ArticlePDF Available

Automatic Ethics: The Effects of Implicit Assumptions and Contextual Cues on Moral Behavior

American Psychological Association
Journal of Applied Psychology
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

We empirically examine the reflexive or automatic aspects of moral decision making. To begin, we develop and validate a measure of an individual's implicit assumption regarding the inherent morality of business. Then, using an in-basket exercise, we demonstrate that an implicit assumption that business is inherently moral impacts day-to-day business decisions and interacts with contextual cues to shape moral behavior. Ultimately, we offer evidence supporting a characterization of employees as reflexive interactionists: moral agents whose automatic decision-making processes interact with the environment to shape their moral behavior.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Automatic Ethics: The Effects of Implicit Assumptions and Contextual
Cues on Moral Behavior
Scott J. Reynolds
University of Washington, Seattle
Keith Leavitt
United States Military Academy, West Point
Katherine A. DeCelles
University of Toronto
We empirically examine the reflexive or automatic aspects of moral decision making. To begin, we
develop and validate a measure of an individual’s implicit assumption regarding the inherent morality of
business. Then, using an in-basket exercise, we demonstrate that an implicit assumption that business is
inherently moral impacts day-to-day business decisions and interacts with contextual cues to shape moral
behavior. Ultimately, we offer evidence supporting a characterization of employees as reflexive inter-
actionists: moral agents whose automatic decision-making processes interact with the environment to
shape their moral behavior.
Keywords: automatic social cognition, moral decision making, moral behavior
Scholars and practitioners alike have argued that moral and
ethical behavior is critical to long-term survival (Donaldson,
2003; George, 2007; Godfrey, 2005).
1
As a result, more and more
research is exploring why employees behave morally and immor-
ally (Trevin˜o, Weaver, & Reynolds, 2006). Research in this area
has traditionally focused on the rational and deliberate aspects of
moral decision making (Jones, 1991; Kohlberg, 1981; Rest, 1986),
but in recent years scholars have paid more attention to the
reflexive, automatic, and intuitive aspects of morality (Haidt,
2001; Reynolds, 2006). As work in this area progresses, one major
hurdle has become apparent. Despite strong theoretical arguments
for the relationship between automatic processes and moral behav-
ior, there is no direct empirical evidence of this relationship.
Furthermore, though many have acknowledged that organizations
can play an important role in shaping automatically driven moral
behavior (e.g., Ashforth & Anand, 2003; Banaji, Bazerman, &
Chugh, 2003; Bandura, 1997; Chugh, 2004; Chugh, Banaji, &
Bazerman, 2005; Sonenshein, 2007), these discussions have been
relatively abstract and inferential in nature. No research has em-
pirically examined how an organization might affect the relation-
ship between an individual’s automatic decision-making process
and the individual’s moral behavior.
The purpose of this research is thus twofold. First, we draw from
literatures on moral decision making and automatic social cogni-
tion to develop a measure of one specific element of an individ-
ual’s automatic processing system: an implicit assumption about
the moral nature of business. Second, we develop a characteriza-
tion of employees as reflexive interactionists, moral agents whose
automatic decision-making processes interact with contextual fac-
tors to influence their moral behavior, and empirically demonstrate
that an implicit assumption about the moral nature of business can
interact with contextual cues to shape moral behavior. Ultimately,
this research extends current research in moral decision making
(Reynolds, 2006; Sonenshein, 2007) and automatic social cogni-
tion (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) and generates prac-
tical implications for improving individual behavior in organiza-
tions.
Automatic Social Cognition and Moral
Decision Making
The literature on automatic (or implicit) social cognition is
predicated upon the argument that social judgments are rapidly
constructed from past experience in a process that is automatic and
without conscious deliberation (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999; Bargh,
Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar, & Troetschel, 2001). In this
literature, automatic or implicit cognitive processing is described
as nonconscious processing of information such that “introspec-
tively unidentified (or inaccurately identified) traces of past expe-
rience mediate attributions” made in current exchanges (Green-
wald & Banaji, 1995, p. 15). In other words, individuals approach
even the most novel social situations informed by underlying
experience. This experience is stored in what are referred to as
knowledge structures, schematic mental structures that describe
1
We consider the terms moral and ethical to be synonymous.
Scott J. Reynolds, Michael G. Foster School of Business, University of
Washington, Seattle; Keith Leavitt, Army Center for the Professional
Military Ethic, United States Military Academy, West Point; Katherine A.
DeCelles, Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto, Toronto,
Ontario, Canada.
We thank Greg Bigley, Tara Ceranic, Jennifer Gephart, Tony Green-
wald, Tom Lee, Linda Trevin˜ o, and Gary Weaver for their help in the
development of this research.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Scott J.
