ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Research suggests that visual impressions of natural compared with urban environments facilitate recovery after psychological stress. To test whether auditory stimulation has similar effects, 40 subjects were exposed to sounds from nature or noisy environments after a stressful mental arithmetic task. Skin conductance level (SCL) was used to index sympathetic activation, and high frequency heart rate variability (HF HRV) was used to index parasympathetic activation. Although HF HRV showed no effects, SCL recovery tended to be faster during natural sound than noisy environments. These results suggest that nature sounds facilitate recovery from sympathetic activation after a psychological stressor.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7, 1036-1046; doi:10.3390/ijerph7031036
International Journal of
Environmental Research and
Public Health
ISSN 1660-4601
www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph
Article
Stress Recovery during Exposure to Nature Sound and
Environmental Noise
Jesper J. Alvarsson *, Stefan Wiens and Mats E. Nilsson
Gösta Ekman Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, SE-106 91 Stockholm,
Sweden; E-Mails: sws@psychology.su.se (S.W.); mats.nilsson@psychology.su.se (M.E.N.)
* Author to whom correspondence should be addressed; E-Mail: jesper.alvarsson@psychology.su.se;
Tel.: +46-8-158-659; Fax: +46- 8-165-522.
Received: 26 January 2010; in revised form: 20 February 2010 / Accepted: 5 March 2010 /
Published: 11 March 2010
Abstract: Research suggests that visual impressions of natural compared with urban
environments facilitate recovery after psychological stress. To test whether auditory
stimulation has similar effects, 40 subjects were exposed to sounds from nature or noisy
environments after a stressful mental arithmetic task. Skin conductance level (SCL) was
used to index sympathetic activation, and high frequency heart rate variability (HF HRV)
was used to index parasympathetic activation. Although HF HRV showed no effects, SCL
recovery tended to be faster during natural sound than noisy environments. These results
suggest that nature sounds facilitate recovery from sympathetic activation after a
psychological stressor.
Keywords: soundscape; nature sounds; environmental noise; skin conductance level; heart
rate variability; stress recovery
Abbreviations: HF HRV = High frequency heart rate variability; SCL = Skin conductance
level
OPEN ACCESS
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7
1037
1. Introduction
In 1984, Ulrich demonstrated that patients whose windows faced a park recovered faster compared
with patients whose windows faced a brick wall [1]. Since then, several studies have demonstrated
restorative effects of natural compared with urban environments; these effects include increased
well-being, decreased negative affect and decreased physiological stress responses [2-7]. Ulrich [6]
suggested that natural environments have restorative effects by inducing positive emotional states,
decreased physiological activity and sustained attention. This agrees with Kaplan and Kaplan’s theory
that nature environments facilitate recovery of directed attention capacity and thereby reducing mental
fatigue [8], and with results showing that positive emotions improves physiological recovery after
stress [9].
Previous research in this area has mainly used visual stimuli, for example videos and photographs
of nature settings and urban areas [1,5,10]. However, sound stimulation is also known to be a potent
stressor, evoking unpleasant feelings (annoyance) and physiological stress reactions, especially at high
sound pressure levels [11,12]. Studies on the connection between sound environment and stress
recovery are currently lacking. Soundscape research has shown that natural sounds are typically
perceived as pleasant and technological noise as unpleasant components of the sound
environment [13-15]. It is therefore plausible that the sound environment may have a similar effect on
stress recovery as the visual environment.
Ulrich et al. [6] used video films with sound and found faster physiological stress recovery during
exposure to films depicting nature compared with urban environments. However, Ulrich et al. did not
control for sound pressure level. Indeed, the soundtrack to their films of urban environmental settings
had considerably higher sound pressure levels than the soundtrack to the films of nature environments.
This makes it difficult to determine whether the effect was related to the characteristics of the
environments or to differences in sound pressure levels. So, although positive effects of visual natural
environments are well established, no research has been done using only auditory stimulation with
controlled stimuli and sound pressure levels.
The autonomic nervous system controls various body functions: the sympathetic branch primarily
controls activation and mobilization, and the parasympathetic branch controls restoration and
relaxation [16]. Sympathetic activity can be indexed by skin conductance level (SCL) [17,18], and
parasympathetic activity can be indexed by the high frequency part of the power spectral density of
heart rate variability (HF HRV) [19,20].
Psychological stress can be elicited by factors such as failure to achieve and marital problems,
psychological stress also often has physiological consequences [21]. In the laboratory, psychological
stress is commonly induced by mental arithmetic and speeded Stroop tasks [22,23].
The purpose of the present study was to induce psychological stress and compare effects of different
sound conditions on the rate of physiological recovery. The sound conditions were chosen so that a
pleasant natural sound environment was compared with three less pleasant urban sound environments
dominated by noise. To study effects of sound pressure level on physiological recovery, the urban
sound conditions had higher, equal, or lower average sound pressure levels than the nature sound. Two
measures of physiological stress were used: SCL as an index of sympathetic activity and HF HRV as
an index of parasympathetic activity. Physiological recovery is associated with a decrease in
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7
1038
sympathetic activation (i.e., SCL decreases) and an increase in parasympathetic activation (i.e., HF
HRV increases). Because physiological stress recovery should be faster during exposure to pleasant
than to unpleasant sounds, we hypothesized that (a) SCL should decrease faster and (b) HF HRV
increase faster during pleasant nature sound than during less pleasant noise.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
Forty university students participated in the experiment (24 women and 18 men, mean age = 27
years). All participants had hearing thresholds lower than 25 dB in their best ear, for all tested
frequencies (0.125, 0.5, 1, 2, 3, 4, and 6 kHz, Interacoustics Diagnostic Audiometer, model AD226).
