Content uploaded by Taina Rantanen
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Taina Rantanen
Content may be subject to copyright.
Aging Clin Exp Res. First published ahead of print July 16, 2010 as DOI: 10.3275/7200
Copyright© 2010, Editrice Kurtis
Benefits of formal voluntary work among older people – A review
Mikaela B. von Bonsdorff, PhD1,2,3, Taina Rantanen, PhD1
1Gerontology Research Centre, Department of Health Sciences, University of Jyväskylä, Finland
2GeroCenter Foundation for Research and Development, Jyväskylä, Finland
3The Central Finland Health Care District, Jyväskylä, Finland
Corresponding author and reprint requests:
Mikaela von Bonsdorff, Researcher, PhD
Gerontology Research Centre
Department of Health Sciences
University of Jyväskylä
PO Box 35 (Viveca)
FIN-40014 University of Jyväskylä
Tel. +358 14 260 4596, Fax +358 14 260 4600, e-mail mikaela.vonbonsdorff@jyu.fi
Keywords: formal volunteering, well-being, health, post-retirement, older people
Running head: Voluntary work among older people
DISCLAIMER: This is an un-copyedited author manuscript copyrighted by Editrice Kurtis. This may
not be duplicated or reproduced, other than for personal use, without permission of the copyright
owner. From the time of acceptance following peer review, the full text of this manuscript is made
freely available by Editrice Kurtis at http://www.kurtis.it/aging/en/list-preview.cfm. The final copyedited
article will be available at the journal website at http://www.kurtis.it/ after publication on Aging Clinical
and Experimental Research. Editrice Kurtis disclaims any responsibility or liability for errors or
omissions in this version of the manuscript or in any version derived from it. The citation of this
article must include the following information: author(s), article title, journal title, year of publication,
and DOI.
Received March 10, 2010; Accepted in revised form June 17, 2010.
2
ABSTRACT
A narrative review of quantitative population-based longitudinal studies was conducted to
examine the association of formal voluntary work and personal well-being among older people
doing the voluntary work and those being served. To be included the study had to be published
in a peer-reviewed journal, written in English and conducted in the Western countries,
participants had to be at least 60 years of age, the study employed a longitudinal or experimental
design, the methodology and outcomes explicitly described and voluntary work quantified as
visits or hours within a certain timeframe. Sixteen studies out of 2897 met the inclusion criteria
for the review reporting on benefits of volunteering for those doing the voluntary work.
Outcomes were collapsed into three categories of personal well-being: physical health, mental
health, and psychosocial resources. All included studies came from the United States and showed
that volunteering in old age predicted better self-rated health, functioning, physical activity and
life satisfaction as well as decreased depression and mortality. However, volunteering did not
decrease the risk of chronic diseases or nursing home admission in old age. Only one study that
met the inclusion criteria on the benefits of volunteering for the older recipients was identified.
Studies predominantly utilized data from large datasets with only limited information about
volunteering which limits more detailed analyses. Randomized controlled trials are needed for
studying the effect of voluntary work for those being served as well as to unmask the health
participant –effect among the volunteers.
3
INTRODUCTION
Volunteering is defined as an activity that involves spending time, unpaid, doing something that
aims to benefit the community in general or its individuals or specified subsets of community
members who are in need, such as older home bound persons (1, 2). This review focused on
formal volunteering and excluded informal volunteering such as helping and caring for close
relatives, friends or neighbors. What differentiates formal and informal volunteering is a sense of
obligation which is often stronger in informal volunteering than it is in formal volunteering,
where volunteering depends more on the ability and opportunity of the helper (1). Volunteering
has been quantified according to its intensity (the amount of time spent), diversity (how many
organizations), and consistency (how regularly the individual volunteers across the lifespan).
Formal volunteering is usually coordinated by public agencies, nonprofit agencies, religious
organizations, or through government programs covering a wide range of activities (1, 3).
Volunteering typically includes activities such as tutoring or mentoring either instrumental,
supportive or nonskilled; skilled or unskilled assistance or technical advice and public safety (4).
The growth in the older population in the 21st century will be a challenge to the public economy
and health care service system but it might also offer possibilities. The predicted shortage in the
labor force will increase the demand for volunteers in the future. The healthy active older
population who are not engaged in paid work constitutes a growing reservoir of human and
social capital (5-7), and volunteering is potentially a good venue to contribute to the society (8).
The generation born between 1946 and 1960 will enter into old age with a great deal of
experience and skills that could be transferred to the non-profit sector. In addition, increasing
longevity, health and wealth allows for non-profit activities such as volunteering in later life (9,
10). Volunteering could be a cornerstone of productive aging, as volunteering helps provide
services that are of economical and social value (11, 12).
