ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract and Figures

Duncan et al. (2007) presented a new methodology for identifying kindergarten readiness factors and quantifying their importance by determining which of children's developing skills measured around kindergarten entrance would predict later reading and math achievement. This article extends Duncan et al.'s work to identify kindergarten readiness factors with 6 longitudinal data sets. Their results identified kindergarten math and reading readiness and attention as the primary long-term predictors but found no effects from social skills or internalizing and externalizing behavior. We incorporated motor skills measures from 3 of the data sets and found that fine motor skills are an additional strong predictor of later achievement. Using one of the data sets, we also predicted later science scores and incorporated an additional early test of general knowledge of the social and physical world as a predictor. We found that the test of general knowledge was by far the strongest predictor of science and reading and also contributed significantly to predicting later math, making the content of this test another important kindergarten readiness indicator. Together, attention, fine motor skills, and general knowledge are much stronger overall predictors of later math, reading, and science scores than early math and reading scores alone.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Fine Motor Skills and Early Comprehension of the World:
Two New School Readiness Indicators
David Grissmer
University of Virginia Kevin J. Grimm
University of California, Davis
Sophie M. Aiyer and William M. Murrah
University of Virginia Joel S. Steele
University of California, Davis
Duncan et al. (2007) presented a new methodology for identifying kindergarten readiness factors and
quantifying their importance by determining which of children’s developing skills measured around
kindergarten entrance would predict later reading and math achievement. This article extends Duncan et
al.’s work to identify kindergarten readiness factors with 6 longitudinal data sets. Their results identified
kindergarten math and reading readiness and attention as the primary long-term predictors but found no
effects from social skills or internalizing and externalizing behavior. We incorporated motor skills
measures from 3 of the data sets and found that fine motor skills are an additional strong predictor of later
achievement. Using one of the data sets, we also predicted later science scores and incorporated an
additional early test of general knowledge of the social and physical world as a predictor. We found that
the test of general knowledge was by far the strongest predictor of science and reading and also
contributed significantly to predicting later math, making the content of this test another important
kindergarten readiness indicator. Together, attention, fine motor skills, and general knowledge are much
stronger overall predictors of later math, reading, and science scores than early math and reading scores
alone.
Keywords: school readiness, fine motor skills, general knowledge, achievement
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020104.supp
Duncan et al. (2007) presented a new methodology for identi-
fying school readiness factors and quantifying their importance by
determining which of children’s developing skills measured
around kindergarten entrance would predict much later reading
and math achievement. This research utilized six international
longitudinal data sets that collected data between birth and kin-
dergarten entry and followed children at least through third grade.
School readiness factors tested in Duncan et al.’s analysis included
measures of early reading and math, attention, internalizing and
externalizing behavior, and social skills. Their regressions in-
cluded as controls a large set of diverse family and child character-
istics measured from birth to kindergarten entry. A meta-analysis of
their results suggested that measures of early math and reading and
attention were significant predictors of later math and reading
achievement and that internalizing and externalizing behavior and
social skills were not significant predictors. Earlier math measures
predicted both later math and reading scores, but early reading mea-
sures predicted only later reading scores. Early math scores predicted
later reading as strongly as early reading scores.
The results suggest that early math skills should receive more
attention in research and curriculum because they predict both later
math and reading scores. The results also suggest that not all
socioemotional skills may be equal in boosting later achievement.
Attention measures appear to be consistently linked to later scores,
but social skills and internalizing and externalizing behavior were
not. Duncan et al. (2007) provided several caveats for these find-
ings. First, the results measured only the association with later
achievement, and including a broader range of long-term devel-
opmental outcomes could show different results. Second, socio-
emotional behavior may affect other students’ achievement more
than one’s own achievement. Third, problem behaviors may
emerge more strongly after school entry, so later measures may
show different results. For instance, class sizes are smaller in
kindergarten compared with later grades, and problem behavior
may be more frequent when children enter larger classes where
teachers have less control.
David Grissmer, Sophie M. Aiyer, William M. Murrah, Center for the
Advanced Study of Teaching and Learning, University of Virginia; Kevin
J. Grimm and Joel S. Steele, Department of Psychology, University of
California, Davis.
This research was supported by National Science Foundation Research
and Evaluation on Education in Science and Engineering Program Grant
(DRL-0815787) and the National Center for Research on Early Childhood
Education, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education
(R305A06021). The opinions and views expressed in this article are those
of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views and opinions of
the U.S. Department of Education.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to David
Grissmer, Center for the Advanced Study of Teaching and Learning,
University of Virginia, 350 Old Ivy Way, Suite 100, Charlottesville, VA
22903. E-mail: dwg7u@virginia.edu
Developmental Psychology © 2010 American Psychological Association
2010, Vol. 46, No. 5, 1008–1017 0012-1649/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0020104
1008
The main objectives of this article are threefold: (a) provide new
empirical evidence that fine motor skills, a developmental skill
measured at school entry but not included in Duncan’s et al.’s
(2007) analysis, is strongly predictive of later scores, (b) present
several sensitivity analyses that extend Duncan et al.’s findings,
including assessing the predictive power of a child’s knowledge of
the world, and (c) review the developmental and neuroscience
literature to assess and suggest mechanisms for a link between
early motor skills and later achievement. For the first objective, we
used three longitudinal data sets that measured early fine motor
skills and were also used in Duncan et al. We added these mea-
sures to similarly specified and estimated equations to test whether
fine motor skills were predictive of later achievement and, if so, its
relative strength compared with attention. For the second objec-
tive, we utilized the Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey–
Kindergarten Cohort (ECLS-K) to (a) test whether the null effects
of social skills, internalizing behavior, and externalizing behavior
are sensitive to different specifications, (b) test whether the relative
strengths of kindergarten math and reading scores in predicting
later math and reading scores are sensitive to inclusion of the
general knowledge score, (c) explore the relative strength of the
motor and attention measures in predicting fifth-grade scores with-
out kindergarten math and reading measures, (d) add results for
later science scores and compare with math and reading results,
and (e) interpret the changing predictive effect of child’s age on
scores from kindergarten to fifth grade to suggest that the key
developmental skills linked to school readiness may be captured
by this analysis.
The link between attention and cognitive development is not
conceptually difficult because of research on executive function as
well as personal experience in maintaining attention when doing
cognitive tasks. However, the link between motor skills and cog-
nitive skills is less apparent. For the third objective, we review the
neuroscience and developmental literature that has linked motor
and cognitive development to provide additional empirical and
theoretical support for the linkage and make this linkage easier to
grasp.
Empirical Evidence From Three Longitudinal Data
Sets With Motor Measures
The ECLS-K, the British Birth Cohort Study (BCS), and the
National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY) included mea-
sures of motor skills that were not included in Duncan et al.
(2007). These data sets were described in Duncan et al. and the
associated supplemental materials. We describe only the motor
measures used in each of these data sets.
Motor Measures in the ECLS-K
Two measures of psychomotor assessment were obtained in the
ECLS-K: gross and fine motor skills. The ECLS-K uses motor
items adapted from the Early Screening Inventory–Revised
(Meisels, Marsden, Wiske, & Henderson, 1997). This inventory is
a well-standardized multidomain screening test widely used to
identify preschool and kindergarten children at risk for school
failure (Kimmel, 2001; Paget, 2001). For the assessment of fine
motor skills, participants used building blocks to replicate a model,
copied five figures on paper, and drew a person. For the assess-
ment of gross motor skills, participants skipped, hopped on one
foot, walked backward, and stood on one foot. Interitem reliability
(alpha coefficient) was .57 for the fine motor scale and .51 for the
gross motor scale. The low reliabilities are partially a function of
the binary scoring system for the items that comprised the scales
and the small number of items.
Motor Measures in the NLSY
The motor skills measure for the NLSY was developed at the
National Center for Health Statistics and comprised items based on
standard and reliable measures of child motor and social develop-
ment (including the Bayley, Gesell, and Denver methods). Age
appropriate sets of dichotomous items were determined on the
basis of previous analyses of 2,714 U.S. children (Peterson &
Moore, 1987). On the basis of the determined age ranges, the
instrument completed by the child’s mother contained eight com-
ponents designed to assess children from ages 22 to 47 months.
Standard scores were created that are comparable for children of
different ages. More detailed information regarding the reliability
and validity of these measures is available in the discussion of
motor skills development in the NLSY79 Child Handbook (Baker,
Keck, Mott, & Quinlan, 1993) and the NLSY Children, 1992 (Mott,
Baker, Ball, Keck, & Lenhart, 1995).
Motor Skills in the BCS
In the BCS, motor skills at age 5 were measured with three
drawing tasks: design copying, human figure drawing, and profile
drawing. The copying task required children to copy eight basic
designs. Children were allowed two attempts with no assessor
assistance. This test was used in previous studies to assess fine
motor and visual control (Davie, Butler, & Goldstein, 1972; Rut-
ter, Tizard, & Whitemore, 1970). In the human figure drawing
task, children were asked to create a full body drawing of a person.
Children received no instructor help during drawing, but clarifi-
cation of aspects of the drawing was allowed after completion. In
the profile drawing test, children completed the profile of a basic
head shape from a test booklet with no assessor help. These tests
have a reported reliability of .94 and good discrimination proper-
ties (Harris, 1963; Koppitz, 1968).
Estimation
In all three data sets, estimation was done with weighted ordi-
nary least squares. Missing data were handled in the ECLS-K with
multiple imputation (20 imputations) and in the NLSY through full
information maximum likelihood. Results reported here for the
BCS are taken from the supplemental materials in Duncan et al.
(2007) because the motor variables were included in the original
analysis. Missing data were handled in the BCS through inclusion
of missing value dummies. The full estimation results are provided
in the supplemental materials.
