ArticlePDF Available

Basal Ganglia Volume Is Associated with Aerobic Fitness in Preadolescent Children

Authors:
  • AdventHealth Research Institute

Abstract and Figures

The present investigation is the first to explore the association between childhood aerobic fitness and basal ganglia structure and function. Rodent research has revealed that exercise influences the striatum by increasing dopamine signaling and angiogenesis. In children, higher aerobic fitness levels are associated with greater hippocampal volumes, superior performance on tasks of attentional and interference control, and elevated event-related brain potential indices of executive function. The present study used magnetic resonance imaging to investigate if higher-fit and lower-fit 9- and 10-year-old children exhibited differential volumes of other subcortical brain regions, specifically the basal ganglia involved in attentional control. The relationship between aerobic fitness, dorsal and ventral striatum volumes and performance on an attention and inhibition Eriksen flanker task was also examined. The results indicated that higher-fit children showed superior flanker task performance compared to lower-fit children. Higher-fit children also showed greater volumes of the dorsal striatum, and dorsal striatum volume was negatively associated with behavioral interference. The results support the claim that the dorsal striatum is involved in cognitive control and response resolution and that these cognitive processes vary as a function of aerobic fitness. No relationship was found between aerobic fitness, the volume of the ventral striatum and flanker performance. The findings suggest that increased childhood aerobic fitness is associated with greater dorsal striatal volumes and that this is related to enhanced cognitive control. Because children are becoming increasingly overweight, unhealthy and unfit, understanding the neurocognitive benefits of an active lifestyle during childhood has important public health and educational implications.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Fax +41 61 306 12 34
E-Mail karger@karger.ch
www.karger.com
Original Paper
Dev Neurosci 2010;32:249–256
DOI: 10.1159/000316648
Basal Ganglia Volume Is Associated with
Aerobic Fitness in Preadolescent Children
LauraChaddock a KirkI.Erickson c RuchikaShauryaPrakash d MattVanPatter a
MichelleW.Voss a MatthewB.Pontifex b LaurenB.Raine b CharlesH.Hillman b
ArthurF.Kramer a
a Department of Psychology, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, and
b Department of
Kinesiology and Community Health, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Ill. ,
c Department of
Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pa. , and
d Department of Psychology, Ohio State University,
Columbus, Ohio , USA
results support the claim that the dorsal striatum is involved
in cognitive control and response resolution and that these
cognitive processes vary as a function of aerobic fitness. No
relationship was found between aerobic fitness, the volume
of the ventral striatum and flanker performance. The find-
ings suggest that increased childhood aerobic fitness is as-
sociated with greater dorsal striatal volumes and that this is
related to enhanced cognitive control. Because children are
becoming increasingly overweight, unhealthy and unfit, un-
derstanding the neurocognitive benefits of an active life-
style during childhood has impor tant public health and edu-
cational implications. Copyr ight © 2010 S. Karger AG , Basel
Introduction
Aerobic exercise and physical activity improve brain
and cognitive health across the lifespan [Hillman et al.,
2008]. While the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus
are the focus of many human and animal studies of fit-
ness and neurocognition [Chaddock et al., 2010; Chad-
dock et al., in press; Colcombe and Kramer, 2003; Cot-
man and Berchtold, 2002; Erickson et al., 2009; Hillman
et al., 2009; Lopez-Lopez et al., 2004; Neeper et al., 1995;
van Praag et al., 1999, 2005; Vaynman et al., 2004], rodent
Key Words
Brain Development Exercise MRI Physical activity
Neurocognition Neuroimaging Striatum
Abstract
The present investigation is the first to explore the associa-
tion between childhood aerobic fitness and basal ganglia
structure and function. Rodent research has revealed that
exercise influences the striatum by increasing dopamine sig-
naling and angiogenesis. In children, higher aerobic fitness
levels are associated with greater hippocampal volumes, su-
perior performance on tasks of attentional and interference
control, and elevated event-related brain potential indices of
executive function. The present study used magnetic reso-
nance imaging to investigate if higher-fit and lower-fit 9- and
10-year-old children exhibited differential volumes of other
subcortical brain regions, specifically the basal ganglia in-
volved in attentional control. The relationship between aer-
obic fitness, dorsal and ventral striatum volumes and perfor-
mance on an attention and inhibition Eriksen flanker task
was also examined. The results indicated that higher-fit chil-
dren showed superior flanker task performance compared
to lower-fit children. Higher-fit children also showed greater
volumes of the dorsal striatum, and dorsal striatum volume
was negatively associated with behavioral interference. The
Recei ved: March 29, 2010
Accepted a fter revision: June 8, 2010
Publish ed online: August 6, 2010
Laura Chaddock
Depa rtment of Psychology, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science a nd Technolog y
University of Illinoi s at Urbana- Champaign
405 Nor th Mathews Avenue, Urba na, IL 61801 (USA)
Tel. +1 610 209 6836, Fax +1 217 333 2922, E-Mail lchaddo2
@ illinois.edu
© 2010 S. Karger AG, Basel
0378–5866/10/0323–0249$26.00/0
Accessible online at:
www.karger.com/dne
Chaddock /Erickson /Prakash /VanPatter /
Vo ss
/Pontifex /Raine /Hillman /Kramer
Dev Neurosci 2010;32:249–256
250
research indicates that wheel running also inf luences the
molecular architecture and behavior of the basal ganglia.
The basal ganglia are a group of structures subdivided
into the dorsal striatum, a subregion implicated in stim-
ulus-response challenges that require response selection
demands, motor integration, response resolution, cogni-
tive flexibility and the execution of learned behaviors,
and the ventral striatum, part of an affect and reward
pathway involved in reinforcement learning and motiva-
tional states [Aron et al., 2009; Casey et al., 2008; Di Mar-
tino et al., 2008; Draganski et al., 2008; Graybiel, 2005,
2008; Ragozzino et al., 2002]. Exercise has been shown to
increase the production and secretion of striatal brain-
derived neurotrophic factor [Aguiar et al., 2008; Marais
et al., 2009] and dopamine [Marques et al., 2008] as well
as increase astrocyte proliferation [Li et al., 2005] and
neural activity [Shi et al., 2004] in the striatum. In addi-
tion, exercise induces angiogenesis and reduces the dam-
aging effects of 6-hydroxydopamine to dopamine neu-
rons in the basal ganglia [Ding et al., 2004; Tillerson et
al., 2001].
The present study is the first to explore the association
between aerobic fitness and striatum volume and func-
tion in humans. A preadolescent population was exam-
ined to add to an emerging literature that physical activ-
ity and high levels of aerobic fitness during childhood
may enhance neurocognition [Buck et al., 2008; Castelli
et al., 2007; Chaddock et al., 2010; Chaddock et al., in
press; Chomitz et al., 2009; Hillman et al., 2005, 2008;
Sibley and Etnier, 2003]. The investigation builds on our
prior work which showed a positive association between
aerobic fitness, hippocampal volume and memory per-
formance in preadolescent children [Chaddock et al.,
2010]. In the present study we investigate the relationship
between fitness and additional brain regions, in particu-
lar the basal ganglia, to understand if higher aerobic fit-
ness in children can affect other regional brain volumes
involved in cognitive task performance.