Reynolds, Michael G. Foster School of Business, University of Washing-
ton, Box 353200, Seattle, WA 98028. E-mail: heyscott@u.washington.edu
Journal of Applied Psychology © 2010 American Psychological Association
2010, Vol. 95, No. 4, 752–760 0021-9010/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019411
752
relationships between concepts (Greenwald et al., 1998). The term
implicit association refers generically to any association of this
type. In recent years, scholars have distinguished between different
kinds of implicit associations, including implicit attitudes (e.g.,
Greenwald et al., 1998), implicit stereotypes (Greenwald, Oakes,
& Hoffman, 2003), implicit self-esteem, and implicit self-concept
(Greenwald & Farnham, 2000). Leavitt, Fong, and Greenwald
(2007) recently argued that implicit assumptions are associations
between concepts (e.g., work and prestige). In this view, such
associations represent the degree of mental “closeness” of con-
cepts and constitute an assumption of abstract relatedness. Though
implicit assumptions may be taken for granted by the individual,
they represent the basic unit of understanding that allows for
day-to-day behavior.
Implicit associations are both theoretically and empirically dis-
tinct from explicit attitudes. Explicit attitudes are beliefs and
opinions developed through cognitive deliberation. Individuals are
self-aware of such attitudes and beliefs, which thus can be captured
through self-report measures. In contrast, implicit associations are
mental representations that allow for automatic responses to ex-
perience; they are activated without conscious effort by exposure
to strongly associated stimuli (Bargh, 2006). Given this, implicit
associations are especially sensitive to cultural context and often
reflect culturally pervasive themes (Banaji, 2001).
It is theorized that implicitly associated concepts are frequently
activated by one another and that the consequent accessibility of
these “related” categories informs judgments, intentions, and be-
havior outside of conscious awareness (Greenwald & Banaji,
1995; Greenwald et al., 2002). Indeed, empirical work has repeat-
edly demonstrated that implicit associations, assumed or mea-
sured, can affect behavior (Bargh, 1997; Bargh, Chen, & Burrows,
1996; Dijksterhuis & Bargh, 2001; Kay, Wheeler, Bargh, & Ross,
2004; Uhlmann, Poehlman, & Bargh, 2007). Scholars have argued
that automatically driven behaviors are most apparent in situations
where norms of behavior are relatively ambiguous or poorly struc-
tured (e.g., Greenwald, Uhlmann, Poehlman, & Banaji, 2009).
Given these conditions, we believe, implicit associations are rel-
evant to the study of moral decision making.
Philosophers have theorized about the processes of moral deci-
sion making for centuries (Singer, 1994). With a few important
exceptions (e.g., Hume, 1983/1751), rational thought has taken a
primary and dominant position in this literature (Haidt, 2001).
Scholars have recently drawn from writings in moral philosophy
on moral intuition (e.g., Kant, 1785/1994; Ross, 1930) and bur-
geoning physiological and clinical research in the area (e.g.,
Greene & Haidt, 2002) to suggest that moral decision making is
also impacted, if not dominated, by reflexive or automatic cogni-
tive processes (Haidt, 2001; Reynolds, 2006). Reynolds (2006)
argued that moral intuition is a result of the fact that an individ-
ual’s descriptive knowledge of any given moral situation is inter-
twined with normative assessments of that situation. In terms of
the automatic social cognition literature, these normative valua-
tions would be manifest as implicit associations between concepts
(e.g., bribery) and their normative valence (e.g., immoral).
Whereas there are perhaps innumerable concepts that can inter-
sect with the moral domain, in this research we chose to examine
the individual’s nonconscious normative valuation of business. We
chose business because the concept is very general and common
and is germane to anyone who encounters moral issues at work. As
such, the implications of any findings could be quite generalizable
and could pave a path for similar research on more specific
concepts. In the following section, we consider implicit assump-
tions about the moral nature of business and develop hypotheses
about how such beliefs can shape behavior.
An Implicit Assumption About the Moral
Nature of Business
Business is a generic concept that in colloquial language repre-
sents a conglomeration of objects, people, activities, and institu-
tions. Though individual understandings of business vary in minor
details, at an abstract level everyone holds a general understanding
of what business represents. Furthermore, business is generally
linked to a particular set of norms and principles. As several
management scholars have suggested (Ghoshal & Moran, 1996;
Morrison & Milliken, 2000; Pfeffer, 1997), business is associated
with an economic paradigm that emphasizes competition, fiduciary
responsibilities, maximizing shareholder returns, and other tradi-
tional, economics-minded, capitalistic values and behaviors (e.g.,
Friedman, 1962).
