatic peers. Results indicated differentiating social-cognitive processes for these groups. First, a greater tendency to engage in everyday social comparison predicted the presence of eating disorder symptoms. Second, social comparisons of one's own body to images of other women's bodies using a range of shapes and sizes also differentiated these 2 groups, with more self-defeating self-appraisals predicting the presence of eating disorder symptoms. Finally, self-esteem partially mediated the relationship between body-related social comparisons and eating disorder symptom status. Results are discussed in terms of their implications for research and practice. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Two studies examined the reliability and validity of the Physical Activity Enjoyment Scale (PACES). In Study 1, each of 37 undergraduates rode an exercise bicycle under control and external focus conditions. As predicted, Ss reported enjoying the exercise more, as measured by the PACES, in the external focus condition. Moreover, there was a significant negative correlation in the control condition between Ss PACES scores and their scores on a measure of boredom proneness. In Study 2, each of 37 undergraduates rode an exercise bicycle and jogged on a minitrampoline in separate sessions; each then chose one of these activities for their 3rd session. As predicted, there was a significant relationship between Ss PACES ratings (completed after each activity) and their choices of activity. Test–retest reliability was high for jogging and moderate for bicycling. The PACES had high internal consistency in both studies. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Article
What factors motivate individuals to participate in sport or fitness activities? This question has been of great interest to researchers. However, most studies on motives have considered only a single or a few variables in their investigation. The purpose of this research is to explore the wide range of factors that motivate people to participate in a fitness activity, namely aerobics. To take into account the broad range of individual and social factors that influence participation in a fitness activity, social identity theory is the basis for a qualitative investigation. The findings that show motives for participation are divergent across groups of similar individuals. The motives that emerged were (a) the atmosphere of an aerobics class, (b) the physical and psychological benefits, (c) social ties related to aerobics, (d) social comparisons, (e) obsession with aerobics, and (f) the feelings participants associate with doing aerobics. A proposed framework of fitness participation is developed based on the findings that could serve as a foundation for future empirical studies.
Article
The unique and interactive effects of mirrored exercise environments and the presence of co-exercisers on sedentary women's exercise-induced feeling states (FS) were examined. Participants (n = 92; mean age = 20.2) performed 20 min of moderate intensity exercise in one of four environments: (a) alone/mirrored, (b) not alone/mirrored, (c) alone/unmirrored, or (d) not alone/unmirrored. FS were measured pre-, mid-, and 5 min post-exercise. Self-consciousness, perceived social evaluation and social comparisons were also assessed post-exercise. Multilevel modeling procedures indicated that women in the not alone/mirrored environment experienced smaller increases in post-exercise revitalization than the other conditions (p
Article
Presents a description of the theory and research underlying the present author's (see record 1979-30156-001) Activation–Deactivation Adjective Check List (AD ACL) and describes a study involving 453 undergraduates that investigated the stability of factor structure of this test as a function of different rating-scale formats. The 2 core dimensions, energetic arousal (including tiredness) and tense arousal (including calmness), are believed associated with a variety of arousal-related characteristics, including physiological changes, sleep–wake cycles, exercise effects, various mood states, and various concomitants of stress. Analyses indicated that the factor structure of activation descriptors remained essentially the same with each scale. The importance of the underlying arousal model in relation to the activation descriptors is discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The specific goal of this volume is to bring together various perspectives and approaches that share a common theme: applying social comparison theory to health-related issues. It also illustrates the broad range of health-related issues to which social comparison theory is applied. Some of these issues and topics are health protective behaviors, such as condom use, and health impairing behaviors, such as smoking and drinking; perceptions of risk; coping with serious diseases, such as cancer and chronic pain disorders; preparation for surgery and postoperative recovery; stress coping during adolescence; seeking medical care; occupational stress and burnout; and depression and well-being. We are hopeful that the present volume will contribute not only to the continued development of health psychology but to the evolution of social comparison theory as well. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)