To this end, the present investigation examined the
relationship between aerobic fitness, the volume of the
dorsal and ventral striatum, and flanker task perfor-
mance. A recent behavioral and event-related potential
study by Hillman et al. [2008] suggested improved selec-
tive attention, interference control and action monitoring
for higher-fit relative to lower-fit preadolescents during
the flanker task [see Colcombe et al., 2004 for a similar
observation with older adults]. Given that the basal gan-
glia have an important role in cognitive control (e.g. pre-
paring, initiating, inhibiting, switching responses) [Aron
et al., 2009], a key skill involved in the performance of the
flanker paradigm, it is possible that fitness differences in
flanker task performance and event-related potential in-
dices are related to differences in the volume of the stria-
tum. In addition, the basal ganglia have been implicated
as an important component of the neural circuitry in-
volved in the flanker paradigm [Casey et al., 2000; Wylie
et al., 2009]. Thus, higher-fit and lower-fit participants
were expected to show different striatal volume patterns
that were related to differential flanker task performance.
Method
Participants
Preadoles cent 9- and 10-year-old children were recr uited from
East-Central Illinois. The children were screened for several fac-
tors that inf luence fitness or cognitive function. Relatively strict
screening criteria were employed to help ensure that the fitness
groups did not differ on variables that could potentially bias cog-
nitive function or basal ganglia volume. To begin, the Kaufman
Brief Intelligenc e Test [Kauf man and Kaufman, 199 0] was admi n-
istered to each child to obtain a composite intelligence quotient
(IQ) score including both crystallized and f luid intelligence mea-
sures. Subjects were excluded if their scores were 1 1 standard de-
viation below the mean (85%). Next, a guardian of the child com-
pleted the Attention Def icit Hyperactivity Di sorder (ADHD) Rat-
ing Scale IV [DuPaul et al., 1998] to screen for the presence of
attentional disorders. Participants were excluded if they scored
above the 85th percentile . Pubertal ti ming was also assessed using
a modified Tanner Staging System [Tanner, 1962; Taylor et al.,
2001] with all included prepubescent participants at or below a
score of 2 on a 5-point scale of developmental stages. In addition,
socioeconomic status was determined by creating a trichotomous
index based on 3 va riables: partic ipation in a free or reduced-price
lunch program at school, the highest level of education obtained
by the mother and father, and the number of parents who worked
full-time [Birnbaum et al., 2002].
Furthermore, eligible participant s were required to (1) qualify
as higher-fit or lower-fit (i.e. subjects with fitness levels between
these 2 extremes were excluded from participation; see ‘Aerobic
Fitness Assessment’ section), (2) demonstrate right-handedness
(as measured by the Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire)
[Oldfield, 1971], (3) report no adverse health conditions, physical
incapacities or neurological disorders, (4) report no use of medi-
cations that influenced central nervous system function, (5) have
a corrected visual acuity of 20/20 and no color blindness, (6) suc-
cessfully perform a ‘mock MRI’ session to test for body size com-
patibility with an MRI machine and to screen for claustrophobia,
and (7) sign an informed assent approved by the University of Il-
linois at Urbana-Champaign. A legal guardian also provided
written informed consent in accordance with the Institutional
Review Board of the University of Il linois at Urbana- Champaign.
The subjects were compensated for participation.
Aerobic Fitness Assessment
The aerobic fitness level of each child was determined by mea-
suring maximal oxygen consumption (VO
2 max.) using a com-
puterized indirect calorimetry system (ParvoMedics True Max
Basal Ganglia and Childhood Fitness Dev Neurosci 2010;32:249–256
251
2400) during a modified Balke protocol [American College of
Sports Med icine, 2006]. Specif ically, the partic ipants ran on a mo-
tor-driven treadmill at a constant speed with increases in grade
increments of 2.5% every 2 min until volitional exhaustion. Aver-
ages for oxygen uptake (VO
2 ) and respiratory exchange ratio (the
ratio between carbon dioxide and oxygen percentage) were as-
sessed every 30 s. In addition, the heart rate was measured
throughout the fitness test [using a Polar heart rate monitor (Po-
lar WearLink + 31, Polar Electro, Finland)], and ratings of per-
ceived exertion were assessed every 2 min using the children’s
OMNI scale [Utter et al., 2002].
V O 2 max. was defined when oxygen consumption remained
at a steady state despite an increase in workload. The relative peak
oxygen consumption was based upon maximal effort as evi-
denced by (1) a peak heart rate 1 185 beats per minute [American
College of Sports Medicine, 2006] accompanied by a heart rate
pl at eau (i. e. an inc re as e i n wo rk rat e w it h no co nc omi ta nt inc re as e
in heart rate) [Freedson and Goodman, 1993], (2) respiratory ex-
change ratio 1 1.0 [Bar-Or, 1983], and/or (3) ratings on the chil-
dren’s OMNI scale of perceived exertion 1 8 [Utter et al., 2002].
The relative peak oxygen consumption was expressed in millili-
ters/kilogram/minute.
Fitness group assignments (i.e. higher-fit and lower-fit) were
based on whether a child’s VO
2 max. value fell above the 70th per-
centile or below the 30th percentile according to normative data
provided by Shvartz and Reibold [1990]. Children who did not
qu a li f y a s h i gh er -f it or lo wer -f it we re ex cl ud ed f ro m p ar t ic ip at io n.
S a m p l e
Fifty-nine subjects were initially eligible for the present study
(after exclusions due to Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test scores,
ADHD, pubertal timing, VO
2 ma x. criteria, etc.). Additional sub-
jects were excluded due to poor scan quality because of excessive
motion (n = 3) and basal ganglia volume outliers (n = 1).
Analyses were conducted on a total of 55 subjects, including
25 higher-fit chi ldren (14 boys, 11 girls) wit h an average age of 10.0
years (SD = 0.6; range 910) and 30 lower-fit children (11 boys, 19
girls) with an average age of 10.0 years (SD = 0.6; range 9–10). No
statist ically reliable differences in age, gender, socioeconomic sta-
tus or Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test scores existed between the
fitness groups. Table1 provides a list of demographic and fitness
information for the final sample.
MR Imaging Protocol and Image Processing
For all participants, high-resolution (1.3 mm ! 1.3 mm ! 1.3
mm) T
1 -weighted structural brain images were acquired using a
3D M PRAGE (magnet iza tion pre par ed rapid gra dient echo ima g-
ing) protocol with 144 contiguous axial slices, collected in as-
cending fashion parallel to the anterior and posterior commis-
sures (echo time = 3.87 ms, repetition time = 1,800 ms, field of
view = 256 mm, acquisition matrix 192 mm ! 192 mm, slice
thickness = 1.3 mm and flip angle = 8°). All images were collected
on a 3T head-only Siemens Allegra MRI scanner.
Segmentation and volumetric analysis of the left and right
dorsal striatum (i.e. caudate nucleus and putamen), ventral stria-
tum (i.e. nucleus accumbens) and globus pa llidus were performed
using a semiautomated, model-based subcortical tool (FMRIB’s
Integrated Registration and Segmentation Tool) in FMR IB’s Soft-
ware Library version 4.1.4 [Patenaude, 2007; Patenaude et al.,
2007a; Patenaude et al., 20 07b]. To begin, a 2-stage affine registra-
tion to a standard space template (MNI space) with 1 mm resolu-
tion using 12 degrees of freedom and a subcortical mask to ex-
clude voxels outside the subcortical regions was performed on
each subject’s MPRAGE. Next, the caudate nucleus, putamen, nu-
cleus accumbens and globus pallidus were segmented with 30, 40,
50 and 40 modes of variation for each structure, respectively.
Please refer to Erick son et al. [in press] and Patenaude et al. [2007a,
b] for a description of the FIRST methodology. Previous studies
have reported high test-retest reliability of this segmentation al-
gorithm [Erickson et al., in press].