Results
Table 1 summarizes the results in format similar to that used by
Duncan et al. (2007). The patterns of results for early math and
reading, attention, social skills, internalizing problems, and exter-
nalizing problems were very similar to those found by Duncan et
1009
SPECIAL SECTION: TWO NEW SCHOOL READINESS INDICATORS
al. For the ECLS-K, early math scores appeared to be the best
predictors of both later math and reading scores, whereas reading
scores also added significant predictive accuracy for later reading
scores but not for later math scores. The NLSY, however, sug-
gested a stronger role for reading in the prediction of later reading
and mathematics achievement than the ECLS-K did. The attention
coefficients for the three data sets showed the same sign as in
Duncan et al., and all were statistically significant at an alpha level
of .01 or better. The coefficients of social skills, internalizing
problems, and externalizing problems also showed patterns similar
to those of Duncan et al., who found mostly nonsignificant results
or significance far less than that for attention or early reading and
math. The exception is for the measure of social skills in the
NLSY, which showed strong significance for both reading and
math. However, the measure of social skills used a rating by the
test administrator when participants were between ages 54 and 78
months. This measure was not included in Duncan et al., perhaps
because of its possible unreliability.
Measures of fine motor skills showed highly significant results
for both math and reading in all three data sets. However, the gross
motor measure in the ECLS-K was not a significant predictor. The
profile drawing test in the BCS was not significant for either
reading or math, but all other fine motor measures showed statis-
tical significance at the .01 alpha level or better. Comparisons of
statistical significance between fine motor skills and attention
measures showed that fine motor skills were almost always as
significant or more statistically significant than attention. The
comparison of the coefficients for effect sizes showed that atten-
tion was much stronger than motor skills for predicting reading in
the ECLS-K but was of similar strength in the NLSY and BCS. For
math, the coefficients for attention were somewhat greater or
approximately similar to those for fine motor skills for the
ECLS-K and NLSY, but the BCS showed stronger effects for fine
motor skills. This evidence suggests that both attention and fine
motor skills were important developmental predictors of later
achievement, controlling for family and child characteristics and
earlier math and reading.
Results From Sensitivity Analyses With the ECLS-K
We used the ECLS-K for analysis of five additional issues. The
first issue was whether the socioemotional variables that were
generally not significant would show stronger effects in the ab-
sence of the attention measure. The attention measure used in the
ECLS-K, a measure of approaches to learning, is a composite
measure that includes attention-related measures (attention, persis-
tence, and concentration) but also measures linked to eagerness to
learn, interest, curiosity, creativity, and responsibility. Such a
broad measure could mask the effects of other socioemotional
measures. Table 2 contrasts the results when the approaches to
learning measure was included and excluded in the analysis. The
results showed that social skills for math and reading became
significant, although still much less significant than attention and
Table 1
Summary of Results Predicting Later Achievement With Earlier Math and Reading Readiness and Socioemotional and Motor Skills
With Three Longitudinal Data Sets
Measure
Reading Math
ECLS-K NLSY BCS ECLS-K NLSY BCS
Earlier cognitive
Reading .08
ⴱⴱⴱⴱ
.10
ⴱⴱⴱⴱⴱ
.13
ⴱⴱⴱ
ns .11
ⴱⴱⴱⴱⴱ
.09
ⴱⴱⴱ
Math .20
ⴱⴱⴱⴱ
.10
ⴱⴱⴱⴱⴱ
.33
ⴱⴱⴱⴱ
.14
ⴱⴱⴱⴱⴱ
Socioemotional
Attention
a
.16
ⴱⴱⴱⴱ
.05
ⴱⴱ
.08
ⴱⴱ
.21
ⴱⴱⴱⴱ
.05
ⴱⴱ
.09
ⴱⴱ
Externalizing–I ns .04
ns ns ns ns
Externalizing–II ns ns
Internalizing .03
ⴱⴱ
ns ns ns ns ns
Social skills
b
ns .06
ⴱⴱⴱⴱⴱ
.04 .04
ⴱⴱ
Motor
Gross motor .02
ns
Fine motor .07
ⴱⴱⴱⴱ
.14
ⴱⴱⴱⴱ
Motor/social .05
ⴱⴱⴱ
.05
ⴱⴱ
Copy 8 designs .26
ⴱⴱⴱ
.36
ⴱⴱⴱ
Human drawing .09
ⴱⴱ
.09
ⴱⴱ
Profile of head ns ns
Family/home controls X X X X X X
Early measures X X
Adjusted R
2
.54 .31 .45 .55 .30 .45
Observations 7,814 5,462 1,778 7,830 5,462 1,753
Note. Blank cells indicate that the measures were not available on a specific data set. X indicates that family/home controls or early measures were
included in the regression. All coefficients are standardized. ECLS-K Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey–Kindergarten Cohort; NLSY National
Longitudinal Survey of Youth; BCS British Birth Cohort Study.
a
The attention measures on the ECLS-K were teacher-rated attention where higher values indicate more attention. For the NLSY and BCS, the attention
measures were of attention problems, and higher values indicate poorer attention.
b
In the NLSY, the social skills measure of sociability, which was a
rating by the test administrator given at 42 months, was not included in Duncan et al. (2007) as a social skills measure.
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p.001.
ⴱⴱⴱⴱ
p.00001.
ⴱⴱⴱⴱⴱ
p.000001.
1010 GRISSMER, GRIMM, AIYER, MURRAH, AND STEELE
fine motor skills. Internalizing and externalizing behavior re-
mained insignificant.
In addition to the early math and reading test, the ECLS-K had
a test measuring knowledge of the world or early science and
social science knowledge. The science items assessed factual
and conceptual understanding as well as the ability to formulate
and answer questions related to the natural world. Social studies
questions assessed children’s knowledge about their environment
across a broad range of categories, including history, economics,
and culture. Duncan et al.’s (2007) results and the results reported
in Table 1 included the test of general knowledge as a control
variable, but its coefficients were not included in the summary
table.
Table 3 shows the effects of including and excluding the test of
general knowledge. The results show that the general knowledge
test was a very strong predictor of later reading, far stronger than
the early reading test, and was also a strong predictor of later math
scores, although early math remained the strongest predictor. Gen-
eral knowledge was a much stronger predictor of both reading and
math than early reading. The pattern of results for the socioemo-
tional, attention, and motor skills was generally unaffected by the
inclusion of general knowledge.
One possible explanation for the strength of the general knowl-
edge test is that it captured comprehension and ability to integrate
knowledge of the external world—skills that may be needed at
fifth grade in both reading and math. Children make a key transi-
tion in reading skills between kindergarten and fifth grade from
learning to read to reading to learn. The general knowledge test
may better track this transition than the early reading test.
Another issue was the sensitivity of the attention, motor, and
socioemotional measures to the removal of the early math and
reading tests. The fifth-grade estimates that included the earlier
readiness tests can underestimate the potential effects of the motor,
attention, and socioemotional measures if these measures and early
readiness scores are correlated. In fact, attention and motor mea-
sures are also strong predictors of early math and reading, so the
effect of attention, motor, and possibly the other socioemotional
measures on fifth-grade achievement may be underestimated (e.g.,
indirect effects through early math and reading).
Table 4 shows the results including and excluding the early
measures of math, reading, and general knowledge. The results
that excluded early reading and math scores may better predict
possible effects of early interventions to improve attention and fine
motor skills. The results showed that the pattern of results for the
Table 2
Sensitivity Test of Socioemotional Variables to Absence of Approaches to Learning
Approaches to learning excluded Approaches to learning included
Math Reading Math Reading
Measure Coef tCoef tCoef tCoef t
Approaches to learning .21 14.71 .16 11.50
Social skills .06 3.86 .03 1.76 .01 0.49 .03 1.66
Externalizing behavior .02 1.24 .00 0.12 .00 0.23 .01 0.67
Internalizing behavior .00 0.45 .01 1.15 .02 1.96 .03 3.03
Self-control .00 0.23 .02 1.50 .04 2.55 .01 0.07
Gross motor .01 0.57 .01 1.57 .00 0.09 .02 1.96
Fine motor .16 15.37 .09 8.26 .14 13.42 .07 6.62
Note. Coef coefficient.
Table 3
Sensitivity Test to Inclusion of General Knowledge
General knowledge excluded General knowledge included
Math Reading Math Reading
Measure Coef tCoef tCoef tCoef t
Attention .21 15.15 .18 12.09 .21 14.71 .16 11.50
Social skills .00 0.07 .01 0.84 .01 0.49 .03 1.66
Externalizing .00 0.09 .01 0.91 .00 0.23 .01 0.67
Internalizing .02 1.69 .02 2.47 .02 1.96 .03 3.03
Self-control .05 2.74 .02 1.07 .04 2.55 .01 0.70
Gross motor .01 1.15 .00 0.05 .00 0.09 .02 1.96
Fine motor .15 14.25 .09 8.03 .14 13.42 .07 6.62
Math .37 25.27 .28 18.60 .33 21.66 .20 13.27
Reading .02 1.76 .11 7.72 .01 0.69 .08 6.04
General knowledge .16 12.30 .30 22.18
R
2
.55 .51 .56 .55
Note. Coef coefficient.
1011
SPECIAL SECTION: TWO NEW SCHOOL READINESS INDICATORS
other socioemotional measures remained similar, mostly insignif-
icant or of far lower significance than the attention and motor
measures. However, the coefficients of attention and fine motor
skills increased markedly with greater statistical significance. The
attention coefficients were larger in both reading and math compared
with the fine motor coefficients. The estimated effect sizes for atten-
tion and fine motor skills were around .3 and .2, respectively Perhaps
more important is that interventions that combine attention and fine
motor skills might predict effect sizes of around .5.