We suggest that beyond holding just a simple description of
what business represents, individuals also hold a normative valu-
ation (make a normative association) of business such that it is
implicitly assumed to be inherently moral or immoral (Reynolds,
2006). As Suchman (1995) argued, morality is an aspect of legit-
imacy. Thus, an implicit assumption that business is inherently
moral would reflect a belief that the predominant economic para-
digm with which business is associated is morally valid and thus
legitimate (Pfeffer, 1981; Pfeffer & Salancik, 1978). So the person
who believes that business is inherently moral (legitimate) would
believe that business should be an exercise in intense competition
emphasizing shareholder obligations, financial performance, and
other traditionally capitalistic practices. In contrast, an implicit
belief that business is immoral would reflect a view that traditional
business norms and practices are perhaps unwarrantedly aggres-
sive and even harmful. As a result, those who hold an implicit
assumption that business is immoral would be more leery of the
traditional, economics-minded, capitalistic business paradigm and
would instead lean toward more collaborative and more people-
oriented views, values, and beliefs (e.g., Freeman, 1984). Thus, we
offer the following:
Hypothesis 1: An implicit assumption that business is moral
will be positively associated with traditional, economics-
minded, capitalistic values and beliefs.
We believe that this implicit assumption will be associated with
definitively moral behaviors (Jones, 1991) in addition to individual
values and beliefs. Empirical evidence of a link between elements
of the traditional, economic paradigm and immoral behavior can
be found in research on deliberate moral decision making. For
example, Kish-Gephart, Harrison, and Trevin˜o’s (2010) meta-
analysis suggests that Machiavellianism, an individual trait that
emphasizes competition and self-advancement, is associated with
immoral behavior. In other research, the traditional, economic
paradigm is often represented as a more realistic and less idealistic
view of the world, and research has demonstrated that realism is
associated with immoral behavior whereas idealism is associated
with moral behavior (O’Fallon & Butterfield, 2005).
753
RESEARCH REPORTS
Though a link between an implicit belief about the moral nature
of business and moral behavior may exist, current theory suggests
that such a link requires a contextual cue in order for the associ-
ation to become salient (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Greenwald et
al., 2002). By definition, a contextual cue refers to the message
imbued in a discernible element of the environment (Weick, 1995).
A contextual cue can convey a great deal of information, and the
relevance of any single piece of information to automatically
driven behavior is a function of the extent to which the information
taps into existing associations (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Green-
wald et al., 2002). Thus, to the extent that a cue is salient to an
existing implicit association, it can activate the association and
initiate a behavioral response. In this research, we focus on the
extent to which a cue communicates key concepts of the predom-
inant economic and capitalistic paradigm of business (e.g., com-
petition, stockholders) as opposed to concepts that contrast such a
view (e.g., collaboration, nonowner stakeholders). We argue that if
an individual implicitly believes that business is inherently moral
and is provided cues by the context that are consistent with this
implicit assumption, that belief will become salient to the individ-
ual and the individual will engage in behaviors consistent with that
paradigm. Moreover, the cues will strengthen the implicit belief’s
influence upon the associated behaviors. As a result, competitive
tendencies can become ultracompetitive behaviors; a preference
for shareholders can become an overemphasis on shareholders;
and capitalistic tendencies can be taken to more extreme conclu-
sions. In many cases, these points of excess will constitute im-
moral behavior. We expected that those who implicitly assume that
business is immoral and are provided cues consistent with that
implicit association would become increasingly less likely to en-
gage in these kinds of behaviors and would thus be less likely to
act immorally.
Empirical findings support an interactionist view of automatic
moral behavior. A host of research supports the general proposi-
tion that individual behavior is shaped by the interaction between
individuals and their environments (Bandura, 1986; Chatman,
1989; Schneider, 1983), and this view has been repeatedly con-
firmed in the ethics literature (Ford & Richardson, 1994; Trevin˜o
et al., 2006). With regard to specific contextual cues, we fully
expected explicit cues to activate these implicit beliefs, but re-
search has shown that individual business behaviors can also be
influenced by subtle contextual cues such as the placement of a
briefcase in a visible location (Kay et al., 2004). Thus, we char-
acterize employees as reflexive interactionists, moral agents whose
moral behavior is shaped by the interplay between their implicit
assumptions about morality and the contextual cues, both explicit
and subtle, that pertain to moral behavior they receive.
Hypothesis 2: Implicit assumptions will interact with contex-
tual cues to shape moral behavior such that an implicit as-
sumption that business is moral and a competitive cue will
result in the most immoral behavior in business-related tasks.
Study 1
We designed Study 1 to achieve two purposes. First, we sought
empirical evidence of the validity of an Implicit Association Test
(IAT) that measures individual implicit assumptions about the
morality of business. Second, we intended to test Hypothesis 1.
Method
Sample and procedure. Participants were intentionally
drawn from two demographically distinct pools. The first pool was
an undergraduate environmental business strategy class at a large
midwestern university. The second was an online forum on moun-
tain biking. Thirty-five students and 24 forum members completed
the instrument. Forum membership can vary on a daily basis, but
we estimate that this represents approximately a 40% response
rate. As expected, the groups were statistically different in terms of
age, F(1, 57) 147.55, p.00, Cohen’s d2.89; full-time work
experience, F(1, 57) 294.46, p.00, Cohen’s d4.29; and
political party affiliation, F(1, 57) 14.01, p.00, Cohen’s d
0.94. In the combined sample, 53% were female and 88% were
Caucasian; 61% were under 25 and 3% were over 55. At least 66%
reported 1 year or more of full-time work experience.