4
Most studies on volunteering have been done in the United States, which is the most active
nation in volunteer work in Western Countries. According to the Current Population Survey, the
rate of volunteering in the U.S among people aged 65 years or over was 23.5% in the year 2008
(13). The long-standing tradition in volunteering in the U.S is partly due to their welfare system
that e.g. builds on non-profit and religious organizations as well as the fact that the U.S policy
makers have actively encouraged volunteering (14). In Europe, the rate of volunteering differs
between the countries, depending on the welfare policy (15). In Nordic countries, where the
public health care system is responsible for social welfare, older people participate more actively
in voluntary work than in the Mediterranean countries. There interfamily exchange is strong and
cohabitation among generations more usual than in north Europe (16). Cross-national data from
the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) on volunteering showed that
the rate of volunteering ranged between 2 and 21%, with an average of 10%, depending strongly
on the countries’ societal context concerning volunteering (17). In Australia about 20% (18) and
in Canada about 18% (19) of the older population had volunteered in the previous year.
In cross-sectional studies, the relationship between volunteering and well-being has been
identified (20-22). There is, however, the problem of causal order between well-being and
volunteering, which can only be addressed with a prospective study design (23). Good health and
adequate social resources tend to increase the probability of doing volunteer work (7, 23-26). It
has been argued that persons who are physically and mentally fit are not only able, but also
likely to be equipped with personal resources such as high self-esteem or control over life that
increase the likelihood of volunteering (1, 23, 26). It is equally plausible that health determines
volunteering than the other way around. Furthermore, the curvilinear relationship between
5
volunteering and well-being (27, 28) also showed that a moderate, but not high, number of hours
spent doing voluntary work enhanced late-life health and well-being.
Although a fairly large body of literature exists regarding the various benefits of volunteering for
those doing the voluntary work, earlier reviews (9, 20, 29, 30) have not investigated the benefits
of volunteering systematically with clearly defined inclusion and exclusion criteria. The aim of
this review was to conduct a systematized narrative literature review on quantitative data to
examine the association of formal voluntary work and personal well-being among older people
doing the voluntary work and those being served.
6
METHODS
Search strategy
We searched for relevant studies published in English through November 31, 2009, without a
start date, using the following databases: PubMed, the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled
Trials, Cochrane Database for Systematic Reviews, CINAHL (Cumulative Index to Nursing and
Allied Health Literature), ERIC (Education Resources Information Center), PsycINFO, Ovid
MEDLINE, ISI Web of Knowledge, and CSA (Sociological Abstracts). Depending on the
database, search terms included combinations of volunteering; “voluntary work”; aged; “older
people”; volunteerism; “benefits of volunteering”; “people being served”; “quality of life”;
intervention; trial. In addition, the reference lists of identified papers and reviews were searched
through for relevant articles. On the basis of the abstract, it was assessed whether the study had
the potential to be included. Based on the full article, it was decided whether the study met the
inclusion criteria.
Inclusion criteria
The following inclusion criteria were used: a) the study was published in a peer-reviewed journal
b) The paper was written in English and conducted in the Western countries c) participants were
at least 60 years of age, d) the study employed a longitudinal or experimental design, e) the study
methodology and outcomes were explicitly described and f) voluntary work was quantified as
visits or hours within a certain timeframe. The selection was made by the reviewer (M.v.B) and
in case of any uncertainty regarding the study inclusion the senior reviewer (T.R) was consulted.
7
RESULTS
2897 studies on older people volunteering were identified in the literature search of which 16
were accepted to this review (see Figure for review flow). The main reasons for exclusion were
that the study was cross-sectional, participants were under 60 years of age, volunteering was
defined to include also informal volunteering, or the study had not been published in a peer-
reviewed journal. The included studies investigated the associations of volunteering with the
well-being of those who did the voluntary work. 13 observational studies analyzed data from
population-based large prospective datasets and 3 experimental studies used data from one
randomized controlled trial. All studies were conducted in the U.S. Studies that used the same
large datasets were reviewed as separate studies when they investigated different outcomes.
Prospective studies included in the review
In the population-based prospective studies the number of participants ranged from 705 to 7.496,
except for one that included 71 women from the Baltimore Experience Corps Extended Pilot
Study matched with a comparison cohort of 150 women from the Women’s Health and Aging
Study (31), for description of included studies see Table 1. Age ranged between 60 to 97 years
(mean age between 70.1 to 77.0 years). Most of the participants (60-70%) were women and
predominantly white. The volunteers were more educated, had better perceived health and fewer
functional impairments than the non-volunteers (28). Most of the studies, however, did not
present baseline data according to the voluntary status, making it hard to compare the volunteers
to the non-volunteers for socio-demographics, health and functional status.
