ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The urgency facet of impulsivity, that is, the tendency to act rashly in response to intense emotional contexts [Cyders, M. A., & Smith, G. T. (2008). Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: positive and negative urgency. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 807-828], has been related to a wide range of maladaptive behaviours. The present study further investigates the role of urgency in problematic behaviours by considering distinct psychological mechanisms that may underlie this component of impulsivity. With this aim, 95 volunteer participants were screened with self-reported questionnaires assessing urgency and three problematic behaviours (compulsive buying, excessive mobile phone use, excessive Internet use). They performed two laboratory tasks: a stop-signal task designed to assess the capacity to inhibit prepotent responses in response to both neutral and emotional stimuli; and the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) measuring the ability to take into account the future consequences of an action. A poor ability to inhibit prepotent responses in the emotional condition of the stop-signal task was found to predict more disadvantageous choices in the IGT, which ultimately results in higher urgency and more problematic behaviours. These findings shed new light on the construct of urgency, its related psychological mechanisms, and its role in problematic behaviours.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/copyright
Author's personal copy
The role of urgency and its underlying psychological mechanisms
in problematic behaviours
Joël Billieux
a
,
b
,
*
, Philippe Gay
a
,
b
, Lucien Rochat
a
,
b
, Martial Van der Linden
a
,
b
,
c
a
Cognitive Psychopathology and Neuropsychology Unit, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
b
Swiss Centre for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
c
Cognitive Psychopathology Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
article info
Article history:
Received 3 December 2009
Received in revised form
8 June 2010
Accepted 16 July 2010
Keywords:
Urgency
Impulsivity
Inhibition
Decision making
Problematic behaviour
abstract
The urgency facet of impulsivity, that is, the tendency to act rashly in response to intense emotional
contexts [Cyders, M. A., & Smith, G. T. (2008). Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: positive and
negative urgency. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 807e828], has been related to a wide range of maladaptive
behaviours. The present study further investigates the role of urgency in problematic behaviours by
considering distinct psychological mechanisms that may underlie this component of impulsivity. With
this aim, 95 volunteer participants were screened with self-reported questionnaires assessing urgency
and three problematic behaviours (compulsive buying, excessive mobile phone use, excessive Internet
use). They performed two laboratory tasks: a stop-signal task designed to assess the capacity to inhibit
prepotent responses in response to both neutral and emotional stimuli; and the Iowa Gambling Task
(IGT) measuring the ability to take into account the future consequences of an action. A poor ability to
inhibit prepotent responses in the emotional condition of the stop-signal task was found to predict more
disadvantageous choices in the IGT, which ultimately results in higher urgency and more problematic
behaviours. These ndings shed new light on the construct of urgency, its related psychological mech-
anisms, and its role in problematic behaviours.
Ó2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Introduction
Emotions are fundamentally adaptive, as they motivate action
tendencies in response to the environment and trigger behaviours
to attain various goals such as satisfying a need or maintaining
homeostasis (i.e., to reduce the discrepancy between the current
state and the desired state; e.g., Frijda, 1986).
Nevertheless, empirical evidence suggests that emotions do not
always result in adaptive behaviours. Indeed, several studies have
shown that intense emotions may trigger risky or problematic
behaviours (i.e., behaviours that are in discordance with long-term
interests). In particular, research has shown that the experience of
intense negative emotions (e.g., anger, sadness) may promote the
occurrence of problematic behaviours such as binge drinking (e.g.,
Selby, Anestis, & Joiner, 2008), binge eating (e.g., Agras & Telch,
1998; Selby et al., 2008), non-suicidal self-injury (Nock &
Prinstein, 2004), compulsive buying (Miltenberger et al., 2003), or
escapism through excessive use of the Internet (Wan & Chiou,
2006). Investigators have proposed that risky or problematic
behaviours occurring in negative emotional contexts may serve to
relieve or reduce the experience of the negative emotion without
the individual considering the potential negative outcomes of these
behaviours (the self-medication hypothesis; e.g., Colder & Chassin,
1997; Jacobs,1986; Swendsen et al., 2000; Thorberg & Lyvers, 2006;
Tice, Bratslavsky, & Baumeister, 2001). Furthermore, it has been
argued that the proneness to enter into maladaptive behaviours in
a condition of negative affect may be at least partly due to
a weakness of the psychological mechanisms (e.g., executive
functions) involved in the voluntary or controlled regulation of
behaviours (e.g., Tice et al., 2001).
Several authors acknowledge that emotional states impair
executive functioning (Blair et al., 2007; Nock, Wedig, Holmberg, &
Hooley, 2008; Pessoa, 2009), which suggests that persons con-
fronted with intense emotions will have greater difculty in
exerting active or effortful control (e.g., to inhibit an aggressive
response in response to an emotion of anger). Pessoa (2009) has
proposed that emotional experiences recruit part of the cognitive
resources (e.g., via focusing on the affectively signicant stimulus
or the event having triggered the emotional experience) available
for effortful control, which impairs its effectiveness.
*Correspondence to: Joël Billieux, Cognitive Psychopathology and Neuropsy-
chology Unit, FPSE, University of Geneva, Boulevard du Pont dArve, 40, CH-1205
Geneva, Switzerland. Tel.: þ41 22 379 93 44; fax: þ41 22 379 93 59.
E-mail address: joel.billieux@unige.ch (J. Billieux).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Behaviour Research and Therapy
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat
0005-7967/$ esee front matter Ó2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.008
Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e1096
Author's personal copy
Therefore, it could be supposed that the occurrence of prob-
lematic or risky behaviours depends both on individual differences
in executive functioning implicated in effortful control (e.g., inhi-
bition functions, the central executive of the working memory) and
on individual differences in the way people experience the inten-
sity of emotions (emotional reactivity; e.g., see Nock et al., 2008).
From this perspective, the concept of urgencydthe facet of
impulsivity that refers to individual differences in the proneness to
act impulsively and rashly in emotional contexts (see Cyders &
Smith, 2008; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001)dmay reect a disposi-
tion toward problematic behaviours, depending on the earlier
mentioned psychological mechanisms.
The urgency facet of impulsivity
Researchers have for a decade recognised that impulsivity
reects a combination of multiple and separable psychological
dimensions (Dick et al., 2010; Enticott & Ogloff, 2006; Evenden,
1999; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). In an attempt to delimit the
facets underlying impulsivity, Whiteside and Lynam (2001)
administered several widely used questionnaires of impulsivity
and the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992)
to a large sample of undergraduate students. A factor analysis
conducted on these questionnaires resulted in a four-factor solu-
tion, which was the basis for the creation of a scale called the UPPS
Impulsive Behaviour Scale (UPPS). The four dimensions of impul-
sivity measured by the UPPS are (1) urgency, dened as the
tendency to experience strong reactions, frequently under
the condition of negative affect; (2) premeditation, dened as the
tendency to take into account the consequences of an act before
engaging in that act; (3) perseverance, dened as the ability to
remain focused on a task that may be boring and/or difcult; and
(4) sensation seeking, considered as a tendency to enjoy and pursue
activities that are exciting and openness to trying newexperiences.
Interestingly, the distinction between these four self-reported
facets of impulsivity has been conrmed by means of semi-
structured interviews (Smith et al., 2007), which provides conver-
gent validity for this model of impulsivity. Among these various
dimensions of impulsive behaviours, urgency has recently received
increased attention, in particular in the domain of problematic or
maladaptive behaviours (for a review, see Cyders & Smith, 2008).
High levels of self-reported urgency (assessed with the UPPS;
Whiteside & Lynam, 2001) are associated with the occurrence of
a wide range of problematic behaviours, encompassing alcohol and
drug abuse (e.g., Anestis, Selby, & Joiner, 2007; Verdejo-García,
Bechara, Recknor, & Pérez-García, 2007); an increased craving for
cigarettes (Billieux, Van der Linden, & Ceschi, 2007; Doran, Cook,
McChargue, & Spring, 2009); eating disorders (e.g., Fischer,
Anderson, & Smith, 2004); aggressive behaviours (Miller, Flory,
Lynam, & Leukefeld, 2003); borderline symptoms (Miller et al.,
2003); problem gambling (e.g., Smith et al., 2007); compulsive
buying (Billieux, Rochat, Rebetez, & Van der Linden, 2008); and
problematic use of the mobile phone (Billieux, Van der Linden,
dAcremont, Ceschi, & Zermatten, 2007; Billieux, Van der Linden,
& Rochat, 2008). Moreover, this facet of impulsivity is also the
best predictor of several problematic behaviours when controlling
for other facets of impulsivity and anxiety or depression symptoms
(e.g., Billieux, Rochat, et al., 2008; Billieux, Van der Linden, et al.,
2008). In this context, researchers have recently suggested that
the construct of urgency is a strong indicator of an individuals
proneness to engage in a variety of maladaptive behaviours to
regulate or relieve negative emotional experience (Cyders & Smith,
2008; Fischer et al., 2004; Selby et al., 2008).
Interestingly, urgency-related behaviours have also been shown
to occur not only in experiences of negative emotions, but also in
contexts of intense positive emotions. Indeed, positive urgency (i.e.,
the tendency to act rashly in intense positive affect) relies on
a variety of problematic behaviours such as problem gambling,
alcohol abuse, or risky sexual behaviour (Cyders & Smith, 2007;
Cyders et al., 2007; Zapolski, Cyders, & Smith, 2009) and has
been proposed to reect a tendency to enter into problematic
behaviours to regulate positive emotions by maintaining or even
enhancing them (Cyders & Smith, 2008). Of note, strong correla-
tions exist between positive and negative urgency (Cyders et al.,
2007), which suggests that these two facets of impulsivity may
be related to common underlying psychological mechanisms.
Urgency and related psychological mechanisms
Although it is now relatively well established that urgency-
related behaviours often aim to regulate emotions, the specic
psychological mechanisms underlying this facet of impulsivity
remain poorly investigated. The next section provides a review of
the studies that have addressed this topic.
Urgency and the ability to inhibit prepotent responses
Bechara and Van der Linden (2005) initially proposed that
a poor capacity for prepotent response inhibition, that is, the ability
to deliberately control or suppress an automatic response
(Friedman & Miyake, 2004; Miyake et al., 2000), may underlie
urgency. Indeed, this type of inhibition has been considered as
central to the occurrence of impulsive and unplanned behaviours
(e.g., Enticott & Ogloff, 2006; Logan, Schachar, & Tannock,1997) and
has also been found to be impaired in a variety of psychopatho-
logical states characterised by high urgency levels, such as patho-
logical gambling (Goudriaan, Oosterlaan, de Beurs, & Van den Brink,
2006) or alcohol dependence (Noël et al., 2001). The ability to
inhibit prepotent or automatic response is generally assessed via
the go/no-goand the stop-signalparadigms (see Verbruggen &
Logan, 2008, for a complete description and a consideration of the
differences between these two tasks). In a classical go/no-go task,
the participant has to respond as quickly as possible to a target
stimulus (the gotrials) presented most of the time (generally 75%
of the trials) and not to respond to another stimulus (the no-go
trials) presented less often (generally 25% of the trials). In a stop-
signal task, participants usually perform a choice reaction task (the
no-stoptrials). On a random part of the trials (generally 25%),
a stop signal (e.g., an auditory tone) is presented. Participants are
instructed not to make the choice reaction task (the stoptrials)
when the stop signal is presented.
To the best of our knowledge, there are currently only two
studies that have investigated the relationships between self-
reported urgency assessed with the UPPS and inhibition of prepo-
tent responses measured by a go/no-go task (Gay, Rochat, Billieux,
dAcremont, & Van der Linden, 2008; Perales, Verdejo-Garcia,
Moya, Lozano, & Perez-Garcia, 2009). These studies have obtained
mixed results: Gay et al. (2008) found a signicant positive rela-
tionship between urgency and the number of commission errors,
whereas Perales et al. (2009) found no relationship between
urgency and performance on a go/no-go task. From a methodolog-
ical point of view, a direct comparison of these two studies may be
hazardous, as they have been done with distinctive samples.
Indeed, Perales et al. (2009) used a small sample of female under-
graduate students (n¼32) characterised by either high or low
impulsivity scores, whereas Gay et al. (2008) used a larger sample
(n¼126) that was more representative of the general population,
consisting of volunteer participants from the community composed
of a quasi equal number of males and females.