The listeners received course credit for their participation. Electrocardiogram data were missing from
three participants (1 man, 2 women) due to electrode failure.
2.2. Experimental Design
The experiment consisted of three different parts: (1) One 5-min quiet baseline period, (2) five
2-min periods of testing (“stressor”), and (3) four 4-min periods of relaxation (“recovery”) during
exposure to one of four experimental sounds. Figure 1 illustrates the experiment schematically. Total
time for the experiment was approximately 35 minutes.
Figure 1. Experimental design with experiment duration on x-axis and expected stress
level on y-axis (S = stress test; R = recovery period for each experimental sound condition).
A 4 × 4 mixed design was used, with sound during relaxation as within subject variable and
presentation order of the four sounds as between subject variable (the sounds are described in detail
below). The participants were randomly assigned to one of four orders of experimental sounds, using a
Latin square matrix.
2.3. Stressor
The stressor was a two minute speeded mental arithmetic task (henceforth “the stress test”). The
task was to decide, within 3 s, whether a displayed equation was correct or false. The participants
answered by pressing one of two keys on a numeric keyboard. Their responses were evaluated as either
“correct”, “false” or “too late” (if later than 3 s). Feedback was presented on the screen (correct, false
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7
1039
or too late) and through earphones with a specific sound for each type of feedback. The equations
consisted of simple arithmetic operations, such as ‘543345 = 193’. The first two terms were integers
between 2 and 999, and the result of the equation was a positive integer below 1,000 which either was
correct or false (correct answer +/ 1–3). The operator could either be addition, subtraction, division or
multiplication. Each operator had 250 equations in a database, half correct and half false. Overall
performance (percent correct) was continuously updated and displayed to the participants in the upper
left corner of the screen.
2.4. Experimental Sounds
During each of the four recovery periods, participants were exposed to either a nature sound or a
noise. The sound pressure levels of the noises were higher, equal or lower than the sound pressure
level of the nature sound. The experimental sounds were selected from a large database of binaural
recordings of environmental sounds. The nature sound was chosen to be more pleasant than the three
noises, including the ambient noise of lower sound pressure level. The four experimental sounds are
described below.
(1) Nature sound. A mixture of sounds from a fountain and tweeting birds. The average sound
pressure level was set to 50 dB (LAeq,4min).
(2) High noise. Road traffic noise recorded close to a densely trafficked road. The average
sound pressure level was set to 80 dB (LAeq,4min).
(3) Low noise. The same noise as (2), but set to a lower average sound pressure level,
50 dB (LAeq,4min).
(4) Ambient noise. A recording from a quiet backyard, with a constant low level ambient noise,
mainly caused by ventilation systems of the buildings surrounding the yard. The average
sound pressure level was set to 40 dB (LAeq,4min).
2.5. Physiological Measures
For SCL measurement two electrodes were fitted by the experiment leader to the hypothenar
eminence of the non dominant hand. The SCL was measured as the conductance between the
two electrodes.
HRV measurements were derived from the electrocardiogram (ECG). Three electrodes were applied
by the participant themselves under supervision of the experimenter. The first electrode was positioned
five centimeters to the right of the middle of the upper sternum and the other two on the left and right
side of the stomach, just below the ribcage. HRV was calculated according to the procedure described
by Berntson and by Hejjel & Kelleny [24,25].
2.6. Procedure
Participants were informed that the goal of the experiment was to study physiological reactions
during a stressful task and that there would be sound presentations during the four minute pauses. The
participants were tested individually. They were first asked to wash their hands and were then seated in
a soundproof room, where they were given a written description of the experiment. After the
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7
1040
participants had given their consent to participate, electrodes were fitted to their bodies. They were
then asked to put on a pair of headphones and one trial version of the stress test was completed in order
to check the equipment.
During the baseline period, the participants were asked to relax in silence. At the end of the period a
prerecorded female voice reminded them that the first stress test was about to begin. After the stress
test, the female voice instructed the participants to relax and one of the four experimental sounds was
presented. This switch between stress test and recovery was repeated three more times (see Figure 1).
After the experiment, participants listened to the four experimental sounds one more time and
assessed the sound’s pleasantness, eventfulness, and familiarity on three bipolar category scales. These
attributes have been suggested as basic perceptual dimensions of sound environments [26]. Finally, the
participants’ thresholds of hearing were tested. Participants were informed only after the experiment
about the true purpose of the study (i.e., our interest in the sounds). The study was conducted in
accordance with regional ethical guidelines.
2.7. Equipment
The sounds were recorded with a binaural head and torso simulator Brüel & Kjær type 4100, with
two microphones type 4190 and two pre-amplifiers type 2669, one conditioning amplifier NEXUS
Brüel & Kjær type 2690 A 0S4 and a calibrator Brüel & Kjær type 4231 plus adapter model 0887. A
portable computer Dolch NPAC-Plus P111 with a 6-channel LynxTwo sound card stored the
recordings with 24 bit resolution and 48 kHz sampling frequency using Sound Forge 7. Editing and
mixing was performed in the same program. In the soundproof room, the signal was fed into a digital
filter and D/A-converter, Rane RPM 26z, and was then presented through Sennheiser HD 600
headphones. The whole listening system was calibrated using a pink-noise signal, measured at the
point of the listener’s ear. The frequency response of the whole listening system was flat within 2 dB,
1/3-octave-band levels, 2516,000 Hz.
The physiological data were recorded with a Biopac System MP100 at 1000 Hz. SCL was measured
with a Biopac GSR100C amplifier and EDA isotonic gel electrodes and ECG was measured with a
Biopac ECG100C amplifier and Red DotTM Ag/AgCl solid gel electrodes.