In the prospective datasets, voluntary work was mostly defined as the number of organizations
the person volunteered for and the total amount of hours dedicated for doing formal voluntary
work during the past 12 months. There was very little information on the nature and duration of
8
the voluntary work or the setting where volunteering took place. Among persons 65 years and
older, the volunteering rate in the past year ranged between 12-34.5% in the population-based
studies. The organizations for which participants had volunteered included, e.g., church or other
religious organization, school or educational organization, and political group or union. The
average number of organizations that the volunteers worked for was 1.7 (32). Number of hours
spent doing voluntary work ranged from 0 to 200 hours. Among those who volunteered, the
average number of hours spent volunteering was 72-73 during the previous year (28, 32). Some
studies (10, 27) reported the average hours spent volunteering among the entire study population,
which reflects the number of persons who did volunteer rather than the amount of volunteering
among those who actually volunteered.
The large datasets such as ACL and LSOA with several data collection waves during long
surveillance periods had relatively high attrition due to non-response, not being able to contact
the participants, and death. Even though in longitudinal studies missing data are often considered
being ubiquitous, it should be accounted for with current methods to avoid biased results (33). In
terms of missing data, it is important to consider whether it is missing at random or not at
random, where in the latter, systematic differences exist between the missing values and the
observed values even after the observed data are taken into account (34). Different imputation
methods, such as multiple imputation, recommended for dealing with data that are missing at
random, were used in some of the prospective studies on the ACL data (3, 10, 32) and the LSOA
data (35). Attrition in the studies drawing on data from the AHEAD study was stated to have
been small throughout the three data waves and weights were used to compensate for it (11, 36).
9
Randomized controlled trials included in the review
The Experience Corps implemented in Baltimore, Maryland (6) is so far the only randomized
controlled trial on volunteering among older people. The RCT was designed to generate social
benefits and simultaneously offer a community-based approach to health prevention. The 128
predominantly African American participants were randomized into an intervention group or a
control waiting list. The eligibility criteria for the trial were: 60 years or older, ability to read and
pass a criminal background check, ability to travel to the schools, a Mini-Mental State
Examination (37) score of 24 or above and/or meeting a threshold score on the Trail Making
Test (38). The intervention group participants served at least 15 hours per week in elementary
schools and assisted the children with e.g. reading achievement, library support, and class room
behavior during an academic year (39).
Longitudinal associations of volunteering and personal well-being
Outcomes were pooled together and collapsed into three categories that constituted personal
well-being: physical health, mental health, and psychosocial resources (23), see Table 2. Several
studies found a nonlinear relationship between volunteering and well-being (32), suggesting that
a certain amount of involvement is optimal but that a lower level of volunteering is beneficial for
well-being (27). Morrow-Howell et al. demonstrated that about 100 hours per year yielded the
greatest benefit of volunteering and that more hours per year did not increase the gains (32).
There were only a few studies that investigated volunteering with more than just the number of
hours committed to it. Building on ACL data, Morrow-Howell et al. reported that the number
and type of organization coordinating the voluntary work was not associated with the well-being
outcomes (32). However, in another study using the same data Musick & Wilson showed first
that the risk for death was lower for those who volunteered for one organization (27) and later
10
that religious volunteering and the longer duration of volunteering predicted better well-being in
older age (3).
Most prospective studies investigated the relationship between volunteering and physical health.
A series of studies found that volunteering predicted better self-rated health. Luoh et al. showed
with the AHEAD data that those who had volunteered 100 hours or more during the last year
rated their health better than those who had volunteered less than 100 hours (36). Two studies
(10, 11), drawing on data from AHEAD and ACL reported, on the other hand, no association
between volunteering and the number of physician diagnosed self-reported chronic diseases. In
all studies, volunteering at the age of 60 years or later predicted less difficulties or disability in
activities of daily living tasks than among the non-volunteers with follow-up ranging from 2 to 8
years (10, 11, 32, 36, 40). Tang used the ACL 1 through 3 data waves for showing that increased
hours spent volunteering at the age of 60 years or later predicted lower level of functional
dependency (10). Lum & Lightfoot found in the AHEAD data that volunteering was not
associated with living in a nursing home at the 7-year follow-up (11). Conversely, volunteering
at the age of 60 years or later predicted lower mortality in the prospective studies drawing on
data from the ACL, AHEAD, and LSOA studies (11, 27, 36, 41, 42).