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e10961086
Author's personal copy
With regard to the processes involved in these tasks, a recent
study by Verbruggen and Logan (2008) has shown that go/no-go
tasks allow the development of automatic associative learning across
trials, as the same stimuli are always associated with go/no-go trials.
Thus, only the beginning of the task could be considered as assessing
effortful inhibition (i.e., the moment in which the associative
learning is not yet effective), (.) whereas the remainder of the task
relies on automatic subcortical processes. In contrast, stop-signal
tasks prevent the development of an automatic associative learning,
as target stimuli are inconsistently associated with the stop signal (as
the stop-signal onset follows the appearance of the stimulus).
Consequently, it is likely that the stop-signal task is more suited than
the go/no-go task to measure effortful or cognitive inhibition and
thus to be a better potential candidate to assess one of the suggested
mechanisms underlying the urgency facet of impulsivity.
No studies have explored the relationships between urgency
and the ability to inhibit a prepotent response in emotional situa-
tions (e.g., by using emotional material). This latter point is of
greater importance because, on the one hand, urgency has been
conceptualized as a tendency to act impulsively in an emotional
context (Cyders & Smith, 2008) and, on the other hand, emotional
contexts have been shown to impair the ability to inhibit a prepo-
tent response (Schulz et al., 2007; Verbruggen & De Houwer, 2007).
More specically, Schulz et al. (2007) have demonstrated by using
a go/no-go task that included faces (with or without an emotional
expression) that it is more difcult to inhibit a prepotent response
for an emotional stimulus than it is for a neutral one, whereas
Verbruggen and De Houwer (2007) found that prepotent response
inhibition (assessed through a stop-signal paradigm) was
compromised when participants viewed high versus low
emotionally arousing pictures. More precisely, emotional scenes of
the International Affective Picture System (Lang, Bradley, &
Cuthbert, 1997) presented before the go and the stop trials of the
task increased the difculty in successfully inhibiting the prepotent
response. The results of this study support the idea that arousal
induced by emotional pictures (both negative and positive)
increased the difculty in inhibiting an automatic response, which
could be because highly emotional stimuli (i.e., stimuli with high
arousal properties) automatically draw attentional resources and
interrupt ongoing activities, which in turn results in fewer
resources available for effortful control (e.g., Schimmack, 2005;
Verbruggen & De Houwer, 2007). Therefore, it is possible that
individuals with a higher level of urgency are characterised by
lower inhibition capacities, which make them less able to inhibit
acting rashly in emotional contexts, and/or they experience
emotions more strongly (i.e., they have high emotional reactivity),
which results in increased difculties in controlling their behav-
iours in such situations.
Urgency may also be underlain by a combination of distinct
psychological processes rather than solely by difculties in inhib-
iting automatized or prepotent response. Such a hypothesis may
also be supported by the facts that (1) this inhibitory function is
related to several other executive processes (e.g., updating of the
central executive of the working memory, shifting, resistance to
proactive interference; Friedman & Miyake, 2004; Miyake et al.,
2000); and (2) the relationship found by Gay et al. (2008)
between urgency and the ability to inhibit prepotent responses is
of relatively small amplitude, which implies that other psycholog-
ical mechanisms may be related to this facet of impulsivity.
Urgency and the ability to take into account
the consequences of an action
Cyders and Smith (2008) have proposed that urgency-related
behaviours are associated with an elevated focus on the present
moment (e.g., the desire to obtain relief from a negative emotion)
and a reduced focus on long-term consequences, which ultimately
results in maladaptive or risky behaviours. Of note,a majority of the
urgency items of the UPPS explicitly refer to acting without
consideration of future consequences in negative emotional
contexts (e.g., Item 14: When I feel bad, I will often do things I later
regret in order to make myself feel better now;Item24:When I
am upset I often act without thinking; Item 36: I often make
matters worse because I act without thinking when I am upset).
Consequently, it may be hypothesized that high urgency is related
to difculty in taking into account the future consequences of an
action in emotional contexts; that is, individuals may have
a tendency to make disadvantageous choices in those situations.
The psychological mechanism underlying the ability to make
decisions by considering its future outcomes has been theorized in
the inuential somatic marker hypothesistheory (Damasio,1994;
Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1991), which argues that in a context
of uncertainty, individuals could unconsciously take into account
the positive and negative outcomes of their actions on the basis of
emotional or somatic markers, that is, anticipatory emotional
reactions provoked by this decision that depend on the conse-
quences associated with similar decisions in the past. From this
perspective, elevated levels of urgency could result in an inability to
act by taking into account somatic markers. Empirical support of
the somatic marker hypothesis is largely based on the ndings of
a laboratory-based emotional decision-making task: the Iowa
Gambling Task (IGT; Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson,
1994). This task has been developed to factor in several aspects of
real-life decision making such as uncertainty, risk, and evaluation
of rewards and punishments. In the IGT, participants have to select
100 cards from four different decks that are apparently identical.
Each choice leads either to a nancial reward or to a combinationof
anancial reward and a penalty. The two decks are, in fact,
disadvantageous in the long term; that is, they are associated with
greater nancial penalty. A central aspect of the IGT, which makes it
relevant for studying urgency-related behaviours, is that in this task
participants have to forego short-term gratication to obtain long-
term prots (by progressively switching their choices from the two
risky decks to the two advantageous decks).
Several authors sustain that the rst part of the IGT reects
decision making under uncertainty, whereas the second part of the
IGT measures decision making under risk (Bechara & Martin, 2004;
Brand, Labudda, & Markowitsch, 2006; Noël, Bechara, Dan, Hanak,
& Verbanck, 2007). Indeed, it has been shown that decisions made
in the IGT progressively switch from uncertain choices involving no
conscious knowledge about reinforcement contingencies of the
decks to choices made in a context in which conscious knowledge
has emerged concerning the reinforcement contingencies of the
various decks (Persaud, McLeod, & Cowey, 2009). Thus, it could be
supposed that the rst part of the IGT relies largely on unconscious
processes (e.g., somatic markers), whereas the second part of the
task also recruits effortful conscious processes. Dunn, Dalgleish,
and Lawrence (2006) proposed that the IGT is multi-determined
and relies on various effortful mechanisms, at least in its second
part. These authors proposed that advantageous choices in the IGT
require updating of the content of working memory (to be able to
bear in mind the reward/punishment schedule of the task), inhib-
iting responding to previously rewarding decks, and shifting ones
preference from risky decks to advantageous decks.
To the best of our knowledge, only four studies have considered
the relationships between the IGT and the various facets of self-
reported impulsivity assessed with the UPPS (Dolan, Bechara, &
Nathan, 2008; Perales et al., 2009; Xiao et al., 2009; Zermatten,
Van der Linden, dAcremont, Jermann, & Bechara, 2005). These
studies have shown that urgency is associated with less
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e1096 1087
Author's personal copy
advantageous choices in the IGT both in adolescents (Xiao et al.,
2009) and in patients with substance use disorder (Dolan et al.,
2008). Zermatten et al. (2005), however, found that another facet
of impulsivity, namely, lack of premeditation, predicted lower
performance in the IGT. In another study, Perales et al. (2009) found
no relationship between the IGT and the various facets of impul-
sivity. However, several reasons lead us to believe that poor
performance on the IGT should be related to higher levels of
urgency.
First, the IGT has for a long time been conceptualized as a task
that assesses decision making in emotional contexts (e.g., Bechara,
2004; Xiao et al., 2009). This implies that the task is probably more
suited as a reection of a psychological mechanism underlying the
urgency facet of impulsivity, that is, acting rashly in emotional
contexts, than as a reection of the lack of premeditation facet of
impulsivity, that is, the capacity to plan and deliberate in general,
not specically in emotional contexts. Second, it has been shown
that urgency is the facet of impulsivity that better predicts the
occurrence of problematic behaviours characterised by poor
performance in the IGT, such as pathological gambling (e.g.,
Goudriaan, Oosterlaan, de Beurs, & van den Brink, 2005) and
alcohol or drug use (e.g., Bechara et al., 2001; Noël et al., 2007).
Finally, the two studies that did not nd a link between the IGT and
urgency used small samples (about 30 participants), and Perales
et al. (2009) incorporated only a small sample of female under-
graduates with high or low impulsivity, which may have had an
impact on the ndings. As a consequence, further research remains
necessary to explore the relationships between the IGT and the
urgency facet of impulsivity.
Understanding the role of inhibition and decision making
in urgency manifestations
It seems that both the ability to inhibit prepotent responses and
the capacity to take into account the future consequences of actions
could represent specic psychological mechanisms underlying the
urgency facet of impulsivity. However, it is also possible that these
two mechanisms act conjointly to promote urgency-related
behaviours. Interestingly, recent evidence has shown that poor
ability to inhibit prepotent response predicts a proneness to take
more risky cards in the IGT, especially in the second part of the task,
which represents making choices under risk (Noël et al., 2007;
Shuster & Toplak, 2009). From this perspective, central to our
argument is how inhibition difculties promote decisions made
without forethought toward future consequences in a situation of
decision making under risk. To this end, an in-depth consideration
of the various processes involved in risky decision making in the
IGT is necessary.
Decision making under risk is a complex process that requires,
on the one hand, consideration of factual knowledgeabout the
choice and, on the other hand, reection about the potential
consequences of that choice (e.g., Bechara & Van der Linden, 2005).
In reference to the IGT, a crucial question is the nature of the choices
made by the participants in its second part: Do they choose a card
on the basis of unconscious factual knowledge, depending on their
previous choices (i.e., mainly by taking into account information
provided by somatic markers), or do they make choices in accor-
dance with conscious knowledge about the reinforcement contin-
gencies of the various decks? This question is important because if
the participants have acquired conscious knowledge about the IGT,
their choices may also be inuenced by controlled or executive
processes (e.g., inhibiting the choice of a card in a deck known to be
risky). In fact, this question has been addressed by Maia and
McClelland (2004), who, by asking participants specic questions
about the game, showed that they acquired conscious conceptual
knowledge about the IGT across trials (e.g., concerning the rein-
forcement contingencies of the four decks). More recently, Persaud
et al. (2009) conducted a study in which participants performed the
IGT and were asked to wager an amount of money after each deck
selection. The hypothesis of the authors is that conscious knowl-
edge about the IGT occurs when participants consistently wage the
maximum amount of money after selecting the positive packs.
Interestingly, this study revealed that advantageous wagering
(which reects an awareness of the strategy used) began approxi-
mately on trial 70 (13).
From this perspective, it can be postulated that in the second
part of the IGT, the participants have to inhibit responses to
previously rewarding decks (the disadvantageous decks associated
with important positive reinforcements in the beginning of the
task) before being able to make a choice that takes into account the
conscious knowledge they have acquired during the task. Indeed,
the last trials of the IGT require participants to make a choice on the
basis of the factual knowledge they have acquired about the task
(e.g., which decks represent greater gains and/or greater losses) and
also on the basis of the consequences of the choice made (e.g., the
likelihood of going bankrupt after that choice; the consideration of
the amount of money already won or lost). In other words, it is
possible that urgency-related behaviours, considered as rash and
non-deliberated actions in emotional contexts, are promoted by
weakness of the inhibitory control that precludes entering into
areexive deliberation about an action (e.g., consideration of the
long-term outcomes and not only the immediate reward). From
a more general point of view, the distinction between an automatic
system on the one hand and an analytical or deliberative system on
the other hand (Evans, 2003, 2004; Figner, Mackinlay, Wilkening, &
Weber, 2009) is of much interest when considering urgency-related
behaviours. More precisely, choices resulting from the automatic
system depend on rapid and parallel processes (i.e., they do not
recruit controlled or executive processes) created via associative
learning and which are unconscious until the decision is made. In
contrast, the analytical system is slower and sequential, requires
controlled cognitive processes (such as executive functions and
working memory), and permits abstract and hypothetical thinking
(which is not the case for the automatic system). Thus, it could be
supposed that persons with high urgency have more difculty
entering into an analytical or deliberative decision-making process,
resulting more frequently in rash and unplanned actions that can
potentially have tangible negative consequences.
The current study
The main goal of the current study was to further investigate the
relations between the urgency facet of impulsivity and both the
capacity to inhibit prepotent responses and the ability to take into
account future consequences of an action. Second, we were also
interested in conrming the critical role of urgency and related
psychological mechanisms in the occurrence of problematic or
risky behaviours. Several a priori hypotheses emerged from our
review and criticism of the existing literature.