Measures.
Independent variable. An implicit assumption about the
moral nature of business was measured with a web-based version
of Greenwald and colleagues’ IAT method (Greenwald et al.,
1998, 2002). The IAT is a computer exercise built upon an obser-
vation that tasks requiring responses to incompatible or unassoci-
ated categories require slightly more time than identical tasks
requiring responses to compatible or associated categories. For a
full discussion and meta-analysis of the IAT, please see Greenwald
et al. (2009).
In this IAT, the primary concept categories were business and
sports, and the secondary concept categories were ethical and
unethical. As per the developed IAT methodology, the control
category sports was used as an arithmetic control parceling out
participants’ tendency to associate a similarly familiar category
with ethical versus unethical behavior (Greenwald et al., 1998).
Sports was chosen as the control category for two reasons. First,
the features of sports are highly analogous to those of business
(e.g., supervisors are similar to coaches). Second, sports and busi-
ness share ideals about fair play and competition, and recent media
exposure has made moral conduct salient in both domains.
Twenty-four conceptually comparable terms were generated for
categorization, six for each category (see Table 1).
To score the measure, we used the Greenwald improved D
algorithm (see Greenwald, Nosek, and Banaji, 2003, for a com-
plete description), which produces a standardized difference score.
A score of zero suggests no relative difference between the
strength of association between the concepts of business and
ethical and business and unethical. Positive scores suggest a stron-
ger association between business and ethical, and negative scores
suggest a stronger association between business and unethical. The
algorithm accounts for time necessary to recover from errors and
removes trials on which participant response is faster than 300 ms
or slower than 3,000 ms. In this sample, a normal distribution with
variance between subjects (M⫽⫺.02, SD .41, range ⫽⫺.73 to
.97) indicated an individual difference in implicit cognitive struc-
tures (Greenwald et al., 1998). We also computed three sub-D
scores for each participant using one third of the trials (Greenwald
& Farnham, 2000). Results (␣⫽.77) indicated that participants
responded consistently throughout the task, and thus we consid-
ered the measure to be internally reliable.
To provide evidence of the IAT measure’s construct validity, we
gathered three direct measures. First, we asked participants
754 RESEARCH REPORTS
“Which response best describes your ‘gut reaction’ to the word
‘business’?” (1 very good, 5very bad;M2.78, SD 0.89).
The implicit belief was correlated as expected with this response
(r⫽⫺.26, p.05). Second, we asked, “Which statement best
captures what you believe?” (1 cheating is more prevalent in
sports than in business, 5cheating is more prevalent in business
than in sports;M3.53, SD 0.70). As expected, the implicit
belief was negatively correlated with this response (r⫽⫺.27, p
.04). Third, we asked, “Which statement best captures what you
believe?” (1 business is much more ethical than sports, 5
sports is much more ethical than business;M3.58, SD 0.91).
As expected, the implicit belief was negatively correlated with this
response (r⫽⫺.44, p.00).
Dependent variables. We utilized two measures of traditional,
economics-minded, capitalistic values and beliefs. First, using an
approach developed by Reynolds, Schultz, and Hekman (2006),
we asked participants, “In general, how much consideration do you
think managers should give the following six (6) groups?” We
listed six major stakeholder groups (e.g., customers, employees)
and asked participants to distribute 100 points among the groups.
Traditional, economics-minded, capitalistic values and beliefs
were measured as the amount of points distributed to the share-
holders/owners.
Second, we employed Mehrabian’s (1996) 20-item measure of
libertarianism. The items (e.g., “We need a stronger government to
create a better society,” “Our society can improve only with more
government controls over individuals and businesses”; 1
strongly disagree, 7strongly agree) assess the extent to which
an individual values personal freedom and the principles of a free
market. We associated more libertarian views with traditional,
economics-minded, capitalistic values and beliefs. The measure
demonstrated high reliability (␣⫽.93).
Results
Means, standard deviations, and a correlation matrix of the
variables are presented in Table 2. Hypothesis 1 held that the
implicit assumption would be positively associated with tradi-
tional, economics-minded, capitalistic values and beliefs. Results
of linear regression (see Table 3) showed that the implicit assump-
Table 1
IAT Categories and Terms
Categories
a
Business Sports Ethical Unethical
Terms Boardroom Locker room Giving Stealing
CEO Coach Being honest Lying
Customers Fans Helping Hurting others
Corporation Stadium Being fair Cheating
Profits Trophy Following rules Breaking rules
Sales Athlete Considering others Insulting others
Note. IAT Implicit Association Test.
a
Pairings (random ordering): (a) business/ethical and sports/unethical; (b) business/unethical and sports/ethical.