We also studied the sensitivity of early and later math and
reading scores to the child’s age. Children in the United States start
kindergarten with a range of ages that can span 18 months. The
large age range arises from the normal 1-year entry window for
kindergarten combined with differences between states in the
lowest kindergarten entry age. Table 5 contains regression coeffi-
cients controlled for family characteristics and for age of child at
testing in equations predicting reading and math achievement at
kindergarten entrance and in the spring of first, third, and fifth
grade. Table 5 also contrasts the age coefficients when the socio-
emotional, attention, and motor variables are included and ex-
cluded. With socioemotional, attention, and motor skills excluded,
the results suggest that differences in age when children were
tested were very significant predictors of kindergarten entry and
spring first-, third-, and fifth-grade scores, but their significance
declined markedly by grade. However, when socioemotional, at-
tention, and motor skills were included, the effect of age com-
pletely faded by third grade.
Sensitivity of scores to age can indicate that a developmental
variable dependent on age was missing from the analysis. That is,
age itself is not a causative mechanism, but rather the significance
of age indicates that developmental differences were present at
different ages that need to be specified in the equations. For
instance, Table 5 illustrates that the absence of attention, motor,
and socioemotional variables at any given grade causes declines in
the strength and statistical significance of the age variable. Thus,
the inclusion of developmental measures reduces the significance
of the age variable. When age becomes insignificant at third grade,
it might suggest that developmental variables measured at kinder-
garten have been accounted for.
Another issue was whether fifth-grade science scores showed
patterns similar to those for math and reading. Table 6 shows that
later science scores were much more strongly predicted by the
general knowledge test, with some contribution from math and
little from reading. The effect of fine motor skills on later science
Table 4
Sensitivity Test to Exclusion of Early Math, Reading, and General Knowledge Scores
Early math, reading,
and general knowledge
excluded
Early math, reading,
and general knowledge
included
Math Reading Math Reading
Measure Coef tCoef tCoef tCoef t
Attention .34 23.13 .30 19.85 .21 14.71 .16 11.50
Social skills .02 1.27 .03 1.81 .01 0.49 .03 1.66
Externalizing behavior .01 0.49 .02 1.35 .00 0.23 .01 0.67
Internalizing behavior .00 0.19 .01 0.78 .02 1.96 .03 3.03
Self-control .06 3.52 .04 2.06 .04 2.55 .01 0.07
Gross motor .03 2.88 .02 1.56 .00 0.09 .02 1.96
Fine motor .22 19.71 .15 13.27 .14 13.42 .07 6.62
Math score .33 21.66 .20 13.27
Reading score .01 0.69 .08 6.06
General knowledge score .16 12.30 .30 22.18
R
2
.46 .43 .56 .55
Note. Coef coefficient.
Table 5
Coefficients of Age for Different Grades and Different Specifications
Grade and
semester
Coefficients of age for reading Coefficients of age for math
Excluding SE and
motor
Including SE and
motor
Excluding SE and
motor
Including SE and
motor
Coef tCoef tCoef tCoef t
Kindergarten, fall .19 21.63 .12 13.43 .27 31.55 .17 20.70
First grade, spring .13 11.08 .04 3.70 .19 15.98 .08 7.23
Third grade, spring .10 8.83 .01 1.00 .11 9.39 .00 0.20
Fifth grade, spring .08 7.43 .00 0.09 .06 4.94 .05 4.98
Note. SE socioemotional; Coef coefficient.
1012 GRISSMER, GRIMM, AIYER, MURRAH, AND STEELE
was stronger than the effect for reading but weaker than the effect
for math. Attention was a somewhat weaker predictor of science
than either later reading or math. However, the general knowledge
test had effect sizes of .2, .3, and .4, respectively, for math,
reading, and science. General knowledge was a much stronger
combined predictor of all three tests than either early math or
reading. Similar to later reading and math, none of the other
socioemotional variables showed statistical significance or they
showed much weaker significance than attention and fine motor
skills.
Neuroscience and Developmental Evidence for a
Motor–Cognitive Link
One possibility that might partially account for a motor–
cognitive causal link is that most activities that build or display
cognitive skills also involve the use of fine motor skills. Writing
requires fine motor skills with the hands as well as hand–eye
coordination. Speaking requires fine motor skills that control the
production of sound. Reading requires the use of fine motor skills
controlling eye movement for word tracking. Poor fine motor skills
can make cognitive learning and performance more difficult be-
cause of the simultaneous need for fine motor skills in cognitive
activities. However, evidence from neuroscience and recent child
development research presents a much more complex relation
between early motor and later cognitive development. Evidence
suggests that even if cognitive development required no simulta-
neous usage of motor and cognitive skills, earlier motor skill
development could have a significant impact on later cognitive
development. In this section, we summarize some of this evidence.
This review is not comprehensive but summarizes some of the
evidence and current thinking about how early motor skills might
be linked to later cognitive development.
Diamond (2000) summarized the links between motor and cog-
nitive skills, using evidence from neuroimaging and neuroanat-
omy, from co-occurrence of effects from brain damage or brain
abnormalities, and from correlations between deficits in motor and
cognitive skills in developmental disorders. Diamond found sig-
nificant evidence for a motor–cognition association in each of
these areas. Historically, cognitive and motor activities were as-
signed to separate brain areas. Diamond’s neuroimaging evidence
suggests that some of the primary brain regions previously thought
to be involved only in motor activities (cerebellum and basal
ganglia) or cognitive activities (prefrontal cortex) are coactivated
when doing certain motor or cognitive tasks. Neuroanatomy also
provides evidence for two-way neural communication linkages
between these motor and cognitive areas. Diamond also proposed
that executive function, primarily located in the prefrontal areas,
may coordinate complex activities requiring several parts of the
brain regardless of whether the task is motor, emotional, or cog-
nitive.
Diamond (2000) also provided evidence for a prevalent co-
occurrence of both motor and cognitive deficits in many developmen-
tal disorders, in movement disorders, and in brain damaged persons.
Individuals with brain damage to either the primary motor or primary
cognitive areas often show impairment in both skill areas. Also,
ADHD and dyslexia, both traditionally classified as childhood cog-
nitive disorders, are often characterized by poor motor coordination.
Individuals with Parkinson’s disease, which is primarily thought of as
a movement disorder, often show areas of significant cognitive im-
pairment. Although these linkages involve primarily disorders outside
the normal variation of motor and cognitive skills in the general
population, the evidence provided from the longitudinal surveys sug-
gests a motor–cognitive linkage due to variations found within pop-
ulations without significant disorders.
Since Diamond’s (2000) study, multidisciplinary research has sub-
stantially strengthened the evidence for the motor–cognitive link and
has elucidated the nature of the linkage. This research suggests that
motor and cognitive development are inextricably linked. Literature
suggests that the linkage occurs primarily for two reasons: (a) many
types of cognitive activities utilize specialized control and modulation
functions located in the cerebellum and basal ganglia that develop
during motor acquisition, and (b) some of the neural infrastructure
linking the prefrontal and motor areas built to adaptively control the
learning process during motor development is also used to control
learning in cognitive development. The evidence to support this view
comes from neuroimaging, neuroanatomy, studies of motor/cognitive
disorders, psychological testing, and recent child development re-
search on motor skills.
Table 6
Comparison of Results for Math, Reading, and Science
Math Reading Science
Measure Coef tCoef tCoef t
Approaches to learning .21 14.71 .16 11.50 .11 7.73
Social skills .01 0.49 .03 1.67 .01 0.82
Externalizing behavior .00 0.23 .01 0.67 .01 0.46
Internalizing behavior .02 1.96 .03 3.03 .03 3.54
Self-control .04 2.55 .01 0.70 .02 1.12
Gross motor .00 0.09 .02 1.96 .02 2.30
Fine motor .14 13.42 .07 6.62 .08 8.25
Math score .33 21.66 .20 13.27 .14 9.56
Reading score .01 0.69 .08 6.04 .04 2.70
General knowledge .16 12.30 .30 22.18 .40 30.00
Adjusted R
2
.56 .55 .57
Note. Coef coefficient.
1013
SPECIAL SECTION: TWO NEW SCHOOL READINESS INDICATORS
Doya (1999) suggested that the prefrontal cortex, basal ganglia,
and cerebellum are all specialized to different types of learning,
with the prefrontal areas more specialized to unsupervised learn-
ing, the basal ganglia more specialized to reinforcement learning,
and the cerebellum more specialized to supervised learning. Seger
(2006) suggested that two important common functions utilizing
the basal ganglia and shared by some motor and cognitive tasks are
performing a sequential, coordinated series of events over time and
complex categorization. The sequential events could be perform-
ing a coordinated motor movement, organizing grammatical ele-
ments in language, or sequencing subgoals in complex reasoning.
Subsequently, cognitive tasks attributed primarily to motor areas
include tracking and estimation of time durations, sequential learn-
ing tasks, nondeclarative and categorical learning, learning based
on implicit or explicit rewards, and the acquisition of new skills
(Ashby & Spiering, 2004; Graybiel, 2005; Nicolson, Fawcett, &
Dean, 2001; Saint-Cyr, 2003; Shohamy, Myers, Grossman, et al.,
2004; Shohamy, Myers, Kalanithi, & Gluck, 2008; Shohamy,
Myers, Onloaor, & Gluck, 2004; Toplak, Dockstader, & Tannock,
2006). For instance, Ashby and Spiering (2004) suggested three
different types of category learning that utilize specialized coor-
dinated subsystems that draw from prefrontal and motor areas
(basal ganglia). These studies provide evidence of a highly devel-
oped specialization of brain areas linked by distinct and special-
ized bidirectional loop circuitry to carry out different types of
learning. Haber (2003), Middleton (2003), and Middleton and
Strick (2000, 2002) reported anatomical evidence that the neces-
sary communication pathways are present between the cerebellum,
basal ganglia, and prefrontal areas. Paquier and Marien (2005),
Middleton (2003), and Middleton and Strick (2000, 2002) sug-
gested that the cerebellum modulates cognitive functions through
the cortico–ponto–cerebellar system and the cerebello–thalamo–
cortical pathways.