First, we postulated that both (1) a lower ability to inhibit
a prepotent response (especially in an emotional context) and (2)
difculty in taking into account future outcomes when making
a choice in an emotional context predict a higher level of urgency.
As a result of the studies that found that low inhibition capacities
predict poor performance in the IGT, we also examined the possi-
bility that decision making mediates the relation between inhibi-
tion and urgency, that is, whether this latter relation may exist
through the impact of inhibition on the ability to consider long-
term outcomes of an action in the IGT.
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e10961088
Author's personal copy
Second, we expected urgency to predict the involvement in
problematic behaviours, as this dimension of impulsivity has been
related to proneness to act rashly with the desire to relieve negative
emotions (e.g., Cyders & Smith, 2008).
To test these hypotheses, we conducted a study in which
volunteer participants from the community performed two labo-
ratory tasks and completed several self-reported questionnaires.
More precisely, the ability to inhibit prepotent responses was
measured by means of a stop-signal task, using faces with different
facial expressions (neutral, joy, sadness). Human faces with positive
and negative expressions (joy and sadness) were used as
emotionally charged stimuli in the stop-signal task (e.g., see Schulz
et al., 2007, for a study using emotional faces in an inhibition task).
We decided to incorporate both negative and positive stimuli
because Cyders et al. (2007) have shown that urgency-related
behaviours may also occur in response to intense positive
emotions. The ability to take into account the future consequences
of an action in an emotional context was measured with the IGT
(Bechara et al., 1994). The urgency and the lack of premeditation
facets of impulsivity were assessed with the UPPS (Whiteside &
Lynam, 2001). The lack of premeditation facet of impulsivity of
the UPPS was also considered in the current study, as it has
frequently been shown to positively correlate with urgency
(Whiteside & Lynam, 2001; Whiteside, Lynam, Miller, & Reynolds,
2005) and has also been associated with an inability to make
advantageous choices in the IGT (Zermatten et al., 2005). Finally,
three distinct potential problematic behaviours were considered,
namely, compulsive buying, problematic use of the Internet, and
problematic use of the mobile phone. We chose to focus on these
three behaviours for three reasons. First, these types of problematic
behaviours have all been related to high levels of urgency (e.g.,
Billieux, Rochat, et al., 2008; Billieux, Van der Linden, dAcremont,
et al., 2007). Second, these behaviours have often been consid-
ered as a way to relieve or enhance emotions or moods (e.g., Cyders
& Smith, 2008; Miltenberger et al., 2003; Wan & Chiou, 2006).
Finally, empirical evidence supports these problematic behaviours
to be well represented in non-clinical participants from the
community (e.g., Billieux, Rochat, et al., 2008; Billieux, Van der
Linden, dAcremont, et al., 2007; Khazaal et al., 2008). The
following self-reported questionnaires were used to assess these
problematic behaviours: the Questionnaire about Buying Behaviour
(QBB; Lejoyeux, Mathieu, Embouazza, Huet, & Lequen, 2007), the
Problematic Use of the Mobile Phone Questionnaire (PUMPQ;
Billieux, Van der Linden, et al., 2008), and the Internet Addiction
Test (IAT; Young, 1999).
Methods
Participants and procedure
Participants were volunteers who received no compensation for
their inclusion in the study. They were recruited by means of
advertisements. The inclusion criterion was being a native speaker
of French. Exclusion criteria were any recent or ongoing major
depressive episode or anxiety disorder, and any reported neuro-
logical disorder. The sample comprised 95 participants (48 females
and 47 males) with an average age of 23.31 years (range 18e30,
SD ¼3.07). The average number of years of education was 14.17
(range 9e20, SD ¼2.13). Participants were individually tested in
a quiet laboratory. They signed an informed consent form before
doing the experiment. Participants performed the stop-signal task
and the IGT (half of the participants performed the stop-signal task
rst and the other half performed the Iowa Gambling Task rst) and
were then screened with various self-reported questionnaires
assessing impulsivity and problematic behaviours. The order of the
questionnaires was randomized across participants to control for
potential boredom and fatigue effects.
Measures
Emotional Stop-Signal Task
The ability to inhibit prepotent response following the presen-
tation of neutral or emotional stimuli was assessed with a task
inspired from a classical stop-signal paradigm (see Logan, 1994;
Logan et al., 1997). The stimuli used in the task consisted of
human faces with three types of emotional expressions: joy,
neutral, and sadness. All pictures were selected from the Karolinska
Directed Expressional Faces set (KDEF; Goeleven, De Raedt,
Leyman, & Verschuere, 2008; Lundqvist, Flykt, & Öhman, 1998).
The task had two distinct parts. In the rst one, used to build up
a prepotent categorization response, participants were presented
with 42 trials in which they had to categorize the gender of faces
(1/3 joy, 1/3 neutral, 1/3 sadness) as quickly as possible without
making mistakes. The cue stimulus (the face) was preceded by
axation cross displayed for 500 ms, followed by a blank screen
shown for 1000 ms. The second part of the task consisted of two
blocks of 96 trials in which participants were told not to respond
(i.e., to inhibit the prepotent response of gender categorization)
when they heard a computer-emitted tone (25% of the trials), but
otherwise to keep performing the same categorization task as
quickly and accuratelyas possible. In the second part of the task, 32
faces (16 male and 16 female, each with the three different
emotional expressions) of the KDEF were used. Each stimulus was
presented twice (once in the rst block and once in the second
block). Stop signals were presented at predetermined intervals
before the subjects expected response. The length of the interval
before the occurrence of the stop signal was 250 ms, 300 ms,
350 ms, or 400 ms (but two identical intervals were never pre-
sented in sequence). The dependent variable that reects the
latency of the inhibitory process is the SSRT (see Logan, 1994),
which corresponds to the latency of the inhibitory process (a high
SSRT corresponds to a lower prepotent response inhibition
capacity). The latency of the stop process can be estimated from the
start and nish of the stop process. The start of the stop process is
experimentally controlled by the interval before the occurrence of
the stop signal, but the nish time has to be inferred from the
reaction time distribution in the observed no-stop trials. If
responses are not stopped in n% of the stop trials, the nish of the
stop process is on average equal to the n
th
percentile of the reaction
time distribution in go trials. The stop-signal interval is subtracted
from the n
th
reaction time to estimate SSRT. SSRTs are calculated for
each interval and then averaged. SSRTs are calculated separately for
the neutral, positive, and negative condition. We also computed an
SSRT for the emotional condition by considering the positive and
negative conditions together. Because several of our statistical
analyses could be sensitive to non-normal variables, the SSRTs were
transformed by using natural logarithm to decrease the skewness
of their distribution. Because of technical problems, data for
the stop-signal task are missing for three participants. Paired
sample t-tests revealed no difference in SSRTs between neutral
and emotional conditions, t(91) ¼.92, p¼.36. Inhibition rates
were 72.93% and 71.19% for the neutral and emotional conditions,
respectively. The comparison of the inhibition rates between
neutral and emotional conditions is not signicant, t(91) ¼.92,
p¼.36.
Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)
In the IGT (Bechara et al., 1994), participants have to choose one
card at a time from four available decks (A, B, C, and D). The task
requires the participant to make 100 choices (100 trials), and in
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e1096 1089
Author's personal copy
each trial, participants may win or lose a certain amount of money.
The aim is to gain as much money as possible, and the participant
starts the task with 2000 Swiss Francs (of fake money) in his or her
account. During game instruction, participants are told that some
decks are more advantageous than others, but they do not know
which decks are better. Actually, two of the four decks (A and B)
produce immediate large rewards but higher punishment, at
unpredictable points, than the other two decks. In the long run,
decks A and B are disadvantageous. The other two decks (C and D)
produce immediate modest rewards, but lower punishment. These
two decks are advantageous in the long run. To calculate a score
that takes into account the evolution of subjectschoices, we
divided performance into ve blocks, representing ve periods of
20-card selection. In each block of 20 cards, the number of cards
selected from advantageous decks was calculated (CþD). A score
for each block, as well as a total score, was obtained. Following
recent proposals by Bechara and Martin (2004) and Brand et al.
(2006), we also made the distinction between the initial phase of
the IGT (the rst 40 trials), in which participants learn to make
choices but without any explicit knowledge about the reinforce-
ment contingencies of the various decks of the task (decision under
uncertainty), and the second part of the IGT (the 40 last trials), in
which choices become more inuenced by explicit knowledge
about the risks associated with each deck (decision under risk).
UPPS Impulsive Behaviour Scale (UPPS)
The French version of the UPPS (Van der Linden et al., 2006),
translated from Whiteside and Lynam (2001), consists of 45 items
that evaluate the four different facets of impulsivity, labelled
negative urgency (12 items, e.g., When I feel bad, I will often do
things I later regret in order to make myself feel better now), (lack
of) premeditation (11 items, e.g., I am a cautious person), (lack of)
perseverance (10 items, e.g., I concentrate easily), and sensation
seeking (12 items, e.g., I will try anything once). All items are
scored on a Likert scale from 1 ¼I agree stronglyto 4 ¼I disagree
strongly, with higher scores reecting higher impulsivity on the
respective facet. A validation study found the high internal
consistency of the French version and its factorial structure to be
similar to the original English UPPS (Van der Linden et al., 2006). In
the present study, internal consistency is very high for the four
facets of impulsivity, namely, negative urgency (Cronbachs
alpha ¼.85), lack of premeditation (Cronbachs alpha ¼.81), lack of
perseverance (Cronbachs alpha ¼.84), and sensation seeking
(Cronbachs alpha ¼.83).
Problematic Use of the Mobile Phone Questionnaire (PUMPQ)
The PUMPQ (Billieux, Van der Linden, et al., 2008) consists of 30
items measuring four different facets of problematic use of the
mobile phone (prohibited use, dangerous use, nancial problems,
and dependence symptoms). The total score of the PUMPQ was
used as a general measure of problematic use of the mobile phone.
All items are scored on a Likert scale from 1 ¼I agree stronglyto
4¼I disagree strongly, with higher scores implying higher
dependence on the mobile phone. The PUMPQ has been shown to
have good psychometrical properties (Billieux, Van der Linden,
et al., 2008). The internal reliability of the PUMPQ in the current
study is very high (Cronbachs alpha ¼.89).
Questionnaire about Buying Behaviour (QBB)
The QBB (Lejoyeux, Tassain, Solomon, & Adès, 1997) consists of
19 items representing major features of compulsive buying (e.g.,
urges to shop and buy, negative feedback from family and friends,
post-purchase guilt). Items on the scales are scored 0 or 1 (ques-
tions with yesor noanswers). Almost all studies using the QBB
have divided their samples into compulsive buyersand normal
buyers(Lejoyeux et al., 1997) on the basis of diagnostic criteria for
uncontrolled buying (see McElroy, Keck, Pope, & Smith, 1994) and
on positive answers to certain items of the QBB (e.g., post-purchase
guilt, tangible negative consequences of the purchasing behaviour).
However, in the present study, compulsive buying was conceptu-
alized from a dimensional perspective. Consequently, total scores
on the QBB were considered as reecting compulsive buying
tendencies, with higher scores reecting higher compulsive buying
proneness. To the best of our knowledge, no data are available for
the psychometric properties of the QBB. The internal reliability of
the QBB found in the present study is high (Cronbachs alpha ¼.76).
Internet Addiction Test (IAT)
The French IAT (Khazaal et al., 2008), adapted from Young
(1999), consists of 20 original items assessing the negative conse-
quences of overuse of the Internet (compromised social and indi-
vidual quality of life, compensatory usage of the Internet,
compromised scholarly/academic/working careers, compromised
time control, excitatory usage of the Internet). All items are scored
on a Likert scale (never, rarely, occasionally, often, always) corre-
sponding to scores of 1e
5, with higher scores implying higher
dependence on the Internet. A validation study found the high
internal consistency of the French version and its factorial structure
to be similar to the original English IAT (Khazaal et al., 2008). In the
current study, the internal reliability of the IAT is very high
(Cronbachs alpha ¼.92).
Statistical analyses
Two-tailed Pearsons correlations (with 5% signicance crite-
rion) were used to evaluate relations between variables. Pearsons
point-biserial correlation was used to evaluate the effect of gender
on task and questionnaire variables. Women were set at 1 and men
at 2; thus, a positive correlation corresponds to a higher score for
men.