Mental health and psychosocial resources are less studied outcomes of volunteering among
persons over 60 years of age, with the focus mainly on the association of volunteering and
depression (3, 11, 26, 32, 35, 40). Building on the three wave data from the ACL study, Li &
Ferrano found a beneficial effect of volunteering on depression during the eight year follow-up
(26). However, they further detected evidence of certain self-selection processes in that persons
with higher socioeconomic status and active church attendees had a lower incidence of
depression and were more actively engaged in volunteering (26). Van Willigen showed, drawing
11
on data from the ACL wave 1 and 2, that voluntary work increased life-satisfaction among
people over 60 years of age (28).
In a prospective study, Tan et al. compared the 71 women from the Experience Corps Pilot study
who did high-intensity voluntary work with 150 non-volunteering women controls from the
Women’s Health and Aging Study with a 3-year follow-up time. The women were aged 65-86
years and had comparable socio-economic status, self-rated health and frailty status. The study
showed that women who volunteered in the high-intensity senior service program reported
sustained increase in their physical activity level compared to the non-volunteering counterparts
during the 3-year follow-up (31).
Effects of volunteering on personal well-being
Three papers investigated in a randomized controlled setting the effect of high-intensity
volunteering on physical functioning (39), physical activity (39, 43) and cognition (39, 44). The
studies showed a positive trend in the effect of volunteering on cognitive and physical
functioning, although the power in the analysis was limited. The intervention increased the over
all physical activity level for the intervention group (39). Further, when measuring in kilocalories
per week, the short-term increase in physical activity was significant among the Experience
Corps volunteers compared to the controls (43).
Benefits of volunteering for those being served
The benefits of volunteering for those being served could not be reviewed, because only one
study that met the inclusion criteria was identified in the literature search. In that study, frail
home-bound older persons participated in the Strong for Life exercise program delivered by
volunteers who had been trained by physical therapists for the task. After the 4-month program,
12
the frail participants showed significant improvements on the Short Form-20 social functioning
scale, however there were no significant differences in physical functioning, mental health or
self-rated health (45). The study did not include a control group so the effect of the program
could not be evaluated in a controlled setting. The basic idea of the study was to investigate
whether an exercise program such as the Strong for Life program could be feasibly, efficiently
and safely delivered by specifically trained lay volunteers in voluntary organizations.
13
DISCUSSION
We identified and critically reviewed 16 studies investigating whether volunteering predicted
personal well-being among older people doing voluntary work. These studies used data from
three population-based prospective datasets and one randomized controlled trial. Volunteering in
old age predicted better self-rated health, functioning, physical activity and life satisfaction, as
well as decreased depression and mortality. However, volunteering did not decrease the risk of
chronic diseases or nursing home admission in old age.
Possible mechanisms for the effect of volunteering on well-being have not been extensively
discussed in the literature. The most commonly used explanation is the beneficial association of
social contacts that develop during volunteering or more general social activities. It has been
shown that social contacts and support have an effect on health and survival among older people
and volunteering fosters such connections (36, 42). Volunteering does involve a certain amount
of physical and mental effort and probably these beneficial physiological effects contribute to the
decreased risk of adverse health outcomes. Furthermore, volunteering is known to support
psychological well-being and sense of control, self-efficacy, which is positively related to health
(23). This might also in part explain the curvilinear beneficial effect of volunteering seen in
several studies (27, 36) in that reasonable amounts of volunteering are salubrious, but excessive
amounts are less likely to enhance health. Little empirical work has been done on specific
outcomes of volunteering (4, 32) as well as on the effect of different type of voluntary work on
health (27, 42). The findings on the association of the number of organizations one volunteers for
and subsequent well-being have been inconsistent (3, 27, 32). Thus, to be able to understand and
to investigate the mechanisms underlying the positive association of volunteering, more detailed
information on the nature of the voluntary work is needed.
14
The problem of causal order between well-being and volunteering is a challenging one.
Furthermore, we cannot completely control for the “healthy participant effect”. It is a fact that
persons who are in good health and possess adequate social and economic resources tend to
volunteer more (23, 26, 27). They are also likely to be equipped with personal resources such as
high self-esteem or control over life that increase the likelihood of volunteering (23, 26). Moen
et al. showed in their prospective study that women who volunteered in 1956 were more likely to
occupy multiple roles 30 years later and have better health outcomes than those who did not
volunteer earlier in their lives (46). That study differs from other studies that compare
volunteering and its benefits across different age groups, like e.g. van Willigen (28), in that the
period and cohort effect is controlled for. Volunteering presumably has a different meaning for
persons in different age groups born in different time periods and in addition, there are
underlying cultural norms and traditions that affect attitudes toward volunteering.