Both programming and presentation of the mental arithmetic stress task was conducted in
Matlab 6.5. The physiological data were analyzed in Matlab, while the HRV power density spectrum
(PDS) was computed in ACQ Knowledge 3.91. Statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS 16.
3. Results
3.1. Perceptual Assessment of Experimental Sounds
The perceptual assessment of the sounds showed that the nature sound was perceived as more
pleasant than the noises (Figure 2). This confirms that the selection of sounds was successful, as the
goal was to find a nature sound that was more pleasant than any of the noises. The low noise and the
ambient noise were similar in perceived pleasantness whereas the high noise sound was rated as the
least pleasant sound. The perceptual evaluation also showed that the high noise was perceived as more
eventful than the other sounds. The ambient noise was the least eventful and also the least familiar
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7
1041
sound, probably because it contained no distinct sound sources and therefore was perceived as an
undifferentiated background noise.
Figure 2. Mean values of perceptual attributes for the nature sound and the high, low and
ambient noises. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.
3.2. Physiological Measures
Skin conductance level (SCL): The mean of successive 10 second periods were computed for the
recovery periods after each stress test. The mean of seconds 150–270 of the baseline period was used
as the baseline measure. Figure 3 shows baseline corrected SCL values over time, and Table 1 shows
descriptive statistics of these data.
Figure 3 suggests that although SCL immediately after the stressor was similar for the different
conditions, recovery was faster during exposure to the nature sound than to the three noise conditions.
The ambient and low noise had the second fastest, and high noise the slowest recovery. A slight
upswing during the last 50 seconds of the recovery period was seen for SCL recovery during the high
noise, possibly reflecting an increased arousal due to prolonged exposure to the unpleasant noise. In a
4 × 4 mixed ANCOVA, the mean SCL for each participant during the recovery period was used as the
dependent variable, sound as a within-subjects variable, and presentation order as a between-subjects
variable. The baseline measure was included in the analysis as covariate [27].
Figure 3. Baseline corrected skin conductance level (SCL) as a function of time, shown
separately for recovery during exposure to nature, high noise, low noise and ambient sound.
050 100 150 200 250
0.15
0.2
0.25
0.3
0.35
0.4
0.45
0.5
0.55
Time (s)
Skin conductance level (SCL)
Deviation from baseline (
S)
Nature sound
High noise
Low noise
Amb ie nt no is e
Nature sound
High noise
Low noise
Ambient noise
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7
1042
Table 1. The mean, median, max, min and standard deviation (SD) of participants
(n = 40) for SCL and HF HRV, baseline measures were computed from second 150–270,
recovery measures were computed over the whole recovery period.
Statistic (N = 40)
Period Sound Mean Median Max Min SD
Skin conductance level (SCL), µS
Baseline Silence 0.55 0.53 2.01 0.09 0.39
Recovery Nature 0.82 0.84 1.84 0.25 0.36
High Noise 0.87 0.90 1.49 0.25 0.35
Low noise 0.84 0.86 1.69 0.17 0.39
Ambient 0.85 0.84 1.80 0.28 0.38
High frequency heart rate variability (HF HRV), ms2/Hz
Baseline Silence 0.60 0.55 1.08 0.29 0.21
Recovery Nature 0.59 0.58 0.91 0.27 0.18
High Noise 0.60 0.57 1.01 0.31 0.20
Low noise 0.60 0.53 1.07 0.25 0.21
Ambient 0.61 0.57 1.04 0.32 0.19
The ANCOVA showed an interaction between presentation order and sound (F9,105 = 6.851,
p = 0.001). This effect reflected general SCL-increase during the experiment. The main effect of sound
was significant (F3,105 = 2.731, p = 0.048). Pairwise comparisons (t-tests) showed that mean SCL was
lower for Nature than High noise (p = 0.045); however, the differences between Nature and the other
two noise conditions did not reach significance (p > 0.05).
In an ANCOVA of sound (4) × time (24, the successive 10 second periods) with baseline as
covariate, the interaction between sound and time was significant (F69,2622 = 1.34, p = 0.034). This
finding suggests that recovery time for SCL differed among the sound conditions. As Figure 3 suggests
that polynomial trends (e.g., linear, quadratic) in an ANCOVA would not describe the recovery
functions well, a non-linear regression analysis was performed to obtain point estimates of recovery
time. To that end, we fitted an exponential function (Equation 1) to the mean SCL data shown in
Figure 3:
3
12
bx
y
bbe , (1)
where y is baseline corrected SCL, x is time (in seconds) and b1, b2 and b3 are constants. Figure 4
shows the fitted functions for the four experimental sounds. The fit, R2, for the nature sound, low noise
and ambient noise was > 0.99, it was slightly lower for the high noise, R2 = 0.96. RMS-error for the
nature, high noise, ambient and low noise sound was 0.0088, 0.017, 0.0090 and 0.0097 µS,
respectively. The half life recovery was calculated using Equation 1, by solving for x at the point
where SCL had been reduced by half, compared with its value at x = 0 (see dotted line in Figure 4).
The high noise had the longest half life of 159.8 s, the half life of the other three were 121.3 s for
ambient noise, low noise 111.4 s and nature sound 101.3 s. Reliable statistical testing of individual half
life values was not possible, since the estimated constants in several cases generated complex numbers,
that resulted in missing data when half life values were calculated.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7
1043
Figure 4. Skin conductance level (SCL) as a function of time, shown separately for the
four sounds. Curves were fitted to the group data. Constants of Equation 1 and half life
value (x) are indicated in each diagram.