Table 2
Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlation Matrix of Variables in Study 1
Variable MSD 1234567891011
1. Implicit assumption
a
0.02 0.41
2. Age (categorical) 1.81 1.17 .29
3. Gender 0.47 0.50 .10 .27
4. Caucasian (dummy) 0.88 0.33 .22 .01 .14
5. Full-time experience
(categorical) 3.15 2.18 .08 .80
ⴱⴱ
.36
ⴱⴱ
.17
6. Political affiliation 1.21 0.40 .21 .48
ⴱⴱ
.45
ⴱⴱ
.19 .48
7. “Business is bad” 2.78 0.89 .26
.22 .19 .09 .11 .38
ⴱⴱ
8. “Cheating is more
prevalent in business
than in sports” 3.53 0.70 .27
.40
ⴱⴱ
.23 .10 .35
ⴱⴱ
.32
.43
ⴱⴱ
9. “Sports is more ethical
than business” 3.58 0.91 .44
ⴱⴱ
.40
ⴱⴱ
.19 .11 .28
.56
ⴱⴱ
.50
ⴱⴱ
.41
ⴱⴱ
10. Consideration for
shareholders 17.90 9.79 .28
.10 .09 .05 .02 .14 .41
ⴱⴱ
.01 .30
ⴱⴱ
11. Libertarianism 4.38 1.04 .30
.46
ⴱⴱ
.50
ⴱⴱ
.08 .52
ⴱⴱ
.60
ⴱⴱ
.42
ⴱⴱ
.43
ⴱⴱ
.44
ⴱⴱ
.01
Note. n 59.
a
The Implicit Association Test measure is continuous. Subsequently, “perceptions of sports” would be the inverse of “perceptions of business” and
“immoral” would be the inverse of “moral.”
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
755
RESEARCH REPORTS
tion was positively associated with a belief about how much
consideration managers should give shareholders: The more they
implicitly believed in the inherent morality (legitimacy) of the
traditional business model, the more consideration they thought
shareholders/owners should receive. In addition, the implicit as-
sumption was significantly correlated with libertarianism: The
more each participant implicitly believed in the inherent morality
of business, the more libertarian were his or her views. These
results provided evidence in support of Hypothesis 1.
Discussion
Study 1 provided evidence of the validity of the IAT measure
and evidence supporting Hypothesis 1. In Study 2, we sought to
gather additional evidence of the validity of the IAT measure and
to test Hypothesis 2.
Study 2
Method
Sample. Participants were 126 undergraduate students en-
rolled in an introductory management course at a West Coast
university. They ranged in age from 18 to 50 years (M21.65,
SD 3.65); 75 (59.5%) were women (2 missing). Twelve (9.8%)
reported three or more years of work experience, and eight (6.5%)
reported none. The sample was 43% Caucasian and 48% Asian.
Procedure. Participants learned about the experiment through
an online system that coordinated a research requirement in the
course. More than 200 students were enrolled in multiple sections
of the course. Given that five other opportunities were available,
the participation rate (60%) was quite high. In each session,
participants completed the IAT via computer. They then completed
a pen-and-paper in-basket exercise. In the exercise, they were
asked to imagine themselves as a manager in a fictitious company
we created with several tasks to complete (e.g., reassigning sales
personnel, ordering office supplies). Tasks relevant to the hypoth-
esis tests were reading a memo from the president and completing
an insurance claim.
The contextual cue was manipulated in the corporate memo. In
the memo, the company CEO praised the corporate culture. In the
competitive condition, he described it as a culture “that emphasizes
success—we do what it takes to be competitive!” In the noncom-
petitive condition, he described it as a culture “that emphasizes
values—we will always do what is right!” Manipulations were
checked with four items (e.g., “[This company’s] culture empha-
sizes success”) scored on a 7-point agreement scale (1 strongly
disagree, 7strongly agree). Results indicated that those reading
the competitive cues viewed the culture as more competitive and
success-oriented than the other condition, F(1, 124) 65.68, p
.00, Cohen’s d1.47. Following this exercise, students completed
a survey. All data were provided anonymously.
Measures. Immoral behavior was measured in the insurance
claim exercise. Participants were informed that company product
had been destroyed en route to the marketplace and that they were
responsible for filing the insurance claim. Participants were pro-
vided the cost of the product, the advertised price of the product,
and the going rate of the product on the black market. They were
then instructed to complete the form by indicating the value of the
product for reimbursement. Immoral behavior was measured as a
dichotomous variable. As values at or below the advertised price
were based on legitimate standards defensible in their own right
(cost or price), they were considered to be moral and were scored
as zero. In contrast, values above the advertised price of the
product were based on illegitimate standards (black market) and
were therefore deemed to be immoral. Immoral values were coded
as 1. To confirm this assessment, we submitted these behaviors to
an expert panel consisting of 10 business school professors with an
average of 9.50 years experience conducting research on business
ethics. As expected, the panel rated submitting a claim based on a
value under the fair market value as ethical (1 very unethical,
7very ethical;M5.00, SD 1.56) and submitting a claim
based on a value over the fair market value as unethical (M1.30,
SD 0.48).