Segmentations were visually checked for errors, and no errors
were noted. Finally, boundary correction was run, a process
which classifies boundary voxels as belonging to the structure (or
not) based on a statistical probability (z-score 1 3.00; p ! 0.001).
The volume of each participant’s caudate nucleus, putamen, nu-
cleus accumbens and globus pallidus was measured in cubic mil-
limeters, and these values were used in all subsequent analyses.
See figure 1 for a sample FIRST segmentation of the basal ganglia.
Flanker Task
The flanker task is a selective attention paradigm, often em-
ployed to examine interference control, one aspect of executive
control [Eriksen a nd Eriksen, 1974; Kramer et al., 1994]. The event-
related task required individuals to respond as quickly as possible
to the direction of a central arrow in an array of arrows presented
on an MRI back-projection. A congruent trial consisted of 1 1 1 1 1
and ! ! ! ! ! arrow displays in which the target arrow was flanked
by arrows of the same direction. An incongruent trial consisted of
1 1 ! 1 1 and ! ! 1 ! ! displays in which the target arrow was
f lanked by the opposing a rrow response. Trials in wh ich the middle
arrow pointed to the right (e.g. 1 1 1 1 1 , ! ! 1 ! ! ) required a right
Tab le 1. Participant mean demographic and fitness data by aero-
bic fitness group
Variable Lower-fit Higher-fit
Participants 30 (11 male) 25 (14 male)
Age, years 10.0 (0.6) 10.0 (0.6)
VO2 max., ml/kg/min 36.5 (3.9)*52.5 (4.8)*
K-BIT composite score (IQ) 114.6 (14.9) 114.4 (7.6)
K-BIT crystallized score
(vocabulary) 110.8 (11.7) 109.4 (7.5)
K-BIT fluid score (matrices) 115.4 (17.6) 116.5 (9.2)
SES (median) 2.8 (0.6) 2.6 (0.6)
ADHD 5.9 (3.8) 7.1 (4.1)
F igures in parentheses represent SD. K-BIT = Kaufman Brief
Intelligence Test [Kaufman and Kaufman, 1990]; SES = socioeco-
nomic status. SES was determined by the creation of a trichoto-
mous index based on 3 variables: child participation in a free or
reduced-price lunch program at school, the highest level of educa-
tion obtained by the child’s mother and father, and the number of
parents who worked full-time [Birnbaum et al., 2002]. ADHD:
scores on the ADHD Rating Scale V [DuPaul et al., 1998].
* p < 0.001: significantly different.
Chaddock /Erickson /Prakash /VanPatter /
Vo ss
/Pontifex /Raine /Hillman /Kramer
Dev Neurosci 2010;32:249–256
252
index finger button press (via an MRI-compatible response box),
while trials in which the middle arrow pointed to the left (e.g.
! ! ! ! ! , 1 1 ! 1 1 ) required a left index finger button press.
During the task, 20 trials of each of the 4 possible arrow pre-
sentations ( 1 1 1 1 1 , ! ! ! ! ! , ! ! 1 ! ! , 1 1 ! 1 1 ) were shown in a
random order. Each array of arrows was presented for 1,500 ms,
and each stimulus array was separated by a fixation cross (+) pre-
sented for 1,500 ms. Forty additional fixation crosses that jittered
between 1,500 and 6,000 ms were also randomly presented after
the constant 1,500-ms fixation cross throughout the task. The jit-
ter prevented participants from expecting a specific frequency of
responding. White arrows and white fixation crosses were pre-
sented on a black backgrou nd. The participant was engaged in the
task for about 6 min, in addition to a 1-min block of 20 practice
trials (5 of each arrow arrays, presented randomly). Stimulus pre-
se nt ation , t im in g and t as k p erfor ma nc e me as ur es w ere co ntr ol le d
by E-Prime software.
R e s u l t s
Participant Demographics
Demographic and fitness data are provided in table1 .
None of the demographic variables (i.e. age, IQ, socioeco-
nomic status, ADHD) differed between higher-fit and
lower-fit groups. Higher-fit children had higher VO
2
max. scores than lower-fit participants as revealed by an
independent t test [t(53) = 13.69, p ! 0.001].
Aerobic Fitness and Flanker Performance
Table2 provides means (SD) for reaction time (RT)
and response accuracy for congruent and incongruent
conditions of the flanker task for higher-fit and lower-fit
groups.
To investigate the amount of behavioral interference
engendered by incongruent flanking items, a percent in-
terference score was computed for each participant as the
percent increase in RT to incongruent stimuli, over and
above the average RT to congruent stimuli [i.e. ([(incon-
gruent – congruent)/congruent] ! 100)] [Colcombe et
al., 2004]. This measure was derived to reflect interfer-
ence unbiased by differences in base RT. Only correct re-
sponses were included in the outcome measure.
The results of an independent t test revealed that high-
er-fit children (M = 5.17%; SD = 7.44%) showed less per-
cent interference compared to lower-fit children (M =
10.86%; SD = 11.58%) [t (53) = 2.20, p = 0.032] (see table2 ).
This suggests that higher-fit children (5% interference) are
more efficient at managing conflicting cues compared to
lower-fit preadolescents (11% interference). There were no
significant fitness-based differences in flanker accuracy
(all p 1 0.15) (although an examination of the means in-
dicated that higher-fit children showed a trend for supe-
rior accuracy for congruent and incongruent trials).
Globus
pallidus
Caudate
nucleus
Volume (mm3)
Nucleus
accumbens
Putamen
0
1,000
2,000
3,000
4,000
5,000
6,000
Caudate nucleus
Left Right
*
Putamen
Left Right
**
Globus pallidus
Left Right
**
Nucleus accumbens
Left Right
Lower-fit
Higher-fit
Fig. 1. FIRST segmentations of the bilateral caudate nucleus (blue), putamen (red), globus pallidus (yellow) and
nucleus accumbens (green) on a structural brain reconstruction as well as basal ganglia volumes (adjusted by
total intracranial volume) as a function of aerobic fitness group (error bars represent standard error). * p < 0.05 .
Basal Ganglia and Childhood Fitness Dev Neurosci 2010;32:249–256
253
Aerobic Fitness and Basal Ganglia Volumes
A multivariate analysis of variance indicated a signif-
icant effect of aerobic fitness group on basal ganglia vol-
ume [F (8, 46) = 2.89, p = 0.01]. This effect was significant
with and without total intracranial volume (the sum of
total gray matter, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid)
as a covariate. To decompose the effects of the omnibus
analysis, univariate ANCOVAs were conducted to com-
pare basal ganglia volumes as a function of aerobic fitness
group, with total intracranial volume (cubic millimeters)
as a covariate to control for variation in head size. The
results of these ANCOVAs are reported below, and table3
and figure 1 provide mean basal ganglia volumes as a
function of aerobic fitness group. Identical ANCOVA re-
sults were found when total gray matter volume was used
as a covariate, when both total intracranial volume and
total gray matter volume were inserted as covariates, and
without covariates. Higher-fit and lower-fit children did
not show differences in total gray matter volume, total
white matter volume, total cerebrospinal fluid or total in-
t racr an ia l v olum e (a ll t 1 0.07, p 1 0.46). In addition, there
were no significa nt effe cts of gender on st riatum volumes
after total intracranial volume was controlled (all F ! 0.7,
p 1 0.4).
Caudate Nucleus. Higher-fit children showed larger
left caudate nucleus volumes compared to lower-fit chil-
dren [F(1, 52) = 4.24, p = 0.04]. No fitness-based differ-
ences in volume were found for the right caudate nucleus
[F(1, 52) = 0.58, p = 0.45].