Evidence of motor–cognitive linkages from developmental dis-
orders, brain abnormalities, and brain damage has also grown
much stronger. Theories about the causal mechanisms in both
ADHD and dyslexia now involve malfunctions in the cerebellum
and/or basal ganglia (Banaschewski et al., 2005; Casey, Nigg, &
Durston, 2007; Castellanos, Sonuga-Barke, Milham, & Tannock,
2006; Castellanos & Tannock, 2002; Durston & Casey, 2006;
Durston & Konrad, 2007; Krain & Castellanos, 2006; Nigg &
Casey, 2005; Schmahmann, 2003, 2004; Sergeant, Geurts, Hui-
jbregts, Scheres, & Oosterlaan, 2003; Smith, Taylor, Warner,
Newman, & Ribia, 2002; Sonuga-Barke, 2003; Sonuga-Barke,
Auerbach, Campbell, Daley, & Thompson, 2005; Sonuga-Barke,
Dalen, & Remington, 2003; Sonuga-Barke, Sergeant, Nigg, &
Willcutt, 2008; Toplak, Dockstader, & Tannock, 2006). For in-
stance, deficits in timing associated with the basal ganglia have
been linked to children with reading problems like dyslexia and
language deficits and ADHD (Nicolson et al., 2001; Smith et al.,
2002). Evidence also suggests that damage to the cerebellum can
produce cognitive deficits, specifically called cerebellar cognitive
affective syndrome, that include impairment in executive function,
spatial cognition, linguistic processing, and verbal working mem-
ory (Ravizza et al., 2006; Schmahmann, 2003).
This research provides a compelling case for significant utiliza-
tion during cognitive development of highly specialized neural
infrastructure built during motor development in the cerebellum
and basal ganglia. The literature also suggests that motor devel-
opment contributes a second type of neural infrastructure, involv-
ing both the motor and prefrontal areas, that adaptively controls the
learning process itself whether motor or cognitive skills are being
learned. Surprisingly, motor development appears to require and
develop a quite sophisticated cognitive control capacity, later used
when learning cognitive skills.
Adolph (2005, 2008; Adolph & Berger, 2006) suggested that
researchers’ views of motor development have been naive because
they have not recognized the quite complex cognitive tasks de-
manded of infants. Adolph proposed that infants are learning to
learn as they master locomotion and subsequent gross and fine
motor skills. Infants continually have to solve complex problems
in adapting and changing each movement in response to their
perception of the current but ever changing environment, their
changing constraints on physical movement because of physical
growth of arms, limbs, and other body parts, and their current
levels of neural maturation and motor capability. In essence, no
two motor movements are ever the same but require continual
adaptation.
New theories of motor development to help explain such child
development research have addressed how children adapt to and solve
these continuing motor challenges. These theories required the pos-
tulation and testing of interplay between cognitive and motor func-
tions. The underlying conceptual model common in theoretical
models of motor and some cognitive tasks is the adaptive control
system that sequentially uses a trial-and-error, reward sensitive, or
reinforcement feedback process that iterates and hones initial rep-
resentations of cognitive or motor actions to desired objectives
(Cohen & Frank, 2008; Doya, 1999, 2000; Ito, 2005).
Ito (2005) suggested that a common function shared by certain
motor and cognitive-learning tasks is needed for control and ma-
nipulation of internal neural representations. In motor develop-
ment, this representation involves a model of the body in the
external environment that predicts, iteratively in a trial-and-error
process, how to control physical movement in a way to achieve the
desired motor action. For some cognitive tasks, the representation
is often of abstract symbols for similar trial and error and adaptive
manipulation toward a solution.
Adolph’s (2005, 2008; Adolph & Berger, 2006) learning to
learn analogy suggests that the neural infrastructure built to control
and guide the adaptive control process for motor skills is also used
when solving cognitive problems. This control process appears
to be orchestrated from the prefrontal areas but also involves the
cerebellum and basal ganglia. This research suggests that virtually
no new motor development or action can occur without an increas-
ingly sophisticated cognitive capacity that adaptively controls and
habituates motor actions. By the time children reach preschool age,
they have developed, during motor development, quite a sophisti-
cated cognitive capacity to initiate learning actions and use exec-
utive function skills in pursuit of motor learning. They have also
developed a significant capacity to orchestrate activities among
prefrontal, cerebellum, and basal ganglia that manipulates increas-
ingly complex representational models to achieve desired motor
adaptations.
The sophistication of this cognitive capacity built during motor
development may depend on the challenges encountered during
motor development. An important part of motor development is a
spiraling process whereby newly developed motor skills provide
expanding opportunity for children to experience more diverse and
1014 GRISSMER, GRIMM, AIYER, MURRAH, AND STEELE
toward strengthening executive function skills with promising
results on cognitive skills.
Current experimental interventions mainly focus on changing
the child’s instructional environment without understanding how
that change will influence the neural development underlying later
cognitive performance. Almost all of these interventions involve
changing the way that math and reading are taught: earlier instruc-
tion (prekindergarten), different curriculum, better classroom cli-
mate and teaching, and additional time on task. The results of our
research would certainly support improved early math and reading
because these early skills are strongly predictive of later math and
reading. As in Duncan et al. (2007), our results suggest that early
math should receive additional emphasis because reading has
already been receiving additional emphasis and math is more
predictive of later math and reading than is early reading. How-
ever, an important question is whether significant marginal gains
can be obtained in early math and reading from instructional
interventions, given the overwhelming focus of past interventions.
Our results suggest that the focus of interventions should shift
from a primary emphasis on changing the direct math and reading
instructional environment to interventions that build better foun-
dational skills of attention and fine motor skills and a better
understanding of the world outside schools. The results suggest
that current direct math and reading instruction is insufficient to
build attention and fine motor skills. Building these skills may rely
more on subjects and curricula that have been de-emphasized to
provide more math and reading instruction: the arts, music, dance,
physical education, and free play. Each of these subjects and
curricula may need to be redesigned to focus on building founda-
tional skills in the same way that math and reading have been
redesigned in recent years. Building stronger knowledge of the
external world also suggests that improving early science and
social studies curricula are important. Paradoxically, higher long-
term achievement in math and reading may require reduced direct
emphasis on math and reading and more time and stronger curric-
ula outside math and reading.
However, it is also likely that building improved attention, fine
motor skills, and knowledge of the world will require family,
parental, and societal emphasis before the start of school and
outside of school once children start school. Disadvantaged chil-
dren from all racial/ethnic groups likely have fewer opportunities
to build these skills than more advantaged children. For instance,
Grissmer and Eiseman (2008) reported that achievements gaps
found later in schooling between racial/ethnic groups are largely
present before school entry, suggesting that differences in earlier
environments and differential access to quality preschools primar-
ily cause score gaps. Grissmer and Eiseman also reported that
substantial gaps are present in attention and fine motor skills
between Black and White students at kindergarten entrance and
may account for up to 40% of the Black–White achievement gap.
The Black–White achievement gaps in reading and math have not
been closed in a sustained way over the past 25 years despite
substantial increases in school spending and the implementation in
virtually all states of standards-based accountability systems that
emphasize math and reading performance (Magnuson & Waldfo-
gel, 2008). Closing these gaps may be less about better math and
reading instruction than about building better attention and fine
motor skills and better awareness and knowledge of the external
world. It is likely that much of this work needs to be done before
kindergarten in extended preschools and in families and perhaps in
after-school programs.
In addition to suggesting new priorities for intervention, exper-
imentation, and educational and social policies, these results
should spur new developmental and neuroscience research that
links these early skills through causative mechanisms to later
achievement. This literature over the past 10 years has provided
substantial support for significant contributions of executive func-
tion and motor development to later cognitive development. Build-
ing stronger theories and knowledge about the interrelationships
among attention, fine motor skills, knowledge of the world, and the
potential causative mechanisms that might link them to later
achievement can result only in better design and increased power
and efficiency of interventions.
References
Adolph, K. (2005). Learning to learn in the development of action. In J. J.
Rieser, J. J. Lockman, & C. A. Nelson (Eds.), Action as an organizer of
learning and development (pp. 91–122). Mawah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Adolph, K. (2008). Learning to move. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 17, 213–218.
Adolph, K., & Berger, S. A. (2006). Motor development. In W. Damon &
R. Lerner (Series Eds.) & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Hand-
book of child psychology: Cognition, perception, and language (pp.
161–213). New York, NY: Wiley.
Ashby, F. G., & Spiering, B. J. (2004). The neurobiology of category
learning. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 101–113.
Baker, P. C., Keck, C. K., Mott, F. L., & Quinlan, S. V. (1993). NLSY child
handbook: A guide to the 19861990 National Longitudinal Survey of
Youth child data. Columbus, OH: Center for Human Resource Research.
Banaschewski, T., Hollis, C., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., Rubia, K.,
Willcutt, E., & Taylor, E. (2005). Towards an understanding of unique
and shared pathways in the psychopathophysiology of ADHD. Devel-
opmental Science, 8, 132–140.
Casey, B., Nigg, J. T., & Durston, S. (2007). New potential leads in the
biology and treatment of attention deficit–hyperactivity disorder. Cur-
rent Opinion in Neurology, 20, 119–124.
Castellanos, F., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Milham, M. P., & Tannock, R.
(2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive dysfunc-
tion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 117–123.