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
Nature sound
Skin conductance level (SCL)
Deviations from baseline (
S)
0.1626
0.3428
-0.1387
101.3
b1 =
b2 =
b3 =
x =
Half-life
High noise
0.2054
0.3071
-0.1185
159.8
b1 =
b2 =
b3 =
x =
Half -lif e
050 100 150 200 250
Ambien t n o ise
Time (s)
0.1607
0.3551
-0.1111
121.3
b1 =
b2 =
b3 =
x =
Half -lif e
050 100 150 200 250
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
Low noise
0.1822
0.3366
-0.1394
111.4
b1 =
b2 =
b3 =
x =
Half -lif e
Heart rate variability (HRV): We found no consistent effect of type of sound on HF HRV. Average
HRV values were not higher for nature sound than for the other sounds, and HF HRV for the high
noise was not substantially lower than for the other sounds. The HF HRV data for each participant and
recovery condition were tested in a 4x4 ANCOVA, with type of sound as within subject variable and
presentation order as between-subject variable. The baseline measure was included in the analysis as
covariate [27]. Neither type of sound, presentation order nor their interaction was statistically
significant (p > 0.05).
4. Discussion
The main purpose of this study was to test whether physiological stress recovery is faster during
exposure to pleasant nature sounds than to noise. Figure 3 suggests that mean SCL during the nature
sound was lower than for the noises. Although this difference was statistically significant only between
the nature sound and the high noise, detailed analyses of the recovery functions showed that half-life
SCL recovery was 937% faster during the nature sound than during the noises. These results suggest
a faster recovery of the sympathetic nervous system [16,17] during the nature sound. Because HF HRV
showed no effects of experimental sounds, this null finding suggests that the parasympathetic
activation may be less affected by sound during recovery.
The present results suggest that recovery from sympathetic arousal is affected by type of sound
(nature sound versus noise). Recovery was faster during the nature sound (50 dBA) compared with the
noises, including the low noise (50 dBA) and the ambient noise (40 dBA). The mechanisms behind the
faster recovery could be related to positive emotions (pleasantness), evoked by the nature sound as
suggested by previous research using non audio film stimuli [9]. Other perceptual attributes may also
influence recovery. The Ambient noise was perceived as less familiar than the other sounds (Figure 2),
presumably because it contained no identifiable sources. One may speculate that this lack of
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7
1044
information might have caused an increased mental activity and thereby an increased SCL, compared
with the nature sound (cf. [28]). An effect of sound pressure level can be seen in the difference
between high and low noise, this difference is in line with previous psychoacoustic research [12] and is
not a surprising considering the large difference (30 dBA) in sound pressure level.
The results from SCL are consistent with those of Ulrich et al. [6], who found a faster decrease in
SCL after audio-visual exposure to natural compared with urban environments. Ulrich et al. found a
similar effect on heart period. This disagrees with our HF HRV results. An explanation might be that
heart period is influenced by both sympathetic and parasympathetic activity [6], whereas our measure,
the HF part of the HRV, only is related to parasympathetic activity [17,19], the effects found on heart
period reflects the influence of the sympathetic rather than the parasympathetic system. The lack of
effects on HF HRV in our study suggests that sound exposure during recovery may not have a strong
influence on parasympathetic activity, at least not with the exposures and the physiologic indexes used
in the current experiment.
Our study used a small set of environmental sounds of short duration, which limits the
generalizability of the results. The nature sound used in the experiment had a relatively high sound
pressure level (50 dBA). The effect of natural sound environments on stress recovery may be greater in
situations with longer exposure times and with lower sound pressure levels commonly found in
recreational and rural areas outside cities. In city parks and other urban outdoor areas, the sound
environment is typically a mix of sound from nature sounds and traffic noise. Based on the present
results, it seems plausible to speculate that recovery from sympathetic activation in such areas would
be less effective than in areas undisturbed by noise.
5. Conclusions
The present results suggest that after psychological stress, physiological recovery of sympathetic
activation is faster during exposure to pleasant nature sounds than to less pleasant noise of lower,
similar, or higher sound pressure level.
Acknowledgements
This research was conducted in the research project Sustaining Acoustic Pleasantness within Rural
and Community Development (SARCADE), funded by the Swedish Research Council FORMAS.
References
1. Ulrich, R.S. View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science 1984,
224, 420-421.
2. Grinde, B.; Patil, G.G. Biophilia: does visual contact with nature impact on health and well-being?
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2009, 6, 2332-2343.
3. Hartig, T.; Kaiser, F.G.; Bowler, P.A. Psychological restoration in nature as a positive motivation
for ecological behavior. Environ. Behav. 2001, 33, 590-607.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7
1045
4. Maller, C.; Townsend, M.; Pryor, A.; Brown, P.; St Leger, L. Healthy nature healthy people:
'contact with nature' as an upstream health promotion intervention for populations. Health Promot.
Int. 2006, 21, 45-54.
5. Parsons, R.; Tassinary, L.G.; Ulrich, R.S.; Hebl, M.R.; Grossman-Alexander, M. The view from
the road: Implications for stress recovery and immunization. J. Environ. Psychol. 1998,
18, 113-140.
6. Ulrich, R.S.; Simons, R.F.; Losito, B.D.; Fiorito, E.; Miles, M.A.; Zelson, M. Stress recovery
during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 201-230.
7. van den Berg, A.E.; Hartig, T.; Staats, H. Preference for nature in urbanized societies: Stress,
restoration, and the pursuit of sustainability. J. Soc. Issues 2007, 63, 79-96.
8. Kaplan, S. The restorative benefits of nature—toward an integrative framework. J. Environ.
Psychol. 1995, 15, 169-182.