Future implications for practice
Volunteering is collectively seen as a feasible and humane way of helping people who cannot
cope on their own and as a potential field of productive activity for older retired fit persons who
still have a lot to offer. The basic element underlying voluntary work is that it is a social activity
which generates a considerable amount of social capital not only to the receiver, but also to the
giver, and consequently also to the society (9, 12). To date, persons who retire have better health
than ever before and they are expected to live longer without disabilities. Thus they have extra
time on their hands, and in some cases, the retired persons’ children and immediate family live
further away and they do not help their own family on a day-to-day basis. However, to what
extent can older people be expected to be a productive part of the society? The sheer increase in
leisure time after retirement cannot be assumed to result in more voluntary work being done
(12). It is potentially harmful for social cohesion to label those who do not, for some reason,
15
volunteer as unproductive in the society (47). Older age increases the risk for debilitating
illnesses and functional decline that hinders such activities even though the person would wish to
productively participate in the society.
Some people, who are near retirement age lead hectic lives and are often very committed to their
working careers. They are likely to transfer some of their energy and ambitious attitudes that
they have adopted during their paid working lives into post retirement volunteering, which will
probably result in changes in the current voluntary work policy. Volunteering could serve as a
good extension of the active working career by offering mental and physical challenges and
social contacts that will otherwise cease when the person is retired (36). However, it remains to
be seen whether the baby-boomers will engage more actively in voluntary work than the older
retired population today.
Even though volunteering is a charitable activity, we should bear in mind that it opens up the
doors of vulnerable older persons who do not necessarily understand the potential dangers and
thus might end up as targets for abuse (48). This is why it is crucial that voluntary work is
coordinated and monitored by responsible organizations that have unified standards for voluntary
work. The people who do ‘hands on’ voluntary work need to be properly trained for the task.
Volunteering should be evaluated rigorously and there should be clearly defined quality criteria
for its implementation.
Future implications for research
There are only a few large datasets in the world that have been used for studying volunteering
among older people. The majority of research data on the associations and effects of
volunteering on health and well-being come from the United States and might not apply to the
16
European countries. Further, the limited information available on the history of volunteer work
in earlier life as well as the frequency and duration of volunteering in the large datasets limits
more detailed analysis of the activity (3). Selection and social causation poses further challenges
to determining the effect of volunteer work on well-being (23). For example, it is challenging to
recruit people for volunteering studies who do not usually participate in any studies, e.g. those
who come from diverse socio-economic backgrounds and who have few social contacts, but who
potentially stand to benefit from social activities such as volunteering. Another potential threat to
the analyses is attrition which is considerable in a prospective large dataset such as ACL (32). In
longitudinal studies on older people, attrition tends to be missing not at random and those who
drop out differ in earlier follow-ups from those who responded. Attrition should be recognized´,
described and dealt with in a proper way in prospective analyses (26). The effect of volunteering
for those being served could be explored among persons who live in similar settings with similar
services and needs. For this purpose, randomized controlled trials are needed to determine the
effect of volunteering on older people. To investigate more deeply the effect of volunteering for
those doing the voluntary work, prospective observational studies preferably starting in young
life are needed. In addition, gender and social class effects should be addressed more specifically
as well as life time engagement in religious and social activities through social networks should
be investigated in relation to voluntary work.
Conclusion
Volunteering has been shown to be positively associated with well-being in old age among those
doing the voluntary work. However, it is not clear whether activities such as volunteering
contribute to the health of older people as opposed to healthy persons being more likely to be
engaged in volunteer activities. Further, the mechanisms underlying the association between
volunteering and well-being in old age have been poorly identified. To date, we know little about
17
the effects of volunteering for those receiving the help. Studies have predominantly utilized data
from large datasets with only limited information about volunteering which prohibits more
detailed analyses. Randomized controlled trials are needed for studying the effect of voluntary
work for those being served as well as to unmask the health participator-effect among the
volunteers.
18
Table 1 Characteristics of prospective studies included in the review
Reference Participants
Voluntary work Confounders Follow-up Outcomes Results
Harris &
Thoreson 2005
(LSOA)
7496,
15.4%
volunteered
70+ y.
Never/rarely/sometime/
frequently volunteers
during past 12 moths
Socio-demographics
Health
Physical activity
Social support
8 years Mortality Reduced mortality
Hong S-I et al.
2009 (LSOA)
5294,
21.6%
volunteered
70+ y.
Yes/no during last 12
months
Socio-demographics
Co-morbidities
Self-rated health
Functional limitation
Functional dependency
6 years Depression Decreased depression
Li & Ferrano
2005
(ACL)
1669
60+ y.