Putamen. Higher-fit children showed larger left puta-
men volumes [F(1, 52) = 13.80, p ! 0.0001] and larger right
putamen volumes [F(1, 52) = 11.40, p = 0.001] compared
to lower-fit children.
Globus Pallidus. Higher-fit children showed larger left
globus pallidus volumes [F (1, 52) = 8.43, p = 0.005] and
larger right globus pallidus volumes [F(1, 52) = 7.61, p =
0.008] compared to lower-fit children.
Nucleus Accumbens. There were no fitness differences
in left [F(1, 52) = 1.20, p = 0.28] or right [F(1, 52) = 0.03,
p = 0.85] nucleus accumbens volumes.
Basal Ganglia Volume and Flanker Performance
Caudate Nucleus. There were no significant Spearman
correlations between left or right caudate nucleus vol-
umes and flanker performance (i.e. accuracy or response
times) (all r ! 0.200, p 1 0.100).
Putamen. Left putamen volume was negatively corre-
lated with flanker percent interference (r = –0.333, p =
0.01). The correlation between right putamen volume and
percent interference was marginally significant (r =
–0.247, p = 0.06). Right putamen volume was also posi-
tively correlated with accuracy during incongruent
flanker task trials (r = 0.266, p = 0.05).
Globus Pallidus. Left (r = –0.261, p = 0.05) and right
(r = –0.350, p = 0.009) globus pallidus volumes were neg-
atively correlated with flanker percent interference.
Nucleus Accumbens. No significant correlations be-
tween nucleus accumbens volume and flanker task per-
formance were found (all r ! 0.210, p 1 0.100).
Discussion
The results revealed an association between aerobic
fitness, t he volume of the dorsal striatum a nd f lanker task
performance. Specifically, children with higher aerobic
fitness levels showed less behavioral interference to mis-
Tab le 2 . T ask performance by aerobic fitness group for congruent
and incongruent conditions of the flanker task
Measure Lower-fit Higher-fit
Mean congruent RT, ms 742.9 (97.1) 746.7 (64.0)
Mean incongruent RT, ms 821.4 (122.9) 786.1 (97.0)
Congruent response accuracy, % 88.3 (17.8) 93.6 (7.7)
Incongruent response accuracy, % 85.6 (15.7) 90.7 (15.2)
Percent interference RT, % 10.9 (11.6)*5.2 (7.4)*
F igures in parentheses represent SD: * p < 0.05: significantly
different.
Tab le 3 . Participant unadjusted mean basal ganglia volumes
(square millimeters) by aerobic fitness group
Variable Lower-fit Higher-fit
Left caudate nucleus*3,692.15 (722.13) 4,061.92 (464.82)
Right caudate nucleus 3,836.29 (716.60) 4,017.73 (568.31)
Left putamen*4,570.68 (1,065.20) 5,554.15 (775.64)
Right putamen*4,588.01 (1,046.08) 5,456.07 (734.18)
Left globus pallidus*1,720.01 (303.33) 1,947.80 (243.23)
Right globus pallidus*1,695.66 (283.44) 1,898.53 (229.29)
Left nucleus accumbens 486.87 (248.94) 566.59 (219.42)
Right nucleus accumbens 402.29 (192.07) 419.24 (200.19)
F igures in parentheses represent SD. * p < 0.05: significantly
different.
Chaddock /Erickson /Prakash /VanPatter /
Vo ss
/Pontifex /Raine /Hillman /Kramer
Dev Neurosci 2010;32:249–256
254
leading and irrelevant flanking cues, coupled with a larg-
er dorsal striatum (i.e. left caudate nucleus and bilateral
putamen). By the same accord, putamen volumes of the
dorsal striatum were negatively correlated with flanker
percent interference scores. The results support the claim
that the dorsal striatum is involved in cognitive control,
motor integration and response resolution [Aron et al.,
2009], processes associated with the performance of the
flanker task. That is, during the stimulus-response para-
digm, subjects must successfully prepare and initiate mo-
tor responses, flexibly switch between congruent and in-
congruent trials, and inhibit/filter incongruent and irrel-
evant information (provided by incongruent flanking
cues). Furthermore, the results support studies which
suggest that these cognitive processes are amenable to
aerobic fitness across the lifespan [Hillman et al., 2009;
Kramer et al., 1999].
No association between aerobic fitness, task perfor-
mance and ventral striatum volume was observed. The
nucleus accumbens is said to involve low-level limbic and
reward processes, functions less involved during the se-
lective attention and interference control paradigm. That
is, subjects were not provided feedback during the task
and thus were unable to adjust response selection based
on reinforcement.
In addition, the results reveal a link between child-
hood aerobic fitness, globus pallidus volume and flanker
task performance. After cortical and dopaminergic in-
puts have been integrated in the striatum, basal ganglia
output converges in the globus pallidus [Aron et al., 2009;
Di Martino et al., 2008; Draganski et al., 2008]. The pres-
ent results raise the possibility that aerobic fitness inter-
acts with both input and output basal ganglia regions,
resulting in a direct effect on behavior.
Human studies across the lifespan have predominant-
ly focused on the positive relation of aerobic fitness to
executive control processes and prefrontal neural net-
works [Colcombe and Kramer, 2003; Hillman et al., 2008,
but see Erickson et al., 2009]. The presence of cortex-stri-
atal loops emphasizes the tight functional and anatomi-
cal connections between the basal ganglia and frontal
cortical systems involved in the cognitive control of at-
tention, response inhibition, working memory and ex-
ecutive function [Aron et al., 2009; Casey et al., 1997; Lis-
ton et al., 2006]. Given the well-established effect of aero-
bic fitness on prefrontal cortex structure and function, it
is plausible that aerobic fitness also impacts subcortical
regions connected to the frontal cortex. While the cur-
rent study is limited to conclusions about subcortical
structures given the chosen brain segmentation tech-
nique, future investigations should examine the role of
visual, parietal and frontal cortical areas as well as corti-
cal-striatal connectivity in the association between child-
hood fitness and f lanker task performance. A study of the
duration of fitness effects on childhood neurocognition
as well as the effects of a physical activity intervention on
basal ganglia structure and function are also important
avenues for future research.
Together, the behavioral and structural imaging re-
sults suggest that higher-fit children exhibit larger vol-
umes in specific subregions of the basal ganglia, which
may impact flanker task performance. The results are im-
portant because they provide additional support suggest-
ing that cognitive enhancement through increased fit-
ness is directly related to differential volumes of brain
regions involved in cognitive function [Hillman et al.,
2008; Chaddock et al., 2010]. In addition, it seems that
childhood aerobic fitness not only impacts the hippo-
campus; it also affects the structure and function of the
basal ganglia. Nevertheless, the results suggest that aero-
bic fitness does not have a general impact on the volume
of all structures in the brain, but rather that there is some
specificity, given that dorsal but not ventral striatal struc-
tures were associated with aerobic fitness.
The results have important public health and educa-
tional implications, given the rise of childhood sedentary
behaviors and obesity rates as well as the reduction and
elimination of physical activity opportunities in schools
[Baker et al., 2007; Hillman et al., 2008; Ludwig, 2007;
Olshansky et al., 2005]. The present findings suggest that
high levels of aerobic fitness in children can positively
impact structural volumes of the basal ganglia involved
in learning and cognitive control, two essential functions
involved in academic success. Hopefully, the present
findings will encourage modifications of educational and
health care policies which emphasize the importance of
physical activity on physical and cognitive health.