9. Fredrickson, B.L.; Mancuso, R.A.; Branigan, C.; Tugade, M.M. The undoing effect of positive
emotions. Motiv. Emotion 2000, 24, 237-258.
10. Laumann, K.; Garling, T.; Stormark, K.M. Selective attention and heart rate responses to natural
and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003, 23, 125-134.
11. Ising, H.; Kruppa, B. Health effects caused by noise: evidence in the literature from the past 25
years. Noise Health 2004, 6, 5-13.
12. Lusk, S.L.; Gillespie, B.; Hagerty, B.M.; Ziemba, R.A. Acute effects of noise on blood pressure
and heart rate. Arch. Environ. Health 2004, 59, 392-399.
13. Brown, A.L.; Muhar, A. An approach to the acoustic design of outdoor space. J. Environ.
Planning Manage. 2004, 47, 827-842.
14. Lavandier, C.; Defréville, B. The contribution of sound source characteristics in the assessment of
urban soundscapes. Acta Acustica united with Acustica 2006, 92, 912-921.
15. Nilsson, M.E.; Berglund, B. Soundscape quality in suburban green areas and city parks.
Acta Acustica United with Acustica 2006, 92, 903-911.
16. Andreassi, J.L. Psychophysiology: Human Behavior and Physiological Response; 5th ed.;
L. Erlbaum, Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2007; p. 10.
17. Blascovich, J.K.; Robert, M. Using electrodermal and cardiovascular measures of arousal in social
psychological research. In Research Methods in Personality and Social Psychology. Review of
Personality and Social Psychology; Hendrick, C., Clark, Margaret S, Eds.; Sage Publications, Inc:
Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 1990; Volume 11, pp. 45-73.
18. Cummings, E.M.; El-Sheikh, M.; Kouros, C.D.; Keller, P.S. Children's skin conductance
reactivity as a mechanism of risk in the context of parental depressive symptoms. J. Child
Psychol. Psychiatry 2007, 48, 436-445.
19. Berntson, G.G.; Bigger, J.T., Jr.; Eckberg, D.L.; Grossman, P.; Kaufmann, P.G.; Malik, M.;
Nagaraja, H.N.; Porges, S.W.; Saul, J.P.; Stone, P.H.; van der Molen, M.W. Heart rate variability:
origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology 1997, 34, 623-648.
20. Pyetan, E.; Toledo, E.; Zoran, O.; Akselrod, S. Parametric description of cardiac vagal control.
Auton. Neurosci. 2003, 109, 42-52.
21. Lovallo, W.R. Stress and Health: Biological and Psychological Interactions; 2nd ed.; Sage
Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2004.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7
1046
22. Dickerson, S.S.; Kemeny, M.E. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration
and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol. Bull. 2004, 130, 355-391.
23. Renaud, P.; Blondin, J.P. The stress of Stroop performance: physiological and emotional
responses to color-word interference, task pacing, and pacing speed. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1997,
27, 87-97.
24. Berntson, G.G.; Stowell, J.R. ECG artifacts and heart period variability: Don't miss a beat!
Psychophysiology 1998, 35, 127-132.
25. Hejjel, L.; Kellenyi, L. The corner frequencies of the ECG amplifier for heart rate variability
analysis. Physiol. Meas. 2005, 26, 39-47.
26. Axelsson, Ö. Individual differences in preferences to Photographs. Psychol. Aesthetics
Creati. Arts 2007, 1, 61-72.
27. Rogers, W.T.; Hopkins, K.D. Power estimates in the presence of a covariate and measurement
error. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1988, 48, 647-656.
28. Adank, P.; Evans, B.G.; Stuart-Smith, J.; Scott, S.K. Comprehension of familiar and unfamiliar
native accents under adverse listening conditions. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2009,
35, 520-529.
© 2010 by the authors; licensee Molecular Diversity Preservation International, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open-access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative
Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
... White noise has been used for a long time to decrease patient anxiety, employing static-like sounds for privacy and sleep (10). Green noise, a variation of white noise utilizing natural sounds at a low frequency, such as waves, flowing rivers, and waterfalls, has also shown promise in reducing heart rate and blood pressure during dental procedures (10,11). ...
... White noise has been used for a long time to decrease patient anxiety, employing static-like sounds for privacy and sleep (10). Green noise, a variation of white noise utilizing natural sounds at a low frequency, such as waves, flowing rivers, and waterfalls, has also shown promise in reducing heart rate and blood pressure during dental procedures (10,11). ...