The explicit belief about business was measured with an estab-
lished five-item scale (Weaver, Trevin˜o, & Cochran, 1999). The
items measured the extent to which the participants believed that
Table 3
Linear Regression Results for Study 1
Variable
Consideration given to shareholders Libertarianism
BSE
p
2
95% CI BSE
p
2
95% CI
Constant 20.86
ⴱⴱ
5.45 .00 [9.93, 31.79] 2.50
ⴱⴱ
0.43 .39 [1.63, 3.36]
Sex 3.32 2.84 .03 [9.01, 2.38] 0.52
0.22 .09 [0.07, 0.97]
Caucasian (dummy) 3.56 4.01 .02 [11.61, 4.49] 0.34 0.32 .02 [0.94, 0.30]
Full-time experience 0.30 0.67 .00 [1.65, 1.04] 0.13
0.05 .11 [0.03, 0.24]
Political affiliation 5.11 3.84 .03 [2.59, 12.81] 0.84
ⴱⴱ
0.30 .13 [0.23, 1.44]
Implicit assumption
a
6.76
3.22 .08 [0.30, 13.23] 0.52
0.25 .07 [0.01, 1.03]
R
2
.13 .52
Adjusted R
2
.05 .47
F1.58 11.36
ⴱⴱ
Note. n 59. Including age and full-time experience resulted in collinearity problems (variance inflation factor 3.81), so the variable with the least
variance (age) was removed. CI confidence interval.
a
The Implicit Association Test measure is continuous. Subsequently, “perceptions of sports” would be the inverse of “perceptions of business” and
“immoral” would be the inverse of “moral.”
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
756 RESEARCH REPORTS
five socially oriented concepts (e.g., “seeking the good of society,”
“treating people fairly”) should be important to a firm. The scale
demonstrated acceptable reliability (␣⫽.82).
The implicit assumption was measured with the IAT exercise
described in Study 1. In this case, the mean for the implicit
assumption was positive (.17), which reflected the expected
strength of the traditional business paradigm in a business school
setting.
Though the IAT method has demonstrated high levels of pre-
dictive validity (Greenwald et al., 2009), we sought additional
evidence of the predictive validity of this particular IAT measure.
In an approach developed by Reynolds et al. (2006), participants
were informed that the company had sold a warehouse and had
$100,000 to distribute at their discretion. They were further in-
formed that coworkers had suggested three different outlets for the
money: a stockholder dividend, an employee-managed scholarship
program, and a community youth development program. Prefer-
ence for stockholders over nonowner stakeholders was measured
as the amount of money (ranging from $0 to $100,000) distributed
to the stockholders (M$42,289, SD $38,374). An implicit
assumption that the traditional, economics-minded capitalistic
model is moral should be manifest in a preference for stockholders
at the expense of stakeholders. Partial correlational analysis dem-
onstrated that after controlling for the effects of the manipulation
and the explicit belief, an implicit assumption that business is
moral was significantly associated with a preference for stockhold-
ers (r.20, p.03). This result provided additional evidence of
the predictive validity of this IAT measure.
Results
Means, standard deviations, and a correlation matrix of the
variables are presented in Table 4. Results of binary logistic
regression are presented in Table 5. As Model 1 reveals, none of
the main effects were significant. This result notwithstanding, the
model was hierarchically well formulated, which allowed for a test
of interaction effects (Jaccard, 2001). Hypothesis 2 held that the
implicit assumption would interact with the contextual cue to
shape immoral behavior such that a belief that business is moral
and a competitive cue would lead to immoral behavior. Results of
Model 2 indicate that the interaction between the implicit assump-
tion and the competitive cue (condition) significantly influenced
the decision to seek reimbursement for more than the advertised
price. The results indicate (see Figure 1) that those who believed
that business is inherently moral and received a competitive cue
were 33 times more likely to file a claim based on an illegitimate
valuation. These results supported Hypothesis 2.
Discussion
Study 2 satisfied two objectives. First, the distribution exercise
provided additional evidence of the predictive validity of the IAT.
Second, the insurance claim exercise provided evidence that the
implicit belief the business is moral can interact with contextual
cues to shape moral behavior, as Hypothesis 2 predicts. We note
that in this exercise neither the explicit measure nor the competi-
tive cue was a significant predictor of moral behavior. In hindsight,
we speculate that these findings are the result of using a dichoto-
mous, less statistically sensitive, dependent variable. Indeed, those
findings make the interaction effect all the more noteworthy.
General Discussion
The research contributes to the literature in at least three ways.
First, the research demonstrates an association between a concept
(business) and its normative valence, thus advancing the notion of
an implicit assumption and opening a door to other similar mea-
sures. Given that a concept can have a moral association, as
Reynolds’ (2006) model of moral decision making suggests, the
empirical possibilities both for concepts with obvious moral di-
mensions (e.g., bribery) and for those with less obvious moral
dimensions (e.g., globalization) would seem to be limitless.
Second, this research demonstrates a relationship between an
implicit assumption about the moral nature of business and moral
behaviors. In particular, these findings provide evidence that im-
plicit assumptions about morality help explain variance in moral
behavior above and beyond that explained by explicit beliefs.