Our various a priori hypotheses were tested by means of path
analysis (PA). PA evaluates the plausibility of a model that is
proposed about the relationships among a set of variables. A benet
of PA is that it permits the analysis of both direct and indirect
relations between the variables selected. PA analysis was computed
with Mplus by using the Maximum Likelihood statistic test
(Muthén & Muthén, 2006). Participants with missing data (n¼3)
were excluded from the PA. Indirect effects were computed via the
product of coefcient strategy (see Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, 2007)
provided by Mplus. To test single parameters, we adopted the 5%
signicance criterion (i.e., t-value of parameters of 1.96). Model t
was evaluated using the chi-square (
c
2
) and root mean square error
of approximation (RMSEA) indices. A non-signicant
c
2
indicates
a good t(Byrne, 1994). The RMSEA measures the difference
between the model and the sample data per degree of freedom,
with values below .05 indicating a close t and below .08 indicating
a reasonable t(Byrne, 2001). We also reported the comparative t
index (CFI). A CFI >.90 is generally interpreted as indicating an
acceptable t.
Results
Summary data of the tasks and the self-report questionnaires
are presented in Table 1.
Correlation analyses
Table 2 reports the two-tailed Pearsons correlations for all
variables of the study.
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e10961090
Author's personal copy
Correlation analysis revealed several signicant relationships
between the various facets of impulsivity. More precisely, we found
positive correlations between negative urgencyand both the lack of
premeditation and perseverance, and between the lack of
premeditation and the lack of perseverance. The sensation seeking
facet of the UPPS did not correlate with the other impulsivity facets.
The urgency facet of impulsivity negatively correlated with both
the total score on the IGT and the last 40 trials of this task. No
correlation was found between negative urgency and the rst 40
trials of the IGT. No signicant correlation was found between
negative urgency and the SSRTs on the stop-signal task, whether for
the emotional or the neutral conditions. Negative urgency nega-
tively correlated with gender, implying that females were found to
have higher scores on this facet of impulsivity. Finally, negative
urgency positively correlated with the various problematic behav-
iours measured (problematic use of the mobile phone, problematic
use of the Internet, and compulsive buying).
The lack of premeditation and lack of perseverance facets of
impulsivity were not related to the stop-signal task or to the IGT.
However, the lack of premeditation was positively correlated with
the proneness to problematic use of the mobile phone, whereas
the lack of perseverance was positivelycorrelated with the overuse
of the Internet. Nevertheless, the sizes of these two correlations are
smaller compared with those taking place between problematic
behaviours and the negative urgency facet of impulsivity.
A specic relation was found between a high level of sensation
seeking and a better ability to inhibit prepotent responses in the
positive and the emotional conditions of the stop-signal task.
However, no relation occured between sensation seeking and the
neutral and the negative conditions of the stop-signal task, the IGT,
and the various problematic behaviours measured.
From the performance of the tasks, relationships were found
between the IGT and the SSRTs on the stop-signal task. More
specically, inhibitory capacities for all emotional conditions of the
stop-signal task correlated with total scores on the IGT and with
scores on the last 40 trials of the task. However, no correlation was
found between the ability to inhibit prepotent responses in the
emotional conditions and the IGT scores for the rst 40 trials. In
addition, no relationship was found between the ability to inhibit
prepotent responses in the neutral condition and all variables of the
IGT.
Measurement model analyses
Our various a priori hypotheses about the psychological mech-
anisms underlying urgency were tested through PAs. The variables
entered in the model were selected on the basis of our hypotheses
and the correlation analysis. We decided to take into account two
variables for the stop-signal task, namely, the SSRTs for the neutral
condition of the task, and the SSRTs for the emotional conditions.
We chose not to distinguish positive and negative stimuli, as
urgency-related behaviours have been shown to rely on both
positive and negative emotional contexts (see Cyders & Smith,
2008), and the correlations found between inhibition in positive
and negative conditions and the IGTare similar. Concerning the IGT,
we chose to consider only the last 40 trials for three distinct
reasons. First, rm conclusions based on the total score of the IGT
are hazardous, as the beginning and the end of the task appear not
to recruit identical types of psychological mechanisms (e.g., Dunn
et al., 2006; Persaud et al., 2009). Second, advantageous choices
in the second part of the IGT positively correlate with negative
urgency, which is not the case for the advantageous choices in the
rst part of the IGT. Third, the ability to inhibit the prepotent
response to emotional stimuli is specically related to the tendency
to make advantageous choices in the second part of the IGT, not in
Table 1
Descriptives for questionnaires and laboratory tasks.
Variables MSD
Impulsivity facets Negative urgency (UPPS) 26.27 6.24
Lack of premeditation (UPPS) 21.51 4.73
Lack of perseverance (UPPS) 19.28 4.82
Sensation seeking (UPPS) 31.31 7.26
Problematic behaviours Internet (IAT) 27.51 12.30
Mobile phone (PUMPQ) 47.62 12.30
Compulsive buying (QBB) 4.2 3.1
Decision making IGT: total score (trials 1e100) 56.78 15.13
IGT: uncertainty (trials 1e40) 20.49 7.00
IGT: risk (trials 61e100) 24.50 8.23
Prepotent response
inhibition
Stop signal: SSRT-neutral 154.88 54.35
Stop signal: SSRT-positive 155.40 50.60
Stop signal: SSRT-negative 160.38 41.88
Stop signal: SSRT-emotional 157.10 36.80
Note. UPPS ¼UPPS Impulsive Behaviour Scale; IAT ¼Internet Addiction Test;
PUMPQ ¼Problematic Use of the Mobile Phone Questionnaire; QBB ¼Questionnaire
about Buying Behaviour; IGT ¼Iowa Gambling Task; SSRT ¼Stop-Signal Reaction
Time.
Table 2
Pearsons correlation between the variables.
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15.
1. Age e
2. Gender .22* e
3. UPPS: negative urgency .14 L.30**e
4. UPPS: lack of premeditation .07 .13 .30**e
5. UPPS: lack of perseverance .06 .09 .32** .48*** e
6. UPPS: sensation seeking L.26* .19 .03 .07 .10 e
7. PUMPQ etotal score .01 .16 .44*** .23* .13 .08 e
8. QBB etotal score .10 .13 .47** .10 .16 .04 .46*** e
9. IAT etotal score .14 .01 .30** .03 .24* .01 .14 .26* e
10. IGT: total score .14 .16 L.25*.07 .01 .11 .12 .04 .09 e
11. IGT: trials 1e40 .06 .14 .15 .09 .01 .07 .11 .02 .00 .74*** e
12. IGT: trials 61e100 .10 .13 L.26**.04 .03 .09 .14 .08 .14 .81*** .27** e
13. Stop signal: SSRT-neutral .18 .12 .03 .15 .02 .17 .08 .10 .02 .03 .06 .02 e
14. Stop signal: SSRT-positive .18 .09 .05 .18 .02 L.26* .10 .11 .02 L.33** .20 L.27*.36** e
15. Stop signal: SSRT-negative .06 .02 .09 .09 .11 .15 .12 .06 .04 L.26*.16 L.28*.30** .31** e
16. Stop signal: SSRT-emotional .20 .11 .06 .08 .11 L.22* .10 .06 .13 L.33** .16 L.34** .41*** .78*** .76***
***p<.001; **p<.01; *p<.05.
Note. UPPS ¼UPPS Impulsive Behaviour Scale; PUMPQ ¼Problematic Use of the Mobile Phone Questionnaire; QBB ¼Questionnaire about Buying Behaviour; IAT ¼Internet
Addiction Test; IGT ¼Iowa Gambling Task; SSRT ¼Stop-Signal Reaction Time.
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e1096 1091
Author's personal copy
its rst part. Because correlation analysis revealed that females
have higher negative urgency scores than males have, gender was
incorporated as a moderator in the model. This moderation was
evaluated through the interaction between urgencyand gender and
the effect of urgency and gender on problematic behaviours. To this
end, a new variable was created by multiplying urgency with
gender (value for females ¼1; value for males ¼2). Urgency scores
were centered to reduce potential multicollinearity between the
main effects and interaction effects in the model (Cohen, Cohen,
West, & Aiken, 2003). The lack of premeditation was not incorpo-
rated into the model, as this facet of impulsivity is not related tothe
IGT or to the stop-signal task.
As depicted in Fig. 1, the following causal paths were computed:
(1) paths leading from the prepotent response inhibition capacities
(either in the emotional or neutral conditions) to the ability to make
advantageous decisions under risk; (2) paths leading from the ability
to make advantageous decisions under risk to the negative urgency
and to the interaction between negative urgency and gender; (3)
paths leading from the negative urgency facet of impulsivity to the
various problematic behaviours; (4) pathsleading from gender to the
various problematic behaviours; (5) paths leading from the inter-
action between urgency and gender to the various problematic
behaviours.The following indirect paths werealso computed: (1) the
indirect effect of inhibition (either in the emotional or neutral
conditions) on negative urgency through decision making; and (2)
the indirect effects of decision making on problematic behaviours
through urgency and through the interaction between urgency and
gender. We allowed the inhibition in response to neutral and
emotional stimuli to correlate, as we did for urgency and gender.
The tested model has a good t(
c
2
(16) ¼20.286, p¼.21;
RMSEA ¼.053, condence interval ¼[0, .115]; CFI ¼.953). Stan-
dardized regression weights (b) for the structural model are pre-
sented in Fig. 1. The model showedthat (1) the emotional condition
of the stop-signal task predicts the ability to make advantageous
choices in the second part of the IGT (b¼.39), which is not the
case for the neutral condition of the stop-signal task (b¼.13); (2)
the ability to make advantageous choices in the second part of the
IGT predicts the negative urgency facet of impulsivity (b¼-.24); (3)
the negative urgency facet of impulsivity predicts the various
problematic behaviours, namely, compulsive buying (b¼.49),
problematic use of the mobile phone (b¼.48), and problematic use
of the Internet (b¼.34). Problematic behaviours are not signi-
cantly predicted by gender or by the interaction between the
urgency facet of impulsivity and gender. Moreover, two signicant
indirect effects were found: (1) an indirect effect from the
emotional condition of the inhibition task to negative urgency
through decision making under risk (b¼.09); and (2) indirect
effects from decision making under risk to compulsive buying
(b¼.12), problematic use of the mobile phone (b¼.12), and
problematic use of the Internet (b¼.07) through the negative
urgency facet of impulsivity.
Discussion
The aim of the present study was rst to investigate the rela-
tions between the urgency facet of impulsivity and two cognitive
mechanismsdthe ability to inhibit a prepotent response and the
capacity to take into account the consequences of an actiondand
second to further conrm the role of urgency in the occurrence of
various problematic or risky behaviours. The results of the study
may be summarized as follows: (1) poor ability to inhibit prepotent
responses in emotional contexts predicts the tendency to make
disadvantageous choices in a situation of decision making under
risk and also indirectly predicts the negative urgency facet of
impulsivity through its effect on the ability to make decisions under
risk; (2) the tendency to make disadvantageous choices in a situa-
tion of decision making under risk predicts the negative urgency
component of impulsivity and also indirectly predicts the occur-
rence of problematic behaviours through its effect on the negative
urgency facet of impulsivity; (3) the negative urgency facet of
impulsivity predicts the occurrence of problematic behaviours; and
(4) being a female is related to higher urgency scores.
The present study shed new light on the role of urgency in
problematic behaviours through a direct investigation of its
underlying psychological mechanisms. Moreover, our data also
conrm and extend previous data highlighting that high urgency is
associated with more risky decisions in the IGT (Dolan et al., 2008;
Xiao et al., 2009). The discussion is divided in two distinct parts.
The rst part addresses in detail the question of the psychological
mechanisms underlying urgency, whereas the second part focuses
on the role of urgency in problematic behaviours.
The psychological mechanisms underlying urgency
The main nding of the study was that a low capacity to inhibit
a prepotent response in an emotional context (reected by the
emotional conditions of the stop-signal task) predicts a proneness
to act without forethought in a situation of decision making under
risk (the second part of the IGT), which ultimately results in more
elevated levels of negative urgency, that is, a proneness to act rashly
in intense negative emotional contexts.