Castellanos, F., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention deficit/
hyperactivity disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 3, 617–628.
Cohen, M. X., & Frank, M. J. (2008). Neurocomputational models of basal
ganglia function in learning, memory and choice. Behavioural Brain
Research, 199, 141–156.
Davie, R., Butler, N. R., & Goldstein, H. (1972). From birth to seven.
London, England: Longman.
Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cog-
nitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child
Development, 71, 44–56.
Diamond, A., Barnett, S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007, November 30).
Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318, 1387–
1388.
Doya, K. (1999). What are the computations of the cerebellum, the basal
ganglia and the cerebral cortex? Neural Networks, 12, 961–974.
Doya, K. (2000). Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in
learning and motor control. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 10,
732–739.
Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C.,
Klebanov, P.,...Japel, C. (2007). School readiness and later achieve-
ment. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428–1446.
1016 GRISSMER, GRIMM, AIYER, MURRAH, AND STEELE
Durston, S., & Casey, B. J. (2006). What have we learned about cognitive
development from neuroimaging? Neuropsychologia, 44, 2149–2157.
Durston, S., & Konrad, K. (2007). Integrating genetic, psychopharmaco-
logical and neuroimaging studies: A converging methods approach to
understanding the neurobiology of ADHD. Developmental Review, 27,
374–395.
Graybiel, A. M. (2005). The basal ganglia: Learning new tricks and loving
it. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 638644.
Grissmer, D. W., & Eiseman, E. (2008). Can gaps in the quality of early
environment and non-cognitive skills help explain persisting Black–
White achievement gaps? In K. Magnuson & J. Waldfogel (Eds.), Steady
gains and stalled progress: Inequality and the Black–White test score
gap. New York, NY: Sage.
Haber, S. N. (2003). Integrating cognition and motivation into the basal
ganglia pathways of action. In M. Bedard, A. Yves, S. Chouinard, S.
Fahn, A. Korczyn, & P. Lesperance (Eds.), Mental and behavioral
dysfunction in movement disorders (pp. 35–50). Totowa, NJ: Humana
Press.
Harris, D. (1963). Children’s drawings as measures of intellectual matu-
rity. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace, & World.
Ito, M. (2005). Bases and implications of learning in the cerebellum—
Adaptive control and internal model mechanism. In C. Zeeuw & F.
Cicarita (Eds.), Progress in brain research: Creating coordination in the
cerebellum (pp. 95–109). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier.
Kimmel, E. (2001). Test review of the Early Screening Inventory–Revised.
In B. S. Plake & J. C. Impara (Eds.), The fourteenth mental measure-
ments yearbook. Retrieved from http://www.unl.edu/buros
Koppitz, E. M. (1968). Psychological evaluation of children’s human-
figure drawings. New York, NY: Grune & Stratton.
Krain, A. L., & Castellanos, F. X. (2006). Brain development and ADHD.
Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 433–444.
Magnuson, K., & Waldfogel, J. (2008). Steady gains and stalled progress.
New York, NY: Sage.
Meisels, S., Marsden, D., Wiske, M., & Henderson, L. (1997). Early
Screening Inventory–Revised. Ann Arbor, MI: Rebus.
Middleton, F. (2003). Fundamental and clinical evidence for basal ganglia
influence on cognition. In M. Bedard, Y. Agid, S. Chouinard, S. Fahn,
A. Korczyn, & P. Lesperance (Eds.), Mental and behavioral dysfunction
in movement disorders (pp. 13–34). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.
Middleton, F. A., & Strick, P. L. (2000). Basal ganglia output and cogni-
tion: Evidence from anatomical, behavioral, and clinical studies. Brain
and Cognition, 42, 183–200.
Middleton, F. A., & Strick, P. L. (2002). Basal-ganglia “projections” to the
prefrontal cortex of the primate. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 926–935.
Mott, F. L., Baker, P. C., Ball, D. E. Keck, C. K., & Lenhart, S. N. (1995).
The NLSY children 1992: Description and evaluation. Columbus, OH:
Ohio State University, Center for Human Resource Research.
Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia:
The cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 508
511.
Nigg, J. T., & Casey, B. J. (2005). An integrative theory of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective neu-
rosciences. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 785–806.
Paget, K. D. (2001). Test review of the Early Screening Inventory–
Revised. In B. S. Plake & J. C. Impara (Eds.), The fourteenth mental
measurements yearbook. Retrieved from http://www.unl.edu/buros
Paquier, P., & Marien, P. (2005). A synthesis of the role of the cerebellum
in cognition. Aphasiology, 19, 3–19.
Peterson, J. L., & Moore, K. A. (1987). Motor and social development in
infancy: Some results from a national survey. Washington, DC: Child
Trends.
Ravizza, S. M., McCormick, C. A., Schlerf, J. E., Justus, T., Ivry, R. B., &
Fiez, J. A. (2006). Cerebellar damage produces selective deficits in
verbal working memory. Brain, 129, 306–320.
Rutter, M., Tizard, J., & Whitmore, K. (1970). Education, health and
behavior. London, England: Longman.
Saint-Cyr, J. A. (2003). Frontal–striatal circuit functions: Context, se-
quence, and consequence. Journal of the International Neuropsycholog-
ical Society, 9, 103–128.
Schellenberg, G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Sci-
ence, 15, 511–514.
Schmahmann, J. (2003). The role of the cerebellum in cognition and
emotion. In M. A. Be´dard (Ed.), Mental and behavioral dysfunction in
movement disorders (pp. 5981). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.
Schmahmann, J. (2004). Disorders of the cerebellum: Ataxia, dysmetria of
thought and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Neuropsychi-
atric Practice and Opinion, 16, 367–378.
Seger, C. A. (2006). The basal ganglia in human learning. Neuroscientist,
12, 285–290.
Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., Huijbregts, S., Scheres, A., & Oosterlaan, J.
(2003). The top and the bottom of ADHD: A neuropsychological per-
spective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 583–592.
Shohamy, D., Myers, C. E., Grossman, S., Sage, J., Gluck, M. A., &
Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Cortico-striatal contributions to feedback-based
learning: Converging data from neuroimaging and neuropsychology.
Brain, 127, 851–859.
Shohamy, D., Myers, C. E., Kalanithi, J., & Gluck, M. A. (2008). Basal
ganglia and dopamine contributions to probabilistic category learning.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 219–236.
Shohamy, D., Myers, C. E., Onlaor, S., & Gluck, M. A. (2004). Role of the
basal ganglia in category learning: How do patients with Parkinson’s
disease learn? Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 676686.
Smith, A., Taylor, E., Warner, J., Newman, S., & Ribia, K. (2002).
Evidence for a pure time perception deficit in children with ADHD.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43,
529–542.
Sonuga-Barke, E. (2003). The dual pathway model of AD/HD: An elab-
oration of neuro-developmental characteristics. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 593–604.
Sonuga-Barke, E., Auerbach, J., Campbell, S. B., Daley, D., & Thompson,
M. (2005). Varieties of preschool hyperactivity: Multiple pathways from
risk to disorder. Developmental Science, 8, 141–150.
Sonuga-Barke, E., Dalen, L., & Remington, B. (2003). Do executive
deficits and delay aversion make independent contributions to preschool
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms? Journal of the Amer-
ican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1335–1342.
Sonuga-Barke, E., Sergeant, J. A., Nigg, J., & Willcutt, E. (2008). Exec-
utive dysfunction and delay aversion in attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder: Nosologic and diagnostic implications. Child and Adolescent
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17, 367–384.
Toplak, M. E., Dockstader, C., & Tannock, R. (2006). Temporal informa-
tion processing in ADHD: Findings to date and new methods. Journal of
Neuroscience Methods, 151, 15–29.
Received April 1, 2009
Revision received October 30, 2009
Accepted November 25, 2009
1017
SPECIAL SECTION: TWO NEW SCHOOL READINESS INDICATORS
... FMS is relevant to writing as an indicator for school readiness (Grissmer et al., 2010), as FMS in kindergarten predicts reading and spelling in higher grades (Doyen et al., 2017;Michel et al., 2020). For instance, one study that examined the effects of FMS on both timed and untimed writing measures found that manual dexterity was a significant predictor for both real word spelling and writing fluency (Doyen et al., 2017). ...
... FMS is a known predictor for math achievement in childhood (Asakawa & Sugimura, 2022;Gashaj et al., 2019) and remains correlated with math skills through age 18 (Grissmer et al., 2010). The relationship between FMS and math varies from r ~ .15-.42, depending on the type of math skill being tested and the age of the sample (Gashaj et al., 2019;Michel et al., 2020). ...
... This aligns with research suggesting that the motor-language connection, prominent in early childhood, weakens in later development (e.g., Iverson, 2010;Rhemtulla & Tucker-Drob, 2011). FMS is typically studied in the context of school readiness, with limited research examining its role in academic achievement among school-aged children (for exceptions, see Gashaj et al., 2019;Grissmer et al., 2010;Michel et al., 2020). Notably, previous studies have proposed that FMS indirectly predicts writing and math achievement through other measures and that the strength of the relationship between FMS and academic outcomes weakens as children age or is relevant only for specific skills (e.g., fluency-based tests). ...
Article
The relationship between writing and math achievement is not well understood. Phonological awareness (PA), working memory (WM), and fine motor skills (FMS) have been individually linked to theories of writing and math, yet are rarely considered together. The current study evaluates the shared cognitive factors underlying writing and math performance, both timed (e.g. fluency/automaticity) and untimed (e.g. spelling and math computation). It does so among third- through fifth-graders (n = 677) who vary in academic abilities. Results revealed differential relationships. WM had a stronger effect on writing than math for timed but not untimed outcomes. PA had a stronger effect on writing compared to math for both timed and untimed outcomes. PA also had a stronger effect on untimed math compared to timed math. Further, WM fully mediated the relationship between FMS and untimed writing but only partially mediated the relationship between FMS and other academic outcomes. Additionally, PA partially mediated the relationship between WM and all writing and math skills. These findings underscore the relevance of FMS, WM, and PA in both writing and math achievement, separately and together. These findings additionally provide guidance for developing a firmer theoretical and empirical understanding of the interrelations of writing and math.