... To obtain the total score for the scale, the scores for each of the 4-item responses were summed, with a total range from 0 to 20. Based on the results of Corah's DAS, the patients were divided into four groups: no anxiety (score < 4), mild (4-8), moderate (9)(10)(11)(12), and severe (>12). Patients with a moderate level of anxiety were chosen for the study. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Anxiety and fear of pain significantly hinder the pursuit of root canal treatments, leading up to 15% of adults to avoid dental care. Since pharmacologic sedation involves risks and typically requires specialist administration, music therapy has demonstrated potential in reducing stress through neurobiological pathways. White noise, known for its static-like sounds, effectively decreases anxiety, while green noise, composed of natural sounds, has shown promise in reducing heart rate and blood pressure during dental procedures. However, their efficacy has never been compared in endodontics. Aim & Objective: This study aims to compare the effectiveness of Green noise (528 Hz) and White noise (432 Hz) in reducing anxiety and pain during root canal treatment, contributing to evidence-based anxiety management strategies in endodontic practice. Materials and Methods: Following ethical approval and clinical trial registration, a double-blind study was conducted with 54 patients aged 18-60 with symptomatic irreversible pulpitis in single-rooted teeth. Patients were randomly assigned to 3 groups i.e control (no music), white noise (432 Hz), or green noise (528 Hz) groups. Anxiety levels were assessed using the Corah Dental Anxiety Scale before treatment. Vital signs such as heart rate (HR), systolic blood pressure (SBP), and diastolic blood pressure (DBP) were recorded pre- and post-treatment. Results: ANOVA and paired t-tests revealed that post-operatively, heart rate (HR) decreased across all groups, except for the white noise group, which experienced an increase. Systolic SBP and DBP rose in the control and white noise groups but decreased in the green noise group to 122.45±5.12 (from 138.09±10.41) and to 76.81±5.41 (from 94.22±9.67), respectively. Pre-operatively, HR differed significantly between the control and both noise groups (p<0.001). Post-operatively, HR differences were notable between white noise and green noise (p=0.011) and between white noise and control (p=0.011). The green noise group showed significant changes in HR, SBP, and DBP pre- and post-op (p<0.001). Conclusion: Green noise (528 Hz) effectively reduces anxiety and modulates physiological responses during root canal treatment, offering a non-invasive alternative to pharmacologic sedation.
... Two promising avenues for acute anxiety reduction are exposure to naturalistic stimuli and guided breathwork. Nature exposure and related therapies have been shown to facilitate stress recovery, mood enhancement, improved cognitive functions, and overall well-being while alleviating anxiety symptoms and boosting health and self-efficacy [13][14][15][16][17][18][19][20][21]. Breathwork techniques demonstrate comparable efficacy through distinct mechanisms, inducing parasympathetic states that promote stress reduction and enhance emotional regulation [22][23][24][25][26][27][28]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The MindGym, a novel immersive technology utilizing a reflective chamber environment, was developed to create standardized experiential content, including anxiolytic experiences. This study examined whether therapeutic experiences originally created in the MindGym could maintain their efficacy when delivered via 360-degree recordings through virtual reality (VR) headsets. A randomized controlled trial (N=126) compared anxiety reduction, cognitive performance, and physiological responses across four conditions: MindGym and VR platforms, each delivering either breathwork or rain stimuli. Results demonstrated significant improvements across all conditions in cognitive performance (Trail Making Test RTACC, p.fdr<.001; Architex Total Speed, p.fdr<.001) and anxiety reduction (STAI, p.fdr<.001). Breathwork conditions produced greater decreases in breath rate compared to rain stimuli (p.fdr=.002). Treatment responses were moderated by individual differences, with absorption (MODTAS) predicting both awe (p.fdr=.004) and ego dissolution (p.fdr=.015), while openness to experience interacted with stimuli type to influence anxiety reduction (p.fdr=.038). The anxiolytic effects originally generated in the MindGym maintained full efficacy when translated to VR delivery, with no significant differences in effectiveness or immersion between the original environment and its virtual reproduction. These findings establish the MindGym as a viable content creation platform for immersive, anxiety-reducing experiences that can be successfully adapted to more accessible delivery systems, while highlighting the potential for personalization based on individual differences. Future research should investigate the translation of more complex MindGym-generated experiences to expand accessible anxiety management tools.
... Step 2: Prior to exposure to a virtual or real restorative setting, participants completed a five-minute arithmetic task on a desktop computer designed to induce stress [29]. Simple arithmetic equations were displayed for 3 s each, and participants decided whether the equation was correct or not by key presses [75], and immediate feedback was provided. Additionally, a loud, fast-tempo song (90-100 dB, 127 bpm) was played on a computer during the task to enhance stress induction [76]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Biophilic design, incorporating natural elements, is known to enhance stress recovery. Immersive virtual environments (IVEs) are increasingly utilized for biophilic design validation and research, but their efficacy in replicating the restorative effects of real environments remains uncertain. Varying responses to virtual settings suggest that specific design factors may affect their effectiveness. This study provides a novel comparison of the stress recovery and restoration effect between real and virtual green walls in a controlled climate chamber. Sixty-three participants were exposed to either a real or virtual green wall for seven minutes after a 5 min stressor task. Physiological stress responses, including heart rate (HR), heart rate variability (HRV), electrodermal activity (EDA), and skin temperature (ST), were recorded, alongside self-reported restoration and thermal comfort surveys. The findings showed that the real green wall provided significantly greater stress recovery, with reduced HR and EDA and increased HRV. The virtual green wall produced similar recovery effects only when considering procedural factors and specific exposure durations. Participants also reported greater restoration and comfort in the real setting. By identifying critical procedural factors and exposure durations, this study advances the application of IVEs in biophilic design, offering a pathway to enhance well-being in built environments through virtual interventions.
... As unspoiled natural environment offering visitors unique and wonderful experiences, World Natural Heritage Sites can relieve modern people from stress, tension and fatigue caused by decreased directed attention, and reduce the incidence of cardiovascular diseases (13). For depressed patients, mental stress level can be maintained at an ideal state in at least one month after returning to normal life (14). ...
Article
Full-text available
Audiovisual environmental perception has been the focus of numerous empirical studies. This study employs virtual reality (VR) to explore how different hydrodynamic waterscapes in Jiuzhaigou World Natural Heritage Site affect physiological and psychological restoration in youth. According to the results, audiovisual interactions, particularly with water sounds and birdsongs, significantly enhance physiological restoration compared to visuals alone. High-intensity hydrodynamic landscapes, regardless of birdsongs, exhibit the highest physiological restoration. There is a linearly positive correlation between physiological restorativeness and hydrodynamic landscapes. Medium-intensity hydrodynamic landscapes with rich forms are most psychologically restorative. In low-medium-intensity settings, visuals contribute more to psychological restoration than soundscapes. It is further found that waterscapes rich in flora and fauna feature a higher level of biodiversity. In the waterscapes with both elements of vegetation and water, the restorativeness of plant and animal resources is greater than that of water. This work highlights the need to focus on the application of different hydrodynamic landscapes in urban areas and the conservation of World Heritage Sites.