First, our results clarify the role of prepotent response inhibition
in the ability to take into account future consequences of an action.
Indeed, disadvantageous choices in the second part of the IGT were
predicted by a weak capacity to inhibit a prepotent response in the
emotional condition of the stop-signal task but were unrelated to
performances in the neutral condition of that task. This latter
dissociation is all the more interesting as it is, to the best of our
knowledge, the rst time that the performances on an emotional
decision-making task have been specically predicted by the
performances on a task assessing prepotent response inhibition in
an emotional context. Moreover, this result conrms previous
ndings highlighting that in the IGT, choices progressively switch
. 41
-. 30
-. 16
-. 04
-. 01
-. 07
. 12 .1 0
. 48 . 49
. 34
-. 39
-. 07
-. 24
. 13
I nhi bi ti on
(n eu tr al co nt ex t)
De ci si on u nde r ri sk
In hi bi ti on
(e mo ti on al co nt ex ts )
Ne ga ti ve
Ur ge nc y
Pr ob le ma ti c
In te rn et
Pr ob le ma ti c
bu yi ng
Pr ob le ma ti c
mo b ile ph on e
Ge nd er
Ne ga ti ve Ur ge nc y x
Ge nd er
Fig. 1. Path analysis (
c
2
(16) ¼20.286, p¼.21; root mean square error of approx-
imation ¼.053, condence interval ¼[0, .115]; comparative t index ¼.953) including
standardized coefcients. Values of the bold single-headed arrows reect signicant
standardized regression weights (at the .05 level). The value of the dashed single-
headed arrow reects a non-signicant standardized regression weight. The value of
the bold double-headed arrow is a signicant correlation (at the .05 level).
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e10961092
Author's personal copy
from a context in which risks associated with each choice are
uncertain and mainly associated with unconscious (automatic)
processes to a situation in which risks associated with each choice
become more conscious and depend on controlled or executive
processes (e.g., Brand et al., 2006; Dunn et al., 2006). However, our
results also extend the ndings of previous studies by emphasizing
that risky decisions in the IGT arespecically predicted by difculty
in inhibiting a prepotent response when faced with emotional
stimuli (and not neutral stimuli). Considering that the IGT is
conceptualized as an emotional decision-making task aimed at
simulating real-life risky situations, that is, a situation in which
a choice has to be made by balancing short-term gratications with
potential negative future outcomes (Bechara et al., 1994), it is not
surprising that risky choices in this task are specically predicted
by the ability to inhibit a prepotent response in emotional or hot
contexts.
Note that, contrary to the current study, previous studies
reported a relationship between a tendency to make disadvanta-
geous choices in the IGT and poor inhibition capacities that were
assessed with a task using only neutral stimuli (Noël et al., 2007;
Shuster & Toplak, 2009). Nevertheless, this may possibly be
allotted to methodological concerns. In the rst study, Noël et al.
(2007) acknowledge that the task they chose to assess prepotent
response inhibition (the Hayling task; Burgess & Shallice, 1996)is
not pure and allows the use of additional conscious strategies (not
related to prepotent response inhibition). Consequently, the multi-
determined task they used to assess inhibition hinders rm
conclusions about the nature of the psychological mechanisms
related to the ability to make advantageous decisions in the IGT. In
the second study, Shuster and Toplak (2009) reported that poor
performances in a go/no-go task (made of neutral stimuli) are
associated with lower abilities to make advantageous choices in the
IGT. In fact, they found a signicant relationship between poor
inhibition and lower effective gains in the IGT (i.e., the amount of
hypothetical money won at the end of the task), but not with the
total of number of cards selected in the two advantageous decks C
and D (i.e., the gold standardmeasure in the IGT; Dunn et al.,
2006). This last result is similar to those of the present study.
The present research helps explain why high urgency individ-
uals often act rashly and without forethought in emotional
contexts, although it must be borne in mind that this tendency is
only indirectly related to impairment in the ability to inhibit
a prepotent response. In fact, we hypothesize that individuals with
a high level of urgency often nd themselves in emotional contexts
that interfere with their ability to act in a deliberative way, which
ultimately results in actions focused on immediate positive and
negative reinforcement. Indeed, emotional experiences have been
shown to interfere with the effectiveness of inhibitory control
(Schulz et al., 2007), meaning that behaviours occurring in intense
emotional contexts are more often rash, automatic, and unplanned.
Moreover, the greater the degree to which an emotional experience
is intense or arousing (whether positive or negative), the more this
emotional experience is associated with difculties in inhibiting
a prepotent response (Verbruggen & De Houwer, 2007).
From this perspective, we think that the absence of a direct
relationship between the inhibition task and the negative urgency
facet of impulsivity is possibly due to the stimuli used (faces with
positive or negative expressions) not being highly arousing. In fact,
emotional arousal properties of the pictures from the KDEF
(Goeleven et al., 2008) used in the current study and those from the
International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang et al., 1997) used
in the study of Verbruggen and De Houwer (2007) can easily be
compared, as both have been validated with the same procedure.
The arousal provoked by these two types of emotional stimuli was
measured with a 9-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all
arousing)to9(completely arousing). The mean arousal for the KDEF
is 3.72 for happy faces and 3.37 for sad faces (Goeleven et al., 2008),
whereas the mean arousal for the IAPS pictures used by Verbruggen
and De Houwer (2007) is 5.75 for positive pictures and 6.16 for
negative pictures. Thus, it appears that pictures from the IAPS are
more arousing than are the faces of the KDEF. Consequently, if
urgency reects poor inhibitory control in response to intense
emotions, we likely would have found a higher relationship
between this facet of impulsivity and inhibitory control by using
a stop-signal task designed with more arousing stimuli (such as the
emotional scenes of the IAPS).
Another potential explanation for the absence of a signicant
relationship between negative urgency and inhibition capacities is
that individual differences in the way people experience emotions
(i.e., individual differences in emotional reactivity; see Nock et al.,
2008) should have an important impact on their tendency to
urgency manifestations. Indeed, elevated levels of emotional
reactivity (e.g., proneness to experience emotion intensely or for
a prolonged period of time) have been shown to be negatively
correlated with self-assessed inhibitory control (Fabes et al., 1999;
Nock et al., 2008). Accordingly, urgency manifestations may be
promoted by a combination of elevated emotional reactivity and
a diminished ability not to act rashly and without forethought in
the face of intense emotional contexts. Nevertheless, further
studies are necessary to explore the relationships between urgency
and individual differences in emotional reactivity, as this latter
construct was not assessed in the present study.
We found, in addition, a gender effect on the negative urgency
facet of impulsivity, as women had higher scores on this facet of
impulsivity than men. However, this gender effect has already been
found in several studies performed with the UPPS (Billieux, Van der
Linden, et al., 2008; Gay et al., 2008) and is in accordance with
studies highlighting a greater occurrence of self-reported experi-
ences of negative affect in women than in men (Fujita, Diener, &
Sandvik, 1991).
Although the present research focuses on the psychological
mechanisms underlying urgency, we have to discuss here the
unexpected relationship found between sensation seeking and the
stop-signal task. More precisely, persons with a higher level of
sensation seeking are better at inhibiting prepotent responses
when confronted with emotional stimuli (especially positive
stimuli), which raise the relevance of considering the tentative
impact of sensation seeking on cognitive control. To this end, we
found the Dual Mechanisms of Control theory by Braver, Gray, and
Burgess (2007) of much interest. The central hypothesis of these
authors is that when persons activate information relevant to
a goal, cognitive control is likely to operate through two distinct
modes: a proactive control mode and a reactive control mode.
Proactive control implies active maintaining of goal-related infor-
mation in working memory (top-down processes), whereas reac-
tive control is characterised by activation of goal-related
information in reaction to the environmental context when
necessary (bottom-up processes). In this vein, a proactive mode of
cognitive control is probably more suited to perform well at the
stop-signal task. Indeed, active maintaining of the contextual
information relative to the task within working memory may
promote top-down bias (e.g., to exert control on a behaviour that
has become dominant), whereas reactive cognitive control remains
relatively unbiased and more inuenced by bottom-up inputs
(implying increased difculty in inhibiting a behaviour that has
become dominant). Consequently, our hypothesis is that high
sensation seekers will preferentially adopt a proactive mode of
cognitive control when performing the stop-signal task, implying
that they are more efcient in inhibiting behavioural responses that
have been previously automatized. Ultimately, the link between
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e1096 1093
Author's personal copy
high sensation seeking and better performances in the emotional
conditions of the stop-signal task can tentatively be allotted to the
fact that the adoption of a proactive mode of cognitive control helps
to overcome the emotional interference provoked by arousing
stimuli.
Urgency and problematic behaviours
The present study conrms that the urgency component of
impulsivity plays a crucial role in the occurrence of various prob-
lematic and maladaptive behaviours (Cyders & Smith, 2008, for
a review). More precisely, wefound negative urgency to predict the
occurrence of all of the problematic behaviours assessed, namely,
compulsive buying, problematic use of the mobile phone, and
problematic use of the Internet. Moreover, the current results add
supplementary support to the view that a high level of negative
urgency may result in maladaptive behaviours that serve to regu-
late affective states through the relief of negative emotions (e.g.,
Cyders & Smith, 2008; Selby et al., 2008). Indeed, research has, for
example, highlighted that both compulsive buying and excessive
use of the Internet may serve as behaviours used to cope with or
manage negative affective states (Miltenberger et al., 2003; Wan &
Chiou, 2006). Considering the psychological mechanisms under-
lying the urgency facet of impulsivity, these maladaptive behav-
iours could be viewed as actions performed with the short-term
perspective of emotion management through immediate positive
or negative reinforcement. In other words, and in accordance with
proposals by other researchers (Evans, 2003, 2004; Figner et al.,
2009), our results suggest that these rash actions may take place
when the experience of intense emotions interferes with inhibitory
control and precludes the activation of the demanding analytical or
deliberative processing, which is necessary to consider the imme-
diate benets in relation to the future costs. Ultimately, it could be
supposed that persons with a higher level of urgency are more at
risk for developing clinically relevant problems (e.g., addictive or
compulsive behaviours) as they become caught in a vicious circle in
which rash or unplanned actions performed in emotional contexts
result in negative outcomes (personal, professional, social),
promoting, in turn, the experience of negative emotions that may
be relieved by overwhelming urgency-related behaviours.
Of particular interest regarding the specicity of the various
impulsivity facets are strong associations between problematic
behaviours and negative urgency; however, the relationships
between these behaviours and lack of premeditation is slim or
non-existent. This nding conrms previous results highlighting
that, contrary to negative urgency, lack of premeditation is not
a predictor of both compulsive buying tendencies and dependence
on the mobile phone when the effect of all impulsivity facets is
controlled (Billieux, Rochat, et al., 2008; Billieux, Van der Linden,
dAcremont, et al., 2007; Billieux, Van der Linden, et al., 2008).
Thus, in contrast to urgency, a low level of premeditation may
reect a proneness to act without forethought in general, that is,
not in highly arousing emotional contexts. In fact, it could be
proposed that low premeditators are likely to make disadvanta-
geous decisions because of inadequate or non-adapted delibera-
tive processes (e.g., they do not use all relevant information when
making a decision). Indeed, it has been found that low premedi-
tation is a predictor of certain behaviours or decisions occurring in
less emotional or arousing contexts but which could have tangible
negative long-term consequences. As an example, low premedi-
tation is a predictor of being a smoker (Miller et al., 2003), which
could be considered as a prototypical behaviour implying devas-
tating long-term consequences for health. From this perspective,
starting to smoke could be viewed as the result of a poor delib-
erative process (if deliberation is referred to as a decision making
process that takes into account the balance between short-term
and long-term benets in the light of all factual relevant infor-
mation available for that decision; Evans, 2003; Figner et al.,
2009). Accordingly, it has been shown that low premeditators
make less advantageous choices than do high premeditators in
adelay discountingtask in which they have to choose between
a small amount of fake money that is immediately available or
a much higher amount of fake money that is delayed (Lynam &
Miller, 2004). However, among smokers, negative urgency (and
not lack of premeditation) predicts the intensity of craving (i.e.,
the desire to smoke that frequently results in an overwhelming
impulse to smoke) related to the relief from negative affect (e.g., in
response to anxiety, depression, or withdrawal symptoms;
Billieux, Van der Linden, & Ceschi, 2007; Doran et al., 2009). Such
distinctions between urgency and premeditation help explain
their similarities and dissimilarities. Although these two compo-
nents of impulsivity are related to a kind of myopiatoward long-
term consequences of certain actions, they especially differ in the
sense that they take place in coldsituations (characterised by
low emotional arousal) versus hotsituations (characterised by
high emotional arousal). This latter difference may account for the
fact that urgency, rather than lack of premeditation, predicts the
occurrence of a wide range of problematic behaviours performed
with the aim of regulating emotional experiences (e.g., Nock et al.,
2008; Selby et al., 2008).