Acknowledgements
We would like to t hank Nancy Dodge a nd Holly Tracy for their
help with data collection.
Basal Ganglia and Childhood Fitness Dev Neurosci 2010;32:249–256
255
References
Aguiar AS, Speck AE, Prediger R D, Kapczinski
F, Pinho RA (2008): Downhill training up-
regulates mice hippocampal and striatal
brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels. J
Neural Transm 115:
1251–1255.
American College of Sports Medicine (2006):
ACSM’s Guidelines for Exercise Testing and
Prescription, ed 7. New York, Lippincott
Williams & Wilkins, p 366.
Aron AR, Poldrack R A, Wise SP (2009): Cogni-
tion: basal ganglia role; in Squire LR (ed.):
Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, vol 2, pp
1069 –1077.
B aker JL, Ol sen LW, Sore nsen TIA (2007): Ch ild-
hood body-mass index and risk of coronary
heart disease in adulthood. N Engl J Med
357:
2329–2337.
Bar-Or O (1983): Pediatric sports medicine for
the prac titioner: From physiologic pr inciples
to clinical applications. New York, Springer,
p 376.
Birnbaum AS, Lytle LA, Murray DM, Story M,
Perry CL, Boutelle KN (2002): Survey devel-
opment for assessing correlates of young ad-
olescents’ eat ing. Am J Hea lth Behav 26:
284–
295.
Buck SM, Hillman CH, Castelli DM (2008): The
relat ion of aerobic fitness to Stroop task per-
formance i n preadolescent child ren. Med Sci
Sports Exerc 40:
166 –172.
Casey BJ, Getz S, Galvan A (2008): The adoles-
cent brain. Dev Rev 28:
62–77.
Casey BJ, Thomas KM, Welsh TF, Badgaiyan
RD, Eccard CH, Jennings JR, Crone EA
(2000): Dissociation of response conflict, at-
tentional selection, and expectancy with
functional magnetic resonance imaging.
Proc Natl Acad Sci 97:
8728–8733.
Casey BJ, Trainor RJ, Orendi JL, Schubert AB,
Nystrom LE , Giedd JN, Castellanos FX,
Haxby JV, Noll DC, Cohen JD, Forman SD,
Dahl R E, Rapopor t JL (1997): A developmen-
tal functional MRI study of prefrontal acti-
vation during performance of a go-no-go
task. J Cog Neurosci 9:
835–847.
Castelli DM, Hil lman CH, Buck SM, Er win HE
(2007): Physical fitness and academic
achievement in third- and fifth-grade stu-
dents. J Sport Exerc Psychol 29:
239–252.
Chaddock L, Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Kim JS,
Voss MW, Va nPatter M, Pontifex MB, Raine
LB, Konkel A, Hillman CH, Cohen NJ,
Kramer AF (2010): A neuroimaging investi-
gation of the association between aerobic fit-
ness, hippo campal volume and memor y per-
formance in preadolescent children. Devel-
opment & Aging. Abstr Cogn Neurosci Soc
B82-76.
Chaddock L, Hillman CH, Buck SM, Cohen NJ
(in press): Aerobic fitness and executive con-
trol of relational memory in preadolescent
children: MSSE.
Chomitz VR, Slining MM, McGowan RJ,
Mitchel l SE, Dawson GF, Hacker KA (20 09):
Is there a relationship between physical fit-
ness and academic achievement? Positive
results from public school children in the
northeastern United States . J Sch Health 79:
30–37.
Colcombe S, Kramer AF (2003): Fitness effects
on the cognitive function of older adults: A
meta-ana lytic study. Psychol Sci 14:
125–130.
Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P,
McAuley E, Cohen NJ, Webb A, Jerome GJ,
Marquez DX, El avsky S (2004): Card iovascu-
lar fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging.
PNAS 101:
3316–3321.
Cotman CW, Berchtold NC (2002): Exercise: a
behavioral intervention to enhance brain
health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci 25:
295–301.
Di Martino A, Scheres A, Marguiles DS, Kelly
AM, Uddin LQ, Shehzad Z, Biswal B, Wal-
ters JR, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2008):
Functional connectivity of human striatum:
a resting state fMRI study. Cereb Cortex 18:
2735–2747.
Di ng YH, Luan XD, Li J, R afols JA, Guthin konda
M, Diaz FG, Ding Y (2004): Exercise-in-
duced overexpression of angiogenic factors
and reduction of ischemia/reperfusion inju-
ry in stroke. Curr Neurovasc Res 1:
411–420.
Draganski B, Kherif F, Kloppel S, Cook PA, Al-
exander DC, Parker GJ, Deichmann R, Ash-
bu rn er J , Fr ac kow ia k R S (2 00 8): E vi den ce for
segregated and integrative connectivity pat-
terns in t he human basal ganglia. J Neurosci
28:
7143–7152.
DuPaul GJ, Power TJ, Anastopoulos A, Reid R
(1998): ADHD Rating Sc ale – IV: Checklist s,
Norms, and Clinical Interpretation. New
York, Guilford Press.
Erickson KI, Boot WR, Basak C, Neider MR,
Prak ash RS, Voss MW, Graybiel AM, Simon s
DJ, Fabiani M, Gratton G, Kramer AF (in
press): Striata l volume predicts level of v ideo
game skill acquisition.
Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Voss MW, Chaddock
L, Hu L, Morris KS, White SM, Wojcicki TR,
McAuley E, Kramer AF (2009): Aerobic fit-
ness is associated with hippocampal volume
in elderly humans. Hippocampus 19:
1030
1039.
Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW (1974): Effects of noise
letters on the identification of a target letter
in a nonsearch task. Percept Psychophys 16:
143 –149.
Fre edson PS, Good man TL (1993): Measurement
of oxygen consumption; in Rowland TW
(ed): Pediatric Laboratory Exercise Test-
ing: Clinical Guidelines. Human Kinetics,
Champaign, pp 91–113.
Graybiel A M (2005): The basal ganglia: learning
new tricks and loving it. Curr Opin Neuro-
biol 15:
638–644.
Gr aybiel AM (2008): Habits, r ituals and t he eval-
u at iv e b ra i n. An nu Re v N eu r os ci 31:
359–387.
Hillman CH, Buck SM, T hemanson JR, Pontifex
MB, Castelli DM (2009): Aerobic fitness and
cognitive development: event-related brain
potential and task performance of executive
control in preadolescent children. Dev Psy-
chol 45:
114–129.
Hillman CH, Castelli DM, Buck SM (2005): Aer-
obic fitness and neurocognitive function in
healthy preadolescent children. Med Sci
Sports Exerc 37:
1967–1974.
Hi llman CH, E rickson KI, K ramer AF (2008): Be
sm a r t, ex e rc is e y o ur he a rt : ex er c is e e f fe c ts on
brain and cognition. Nature Rev Neurosci 9:
58–65.
Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL (1990): Kaufman
Brief Intelligence Test. Circle Pines, AGS.
K ramer AF, Hahn S , Cohen N, Banich M, McAu-
ley E , Harrison C, Chason J, Vakil E, Bardell
L, Boileau RA, Colcombe A (1999): Aging,
fitne ss, and neurocog nitive func tion. Nature
400:
418–419.
Kramer A, Humphrey D, Larish J, Logan G,
Strayer D (1994): Aging and inhibition: be-
yond a unitary view of inhibitory processing
in attention. Psychol Aging 9:
491–512.
Li J, Ding YH, Rafols JA, Lai Q, McAllister JP,
Ding Y (2005): Incre ased astroc yte prolifera-
tion in rats after running exercise. Neurosci
Lett 386:
160 –164.