Volunteering for 0-5
organizations,
hrs/ past year
Socio-demographics
Church attendance
Functioning
Informal social
integration
8 years Depression Decreased depression
Li & Ferrano
2006 (ACL)
1669
60+ y.
875 45-59 y.
Volunteering for 0-5
organizations,
hrs/ past year
Socio-demographics
Church attendance
Functioning
Informal social
integration
8 years Functional limitations Decreased depression and
reduced pace of functional
limitations
Lum & Lightfoot
2005 (AHEAD)
7322, 13%
volunteered
70+ y.
0-99 or 100 hours or
more work for
charitable/religious
organization /past year
Socio-demographics
7 years Self-reported health
Medical conditions
Depression level
Functioning level
Nursing home entry
Mortality
Decreased depression, reduced
loss of functioning,
not associated with medical
conditions or nursing entry
Luoh & Herzog
2002 (AHEAD)
4860,
12%
volunteered
100 hrs
70+ y.
0-99 or 100 hours or
more work for
charitable/religious
organization /past year
Socio-demographics
Physical activity
Social contact
Cognitive functioning
Depressive symptoms
2 years Self-reported health
Functional status
Mortality
Volunteering 100 hrs lowered
the risk of ill health, daily
living limitations and mortality
Morrow-Howell
et al. 2003
(ACL)
1669,
34.5%
volunteered
60+ y
Volunteering for 0-5
organizations,
hrs/ past year
Socio-demographics
Informal social
integration
8 years Functional dependency
Self-rated health
Depression
Reduced functional dependency
and depression, positively
associated with self-rated
health
19
Musick et al.
1999 (ACL)
1211,
35%
volunteered
65+ y.
Volunteering for 0-5
organizations,
hrs/ past year
Socio-demographics
Health
Physical activity
Informal social
integration
7.5 years Mortality Volunteering decreased
mortality, curvilinear
association according to
number of hours volunteered
Musick &
Wilson (2003)
ACL
2348, 24-90
y., (for the
number of
65+ y. no
data
available)
Volunteering for 0-5
organizations,
Secular/religious work,
periods of volunteering
1-3
Socio-demographics
Social resources
Psychological resources
Health and functioning
Physical activity
Church attendance
8 years Depression Decreased depression
Sabin 1993
(LSOA)
7485,
15.6%
volunteered
70+ y.
Yes/no during last 12
months
Socio-demographics
Self-rated health
Functional dependency
4 years Mortality Reduced mortality
Tan et al. 2009
(EC, WHAS)
60+
EC=71
WHAS=150
African
American
women
15h/ week for an
academic year in an
elementary school
Socio-demographics
Health
Mobility disability
Frailty category
3 years Physical activity Volunteering increased the
level of physical activity
compared to the controls
Tan 2009
(ACL)
1669,
34.5%
volunteered
60+ y.
Volunteering for 0-5
organizations,
hrs/ past year
Socio-demographics
Informal social contact
8 years Self-rated health
Functional dependency
Chronic diseases
Volunteering associated with
improved self-rated health,
decreased functional
dependency, but not with
diseases
Van Willigen
2000 (ACL)
705,
39%
volunteered
60+ y.
Volunteering for 0-5
organizations,
hrs/ past year
Socio-demographics
Social integration
Social support
3 years Life satisfaction
Self-rated health
Volunteering was positively
associated with both outcomes
American’s Changing Lives (ACL), Asset and Health Dynamics Among the Oldest Old Study (AHEAD), Baltimore Experience Corps (EC), The Longitudinal Study on
Aging (LSOA), (WHAS) Women’s Health and Aging Studies
M=mean, SES=socioeconomic status, CES-D=Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression(49)
20
Table 2 Personal well-being framework
Personal well-being Measure Reference
I Physical health
Morbidity Physician diagnosed
chronic diseases
Lum & Lightfoot 2005
Tan 2009
Self-rated health Self-repot, 5 categories Van Willigen 2000
Luoh & Herzog 2002
Morrow-Howell et al. 2003
Lum & Lightfoot 2005
Tan 2009
Functional status ADL and IADL disability Luoh & Herzog 2002
Morrow-Howell et al. 2003
Fried et al. 2004
Lum & Lightfoot 2005
Li & Ferrano 2006
Tan 2009
Physical activity Minnesota leisure time
physical activity
questionnaire (MLTPAQ)
Self-reported physical
activity questions
Strength*, endurance*,
balance*, physical tasks*
Fried et al. 2004*
Tan et al. 2006
Tan et al. 2009
Nursing home
admission
Verified at follow-up data
collection
Lum & Lightfoot 2005
Mortality Date of death,
National Death Index†
Sabin 1993
Musick et al. 1999†
Luoh & Herzog 2002
Harris & Thoreson 2005†
Lum & Lightfoot 2005
II Mental health
Depression CES-D score
Self-reported single
question‡
Lum & Lightfoot 2005
Musick & Wilson 2003
Morrow-Howell et al. 2003
Li & Ferrano 2005
Li & Ferrano 2006
Hong et al. 2009‡
Memory
Executive functioning
Psychomotor speed
TMT, Rey-Osterrieth CFT
Word lists
Carlson et al. 2008
Cognitive activity Standardized questionnaire,
self-report
Fried et al. 2004
III Psychosocial resources
Life-satisfaction Self-reported, 5 categories Van Willigen 2000
ADL= Activities of Daily Living, IADL=Instrumental Activities of Daily Living, CES-D=Center for
Epidemiologic Studies Depression, TMT= Trail Making Test, Rey-Osterrieth CTF= Rey-Osterrieth Complex
Figure Test
21
Figure. Literature review flow diagram.