Thus, we provide empirical evidence of the importance of both the
deliberate and the intuitive aspects of decision making (Haidt,
2001; Reynolds, 2006; Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977).
Finally, this research makes a contribution by developing and
empirically assessing the characterization of employees as reflex-
ive interactionists. Though the field has acknowledged the roles of
deliberate reasoning, automatic processes, and context in shaping
moral behavior (Banaji et al., 2003; Caruso, Epley, & Bazerman,
2006; Chugh, 2004; Chugh et al., 2005; Wade-Benzoni, Tenbrun-
sel, & Bazerman, 1996), no studies have empirically examined
how individual behavior might be a product of the interaction
between automatic cognitive processes and environmental factors.
Accordingly, this research makes a contribution by demonstrating
Table 4
Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlation Matrix of Variables in Study 2
Variable MSD123456
1. Claim (dichotomous) 0.17 0.38
2. Age 21.65 3.58 .09
3. Gender 0.39 0.48 .17 .03
4. Contextual cue (condition) 0.58 0.50 .05 .07 .04
5. Explicit belief 5.82 0.86 .07 .11 .06 .07
6. Implicit assumption 0.17 0.42 .10 .05 .02 .15 .00
Note. n 126.
757
RESEARCH REPORTS
that implicit assumptions about the moral nature of business in-
teract with cues from the organization environment salient to the
implicit association and thereby shape moral behavior.
Limitations
This research is not without limitations. For example, the IAT
method has been subject to many criticisms (e.g., Arkes & Tetlock,
2004). Though the most common objections have been addressed
at length (Greenwald & Nosek, 2001; Greenwald, Rudman, Nosek,
& Zayas, 2006; Greenwald et al., 2009), we acknowledge continu-
ing concerns with the method. With regard to possible objections
to the IAT we developed, we suggest that the variance found
between individuals, the use of a prevalent control concept (i.e.,
sports), and evidence of construct and predictive validity indicate
that the measure captured a powerful and important implicit asso-
ciation that, though likely shaped by a predominant cultural par-
adigm, still varies greatly across individuals. Future research can
assess our claim.
Additionally, though our research measured this implicit asso-
ciation, we did not consider its origins. Reynolds’ (2006) discus-
sion of ethical prototypes suggests that such assumptions are
formed from years of experience but can nevertheless be altered
through new experiences and personal reflection. Thus, we expect
such factors as educational and functional backgrounds (Hambrick
& Mason, 1984), social networks (Brass, Butterfield, & Skaggs,
1998), and ethics training (Frisque & Kolb, 2008) to be relevant.
Future research can explore these issues in detail.
We also recognize limitations specific to Study 2. First, we have
argued that salience is the key characteristic of contextual cues, but
the cue we utilized was explicit and had a normative tone. Future
research could explore our hypothesis with a more subtle and more
agnostic contextual cue to verify our core argument. Second, we
used a dichotomous dependent variable. A scalar measure of moral
behavior would provide more generalizable results. Third, we note
that whereas Study 2 revealed a relationship between an implicit
belief, a contextual cue, and moral behavior, it did not investigate
the process by which the cue turned or translated that implicit
belief into a behavior. Future research could explore the mechanics
of these automatic processes while controlling for well-
documented moral traits (e.g., relativism, moral attentiveness) and
thereby provide a more thorough treatment of this phenomenon.
Theoretical and Practical Implications
Our research has important theoretical and practical implica-
tions. For example, our findings spark questions about the tension
between organizational responsibility and individual moral agency
(Ashforth & Anand, 2003; Bandura, 1999; Sykes & Matza, 1957).
This research suggests that implicit assumptions can be activated
by the social context to influence immoral behavior independent of
cognitive and deliberate attempts to justify one’s actions. In other
words, perhaps there is less moral agency in moral decisions than
previously considered, and perhaps organizations bear more re-
sponsibility for the actions of their members than is currently
understood. Our research suggests that organizations might take a
proactive approach to influencing employees’ perceptions of busi-
ness as a complex activity filled with moral obstacles. Rather than
displaying confidence that business is a moral venture, leaders
instead might warn employees against possible immoral behavior
within the business context. This might be accomplished in new
employee socialization (Ashforth & Anand, 2003), as well as in
employee training (Frisque & Kolb, 2008). To the extent that
organizations are aware of the contextual cues they propagate,
perhaps there is power to improve moral behavior within their
boundaries.
Last, we acknowledge that for some these findings might pro-
vide an excuse or a justification for immoral actions. For example,
Non-competitive cue
Probability
of acting
immorally
Business is
immoral
1.0
Business is
moral
Competitive cue
Figure 1. The interaction of implicit assumption and contextual cue on
immoral behavior.