... Fine motor skills involve precise movements of small muscles, primarily the hands and fingers, to accomplish specific tasks in coordination with psychological processes like perception and attention (2). The development of fine motor skills in children is intricately linked to cognitive enhancement, where the acquisition of fine motor skills lays the foundation for higher-level cognitive activities, while improved cognitive abilities facilitate the refinement of fine motor skills (9)(10)(11)(12)(13). ...
... Each item was scored as "0" if the answer was "unclear" or "no," and "1" if the answer was "yes." The quality assessment criteria for this study were as follows: low quality (0-3), moderate quality (4-7), and high quality (8)(9)(10)(11). Any disagreements between the two independent assessors were resolved through consultation with a third party. ...
... Moreover, Greenburg's subsequent longitudinally inclined study delves deeper, encompassing the tracking of children's academic performance from preschool to elementary school. The results show that children who exhibited robust visual-motor integration capabilities during preschool subsequently outperformed their peers in standardized mathematics tests during third, fourth, and fifth grades (11). This finding offers novel pedagogical implications for educators working with young children. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background In recent years, an increasing number of scholars have begun to focus on the relationship between children's motor development and school activities, with the relationship between children's fine motor skills and academic achievement being a particularly researched area. However, due to different research perspectives among scholars, the results in this field have been somewhat controversial. Therefore, this study aims to delve deeper into the relationship between children's fine motor skills and their various academic abilities through systematic review and meta-analysis. Method English databases (PubMed, Web of Science, Embase) and Chinese databases (CNKI, Wei Pu) were searched, and a quantitative meta-analysis was conducted using STATA software, along with a systematic descriptive analysis of the included literature. Results From the 1,147 documents retrieved, 11 studies were ultimately included. All meta-analysis results are significant, and there is a medium correlation between fine motor skills and reading ability, a larger correlation is observed with mathematical ability. In the subgroup analysis of each fine motor skill component and academic ability, except for the fine motor coordination, which shows only a small correlation with reading ability, the variables in the other subgroups all exhibit a medium degree of correlation. Notably, the correlation between visual-motor integration and mathematical ability is the strongest in subgroup ( r = 0.47). Conclusion The meta-analysis provides evidence supporting a positive and statistically significant correlation between preschool children's fine motor skills and learning outcomes. However, the scope of academic abilities examined in this domain is predominantly confined to mathematics and reading. Moreover, existing research largely focuses on surface-level correlational analyses, necessitating deeper exploration into the underlying mechanisms. Systematic Review Registration https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/ , identifier (CRD42023415498).
... Specifically, fine motor skills have been shown to predict reading proficiency in Grade 1 [30], while fine motor integration at the same grade level is a significant predictor of mathematical performance [31]. These findings underscore the importance of incorporating MC assessment as a fundamental component of school readiness indicators [32]. Indeed, regardless of the type of assessment, participant age or class duration, physical education classes consistently have a positive impact on the development of MC during childhood and adolescence [33]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Motor competence (MC) refers to the ability to execute a range of motor tasks, including the coordination and control necessary to perform everyday activities effectively. Interest in MC has grown over the past two decades, as reflected in an increasing number of publications on the topic. However, there is still no consensus on how to assess MC, with various test batteries available. A single test is insufficient for evaluating MC, as it encompasses a set of fundamental skills essential for daily life. These skills are typically categorised into fundamental movement skills, although discrepancies exist in their definitions. Some frameworks recognise two components-locomotor and manipulative skills-while others include a third component: stability skills. This review aims to analyse why, when and how MC should be assessed in children and adolescents. It was observed that MC assessments are essential at a young age, with physical education classes providing an ideal context for their implementation. Such assessments promote sports participation, aid in talent identification and highlight the positive correlation between MC and lifelong physical activity. Selecting an appropriate test battery requires careful consideration of the evaluator's objectives, participant age, required materials and the three pillars of fundamental movement skills: locomotor, stabilising and manipulative skills. Future research should refine the concept of MC and ensure that the validity of test batteries is rigorously examined. Additionally, using the same test battery across identical subjects and evaluating ecological validity will enhance the sensitivity and applicability of assessments, facilitating their use for both characterisation and intervention. Citation: Silva A. Evaluating motor competence in children and youth: a narrative review. Hum Mov. 2025;26(1):42-57; doi: https://doi.org/10.5114/hm/199533. © Wroclaw University of Health and Sport Sciences 2025; 26(1): 42-57
... Motor skills during infancy are measurable milestones and are positively correlated with later cognitive and language skill performance (Bolger et al., 2021;Collett et al., 2019;Ghassabian et al., 2016;Gonzalez et al., 2019;Holloway & Long, 2019;Sansavini et al., 2021). For example, fine motor skill performance can serve as a predictor of other developmental outcomes, such as school readiness (Grissmer et al., 2010;Iverson, 2010). Tracking motor skill acquisition can provide unique insights into an infant's developmental trajectory. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Infants undergo significant developmental changes in the first few months of life. While some risk factors increase the risk of developmental disability, such as preterm birth, the developmental trajectories of infants born pre-term (PT) and full-term (FT) present with individual variability. This study aims to investigate whether the utilization of data-driven unsupervised machine learning can identify patterns within groups of infants and categorize infants into specific developmental trajectories. Thirty-four infants, 19 FT and 15 PT, were assessed with the gross and fine motor subscales of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, version III (BSID-III) monthly for 2-5 visits between the ages of 1 and 6 months. Latent class growth analysis (LCGA) models were adopted to identify clusters of motor developmental trajectories during this critical time. Based on statistical significance, the linear, 2-class trend was selected as the best-fitting model for both gross and fine motor trajectories. Within this, LCGA reveals 2 developmental trends with varying beginning scores and developmental rates, including the low-baseline slow-growth (LBSG) subgroup, and the high-baseline fast-growth (HBFG) subgroup, with age (adjusted for prematurity) being equally distributed across both subgroups. Both subgroups, HBFG and LBSG, had a combination of infants born FT and PT (55% FT in HBFG, 56% FT in LBSG), supporting that preterm birth alone may not sufficiently categorize an infant’s developmental trajectory. The later BSID-III gross motor score showed marginal difference between groups (p = 0.062). Similarly, the fine motor model displayed a mixture of both infants born FT and PT (68% FT in HBFG, 40% FT in LBSG). In this case, the late motor composite BSID score was different between groups (p = 0.04). Our study uses a novel approach of LCGA to elucidate heterogeneous trajectories of motor development for gross and fine motor skills during the first half of life and offers potential for early identification of subgroup membership. Furthermore, these findings underscore the necessity for individualized risk assessments and intervention strategies tailored to individual needs. Ultimately, further validation of these models may provide usefulness in uncovering distinct motor development trajectories in infants. Author Note The authors would like to thank the infants and families who participated in these study, and Katy Kelley, Carolina Panceri, Judy Zhou, and Dana Fine for assisting with recruitment and data collection. Thank you to Eisner Health, Children’s Hospital Los Angeles, and Ventura County Medical Center for assistance with recruitment. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The studies secondarily analyzed in this work were supported by grants from the Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation [OPP1119189] (PI: B.A.S.) and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R03HD096137 (PI: Smith). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Study data were collected and managed using REDCap electronic data capture tools hosted at the Southern California Clinical and Translational Science Institute at the University of Southern California NIH grant UL1TR001855 (PI: T. Buchanan). REDCap (Research Electronic Data Capture) is a secure, web-based application designed to support data capture for research studies, providing (i) an intuitive interface for validated data entry; (ii) audit trails for tracking data manipulation and export procedures; (iii) automated export procedures for seamless data downloads to common statistical packages; and (iv) procedures for importing data from external sources.
... Physical movements such as going through obstacle courses or walking on a balance beam, for example, involve the sensations that promote the performance of cognitive tasks (Alibali & Nathan, 2012). Motor skills are a critical component of a child's development and readiness for school (Grissmer et al., 2010) and childcare providers would do well to provide developmentally appropriate movement programs for young children. Research on the effects of mindfulness practices on young children's balancing skills is scarce. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The quality of education is influenced not only by students' ability to learn and the curriculum, but also by the professionalism of teachers. While many educators engage in various professional development activities, one particularly effective method for enhancing teaching skills - peer observation- remains underused. Peer observation involves teachers observing one another's lessons to share best practices, offer constructive feedback, and learn new teaching strategies. Despite its potential, this practice is not as widely adopted as other professional development methods. This study investigates the views of 47 general and vocational education teachers on the key benefits of peer observation. The results indicate that teachers perceive peer observation as a valuable tool for professional growth. By observing their colleagues, teachers can share effective teaching techniques, gain new insights into classroom management, and reflect on their own teaching methods. When peer observation is conducted regularly, it fosters a more open and collaborative teaching environment, where teachers are more receptive to different viewpoints and teaching approaches. Additionally, teachers become better at identifying both the strengths and areas for improvement in their colleagues' work, leading to a more reflective and supportive teaching culture. Overall, the findings highlight the positive impact of peer observation on teachers' professional development. Encouraging its more widespread use could lead to enhanced teaching practices and, ultimately, improve the quality of education for students.