2897 Studies identified and screened for retrieval
66 Papers retrieved for detailed evaluation
2831 Studies excluded on the basis
of title or abstract (did not include
volunteering, under 60 years of age
at baseline, cross-sectional, reviews,
editorials, duplicates, full articles not
available)
50 Papers excluded after full article
screening (included informal
volunteering, participants under 60
years of age at baseline, cross-
sectional)
16 Papers included in the systematized review
22
REFERENCES
1. Wilson J, Musick MA. Who cares? Toward an integrated theory of volunteer work. Am
Sociol Rev. 1997;62:694-713.
2. Davis Smith J. Civic service in Western Europe. Nonprof Volunt Sec Q. 2004;33(4):64S-
78S.
3. Musick MA, Wilson J. Volunteering and depression: The role of psychological and social
resources in different age groups. Soc Sci Med. 2003 Jan;56(2):259-69.
4. Morrow-Howell N, Hong SI, Tang F. Who benefits from volunteering? Variations in
perceived benefits. Gerontologist. 2009 Feb;49(1):91-102.
5. Moen P, Fields V. Midcourse in the United States: Does unpaid community participation
replace paid work? Ageing International. 2002;27(3):21-48.
6. Glass TA, Freedman M, Carlson MC, Hill J, Frick KD, Ialongo N, McGill S, Rebok GW,
Seeman T, Tielsch JM, Wasik BA, Zeger S, Fried LP. Experience Corps: Design of an
intergenerational program to boost social capital and promote the health of an aging society. J
Urban Health. 2004 Mar;81(1):94-105.
7. Choi NG, Burr JA, Mutchler JE, Caro FG. Formal and informal volunteer activity and
spousal caregiving among older adults. Res Aging. 2007;29(2):99-124.
8. Butler RN. The study of productive aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002
Nov;57(6):S323.
9. Gottlieb BH, Gillespie AA. Volunteerism, health, and civic engagement among older adults.
Can J Aging. 2008 Winter;27(4):399-406.
10. Tang F. Late-life volunteering and trajectories of physical health. J Appl Gerontol.
2009;28(4):524-33.
11. Lum T, Lightfoot E. The effects of volunteering of the physical and mental health of older
people. Res Aging. 2005;27(1):31-55.
12. Rozario PA. Volunteering among current cohorts of older adults and bay boomers.
Generations. 2006;29(4):31-6.
13. United States Department of Labor. Volunteering in the United States, 2008. United States
Department of Labor;2009
14. Baldock CV. Seniors as volunteers: An international perspective on policy. Ageing Soc.
1999;19:581-602.
15. Esping-Andersen, G. The three worlds of welfare capitalism. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press; 1990.
23
16. Hank K, Erlinghagen M. Dynamics of volunteering in older Europeans. Gerontologist.
2009 Aug 7
17. Erlinghagen M, Hank K. The participation of older Europeans in volunteer work. Ageing
& Society. 2006;26:567-84.
18. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Voluntary Work, Australia 2006, catalogue no. 4441.0.
Canberra: ABS; 2007.
19. Lasby, D, McIver, D. Where Canadians volunteer: volunteering by type of organization.
Toronto, Ontario: Canadian Centre for Philanthropy; 2004.
20. Wheeler JA, Gorey KM, Greenblatt B. The beneficial effects of volunteering for older
volunteers and the people they serve: A meta-analysis. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 1998;47(1):69-
79.
21. Greenfield EA, Marks NF. Formal volunteering as a protective factor for older adults'
psychological well-being. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2004 Sep;59(5):S258-64.