Liston C, Watts R, Tottenham N, Davidson MC,
Niogi S, Ulug AM, Casey BJ (2006): Fronto-
striatal microstructure modulates efficient
recruit ment of cognitive control. C ereb Cor-
tex 16:
553–56 0.
Lopez-Lopez C, LeRoith D, Torres-Aleman I
(2004): Insulin-like grow th factor I is re-
quired for vessel remodeling in the adult
brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:
9833–
9838.
Ludwig DS (2007): Ch ildhood obesity : the shape
of things to come. N Engl J Med 357:
2325–
2327.
Mar ais L, Stein DJ, Da niels WM (2009): Exercise
increases BDNF levels in the striatum and
decreases depressive-like behavior in chron-
ically stressed rats. Metab Brain Dis 24:
587–
597.
Marques E, Vasconcelos F, Rolo MR, Pereira FC,
Silva AP, Macedo TR, Ribeiro CF (2008): In-
fluence of chronic exercise on the amphet-
amine-induced dopamine release and neu-
rodegeneration in the striatum of the rat.
Ann NY Acad Sci 1139:
222–231.
Neeper S, Gomez-Pinilla F, Choi J, Cot man CW
(1995): Exercise and brain neurotrophins.
Nature 373:
109.
Oldfield RC (1971): The assessment and analysis
of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.
Neuropsychologia 9:
97–113.
Olshansky SJ, Passaro DJ, Hershow RC, Layden
J, C ar nes B A, Br ody J, Hayf lic k L, But ler R N,
Allison DB, Ludwig DS (2005): A potential
decline in life expectancy in the United
States in the 21st century. N Engl J Med 352:
1138 –1143.
Chaddock /Erickson /Prakash /VanPatter /
Vo ss
/Pontifex /Raine /Hillman /Kramer
Dev Neurosci 2010;32:249–256
256
Patenaude B (2007): Bayesian statistical models
of shape and appearance for subcortical
brain segmentation (D. Phil. thesis). Oxford,
University of Oxford.
Patenaude B, Smith SM, Kennedy D, Jenk inson
M (2007a): FIRST-FMRIB’s integrated regis-
tration and segmentation tool. Chicago, Hu-
man Brain Mapping Conference.
Patenaude B, Smith SM, Kennedy D, Jenk inson
M (2007b): Bayesian shape and appearance
models. Technical report TR07BP1. Oxford,
FMRIB Center, University of Oxford.
Ragozzino ME, Jih J, Tzavos A (2002): Involve-
ment of the dorsomedial striatum in behav-
ioral f lexibility: role of muscarinic choliner-
gic receptors. Brain Res 953:
205–214.
Shi LH, Luo F, Woodward DJ, Chang JY (2004):
Neural responses in multiple basal ganglia
regions during spontaneous and treadmill
locomotion tasks in rats. Exp Brain Res 157:
303–314.
Shvartz E, Reibold RC (1990): Aerobic fitness
norms for males and females aged 6 to 75
years: a rev iew. Aviat Space Environ Med 61:
3–11.
Sibley BA, Etnier JL (2003): The relationship be-
tween physic al activ ity and cognit ion in chil-
dren: a meta-a nalysis. Pediatr Exerc Sci 15:
243–256.
Tanner JM (1962): Growth at Adolescence.
Oxford, Blackwell Scientific Publications,
p 340.
Taylor SJC, Whincup PH, Hindmarsh PC,
Lampe F, Odoki K, Cook DG (2001): Perfor-
mance of a new pubertal self-assessment
questionnaire: a preliminary study. Paediatr
Perinat Epidemiol 15:
88–94.
Ti llerson JL, C ohen AD, Philhower J, Mi ller GW,
Zigmond MJ, Schallert T (2001): Forced
limb-use effects on the behavioral and neu-
rochemical effects of 6-hydroxydopamine. J
Neurosci 21:
4427–4435.
Utter AC, Roberson RJ, Nieman DC, Kang J
(2002): Children’s OMNI scale of perceived
exertion: walking/running evaluation. Med
Sci Sports Exerc 34:
139–144.
Van Praag H, Christie BR, Sejnowski TJ, Gage
FH (1999): Running enhances neurogenesis,
learning, and long-term potentiation in
mice. Neurobiology 96:
13427–13431.
Van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao C, Gage FH
(2005): Exercise enhances learning and hip-
pocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J Neu-
rosci 25:
8680–8685.
Vaynman S, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F (2004):
Hippoca mpal BDNF mediates t he efficac y of
exercise on synaptic plasticity and cognition.
Eur J Neurosci 20:
2580–2590.
Wylie SA, van den Wildenberg WP, Ridderink-
hof KR, Bashore TR, Powell VD, Manning
CA, Wooten GF (2009): The effe ct of Parkin-
son’s disease on interference control during
action selection. Neuropsychologia 47:
145
157.
... A number of hypotheses were posited in the 1980s and 90s but it was not until van Praag et al., 1999 demonstrated that chronic exercise results in the synthesis and release of the protein brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in the brains of rodents that any possible neuroscientific, or indeed any psychological mechanisms, were discussed. As BDNF was known to be vital for neurogenesis and neuroplasticity (Chen & Nakagawa, 2023), chronic exercise-induced brain BDNF activation soon formed the major rationale for the positive effects of chronic exercise on cognition in a variety of populations (e.g., Chaddock et al., 2010;Erickson et al., 2011). ...
... Neuroscientific research suggests that physical fitness, particularly aerobic exercise, induces structural and functional changes in brain regions associated with cognitive control. Studies have shown that higher fitness levels are linked to increased hippocampal volume and enhanced thalamocortical connectivity, which play crucial roles in attention and memory [6,7]. One potential mechanism is the upregulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in response to physical activity, promoting synaptic plasticity and enhancing neural signal transmission efficiency in the prefrontal cortex. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective This study aimed to explore the relationship between physical fitness and cognitive control in Chinese children, with a focus on gender differences and the moderating role of family support for physical activity (FSFPA). Method This study employed a cross-sectional design to assess 148 children aged 12–14 years from Guangzhou. Physical fitness was evaluated using the National Student Physical Fitness Standard, family support for physical activity (FSFPA) was measured with the validated Family Support for Physical Activity Scale, and cognitive control was assessed using the Stroop color-word task, Go/No-Go task, and task-cue paradigm. The statistical analyses included descriptive statistics, correlation analysis, and hierarchical regression analysis to examine the relationships between variables. Results Gender differences were observed in physical fitness and cognitive control. Girls scored higher in physical fitness and showed better accuracy in interference suppression, with faster reaction times in impulse control. BMI and speed were positively correlated with cognitive flexibility in both genders. For boys, cardiopulmonary endurance positively affected interference suppression, and muscle strength influenced impulse control. BMI and speed were linked to cognitive flexibility. For girls, speed, cardiopulmonary endurance, and muscle strength improved interference suppression, while BMI influenced cognitive flexibility. FSFPA moderated the relationship between physical fitness and cognitive control for both genders. It positively impacted interference suppression and cognitive flexibility for both boys and girls. Conclusion Physical fitness in Chinese children is positively associated with cognitive control, with gender-specific differences in the fitness components influencing cognitive outcomes. FSFPA significantly moderates this relationship, enhancing the positive effects of physical fitness on cognitive control. These findings suggest that promoting physical fitness, particularly through family-based physical activity support, may improve children’s cognitive control abilities.