... In fact, over the last two decades, certain findings have provided support for this hypothesis: Murray et al. (2006) found that within normal developmental ranges (in the general population), earlier maturation in gross motor function was predictive of superior executive function skills; Piek et al. (2008) showed that better infant motor coordination was associated with better working memory and processing speed, while inferior gross motor function was associated with anxiety and depression symptoms at school age (Piek et al. 2010), namely, mental health symptoms that are closely linked with self-regulation in that age group (Robson, Allen, and Howard 2020;Sawyer et al. 2015;Wyman et al. 2010). Similarly, Grissmer et al. (2010) provided evidence that fine motor skills were associated with a range of cognitive abilities and were predictive of later academic achievement, while Gandotra et al. (2023) showed that fine motor function was a strong predictor of response inhibition, whereas gross motor function was a strong predictor of prosocial behavior in childhood. Zhou and Tolmie (2024) provided further evidence for a prospective association between infant motor function and cognitive skills (including executive function) and academic achievement in late childhood using a longitudinal dataset drawn from the general UK population. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective To explore the developmental pathways linking infant psychomotor function with personality in late adolescence through cognitive, social, and self‐regulation skills. The broader research question, seen through the lens of embodied cognition, is whether cognition and personality in youth develop from basic sensorimotor and communicative systems in infancy. Method The sample included 9202 participants from a representative UK birth cohort. A structural equation model examined the prospective associations between motor and communicative functions at age 9 months, cognition, self‐regulation, and prosociality at 5 years, and the five‐factor model of personality at 17 years. The associations between psychomotor function and the meta‐traits of stability and plasticity were also explored. Results Even after controlling for confounders and correcting for multiple paths, there was robust evidence that psychomotor development significantly predicts personality structure, with indirect pathways mediated by self‐regulation skills and general or social cognitive skills in middle childhood. While infant communicative function was significantly associated with both meta‐traits, gross motor function was significantly associated with plasticity but not stability. Conclusions Early psychomotor function may have long‐term effects on personality, mediated by cognitive, social, and self‐regulation skills. This finding can inform the development of socio‐educational interventions and tailored curricula in early childhood education.
Article
Motor skill acquisition is pivotal for human development. Traditionally, teaching motor skills has occurred in person, where teachers are in the exact location as learners. Recently, there has been a substantial shift in education and skill training to transfer to an online environment without robust evidence of online motor skill training’s effectiveness. In the context of exercise, motor skills have primarily been taught in online physical education and medical education, and this trend has been exacerbated by COVID-19. Considering the popularity of online skill learning and a lack of robust evidence of its effectiveness, this study compared the effectiveness of motor skill acquisition, motivation, and enjoyment of synchronous and asynchronous online groups to the in-person group. A sample of 83 college students ( M age = 20.51, SD = 1.780; male = 26, female = 57) were assigned to the in-person, synchronous, or asynchronous groups. The analysis of covariance showed a statistically significant difference in skill performance, F (2, 68) = 3.63, p = .032, η ² = .096, enjoyment, F (2, 67) = 5.69, p = .005, η ² = .15, and motivation, F (2, 67) = 3.92, p = .025, η ² = .11, favoring in-person group over the online groups. In conclusion, although teaching a motor skill online was practical and feasible, in-person training was more effective in learners’ skill acquisition and had higher positive perspectives (enjoyment and motivation) toward the training session.
Article
Full-text available
This research looks at how teachers use purposeful interventions, with specific consideration for parent engagement, planned errors, sensory activities, and scaffolding practices to help kids (3.5–4 years old) develop early writing competencies. A quasi-experimental research method separated 26 students into two study groups to conduct the investigation. The intervention group experienced structured support because their teachers demonstrated letter formations and employed error correction methods while guiding selected fine motor activities and using kinesthetic methods. It showed better fine motor performance as well as improved letter writing abilities and enhanced writing quality than students in the control group. The techniques led educational students to develop better pincer grip skills together with improved written proportions and improved entire letter formation structure. Students’ skill acquisition speeded up as well as their motivation because of the combined approach of multisensory learning with parent involvement and both groups showed improved application of learned skills during autonomous play sessions. Data confirms that students need purpose-driven training methods combined with interactive activities along with scaffolding support systems to build their writing competencies along with self-assurance and base skills. Future educational research requirements must examine the long-term effects of these approaches in different educational contexts.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Fine motor skill (FMS) development during childhood is essential to many learning processes, especially in school. FMS impairment can have a major impact on children’s quality of life. Developing effective and engaging rehabilitation solutions to train FMS that engage children in the abundant practice required for motor learning can be challenging. Virtual reality (VR) is a promising intervention option offering engaging FMS training tasks and environments that align with evidence-based motor learning principles. Other potential advantages of VR for rehabilitation include accessibility for home-based use and adaptability to individual needs. The objective of this scoping review is to map the extent, range and nature of VR applications focused on FMS training in paediatric rehabilitation, including hardware, software and interventional parameters. Methods and analysis We are following methodological guidelines for scoping review conduct and reporting from the Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) Manual for Evidence Synthesis and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for scoping reviews recommendations. We will search four databases (Pubmed, Web of Science, PsycInfo and Scopus) for articles that meet inclusion criteria defined by the Population, Concept, Context method; specifically studies focused on development or evaluation of immersive or non-immersive VR applications to deliver FMS training in paediatric rehabilitation. Different populations of children with FMS impairments will be included (such as children with cerebral palsy, children with developmental coordination disorder or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder). The first search took place in December 2023, and a second is planned for February 2025. One reviewer will complete title, abstract and full paper screening, with consultation by a second reviewer in case of uncertainty. A data extraction framework will be tested by two reviewers on five randomly selected studies to ensure inter-rater reliability, and one reviewer will complete data extraction. Quantitative and qualitative extraction will follow JBI guideline recommendations. Results will be presented in a descriptive and tabular format, including a narrative summary. Results will enhance understanding of the potential of FMS training in VR and inform subsequent directions for research and clinical practice. Ethics and dissemination Data for this review will be collected from the published literature. Ethical approval is not required. We will present our findings at scientific conferences and submit this review to a peer-reviewed journal for publication.
Article
Full-text available
The cerebellum is known to project via the thalamus to multiple motor areas of the cerebral cortex. In this study, we examined the extent and anatomical organization of cerebellar input to multiple regions of prefrontal cortex. We first used conventional retrograde tracers to map the origin of thalamic projections to five prefrontal regions: medial area 9 (9m), lateral area 9 (9l), dorsal area 46 (46d), ventral area 46, and lateral area 12. Only areas 46d, 9m, and 9l received substantial input from thalamic regions included within the zone of termination of cerebellar efferents. This suggested that these cortical areas were the target of cerebellar output. We tested this possibility using retrograde transneuronal transport of the McIntyre-B strain of herpes simplex virus type 1 from areas of prefrontal cortex. Neurons labeled by retrograde transneuronal transport of virus were found in the dentate nucleus only after injections into areas 46d, 9m, and 9l. The precise location of labeled neurons in the dentate varied with the prefrontal area injected. In addition, the dentate neurons labeled after virus injections into prefrontal areas were located in regions spatially separate from those labeled after virus injections into motor areas of the cerebral cortex. Our observations indicate that the cerebellum influences several areas of prefrontal cortex via the thalamus. Furthermore, separate output channels exist in the dentate to influence motor and cognitive operations. These results provide an anatomical substrate for the cerebellum to be involved in cognitive functions such as planning, working memory, and rule-based learning.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Traditional neurological tenets posit that the cerebellum coordinates skilled voluntary movements, and controls motor tone, posture, and gait. However, anatomical, clinical, and neuroimaging studies conducted over the past decades have shown that the cerebellum is implicated in diverse higher cognitive functions, such as language, memory, visospatial skills, executive functions, thought modulation, and emotional regulation of behaviour. Aims: To provide an introductory overview of the recently acknowledged role of the cerebellum in a number of cognitive processes, with special emphasis on its implication in speech and language functions. Main Contribution: The role the cerebellum plays in motor speech disorders is not limited to (ataxic) dysarthria, but also encompasses mutism and possibly apraxia of speech. Cerebellar-induced language impairments may consist of agrammatism and frontal (dynamic) aphasia-like phenomena, along with semantic retrieval deficits, syntactic comprehension difficulties, and depressed phonological verbal fluency tasks. In addition to speech and language functions, the cerebellum seems also to be implicated in several aspects of memory, such as procedural learning, paired-associative learning, long-term memory, and working memory. The cerebellum appears to modulate executive functions, such as planning, set-shifting, and abstract reasoning as well. Furthermore, it has been shown that the cerebellum intervenes in utilising the precise representation of temporal information not only for motor but also for perceptual tasks. The cerebellum further seems able to optimise the quality of sensory information for coordinating orientation, distribution, and shifting of attentional processes. Visuospatial skills and spatial cognition also appear to depend on cerebellar participation. Finally, a relationship between visuomotor imagery and cerebellar activation in the absence of real movement has been demonstrated.
Chapter
Over the past century, the term “basal ganglia” has at one time or another been used to indicate a wide variety of brain structures. In its present and most restrictive use, however, the term is generally reserved for three groups of brain structures called the “striatum,” “pallidum,” and “substantia nigra,” and an additional structure termed the “subthalamic nucleus” (STN). Table 1 outlines the basic subdivisions of the basal ganglia. The striatum can be separated into two general components, the dorsal striatum, which consists of the caudate and putamen, and the ventral striatum, which consists of the nucleus accumbens, septum, and olfactory tubercle. The nucleus accumbens, in turn, is subdivided into a lateral core region and medial shell region. In a similar manner, the pallidum can also be divided into multiple divisions, including a lateral or external segment of the globus pallidus (GPe), a medial or internal segment of the globus pallidus (GPi), and a portion that lies ventral and anterior to the anterior commissure, designated the ventral pallidum (VP). The GPi is further divided anatomically by a distinct fiber bundle into a lateral outer portion and a medial inner portion. Finally, the substantia nigra is also composed of more than one component, a cell group rich in neuromelanin called the pars compacta (SNpc), which is responsible for the black appearance of the nucleus in gross specimens, and an unpigmented cell group known as the pars reticulata (SNpr). Further subdivision of the SNpr is also possible, due to the presence of a distinct subpopulation of cells located dorsolaterally in the SNpr, and is referred to as the pars lateralis.