Limitations
Some limitations of the current study warrant discussion. First,
potential predictions concerning maladaptive behaviours occurring
when individuals experience intense positive emotions are infer-
ential, as positive urgency was not directly assessed in the version
of the UPPS used in the present study. Nevertheless, we found that
a poor ability to inhibit prepotent responses in emotional contexts
(both positive and negative) results in more frequent rash and
short-term based actions (i.e., less advantageous choices in the IGT)
and higher levels of negative urgency. Thus, in accordance with
Cyders et al. (2007), who highlighted by using conrmatory factor
analysis that positive and negative urgency are highly correlated
and load on a common factor of emotionally based rash actions
we assume that these two constructs are possibly underlain by
similar psychological mechanisms. Such an explanation could on
the one hand account for the fact that urgency-related behaviours
may be triggered by both positive and negative intense emotional
contexts (Cyders & Smith, 2008) and on the other hand for the very
strong correlations taking place between positive and negative
urgency (e.g.,r¼.67, Verdejo-García, Lozano, Moya, Alcasar, &
Pérez-García, 2010;r¼.67, Zapolski et al., 2009). However, further
research assessing both positive and negative urgency is needed to
empirically test that these two impulsivity facets are underlain by
common psychological mechanisms.
Second, the current study comprises no mood manipulation or
measurement (e.g., pre-, post-, or during the stop-signal task). This
choice was motivated by our wish to investigate the relationship
between urgency and the ability to inhibit a prepotent response in
response to an emotional stimuli and not to induce a mood state in
participants (e.g., through lm or music) and then test the partic-
ipants with a classical stop-signal paradigm (not designed with
emotional stimuli). More precisely, on the basis of a conceptuali-
zation of urgency as the tendency to act rashly in response to
emotions (Cyders & Smith, 2008), we found it more relevant to use
a task designed to trigger emotions (the presentation of faces with
positive or negative expression activates the amygdala and related
emotion processing networks, see e.g., Hare, Tottenham, Davidson,
Glover, & Casey, 2005) rather than to induce mood states in the
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e10961094
Author's personal copy
participants. Indeed, mood states generally refer to an affective
state that is not related to a specic event and that is characterised
by longer duration, lower intensity, and a certain diffuseness
(Frijda, 1993). Nevertheless, we acknowledge that the use of
experimental mood induction is a relevant topic for further
research on urgency, especially considering that urgency-related
problematic behaviours have been associated with not only the
relief of intense negative emotions, but also with the relief of more
stable negative mood states (e.g., Anestis et al., 2007).
Third, the current study is cross-sectional in nature. Thus,
longitudinal studies are needed to investigate whether the
psychological mechanisms related to the urgency facet of impul-
sivity contribute to the progression and maintenance of problem-
atic behaviours. In addition, further research should also consider
the possibility of using more ecological measures to assess prone-
ness to problematic behaviour, rather than being restricted to self-
reports. For example, problematic behaviours could be measured
with diaries in which participants can record behaviours daily
during a certain period. In the framework of research on urgency,
this method is all the more relevant, as participants could be
instructed to pay particular attention to their emotional state
before their involvement in the concerned behaviour.
To summarize, the present study opens new prospects for the
comprehension of mechanisms underlying the urgency facet of
impulsivity and its role in problematic or maladaptive behaviour
(e.g., compulsive buying or overuse of the mobile phone and the
Internet). Ultimately, we think that our ndings could be general-
ized to other maladaptive behaviours occurring in response to
intense emotions (e.g., non-suicidal self-injury, risky sex, binge
drinking or eating).
Acknowledgments
We thank Bénédicte Wildhaber, Seraina Agramunt, Sabrina
Dunon, and Séverine Portier for their help in data collection. We
also thank an anonymous reviewer for his relevant comments on an
earlier version of the manuscript.
References
Agras, W. S., & Telch, C. F. (1998). The effect of caloric deprivation and negative affect
on binge eating in obese binge-drinking disordered woman. Behavior Therapy,
29,491e503.
Anestis, M. D., Selby, E. A., & Joiner, T. E. (2007). The role of urgency in maladaptive
behaviors. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45,3018e3029.
Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: evidence from neuro-
logical patients with orbito-frontal damage. Brain and Cognition, 55,30e40.
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to
future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition,
50,7e15.
Bechara, A., Dolan, S., Denburg, N., Hindes, A., Anderson, S. W., & Nathan, P. E.
(2001). Decision-making decits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial
prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia,
39,376e389.
Bechara, A., & Martin, E. M. (2004). Impaired decision making related to working
memory decits in individuals with substance addictions. Neuropsychology, 18,
152e162.
Bechara, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2005). Decision-making and impulse control
after frontal lobe injuries. Current Opinion in Neurology, 18,734e739.
Billieux, J., Rochat, L., Rebetez, M. M. L., & Van der Linden, M. (2008). Are all facets of
impulsivity related to self-reported compulsive buying behavior? Personality
and Individual Differences, 44, 1432e1442.
Billieux, J., Van der Linden, M., & Ceschi, G. (2007). Which dimensions of impulsivity
are related to cigarette craving? Addictive Behaviors, 32,1189e1199.
Billieux, J., Van der Linden, M., dAcremont, M., Ceschi, G., & Zermatten, A. (2007).
Does impulsivity relate to perceived dependence and actual use of the mobile
phone? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21,527e537.
Billieux, J., Van der Linden, M., & Rochat, L. (2008). The role of impulsivity in actual
and problematic use of the mobile phone. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22,
1195e1210.
Blair, K. S., Smith, B. W., Mitchell, D. G. V., Morton, J., Vythilingam, M., Pessoa, L.,
et al. (2007). Modulation of emotion by cognition and cognition by emotion.
NeuroImage, 35, 430e440.
Brand, M., Labudda, K., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2006). Neuropsychological correlates
of decision-making in ambiguous and risky situations. Neural Networks, 19,
1266e1276.
Braver, T. S., Gray, J. R., & Burgess, G. C. (2007). Explaining the many varieties of
working memory variation: dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In
A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, & A. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working
memory (pp. 76e106). New York: Oxford University Press.
Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1996). Response suppression, initiation and strategy
use following frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 34, 263e272.
Byrne, B. M. (1994). Structural equation modeling with EQS and EQS/Windows.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, appli-
cations, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple re gres-
sion/correlation analysis for the behavioral scienc es (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Colder, C. R., & Chassin, L. (1997). Affectivity and impulsivity: temperament risk
for adolescent alcohol involvement. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 11,
83e97.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R)
and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional manual. Odessa, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources.
Cyders, M. A., & Smith, G. T. (2007). Mood-based rash action and its components:
positive and negative urgency. Personality and Individual Differences, 43,
839e850.
Cyders, M. A., & Smith, G. T. (2008). Emotion-based dispositions to rash action:
positive and negative urgency. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 807e828.
Cyders, M. A., Smith, G. T., Spillane, N. S., Fischer, S., Annus, A. M., & Peterson, C.
(2007). Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior:
development and validation of a measure of positive urgency. Psychological
Assessment, 19,107e118 .
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descarteserror: Emotion, reason, and the human brain.New
York: Grosset/Putnam.
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1991). Somatic markers and the guidance
of behaviour: theory and preliminary testing. In H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg, &
A. L. Benton (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp. 217e229). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Dick, D. M., Smith, G., Olausson, P., Mitchell, S. H., Leeman, R. F., OMalley, S. S., et al.
(2010). Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to
alcohol use disorders. Addiction Biology, 15,217e226.
Dolan, S. L., Bechara, A., & Nathan, P. E. (2008). Executive dysfunction as a risk
marker for substance abuse: the role of impulsive personality traits. Behavioral
Sciences and the Law, 26, 799e822.
Doran, N., Cook, J., McChargue, D., & Spring, B. (2009). Impulsivity and cigarette
craving: differences across subtypes. Psychopharmacology, 207, 365e373.
Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., & Lawrence, A. D. (2006). The somatic marker hypothesis:
a critical evaluation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 239e271.
Enticott, P. G., & Ogloff, J. R. P. (2006). Elucidation of impulsivity. Australian
Psychologist, 41,3e14.
Evans, J. S. B. T. (2003). In two minds: dual process accounts of reasoning. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 7, 454e459.
Evans, J. S. B. T. (2004). Dual processes, evolution and rationality. Thinking &
Reasoning, 10, 405e410.
Evenden, J. (1999). Impulsivity: a discussion of clinical and experimental ndings.
Journal of Psychopharmacology, 13,180e192.
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., Jones, S., Smith, M., Guthrie, I., Poulin, R., et al. (1999).
Regulation, emotionality, and preschoolerssocially competent peer interaction.
Child Development, 70, 432e442.
Figner, B., Mackinlay, R. J., Wilkening, F., & Weber, E. U. (2009). Affective and
deliberative processes in risky choice: age differences in risk taking in the
Columbia card task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and
Cognition, 35,709e730.
Fischer, S., Anderson, K. G., & Smith, G. T. (2004). Coping with distress by eating or
drinking: role of trait urgency and expectancies. Psychology of Addictive
Behaviors, 18, 269e274.
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interfer-
ence control functions: a latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 133,101e135.
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Frijda, N. H. (1993). Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. In M. Lewis, &
J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (1st ed.). (pp. 381e403) New York:
Guilford Press.
Fujita, F., Diener, E., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Gender differences in negative affect and
well-being: the case for emotional intensity. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 61,427e434.
Gay, P., Rochat, L., Billieux, J., dAcremont, M., & Van der Linden, M. (2008).
Heterogeneous inhibition processes involved in different facets of self-reported
impulsivity: evidence from a community sample. Acta Psychologica, 129,
332e339.
Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & Verschuere, B. (2008). The Karolinska
directed emotional faces: a validation study. Cognition and Emotion, 22,
1094e1118 .
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e1096 1095
Author's personal copy
Goudriaan, A. E., Oosterlaan, J., de Beurs, E., & van den Brink, W. (2005). Decision
making in pathological gambling: a comparison between pathological
gamblers, alcohol dependents, persons with Tourette syndrome, and normal
controls. Cognitive Brain Research, 23,137e151.
Goudriaan, A. E., Oosterlaan, J., de Beurs, E., & Van den Brink, W. (2006). Neuro-
cognitive functions in pathological gambling: a comparison with alcohol
dependence, Tourette syndrome and normal controls. Addiction, 101, 534e547.
Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Davidson, M. C., Glover, G. H., & Casey, B. J. (2005).
Contributions of amygdala and striatal activity in emotion regulation. Biological
Psychiatry, 57, 624e632.
Jacobs, D. F. (1986). A general theory of addictions: a new theoretical model. Journal
of Gambling Behavior, 2,15e31.
Khazaal, Y.,Billieux, J., Thorens, G., Khan, R., Louati, Y., Scarlatti, E., et al. (2008). French
validation of the Internet addiction test. Cyberpsychology & Behavior,11, 703e706.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture
system (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: University
of Florida, NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention.
Lejoyeux, M., Mathieu, K., Embouazza, H., Huet, F., & Lequen, V. (2007). Prevalence
of compulsive buying among customers of a Parisian general store. Compre-
hensive Psychiatry, 48,42e46.
Lejoyeux, M., Tassain, V., Solomon, J., & Adès, J. (1997). Study of compulsive buying
in depressed patients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 58,169e173 .
Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a users guide to
the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach, & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory
processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189e239). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Logan, G. D., Schachar, R. J., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control.
Psychological Science, 8,60e64.
Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces.
Stockholm: Karolinska Institute, Psychology Section, Department of Clinical
Neuroscience.
Lynam, D. R., & Miller, J. D. (2004). Personality pathways to impulsive behavior and
their relations to deviance: results from three samples. Journal of Quantitative
Criminology, 20,319e341.