... Increased childhood cardiorespiratory fitness is associated with higher cue-P300 amplitudes, reflecting more effective allocation of attentional resources during encoding, and greater CNV amplitude associated with enhanced preparatory and anticipatory processes during maintenance. This relation suggests that in children, cardiorespiratory fitness translates into better cognitive control and the ability to adapt to varying task demands (Chaddock et al. 2010;Kamijo, Bae, and Masaki 2016;Ludyga, Mücke, et al. 2020;Scudder et al. 2016). This pattern also seems to apply to older adults as greater CNV and cue-P300 amplitudes and decreased cue-P300 latency were observed in higher-fit compared to lower-fit participants, suggesting improved working memory effectiveness, indicating a more proactive cognitive control profile (Chang et al. 2013;Hillman et al. 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
Working memory is a crucial component of cognitive performance, supporting well-being, mental health, and successful personal and academic endeavors. Working memory performance and capacity peak during young adulthood, a critical period for managing increased life challenges. Emerging evidence indicates that cardiorespiratory fitness holds potential to enhance working memory performance; however, the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. This study examined the association between cardiorespiratory fitness and neurophysiological markers of cognitive control during a task requiring working memory maintenance. Young healthy adults (N = 112) completed a submaximal ergometer test to estimate cardiorespiratory fitness. Working memory maintenance was assessed using the Sternberg task under low and high cognitive loads, while event-related potentials (ERP) components (cue-P300, CNV, and probe-P300) were recorded. In addition, an immediate free recall task was administered. Path analysis revealed that higher cardiorespiratory fitness was significantly associated with better accuracy in the Sternberg task exclusively under high cognitive load (β = 0.21, p = 0.03). In the high load condition, higher negativity of the contingent negative variation (CNV) amplitude was significantly associated with higher fitness levels (β = −0.20, p = 0.03) and with better performance on the immediate free recall task (β = −0.23, p = 0.02), suggesting enhanced neurophysiological preparatory processes and dominance of proactive cognitive control strategy in fitter individuals. Cue-P300 and probe-P300 did not show significant associations with cardiorespiratory fitness nor working memory performance. These findings underscore the role of proactive cognitive control in mediating the benefits of cardiorespiratory fitness on cognitive performance.
... Zudem unterstützen neurowissenschaftliche Befunde infolge von Sportprogrammen bei Kin-dern eine erhöhte Aktivierung des präfrontalen Kortex (Davis et al., 2011) oder eine Ver bes se rung von kognitiven Prozessen, die von dieser Struktur abhängig sind (Ludyga et al., 2018a, b). Außerdem scheinen Aspekte der körperlichen Fitness mit dem Vo lu men der Basalganglien verbunden zu sein (Chaddock et al., 2010). Zusammengefasst weisen diese Studien auf direkte Effekte der sportlichen Aktivität auf Gehirnstrukturen hin, die für die exekutiven Funktionen und motorische Leistung gleichermaßen verantwortlich sind (Leisman et al., 2016). ...
... In addition, better aerobic fitness in adolescents was associated with greater bilateral hippocampal volume, which was positively correlated with attention in adolescents (79). Adolescents with high aerobic fitness also had larger dorsal striatum volumes, which are associated with cognitive control (80). Moderate-to vigorousintensity exercise directly and positively impacts attention (81). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose To explore the association between physical activity and attention among children aged 6–12 years and to determine whether fundamental movement skills and aerobic fitness mediate the association between physical activity and attention. Methods A total of 1,086 school-aged children (mean age: 9.40 ± 2.03 years) were included in the present study. Physical activity was assessed using the International Physical Activity Scale-Short Form. Fundamental movement skills were assessed using the Test of Gross Motor Development, third edition, and aerobic fitness was evaluated using the 20-metre shuttle run. Attention was assessed by the d2 Attention Test. Results There were significant positive correlations between physical activity and fundamental movement skills, physical activity and aerobic fitness, and physical activity and attention (all p < 0.05). Both fundamental movement skills and aerobic fitness fully mediated the relationship between physical activity and attention. In addition, fundamental movement skills and aerobic fitness had chain-mediating effects on the relationship between physical activity and attention. Conclusion High levels of physical activity were positively associated with attention among school-aged children. Fundamental movement skills and aerobic fitness played a chain-mediating role in the relationship between physical activity and attention.
... The basal nuclei are responsible for actions such as motor, spatial, visual, and affective. Specifically, the putamen region of the basal nuclei is responsible for motor and visual tasks, being activated during sports, and acting for better inhibitory control [68,96,97] and these regions are known to be enhanced during sports, mainly for open skill sports [68]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The impact of cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF) on cognition is thought to be mediated by brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Aerobic exercise can increase CRF through various activities, including sports participation. The relationship between these factors in females has yet to be elucidated. Objective This review aims to map the current literature on the effects of aerobic exercise, sports participation, and CRF in healthy adult females, with sub-topics of pregnancy and menstrual cycle periodicity. Methods A scoping review of the literature was conducted following PRISMA guidelines and the PCC mnemonic (population, concept, and context). The following five databases were screened: CINAHL, Medline, Web of Science, SPORTDiscus, and Scopus. Eligible articles included healthy adult females, investigated aerobic exercise, sports participation or CRF, and linked outcomes to cognition. Data from included manuscripts was extracted and analyzed. Two sub-population groupings (pregnant individuals and menstrual cycle) were established to further aid the interpretation of the findings. Results Of the 300 titles and abstracts screened, 74 were eligible for full-text screening, and 28 were included in the scoping review. Of the 28 included, 14 did not control for or report on menstrual cycle phase or sex hormones. Conclusion This scoping review found an inverse ‘U’ relationship between aerobic exercise and cognition, demonstrating an optimal dose of aerobic exercise to benefit cognitive functions. As estrogen may impact the relationship between CRF and neural growth factors, more research is needed on this pathway, independent of the menstrual cycle, to determine potential beneficial effects. It is currently unknown whether sports participation can independently impact cognition.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: To explore the association between cardiorespiratory fitness and inhibitory control in elementary school children, and compare the difference in brain activation patterns between children with high and low cardiovascular fitness groups, so as to provide empirical evidence for revealing the neural mechanisms underlying the inhibitory control of cardiorespiratory function in children. Methods: A total of 140 children aged 7-12 were recruited. They were divided into high and low cardiovascular fitness groups based on the 20-meter shuttle run test. The Flanker task was used to assess the inhibition control of the participants. The 8-channel functional near-infrared spectroscopy was used to monitor the Hemoglobin oxygenation (oxy-Hb) concentration of prefrontal cortex (PFC). Results: Compared to low cardiorespiratory fitness group, children in high cardiorespi-ratory fitness had better task performance, as evidenced by faster reaction time. And high cardiorespiratory fitness group exhibited higher concentrations of oxy-Hb and higher activation in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex(R-DLPFC) and in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (L-DLPFC) during the Flanker task. Relevant regression analysis showed that the activation of the R-DLPFC plays a fully mediating role between cardiorespiratory fitness and inhibitory control. Conclusion: The activation of the R-DLPFC is influenced by the cardiovascular fitness, which in turn improves inhibitory control function.