Chapter
The involvement of the basal ganglia in motor control is well documented, particularly in its association with the neurological disorders, Parkinson’ s disease (PD) and Huntington’ s disease (HD). It is the dorsal basal ganglia that is most affected in these diseases. In contrast, the ventral basal ganglia (the ventral striatum [VS] and ventral pallidum [VP]) are associated with mental health problems including schizophrenia and drug abuse and addiction (1–5). Taken as a whole, the basal ganglia, both dorsal and ventral components, are involved in motor, cognitive, and limbic functions. These functions are thought to be contained in separate, segregated corticobasal ganglia circuits (6). However, although severe motoric dysfunctions are associated with the dorsal striatum, and cognitive, emotional, and motivational problems are associated with the VS diseases effecting these striatal regions often have a mixed set of dysfunctions (7–15). For example, often the earliest detectable symptoms in PD patients occur on cognitive tasks requiring attentional set-shifting and tasks requiring organizational skills and use of working memory. Thus, although different basal ganglia regions are involved in various functions, the basal ganglia as a whole operates in concert with cortex in mediating overall behavioral responses. This involves a complex coordination of motivational, cognitive, and motor elements.
Book
Addressing the disparity in test scores between black and white children remains one of the greatest social challenges of our time. Between the 1960s and 1980s, tremendous strides were made in closing the achievement gap, but that remarkable progress halted abruptly in the mid 1980s, and stagnated throughout the 1990s. How can we understand these shifting trends and their relation to escalating economic inequality? In Steady Gains and Stalled Progress, interdisciplinary experts present a groundbreaking analysis of the multifaceted reasons behind the test score gap-and the policies that hold the greatest promise for renewed progress in the future. Steady Gains and Stalled Progress shows that while income inequality does not directly lead to racial differences in test scores, it creates and exacerbates disparities in schools, families, and communities-which do affect test scores. Jens Ludwig and Jacob Vigdor demonstrate that the period of greatest progress in closing the gap coincided with the historic push for school desegregation in the 1960s and 1970s. Stagnation came after efforts to integrate schools slowed down. Today, the test score gap is nearly 50 percent larger in states with the highest levels of school segregation. Katherine Magnuson, Dan Rosenbaum, and Jane Waldfogel show how parents' level of education affects children's academic performance: as educational attainment for black parents increased in the 1970s and 1980s, the gap in children's test scores narrowed. Sean Corcoran and William Evans present evidence that teachers of black students have less experience and are less satisfied in their careers than teachers of white students. David Grissmer and Elizabeth Eiseman find that the effects of economic deprivation on cognitive and emotional development in early childhood lead to a racial divide in school readiness on the very first day of kindergarten. Looking ahead, Helen Ladd stresses that the task of narrowing the divide is not one that can or should be left to schools alone. Progress will resume only when policymakers address the larger social and economic forces behind the problem. Ronald Ferguson masterfully interweaves the volume's chief findings to highlight the fact that the achievement gap is the cumulative effect of many different processes operating in different contexts. The gap in black and white test scores is one of the most salient features of racial inequality today. Steady Gains and Stalled Progress provides the detailed information and powerful insight we need to understand a complicated past and design a better future.
Article
Alarge body of research in social science has been directed toward addressing the causes of and solutions to continuing inequality of outcomes between black and white Americans (for recent summaries, see Neckerman 2004; Jencks and Phillips 1998). Historically, this research has created an expectation that such gaps would close over generations in a competitive economy if educational and labor market opportunities were equalized. The research, however, has been unable to account for several key aspects of the empirical data-specifically, to explain why progress is so slow, why there can be periods of either rapid or no progress, and why substantial gaps might remain. In this chapter, we examine three factors that might help explain these more complex dynamics in the empirical data: • Recognizing that early childhood environments create much of the achievement gap and may set limits on later achievement, educational attainment and labor force outcomes • Recognizing that the behavior and proximal processes that underlie early cognitive development may be different for racial-ethnic groups, and these differences may not be strongly linked to differences in commonly used surrogate variables (income, parent education), and may not change even as social and economic equality improves • Recognizing that a narrow focus on cognitive achievement measures rather than a broader focus on noncognitive skills such as behavioral, emotional, social and motor skills can leave persisting gaps because long-term educational attainment and labor force outcomes depend not only on cognitive skills, but also on noncognitive skills ■ some of these noncognitive skills may be implicit cognitive skills,1 that is, linked to the development or performance of cognitive skills The identification of these factors is done through reviews of recent literature and empirical estimations with the Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey of Kindergartners (ECLS-K). The major contributions from the literature are related to the interactive genetic and environmental mechanisms driving development from conception to kindergarten entrance, the emerging importance of noncognitive skills in explaining educational and labor market outcomes, and identifying implicit cognitive skills. Historically, researchers assumed that achievement gaps emerge during schooling through inequality of schools and family characteristics, and looked to the equalization of schooling opportunity as a major policy lever. However, research now shows that a substantial share of the gap is present at school entry, and that school equalization may therefore not fully close score gaps (Lee and Burkam 2002; Fryer and Levitt 2004, 2006).2 Research also suggests that closing these gaps may be more difficult at later ages (Heckman and Masterov 2007; Cunha et al. 2006) and that some intense early environments may limit later cognitive development even in the best of later circumstances (O'Connor, Rutter, and Beckett 2000; Beckett et al. 2006). Eliminating gaps before school entrance now appears to be critical to achieving social and economic equality in adulthood. This task may be even more challenging than equalizing schooling, given the numerous, diverse, and complex influences on child development in the early years. The behaviors and proximal processes that underlie the causative mechanisms in early cognitive development may not be adequately captured by the traditional surrogate variables focusing on family and neighborhood characteristics. Research suggests that interactions between genes and these behaviors and processes may account for much variance making their identification and measurement difficult (Rutter 2002; Shonkoff and Phillips 2000). Yet it will likely be in the discovery of these behaviors and processes and the possible differences across groups that will lead to the knowledge required for efficient early interventions to eliminate gaps. Finally, an implicit assumption in achieving social equality has been that cognitive development plays the dominant role in predicting gaps in later, long-term outcomes. However, noncognitive skills such as social, motor, emotional, and behavioral skills are now also known to affect educational attainment and other long-term outcomes and are receiving increasing attention in early development (Bowles, Gintis, and Osborne 2001, 2002; Heckman and Rubinstein 2001; Heckman, Stixrud, and Urzua 2006; Borghaus, Duckworth, and Heckman forthcoming). In fact, such noncognitive skills may directly affect more than longer-term outcomes. Developmental research and the evidence in this chapter suggest that some noncognitive skills may also be implicitly linked to development and cognitive skills at kindergarten entrance (Diamond 2000; Duncan et al. 2007; Raver, Garner, and Smith-Donald 2007; Snow 2007; Blair et al. 2007; Diamond et al. 2007). Historically, educational policy makers and researchers who have studied schooling effects have focused on cognitive skills as measured by achievement scores as the most important and often as their sole measure of interest. Such focus might help explain persisting score gaps if noncognitive skills also influence later, long-term outcomes. Developmental researchers studying early childhood have included both cognitive and noncognitive skills in their research, but the specific links between cognitive and noncognitive skills are not yet well understood, and persisting score gaps are possible until these links become better known.
Article
The classical notion that the cerebellum and the basal ganglia are dedicated to motor control is under dispute given increasing evidence of their involvement in non-motor functions. Is it then impossible to characterize the functions of the cerebellum, the basal ganglia and the cerebral cortex in a simplistic manner? This paper presents a novel view that their computational roles can be characterized not by asking what are the "goals" of their computation, such as motor or sensory, but by asking what are the "methods" of their computation, specifically, their learning algorithms. There is currently enough anatomical, physiological, and theoretical evidence to support the hypotheses that the cerebellum is a specialized organism for supervised learning, the basal ganglia are for reinforcement learning, and the cerebral cortex is for unsupervised learning.This paper investigates how the learning modules specialized for these three kinds of learning can be assembled into goal-oriented behaving systems. In general, supervised learning modules in the cerebellum can be utilized as "internal models" of the environment. Reinforcement learning modules in the basal ganglia enable action selection by an "evaluation" of environmental states. Unsupervised learning modules in the cerebral cortex can provide statistically efficient representation of the states of the environment and the behaving system. Two basic action selection architectures are shown, namely, reactive action selection and predictive action selection. They can be implemented within the anatomical constraint of the network linking these structures. Furthermore, the use of the cerebellar supervised learning modules for state estimation, behavioral simulation, and encapsulation of learned skill is considered. Finally, the usefulness of such theoretical frameworks in interpreting brain imaging data is demonstrated in the paradigm of procedural learning.
Article
Locomotion - moving the body from place to place - is one of infants' greatest achievements. In addition to conquering gravity, infants must cope with variable and novel constraints on balance and propulsion. At the same time that they are learning to move, changes in infants' bodies, skills, and environments change the biomechanical constraints on movement. Recent work highlights both flexibility and specificity in infants' responses to novel and variable situations, demonstrating that infants are learning to learn as they master locomotion. Within sitting, crawling, cruising, and walking postures, experienced infants adapt their locomotor responses to the current biomechanical constraints on movement. However, what infants have learned about coping with variability and novelty in earlier-developing postures does not transfer to later-developing postures.