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has shown that natural environments and landscape‐related expertise affect psychological restoration and visual behaviour. However, little is known about how subtle differences in naturalness across similar settings, and the role of expertise, influence restorative benefits and eye movements. This study addresses these gaps by investigating how different nature settings—wild nature, tended nature (includes plaza, lawn, path, pool and waterfall settings), and urban environments—influence human preferences, restorative perceptions and eye movements. A between‐subjects experimental design was used, with 280 participants randomly assigned to one of seven landscape settings (40 participants per setting). Eye movements were tracked during the experiment to assess visual engagement. The study found that the type of setting significantly influenced preferences, restorative potential and outcomes. Wild nature, along with two tended nature settings (pool and waterfall), was rated higher in restorative potential compared with urban and plaza settings by both expert and non‐expert groups. No significant differences were observed between pool, waterfall and wild nature settings in these ratings. While expertise influenced preferences in lawn, path and waterfall settings, it did not significantly impact perceptions of restorative potential or outcomes in most settings. Furthermore, settings had a stronger effect on eye movements in non‐experts, with notable differences in eye movement behaviour between the two groups. This study emphasizes the importance of designing landscapes that cater to the diverse needs and preferences of different user groups. The findings contribute to the interdisciplinary field of people–nature relationships, providing insights for landscape architecture, urban planning and public health. Read the free Plain Language Summary for this article on the Journal blog.
Article
In recent years, mindfulness has been gaining attention in the business world as a way to improve concentration and reduce stress. Meditation is a well-known method of practicing mindfulness. On the other hand, there is also a method called “mindfulness-in-action,” in which mindfulness is practiced while moving the body. In this study, we considered ikebana, a traditional Japanese culture, as a method of mindfulness-in-action. Ikebana requires flower materials, vases, and other tools, and is labor-intensive as a mindfulness practice. Therefore, the purpose of this experiment was to clarify the changes in mind and body caused by watching videos of ikebana practice. In the experiment, the effect of stress reduction was tested using EDA. The results revealed that viewing ikebana videos and photos of ikebana had a stress reduction effect. Furthermore, ikebana videos were found to have a greater stress reduction effect than ikebana photos.
Article
Rapid urbanization has led to increased stress and anxiety, heightening concerns about mental health. Hospitals are essential to provide medical treatment, and they also support patients’ mental well-being. The effects of hospital healing landscapes to promote patients’ mental health have been studied to some degree. Previous studies have primarily concentrated on one or two sensory modalities while overlooking the synergistic effects of visual, auditory, and olfactory stimuli. From a biophilic design approach and grounded in Attention Restoration Theory (ART) and Stress Reduction Theory (SRT), this study explores the synergistic impact of vision, audition, and olfaction on the restorative potential of hospital healing landscapes. The rooftop garden of Nanjing Drum Tower Hospital served as a case study. Nine experimental scenarios were developed using an orthogonal experimental design. They involved a total of 270 volunteers participating in virtual reality experiments. Psychological assessments included the Semantic Differential Scale (SDS), State-Trait Anxiety Inventory State (STAI-S), and Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS), while physiological measures included Heart Rate (HR), Root Mean Square Successive difference (RMSSD), Skin Conductance Level (SCL), and Skin Conductance Response (SCR). The study demonstrated that hospital healing landscapes promoted psychological and physiological restoration. Visual and olfactory stimuli primarily influenced psychological responses, while auditory elements mainly affected physiological reactions. Environments with high plant diversity, natural sounds, and complex plant aromas were the most effective for psychological and physiological restoration. This study highlights the importance of integrating multisensory elements in healing environments and offers new insights for the design and management of hospital healing landscapes.
Article
Full-text available
Shifting the focus from fear, guilt, and indignation related to deteriorating environmental quality, the authors hypothesized that people who see greater potential for restorative experiences in natural environments also do more to protect them by behaving ecologically, as with recycling or reduced driving. University students (N = 488) rated a familiar freshwater marsh in terms of being away, fascination, coherence, and compatibility, qualities of restorative person-environment transactions described in attention restoration theory. They also reported on their performance of various ecological behaviors. The authors tested a structural equation model with data from a randomly drawn subset of participants and then confirmed it with the data from a second subset. For the combined subsets, perceptions of the restorative qualities predicted 23% of the variance in general ecological behavior. As the only direct predictor, fascination mediated the influences of coherence, being away, and compatibility.
Article
Full-text available
According to guidelines proposed in Sweden, at least 80% of the visitors in quiet areas should perceive the sound environment as good. This was the starting point for a questionnaire study on "soundscape quality" in four suburban green areas and in four city parks. The soundscapes in the suburban areas were completely dominated by sounds from nature (e.g., bird song and sounds from water), whereas traffic noise was a main component of the city-park soundscapes. Measured equivalent sound levels (from all sources) ranged from 42 to 50 dBA in the suburban green areas, and from 49 to 60 dBA in the city parks (LAeq, 15min). "Soundscape quality" was assessed by a five-point bipolar category scale. Among the respondents, 84-100% in the suburban green areas and 53-65% in the city parks assessed the soundscape as "Good" or "Very good". Thus, all suburban green areas but none of the city parks reached the stipulated goal (at least 80%). The soundcape quality was confirmed by attribute profiling using a set of 12 adjectives. Based on the visitor's responses, it is concluded that good soundscape quality can only be achieved if the traffic noise exposure in suburban green areas and city parks during day time is below 50 dBA.