Article
Chronic diseases are major killers in the modern era. Physical inactivity is a primary cause of most chronic diseases. The initial third of the article considers: activity and prevention definitions; historical evidence showing physical inactivity is detrimental to health and normal organ functional capacities; cause versus treatment; physical activity and inactivity mechanisms differ; gene‐environment interaction (including aerobic training adaptations, personalized medicine, and co‐twin physical activity); and specificity of adaptations to type of training. Next, physical activity/exercise is examined as primary prevention against 35 chronic conditions [accelerated biological aging/premature death, low cardiorespiratory fitness (V o 2 max), sarcopenia, metabolic syndrome, obesity, insulin resistance, prediabetes, type 2 diabetes, nonalcoholic fatty liver disease, coronary heart disease, peripheral artery disease, hypertension, stroke, congestive heart failure, endothelial dysfunction, arterial dyslipidemia, hemostasis, deep vein thrombosis, cognitive dysfunction, depression and anxiety, osteoporosis, osteoarthritis, balance, bone fracture/falls, rheumatoid arthritis, colon cancer, breast cancer, endometrial cancer, gestational diabetes, pre‐eclampsia, polycystic ovary syndrome, erectile dysfunction, pain, diverticulitis, constipation, and gallbladder diseases]. The article ends with consideration of deterioration of risk factors in longer‐term sedentary groups; clinical consequences of inactive childhood/adolescence; and public policy. In summary, the body rapidly maladapts to insufficient physical activity, and if continued, results in substantial decreases in both total and quality years of life. Taken together, conclusive evidence exists that physical inactivity is one important cause of most chronic diseases. In addition, physical activity primarily prevents, or delays, chronic diseases, implying that chronic disease need not be an inevitable outcome during life. © 2012 American Physiological Society. Compr Physiol 2:1143‐1211, 2012.
Article
Purpose This paper aims to present a theoretical framework and underpinning evidence to support researchers and practitioners to systematically design sport and exercise interventions. Design/methodology/approach The framework highlights the theoretical underpinnings and practical feasibility considerations that are important when designing sport and exercise interventions for children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Findings Future research may wish to apply and evaluate the utility of this framework and research and practice and make recommendations for further refinement. Originality/value The quality and strength of conclusions from this body of research has been undermined by a lack of methodological quality and clarity in these studies. One criticism has been the lack of an evidence based, transparent and consistent approach to the design of physical activity and sport interventions and selection of outcome measures. This paper will facilitate the transparent and evidence-based decision-making to guide the design of sport/physical activity interventions for children/adolescents with ADHD.
Article
Full-text available
Extensive research on humans suggests that exercise could have benefits for overall health and cognitive function, particularly in later life. Recent studies using animal models have been directed towards understanding the neurobiological bases of these benefits. It is now clear that voluntary exercise can increase levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and other growth factors, stimulate neurogenesis, increase resistance to brain insult and improve learning and mental performance. Recently, high-density oligonucleotide microarray analysis has demonstrated that, in addition to increasing levels of BDNF, exercise mobilizes gene expression profiles that would be predicted to benefit brain plasticity processes. Thus, exercise could provide a simple means to maintain brain function and promote brain plasticity.
Article
Full-text available
This study examines important developmental differences in patterns of activation in the prefrontal cortex during performance of a Go-No-Go paradigm using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Eighteen subjects (9 children and 9 adults) were scanned using gradient echo, echo planar imaging during performance of a response inhibition task. The results suggest four general findings. First, the location of activation in the prefrontal cortex was not different between children and adults, which is similar to our earlier pediatric fMRI results of prefrontal activation during a working memory task (Casey et al., 1995). Second, the volume of activation was significantly greater for children relative to adults. These differences in volume of activation were observed predominantly in the dorsal and lateral prefrontal cortices. Third, although inhibitory processes have typically been associated with more ventral or orbital frontal regions, the current study revealed activation that was distributed across both dorsolateral and orbitofrontal cortices. Finally, consistent with animal and human lesion studies, activity in orbital frontal and anterior cingulate cortices correlated with behavioral performance (i.e., number of false alarms). These results further demonstrate the utility of this methodology in studying pediatric populations.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to quantitatively combine and examine the results of studies pertaining to physical activity and cognition in children. Studies meeting the inclusion criteria were coded based on design and descriptive characteristics, subject characteristics, activity characteristics, and cognitive assessment method. Effect sizes (ESs) were calculated for each study and an overall ES and average ESs relative to moderator variables were then calculated. ESs (n = 125) from 44 studies were included in the analysis. The overall ES was 0.32 (SD = 0.27), which was significantly different from zero. Significant moderator variables included publication status, subject age, and type of cognitive assessment. As a result of this statistical review of the literature, it is concluded that there is a significant positive relationship between physical activity and cognitive functioning in children.
Chapter
From the early twentieth century, neurologists noted that diseases affecting the basal ganglia caused cognitive deficits, including problems with memory, attention, and other mental activities. After a protracted period in which the contribution of the basal ganglia to motor control received the most attention, the past two decades have seen a resurgence of interest in their cognitive roles. It is now clear that the structures composing the basal ganglia take part in a wide range of cognitive functions, including learning, memory, skill, planning, switching, sequencing, classification, timing, and the processing of rewards, as well as other feedback.
Article
Physicians who care for children are often called upon to render advice on sports and exercise. Long before the physical fitness fad struck the adult sector, children used sports to have fun, work off steam, test themselves against their peers, and learn about real life. Their physicians, once called on only to tend their wounds, now must be prepared to advise on exercise appropriate to age, size, development, and any concurrent disease, and to control access to many competitive activities.This well-written, carefully documented book provides a great deal of information by which such decisions can be informed. Its extended title accurately describes the focus of the book: exercise physiology in the immature human, with special applications to children with several forms of chronic disorders. Two initial chapters clearly describe the response of children of differing age, sex, and size to exercise. Applications to children with pulmonary, cardiovascular, endocrine, nutritional,
Article
In the ageing process, neural areas¹,² and cognitive processes³,⁴ do not degrade uniformly. Executive control processes and the prefrontal and frontal brain regions that support them show large and disproportionate changes with age. Studies of adult animals indicate that metabolic⁵ and neurochemical⁶ functions improve with aerobic fitness. We therefore investigated whether greater aerobic fitness in adults would result in selective improvements in executive control processes, such as planning, scheduling, inhibition and working memory. Over a period of six months, we studied 124 previously sedentary adults, 60 to 75 years old, who were randomly assigned to either aerobic (walking) or anaerobic (stretching and toning) exercise. We found that those who received aerobic training showed substantial improvements in performance on tasks requiring executive control compared with anaerobically trained subjects.
Article
The purpose of this manual is to describe two behavior questionnaires (the ADHD Rating Scale—IV: Home Version and the ADHD Rating Scale—IV: School Version) that are based on the diagnostic criteria for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as described in the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Information is presented about the development and standardization of these scales, collection of normative data, factor structure, psychometric properties (i.e., reliability and validity), as well as the interpretive uses of these scales in clinical and school settings. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
During a 1-sec tachistoscopic exposure, Ss responded with a right or left leverpress to a single target letter from the sets H and K or S and C. The target always appeared directly above the fixation cross. Experimentally varied were the types of noise letters (response compatible or incompatible) flanking the target and the spacing between the letters in the display. In all noise conditions, reaction time (RT) decreased as between-letter spacing increased. However, noise letters of the opposite response set were found to impair RT significantly more than same response set noise, while mixed noise letters belonging to neither set but having set-related features produced intermediate impairment. Differences between two target-alone control conditions, one presented intermixed with noise-condition trials and one presented separately in blocks, gave evidence of a preparatory set on the part of Ss to inhibit responses to the noise letters. It was concluded that S cannot prevent processing of noise letters occurring within about 1 deg of the target due to the nature of processing channel capacity and must inhibit his response until he is able to discriminate exactly which letter is in the target position. This discrimination is more difficult and time consuming at closer spacings, and inhibition is more difficult when noise letters indicate the opposite response from the targe