Maia, T. V., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). A reexamination of the evidence for the
somatic marker hypothesis: what participants really know in the Iowa
gambling task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 101, 16075e16080.
McElroy, S. L., Keck, P. E., Pope, H. G., & Smith, J. M. R. (1994). Compulsive buying:
a report of 20 cases. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 55, 242e248.
Miller, J., Flory, K., Lynam, D. R., & Leukefeld, C. (2003). A test of the four-factor
model of impulsivity-related traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 34,
1403e1418.
Miltenberger, R. G., Redlin, J., Crosby, R., Stickney, M., Mitchell, J., Wonderlich, S., et al.
(2003). Direct and retrospective assessment of factors contributing to compul-
sive buying. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 34,1e9.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P.,Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D.
(2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to
complex frontal lobetasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41,
49e100.
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2006). Mplus users guide (4th ed.). Los Angeles:
Muthén & Muthén.
Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2004). A functional approach to the assessment of
self-mutilative behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72,
885e890.
Nock, M. K., Wedig, M. M., Holmberg, E. B., & Hooley, J. M. (2008). The emotion
reactivity scale: development, evaluation, and relation to self-injurious
thoughts and behaviors. Behavior Therapy, 39,107e116 .
Noël, X., Bechara, A., Dan, B., Hanak, C., & Verbanck, P. (2007). Response inhibition
decit is involved in poor decision making under risk in nonamnesic individ-
uals with alcoholism. Neuropsychology, 21, 778e786.
Noël, X., Paternot, J., Van der Linden, M., Sferrazza, R., Verhas, M., Hanak, C., et al.
(2001). Correlation between inhibition, working memory and delimited frontal
area blood ow measured by 99MTC-Bicisate SPECT in alcohol-dependent
patients. Alcohol & Alcoholism, 36, 556e563.
Perales, J. C., Verdejo-Garcia, A., Moya, M., Lozano, O., & Perez-Garcia, M. (2009).
Bright and dark sides of impulsivity: performance of women with high and low
trait impulsivity on neuropsychological tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experi-
mental Neuropsychology, 8,1e18.
Persaud, N., McLeod, P., & Cowey, A. (2009). Post-decision wagering objectively
measures awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 10,257e261.
Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 13,160e166.
Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation
hypotheses: theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral
Research, 42,185e227.
Schimmack, U. (2005). Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: threat,
negativity or arousal? Emotion, 5,55e66.
Schulz, K. P., Fan, J., Magidina, O., Marks, D. J., Hahn, B., & Halperin, J. M. (2007). Does
the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition? Convergence
with measures on a non-emotional analog. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
22,151e160.
Selby, E. A., Anestis, M. D., & Joiner, T. E. (2008). Understanding the relationship
between emotional and behavioral dysregulation: emotional cascades. Behav-
iour Research and Therapy, 46, 593e611.
Shuster, J., & Toplak, M. E. (2009). Executive and motivational inhibition: associa-
tions with self-report measures related to inhibition. Consciousness and Cogni-
tion, 18,471e480.
Smith, G. T., Fischer, S., Cyders, M. A., Annus, A. M., Spillane, N. S., & McCarthy, D. M.
(2007). On the validity and utility of discriminating among impulsivity-like
traits. Assessment, 14,155e170.
Swendsen, J. D., Tennen, H., Carney, M. A., Afeck, G., Willard, A., & Hromi, A. (2000).
Mood and alcohol consumption: an experience sampling test of the self-
medication hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109,198e204.
Thorberg, F. A., & Lyvers, M. (2006). Negative mood regulation (NMR) expectancies,
mood, and affect intensity among clients in substance disorder treatment
facilities. Addictive Behaviors, 31,811e820.
Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotion distress regulation
takes precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 80,53e67.
Van der Linden, M., dAcremont, M., Zermatten, A., Jermann, F., Laroi, F., Willems, S.,
et al. (2006). A French adaptation of the UPPS impulsive behavior scale:
conrmatory factor analysis in a sample of undergraduate students. European
Journal of Psychological Assessment, 22,38e42.
Verbruggen, F., & De Houwer, J. (2007). Do emotional stimuli interfere with
response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cognition and
Emotion, 21,391e403.
Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition:
associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 137,649e672.
Verdejo-García, A., Bechara, A., Recknor, E. C., & Pérez-García, M. (2007). Negative
emotion-driven impulsivity predicts substance dependence problems. Drug and
Alcohol Dependence, 91,213e219.
Verdejo-García, A., Lozano, O., Moya, M., Alcasar, M. A., & Pérez-García, M. (2010).
Psychometric properties of a Spanish version of the UPPSeP impulsive behavior
scale: reliability, validity and association with trait and cognitive impulsivity.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 92,70e77.
Wan, C. S., & Chiou, W. B. (2006). Psychological motives and online games addic-
tion: a test of ow theory and humanistic needs theory for Taiwanese adoles-
cents. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 9,317e324.
Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The ve factor model and impulsivity: using
a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and
Individual Differences, 30, 669e689.
Whiteside, S. P., Lynam, D. R., Miller, J., & Reynolds, B. (2005). Validation of the UPPS
impulsive behaviour scale: a four-factor model of impulsivity. European Journal
of Personality, 19,559e574.
Xiao, L., Bechara, A., Grenard, L. J., Stacy, W. A., Palmer, P., Wei, Y., et al. (2009).
Affective decision-making predictive of Chinese adolescent drinking behaviors.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15,547e557.
Young, K. S. (1999). Internet addictions: symptoms, evaluation and treatment. In
L. Vande Creek, & T. Jackson (Eds.), Innovations in clinical practice: A source book
(pp. 19e31). Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press.
Zapolski, T. C. B., Cyders, M. A., & Smith, G. T. (2009). Positive urgency predicts
illegal drug use and risky sexual behavior. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23,
348e354.
Zermatten, A., Van der Linden, M., dAcremont, M., Jermann, F., & Bechara, A. (2005).
Impulsivity and decision making. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 193,
647e650.
J. Billieux et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 1085e10961096
... Additionally, some individuals might prioritize relationships within the virtual realm over real-world connections 63 . These patterns of uncontrolled usage might lead to mobile phone addiction, characterized by problematic behaviors 64 . Moreover, it is crucial to examine the theoretical foundations that underpin the proposed relationships between variables in this study. ...
... Moreover, age positively predicts sport commitment and negatively predicts life satisfaction. These findings contrast with a study conducted by Billieux et al. 64 , which reported that gender was not a predictor of problematic mobile phone use. However, this inconsistency may be attributed to differences in study populations. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to investigate the relationship between perceived stress and life satisfaction under the influence of mobile phone addiction and sport commitment. Participants were recruited from eight universities located in six Chinese cities, naming Chongqing, Maoming, Nanjing, Suzhou, Shijiazhuang, and Zhengzhou. The sample consisted of 575 participants enrolled in Chinese universities, with 309 (53.7%) being female students. The mediation model was tested under the structural equation modeling framework using Mplus. Results showed that (1) perceived stress had a direct and negative impact on life satisfaction, and it also had indirect effects through the two mediators; (2) perceived stress positively predicted mobile phone addiction, which, in turn, negatively impacted life satisfaction; (3) perceived stress negatively predicted sport commitment, which, in turn, positively impacted life satisfaction. By emphasizing the mediating roles of mobile phone addiction and sport commitment, our findings highlight the importance of addressing these factors in interventions aimed at encouraging college students’ well-being. Implications for intervention design to promoter health among university students should take into account the mediating roles of mobile phone addiction and sport commitment.
... PC1 included variables from all impulsivity-related assessments (though not all subscales). This PC, which may be thought of as a sort of 'general impulsivity' dimension, captured common facets of impulsivity, including self-/caregiver-reports related to motivation and urgency that often influence decision-making processes [44,45] and social interactions [46][47][48], as well as performance on behavioral tasks. Adolescents with higher scores on PC1 exhibited heightened levels of externalizing behaviors, as indicated by significant positive loadings on the ADHD, attention, and externalizing subscales of the CBCL. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Impulsivity is a multifaceted construct that typically increases during adolescence and is implicated in risk for substance use disorders that develop later in life. Here, we take a multivariate approach to identify latent dimensions of impulsivity, broadly defined, among youth enrolled in the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development (ABCD) study and explore associations with individual differences in demographics, substance-use initiation and canonical resting state networks (N=11,872, ages ~9-10). Using principal component analysis, we identified eight latent impulsivity dimensions, the top three of which together accounted for the majority of the variance across all impulsivity assessments. The first principal component (PC1) was a general impulsivity factor that mapped onto all impulsivity-related assessments. PC2 mapped onto a 'mixed' impulsivity style related to both poorer, less attentive performance on the SST and decreased delay discounting. PC3 linked externalizing behaviors across multiple measures with indices of delay discounting, making delay discounting the only impulsivity-related assessment to load on all three of the top PCs. Multiple impulsivity PCs were significantly associated with subsequent initiation of alcohol and cannabis use. Finally, we found both cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between the PCs and functional connectivity between and within frontoparietal, cingulo-opercular, and default mode networks. These data provide a critical empirical baseline for how facets of impulsivity covary in early adolescence which may be tracked through future waves of ABCD data, enabling longitudinal elucidation of how dimensions of impulsivity interact with other individual and environmental factors to influence risk for substance use later in life.
... (10) Other studies have also emphasised previous psychopathologies, psychopathy, congenital disorders, and other genetic aspects. (19,20) Thus, modern scientists have intensified the study of addictive behaviour and characterised various manifestations and causes of this phenomenon. However, this research issue is not fully characterised in the current scientific discourse. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: The Russian aggression against Ukraine highlights the urgent need to address psychological health issues, particularly in combating addictive behaviours. This study aims to explore the psychological mechanisms underpinning the development of addiction in Ukraine’s current context.Methods: The research employed surveys (40 patients and 20 experts), content analysis of scholarly literature, response analysis, and comparative methods.Results: Findings reveal that childhood trauma, especially familial stress, plays a pivotal role in fostering addictive behaviours. Additional contributing factors include parental divorce, adverse living conditions, and personal setbacks during childhood and adolescence. Alcoholism emerges as the most prevalent form of addiction, while drug addiction, gambling, and Internet dependency also represent significant issues. The proliferation of gambling addiction is attributed to martial law and the increased availability of online gaming platforms. Similarly, Internet addiction correlates with widespread digitalisation and globalisation. Effective strategies for overcoming addiction involve professional psychological support strengthened by social assistance, family counselling, educational initiatives, and guidance from spiritual or religious organisations. However, a critical shortage of high-quality psychological rehabilitation and treatment services persists in Ukraine. Proposed solutions include establishing a national assistance programme, reforming psychologist training, and leveraging international support systems.Conclusions: The study underscores the necessity of a multifaceted psychological approach to address addictive behaviours effectively. Comprehensive measures combining professional, social, educational, and spiritual resources are vital for mitigating the impact of addiction in Ukraine’s challenging circumstances.
... Regarding the influence of gender, the probability of deceleration rate for male drivers was 0.64 times lower, compared to female drivers. Higher urgency levels in females could have resulted in harsher deceleration as urgency levels positively impact the deceleration magnitude (Billieux et al., 2010;Durrani et al., 2021). Despite the harsh deceleration rate, female had approximately 16.31 % more cases at a very high crash risk within lower speed limit conditions, while 9.53 % more cases were at a very high crash risk in the higher speed limit conditions. ...
... It is entirely possible that lower research self-efficacy leads to increased mental stress, which might, in turn, result in problematic smartphone use as a coping mechanism. Some studies indicated the component of urgency to be a robust predictor of problematic mobile phone use [54]. Some forms of psychopathology, including stress and depression, have also been found in longitudinal studies as outcomes of problematic smartphone use [19,55]. ...
Article
Full-text available
As Generation Z youth grow up with the Internet, problematic smartphone use is growing more prevalent. This study administered questionnaires containing measures such as the Mobile Phone Addiction Index, the Research Self-Efficacy Scale, the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale, and the Stress Mindset Measure. The survey targeted 2278 graduate students and explored the mechanism through which problematic smartphone use affects research self-efficacy (RSE). The results reveal that problematic smartphone use has significant negative effects on self-efficacy, with mental stress playing a mediating role in this process; that is, problematic smartphone use lowers RSE by increasing mental stress. Meanwhile, the aforementioned negative impacts caused by problematic smartphone use are moderated by stress mindsets: the “stress-is-enhancing” mindset reduces the negative effects of smartphone use on RSE, whereas the “stress-is-debilitating” mindset amplifies these negative effects by enhancing the mediating effect of mental stress.