22. Windsor TD, Anstey KJ, Rodgers B. Volunteering and psychological well-being among
young-old adults: How much is too much? Gerontologist. 2008 Feb;48(1):59-70.
23. Thoits P, Hewitt L. Volunteer work and well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior.
2001;42:115-31.
24. Warburton J, Le Brocque R, Rosenman L. Older people--the reserve army of volunteers?:
An analysis of volunteerism among older Australians. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 1998;46(3):229-
45.
25. Musick MA, Wilson J, Bynum WB. Race and formal volunteering: The differential effects
of class and religion. Social Forces. 2000;78(4):1539-71.
26. Li Y, Ferrano K. Volunteering and depression in later life: Social benefit or selection
processes? J Health Soc Behav. 2005;46:68-84.
27. Musick MA, Herzog AR, House JS. Volunteering and mortality among older adults:
Findings from a national sample. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1999 May;54(3):S173-80.
28. Van Willigen M. Differential benefits of volunteering across the life course. J Gerontol B
Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2000 Sep;55(5):S308-18.
29. Onyx J, Warburton J. Volunteering and health among older people: A review. Australas J
Ageing. 2003;22(2):65-9.
30. Corporation for National & Community Service, Office of Research and Policy
Development. The health benefits of volunteering: a review of recent research. Washington,
DC: Corporation for National & Community Service; 2007
31. Tan EJ, Rebok GW, Yu Q, Frangakis CE, Carlson MC, Wang T, Ricks M, Tanner EK,
McGill S, Fried LP. The long-term relationship between high-intensity volunteering and
24
physical activity in older African American women. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009
Mar;64(2):304-11.
32. Morrow-Howell N, Hinterlong J, Rozario PA, Tang F. Effects of volunteering on the well-
being of older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2003 May;58(3):S137-45.
33. Sterne JA, White IR, Carlin JB, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward MG, Wood AM,
Carpenter JR. Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research:
Potential and pitfalls. BMJ. 2009 Jun 29;338:b2393.
34. Graham JW. Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annu Rev Psychol.
2009;60:549-76.
35. Hong SI, Hasche L, Bowland S. Structural relationships between social activities and
longitudinal trajectories of depression among older adults. Gerontologist. 2009 Feb;49(1):1-
11.
36. Luoh M, Herzog R. Individual consequences of volunteer and paid work in old age:
Health and mortality. J Health Soc Behav. 2002;43:490-509.
37. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-Mental State". A practical method for
grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975 Nov;12(3):189-
98.
38. Reitan R. Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept
Motor Skill. 1958;8:271-6.
39. Fried LP, Carlson MC, Freedman M, Frick KD, Glass TA, Hill J, McGill S, Rebok GW,
Seeman T, Tielsch J, Wasik BA, Zeger S. A social model for health promotion for an aging
population: Initial evidence on the Experience Corps model. J Urban Health. 2004
Mar;81(1):64-78.
40. Li Y, Ferrano K. Volunteering in middle and later life: Is health a benefit, barrier or both?
Soc Forces. 2006;85(1):497-519.
41. Sabin E. Social relationships and mortality among the elderly. J Appl Gerontol.
1993;12(1):44-60.
42. Harris AH, Thoresen CE. Volunteering is associated with delayed mortality in older
people: Analysis of the longitudinal study of aging. J Health Psychol. 2005 Nov;10(6):739-52.
43. Tan EJ, Xue QL, Li T, Carlson MC, Fried LP. Volunteering: A physical activity
intervention for older adults--the experience corps program in Baltimore. J Urban Health.
2006 Sep;83(5):954-69.
44. Carlson MC, Saczynski JS, Rebok GW, Seeman T, Glass TA, McGill S, Tielsch J, Frick
KD, Hill J, Fried LP. Exploring the effects of an "everyday" activity program on executive
function and memory in older adults: Experience Corps. Gerontologist. 2008 Dec;48(6):793-
801.
25
45. Etkin CD, Prohaska TR, Harris BA, Latham N, Jette A. Feasibility of implementing the
strong for life program in community settings. Gerontologist. 2006 Apr;46(2):284-92.
46. Moen P, Dempster-McClain D, Williams M. Successful aging: A life-course perspective
on women's multiple roles and health. Am J Sociol. 1992;97(6):1612-38.
47. Hank K, Erlinghagen M. Volunteering in "old" Europe. J Appl Gerontol. 2010;39(1):3-20.
48. Cooper C, Selwood A, Livingston G. The prevalence of elder abuse and neglect: A
systematic review. Age Ageing. 2008 Mar;37(2):151-60.
49. Radloff L. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general
population. Appl Psych Meas. 1977;1:385-401.