Article
Full-text available
Individual differences in preferences to photographs were explored based on an alternative framework. This framework predicts that the primary difference between individuals in this respect is their ability to process photographic information, which in turn influences their preferences. Chiefly, people with well-developed schemes in photography (e.g., photo professionals) should have a higher ability to process photographic information than people with less developed schemes (e.g., psychology students). Consequently, people with well-developed schemes in photography should prefer photographs that are relatively more demanding to process. Ten psychology students and 5 photo professionals assessed 32 photographs on six general concepts: Preference, Hedonic Tone, Expressiveness, Familiarity, Uncertainty, and Dynamics. As predicted, photo professionals had a higher ability to process photographic information and preferred photographs that were relatively uncertain and unfamiliar. These results are in concordance with previous research and give strong support to the utility of the present framework in experimental aesthetics. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Reviews the book, Psychophysiology: Human behavior and physiological response (4th ed.) by John L. Andreassi (see record 2000-07165-000). This is the fourth edition of a well-known and highly acclaimed book. Although the author modestly states, "The plan of this book is to provide students with elementary information...," the book is almost an encyclopaedic account of this extremely extensive field. Given this comprehensiveness, the book represents an excellent introduction and overview to the whole field of psychophysiology. There are two introductory chapters, the first of which provides a general introduction to the field of psychophysiology, starting very conventionally with definitions and followed by a short historical account. The following chapter provides information on the basic anatomy and physiology of the nervous system, as well as the principles behind the main techniques used. The remaining 17 chapters are grouped into three main conceptual areas: (1) measures of central nervous system activity, (2) measures of the activity of behaviourally relevant peripheral systems, and (3) applications and methodological issues including an appendix dealing with the extremely important issue of laboratory safety. Andreassi has written a comprehensive guide on research methods in psychophysiology, clearly written and very suitable for the group for whom the book is primarily intended: students of behavioural science. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The statistical power of a test of significance for the difference among means is dependent upon a number of factors, among them the presence of a covariate, which often serves to increase power, as well as measurement error, which reduces power. Formulas are provided for adjusting Cohen's (1977) f value for these two sources of variability. These adjusted f values can then be used with Cohen's power tables. An example of the use of these formulas in the planning stage of a study is given, followed by a description of the complex nature of the relationship between power and covariance and measurement error.
Article
discuss specific theoretical and methodological issues related to the use of psychophysiological measures of arousal / describe state-of-the-art electrodermal and cardiovascular measurement techniques / emphasize the use of patterns of electrodermal and cardiovascular responses as indexes of arousal specifying an arousal construct / maintaining a psychophysiologically consonant experimental situation / multiple psychophysiological response measures / continuous measurement (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
A considerable body of folklore and scientific research alludes to the efficacy of the vernacular environment to influence both aesthetic experience and general well-being. To examine explicitly whether stress recovery and/or immunization varies as a function of the roadside environment, 160 college-age participants, both male and female, viewed one of four different video-taped simulated drives through outdoor environments immediately following and preceding mildly stressful events. Overall, it was anticipated that participants who viewed artifact-dominated drives, relative to participants who viewed nature-dominated drives, would show greater autonomic activity indicative of stress (e.g. elevated blood pressure and electrodermal activity), as well as show altered somatic activity indicative of greater negative affect (e.g. elevated electromyographic (EMG) activity over the brow region and decreased activity over the cheek region). In addition, it was expected that participants who viewed nature-dominated drives would experience quicker recovery from stress and greater immunization to subsequent stress than participants who viewed artifact-dominated drives. The overall pattern of results is consistent with both hypotheses and the findings are interpreted to support postulating a sympathetic-specific mechanism that underlies the effect of nature on stress recovery and immunization.
Article
Urbanicity presents a challenge for the pursuit of sustainability. High settlement density may offer some environmental, economic, and social advantages, but it can impose psychological demands that people find excessive. These demands of urban life have stimulated a desire for contact with nature through suburban residence, leading to planning and transportation practices that have profound implications for the pursuit of sustainability. Some might dismiss people's desire for contact with nature as the result of an anti-urban bias in conjunction with a romantic view of nature. However, research in environmental psychology suggests that people's desire for contact with nature serves an important adaptive function, namely, psychological restoration. Based on this insight, we offer a perspective on an underlying practical challenge: designing communities that balance settlement density with satisfactory access to nature experience. We discuss research on four issues: how people tend to believe that nature is restorative; how restoration needs and beliefs shape environmental preferences; how well people actually achieve restoration in urban and natural environments; and how contact with nature can promote health. In closing, we consider urban nature as a design option that promotes urban sustainability.
Article
We tested the hypothesis that exposure to nature stimuli restores depleted voluntary attention capacity and affects selective attention. Before viewing a video of either a natural or an urban environment, 28 subjects first completed a proofreading task to induce mental load and then performed Posner's attention-orienting task. After viewing the video they performed the attention-orienting task a second time. Cardiac inter-beat interval (IBI) was measured continuously to index autonomic arousal. Before the video both groups reacted faster to validly versus invalidly cued targets in the attention-orienting task. After the video, the urban group was still faster on validly versus invalidly cued trials, but in the nature group this difference disappeared. During the video the nature group had a longer mean IBI (lower heart rate) measured as the difference from baseline than the urban group. The results suggest that reduced autonomic arousal during the video engendered less spatially selective attention in the nature group compared to the urban group.