... IA has been described as impulsive, with individuals reporting an increased sense of tension or arousal before connecting to the Internet, and a relief of that tension once they were online (Shapira et al. 2000). Specifically, studies have shown that both lack of perseverance and urgency are positively related to Internet addiction as facets of impulsivity (Mottram & Fleming 2009, Billieux et al. 2010). Recently, this classification has been viewed as too restrictive considering that other factors besides impulsivity traits also influence IA (Billieux et al. 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background With the increasing prevalence of digital technology, adolescent internet addiction (IA) has become a global concern. Excessive internet use, especially among adolescents, has been linked to various negative outcomes such as poor academic performance, social isolation, and mental health issues. Conducted among adolescents of Arab origin, our study addressed the limitations of the literature, which predominantly focuses on Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic populations. Objective This study aimed to differentiate between essential and nonessential internet use and how they relate to IA in early and middle adolescents, as well as the relationship between subjective happiness with the amount of time spent on nonessential internet use and IA. Methods A cross-sectional survey was conducted among 377 students from 16 schools in Qatar. The survey measured essential and nonessential internet use, subjective happiness with nonessential use, and IA symptoms using the Internet Addiction Diagnostic Questionnaire, as well as participant demographics. To explore age-specific associations, participants were categorized into early (age 11-13 years) and middle (age 14-17 years) adolescents. Factorial analysis, multiple regression, and logistic regression were used for statistical analysis. Results Nonessential internet use significantly predicted IA in both early (P<.001) and middle (P<.001) adolescents, with early adolescents showing a stronger association. Subjective happiness with nonessential internet use negatively predicted IA only in middle adolescents (P<.001) as greater dissatisfaction led to a higher IA risk. Essential internet use did not predict IA in either group. Conclusions Differentiating between essential and nonessential internet use is crucial in understanding IA. This study highlights the importance of developmental differences in shaping IA symptoms. The findings suggest that interventions aimed at addressing IA should be age specific and focus on addressing nonessential use specifically rather than considering internet use and screen time in general as a single entity. Cultural and regional factors also play a role in shaping internet use patterns and IA in the Middle East, necessitating context-specific, culturally sensitive approaches to IA prevention.
... This heightened impulsivity could drive females to seek refuge in online activities as a coping mechanism, potentially exacerbating their risk of technological addiction [111]. Several studies have demonstrated the existence of the relationship between negative urgency and IA, particularly among females [102,112]. Overall, the results indicated significant gender differences in several measures, including the IAT, DERS, and UPPS. Although, the gender effect on these variables could be limited, as indicated by the small effect size, it is important to consider their practical significance, particularly in designing interventions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Abstract: Background/Objectives: Internet addiction (IA) and related behaviors, such as Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) and social media addiction (SMA), have gained increasing research attention. Studies show gender differences, with males more likely to develop gaming-related addictions and females more prone to social media and phubbing behaviors. This study aimed to explore gender differences in Internet addiction and related behaviors in a Spanish sample, to identify predictors and gender-specific patterns of IA. Methods: We conducted a cross-sectional study with 585 participants (265 male, 320 female) aged 18 to 35 years (M = 22.11, SD = 3.08). Data were collected using standardized questionnaires to assess IA, IGD, SMA, phubbing, Fear of Missing Out (FoMO), emotional dysregulation, personality traits, and prosociality. Correlation and regression analyses were used to identify gender-specific predictors of IA. Results: Males exhibited significantly higher scores for IA and IGD, while females showed higher scores for SMA and phubbing's "phone obsession" dimension. No significant gender differences were found in the "communication disturbance" dimension of phubbing or FoMO. Correlation analyses revealed significant associations between IA and psychological and technological variables. Gender-specific predictors of IA included social media engagement and emotional regulation for females, while gaming behaviors and communication patterns were more relevant for males. Conclusions: These findings highlight gender differences in IA, suggesting that tailored interventions should address unique online behaviors and emotional regulation challenges in males and females. Future research should refine gender-specific patterns to develop more effective, targeted prevention and treatment strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Background Cognitive rigidity and working memory impairment are established features of internalizing syndromes. Growing evidence suggests that deficits in affective control –cognitive control in the context of emotion – may underpin elevated emotion-related impulsivity in various psychiatric disorders. Objective This study examines two components of affective control (affective flexibility and emotional working memory) as potential neurocognitive processes linking emotion-related impulsivity to internalizing psychopathology. Method Undergraduate participants (analysis n = 120) completed the Memory and Affective Flexibility Task (MAFT), a novel behavioral assessment designed to assess hot cognition in affective flexibility and emotional working memory performance, alongside self-report measures of impulsivity and symptoms of internalizing disorders. Results Structural equation modeling suggested that less accurate working memory during neutral trials (cool cognition) was associated with more symptoms of internalizing psychopathology. However, effects of hot working memory and affective flexibility were not significantly related to emotion-related impulsivity or psychopathology scores. Conclusions Although findings provide no support for the validity of MAFT indices of hot cognition, these results replicate and extend work on the importance of cool working memory and emotion-related impulsivity as correlates of psychopathology.
Article
Full-text available
Background/Aims: Body-focused repetitive behaviors (BFRBs) such as skin-picking, trichotillomania, nail-picking, nail-biting, lip-biting and skin-biting have adverse physical and psychological effects. This study aimed to investigate the prevalence and associated factors of BFRBs in a sample of Turkish medical students. Methods: An online survey was used to investigate the prevalence of six BFRBs —skin-picking, trichotillomania, nail-biting, nail-picking, lip-biting and skin-biting-- and psychological factors such as anxiety, depression, stress levels, impulsivity and difficulties in emotional regulation (DER) in a sample of 200 medical students. Skin Picking Scale-Revised (SPS-R), Massachusetts General Hospital Hairpulling Scale (MGH-HPS), Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21), Barratt Impulsiveness Scale–Short Form and Difficulty in Emotion Regulation Scale—Brief Form and specific questions for four BFRBs (nail-picking, nail-biting, lip biting and skin biting) were administered. Results: The prevalence of at least one BFRB disorder was 28.0% (n=56). The most common diagnoses were skin-picking (17.0%) and trichotillomania (10.5%), followed by skin-picking (9.0%), lip biting (8.5%), nail-picking (7%) and nail-biting (5.0%). The gender difference was found only in skin-picking disorders (females=22.9%; males=8.5%). Students with BFRB disorder (n=56) were found to have higher levels of anxiety, depression, stress, impulsivity and DER. Students in 2nd, 3rd and 5th grade are more likely to have BFRB disorders than interns. DER was a partial mediating variable between impulsivity and the presence of BFRB disorder. Conclusions: The findings of the study suggest that BFRBs are common among medical students and that these behaviors are associated with psychological factors such as depression, anxiety, stress levels, impulsivity and difficulties in emotion regulation. Screening programs and intervention strategies for BFRBs in medical students should be developed, and it is recommended that psychological factors should be considered in these interventions, emotion regulation and stress management skills should be significantly improved, and comorbid depression and anxiety should be treated.
Article
Full-text available
Affect intensity (AI) may reconcile 2 seemingly paradoxical findings: Women report more negative affect than men but equal happiness as men. AI describes people’s varying response intensity to identical emotional stimuli. A college sample of 66 women and 34 men was assessed on both positive and negative affect using 4 measurement methods: self-report, peer report, daily report, and memory performance. A principal-components analysis revealed an affect balance component and an AI component. Multimeasure affect balance and AI scores were created, and t tests were computed that showed women to be as happy as and more intense than men. Gender accounted for less than 1% of the variance in happiness but over 13% in AI. Thus, depression findings of more negative affect in women do not conflict with well-being findings of equal happiness across gender. Generally, women’s more intense positive emotions balance their higher negative affect.
Article
Full-text available
The importance of the relation between impulsivity and deviance is well-acknowledged among criminologists. However, differences in the representations of impulsivity, some merely titular and others substantive, may cloud our understanding of these relations. The current study examines the argument, offered by Whiteside and Lynam Pers. Individuals Diff. (2000) 30: 669–689, that there may be four distinct personality pathways through which impulsive behavior may be manifested. Across three samples (two undergraduate, one community), we examine the validity of a four-factor structure of impulsivity, test whether these four pathways manifest divergent relations with various forms of deviant behavior such as crime and substance use, as well as laboratory manifestations of aggressive and impulsive behavior, and examine the invariance of these results across gender. The results support the existence of a four-factor model of impulsivity, the importance of two specific personality pathways in relation to self-reports of deviance (lack of premeditation and sensation seeking), as well as actual behavior, and suggest that these pathways are important for both men and women.
Article
Full-text available
This chapter shows how all working memory (WM) theorists agree that control processes are a critical component of WM function. The general theoretical framework is the dual mechanisms of control (DMC): proactive control and reactive control. They can be differentiated on the basis of their information-processing characteristics, computational properties, temporal dynamics, and underlying neural systems. Regardless of the source of WM variation, the proximal mechanisms of variation remain the same and have the same impact on brain activity and behavior. Thus, the DMC framework provides a unifying account that has the potential to synthesize and integrate a large body of literatures on WM function. By recognizing that there are multiple alternative routes to cognitive control, investigators may be in a better position to explore and investigate the complexity of empirical findings.
Article
The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces (KDEF; Lundqvist, Flykt, & Öhman, 1998) is a database of pictorial emotional facial expressions for use in emotion research. The original KDEF database consists of a total of 490 JPEG pictures (72x72 dots per inch) showing 70 individuals (35 women and 35 men) displaying 7 different emotional expressions (Angry, Fearful, Disgusted, Sad, Happy, Surprised, and Neutral). Each expression is viewed from 5 different angles and was recorded twice (the A and B series). All the individuals were trained amateur actors between 20 and 30 years of age. For participation in the photo session, beards, moustaches, earrings, eyeglasses, and visible make-up were exclusion criteria. All the participants were instructed to try to evoke the emotion that was to be expressed and to make the expression strong and clear. In a validation study (Goeleven et al., 2008), a series of the KDEF images were used and participants rated emotion, intensity, and arousal on 9-point Likert scales. In that same study, a test-retest reliability analysis was performed by computing the percentage similarity of emotion type ratings and by calculating the correlations for the intensity and arousal measures over a one-week period. With regard to the intensity and arousal measures, a mean correlation across all pictures of .75 and .78 respectively was found. (APA PsycTests Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
SUMMARY The Internet itself is a neutral device originally designed to facilitate research among academic and military agencies. How some people have come to use this medium, however, has created a stir among the mental health community by great discussion of Internet addiction. Addictive use of the Internet is a new phenomenon which many practitioners are unaware of and subsequently unprepared to treat. Some therapists are unfamiliar with the Internet, making its seduction difficult to understand. O ther tim es, its im pact on the individual's life is m inim ized. T he purpose of this chapter is to enable clinicians to better detect and treat Internet addiction. The chapter will first focus on the complications of diagnosis of Internet addiction. Second, the negative consequences of such Internet abuse are explored. Third, how to properly assess and identify triggers causing the onset of pathological Internet use are discussed. Fourth, a number of recovery strategies are presented. Lastly, since Internet addiction is an emergent disorder, implications for future practice are presented. Complications In Diagnosing Internet Addiction Negative Consequences Of Addictive Use Of The Internet
Article
We report an experiment testing the hypothesis that impulsive behavior reflects a deficit in the ability to inhibit prepotent responses Specifically, we examined whether impulsive people respond more slowly to signals to inhibit (stop signals) than non-impulsive people In this experiment, 136 undergraduate students completed an impulsivity questionnaire and then participated in a stop-signal experiment, in which they performed a choice reaction time (go) task and were asked to inhibit their responses to the go task when they heard a stop signal The delay between the go signal and the stop signal was determined by a tracking procedure designed to allow subjects to inhibit on 50% of the stop-signal trials Reaction time to the go signal did not vary with impulsivity, but estimated stop-signal reaction time was longer in more impulsive subjects, consistent with the hypothesis and consistent with results from populations with pathological problems with impulse control