Table 5
Binary Logistic Regression Results for the Hierarchically Well-Formulated Model in Study 2
Variable
Model 1 Model 2
BSEExp(B)BSEExp(B)
Constant 2.95 1.84 0.05 8.04
4.11 0.00
Contextual cue (condition) 0.12 0.49 0.89 7.40 4.75 1629.10
Explicit belief 0.22 0.30 1.24 1.12 0.66 3.06
Implicit assumption 0.68 0.59 1.97 1.95 5.77 0.14
Contextual Cue Explicit Belief 1.40 0.79 0.25
Contextual Cue Implicit Assumption 3.50
ⴱⴱ
1.35 33.11
Explicit Belief Implicit Assumption 0.14 0.91 1.14
2 log likelihood 99.65
Note. n 126. Unstandardized regression coefficients are shown. Dependent variable equals claim amount.
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
758 RESEARCH REPORTS
recent theoretical work suggests that certain occupations are reg-
ularly associated with implicit beliefs that can “morally seduce”
employees to do wrong (Moore, Tetlock, Tanlu, & Bazerman,
2006, p. 3). Whereas our research may help to explain immoral
behavior, it is not an excuse or a justification for immoral behav-
ior. We urge researchers to explore these issues to find strategies
to prevent such behaviors.
In closing, we believe that our research represents a promising
new approach to understanding individual moral behavior in or-
ganizations. In our opinion, the potential contributions of such an
approach are not just empirical but also theoretical and practical.
For these reasons, we are hopeful that more research along these
lines will dramatically increase our knowledge of moral behavior
in organizations.
References
Arkes, H. R., & Tetlock, P. E. (2004). Attributions of implicit prejudice, or
“Would Jesse Jackson ‘fail’ the Implicit Association Test?” Psycholog-
ical Inquiry, 15, 257–278.
Ashforth, B. E., & Anand, V. (2003). The normalization of corruption in
organizations. In R. M. Kramer & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in
organizational behavior (pp. 1–52). Amsterdam, the Netherlands:
Elsevier.
Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitudes can be measured. In H. L. Roedeger
III, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of
remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 117–150).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Banaji, M., Bazerman, M., & Chugh, D. (2003). How (un)ethical are you?
Harvard Business Review, 81, 56 64.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral
change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.
Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhuman-
ities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 193–209.
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer Jr.
(Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition
(Vol. 10, pp. 1– 61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bargh, J. A. (2006). What have we been priming all these years? On the
development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behav-
ior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 147–168.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of
being. American Psychologist, 54, 462– 479.
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social
behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on
action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230 –244.
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A. Y., Barndollar, K., &
Troetschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and
pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 81, 1014 –1027.
Brass, D. J., Butterfield, K. D., & Skaggs, B. C. (1998). Relationships and
unethical behavior: A social network perspective. Academy of Manage-
ment Review, 23, 14 –31.
Caruso, E., Epley, N., & Bazerman, M. H. (2006). The costs and benefits
of undoing egocentric responsibility assessments in groups. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 857– 871.
Chatman, J. A. (1989). Improving interactional organizational research: A
model of person– organization fit. Academy of Management Review, 14,
333–349.
Chugh, D. (2004). Societal and managerial implications of implicit social
cognition: Why milliseconds matter. Social Justice Research, 17, 203–
222.
Chugh, D., Banaji, M., & Bazerman, M. (2005). Bounded ethicality as a
psychological barrier to recognizing conflicts of interest. In D. Moore,
D. Cain, G. Loewenstein, & M. Bazerman (Eds.), Conflicts of interest:
Problems and solutions from law, medicine and organizational settings
(pp. 74 –95). London, England: Cambridge University Press.
Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception– behavior express-
way: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. In M. P.
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33, pp.
1– 40). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Donaldson, T. (2003). Editor’s comments: Taking ethics seriously—A
mission now more possible. Academy of Management Review, 28, 363–
366.
Ford, R. C., & Richardson, W. D. (1994). Ethical decision-making: A
review of the literature. Journal of Business Ethics, 13, 205–221.
Freeman, R. E. (1984). Strategic management: A stakeholder approach.
Boston, MA: Pitman.
Friedman, M. (with Friedman, R. D.). (1962). Capitalism and freedom.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Frisque, D., & Kolb, J. A. (2008). The effects of an ethics training program
on attitude, knowledge, and transfer of training of office professionals:
A treatment- and control-group design. Human Resource Development
Quarterly, 19, 35–53.
George, B. (2007). True north: Discover your authentic leadership. San
Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass.
Ghoshal, S., & Moran, P. (1996). Bad for practice: A critique of the
transaction cost theory. Academy of Management Review, 21, 13– 47.
Godfrey, P. C. (2005). The relationship between corporate philanthropy
and shareholder wealth: A risk management perspective. Academy of
Management Review, 30, 777–798.
Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment
work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 517–523.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition:
Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102,
4 –27.
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek,
B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes,
stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109,
3–25.
Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the Implicit Associa-
tion Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 79, 1022–1038.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring
individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association
Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464 –1480.
Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2001). Health of the Implicit Associ-
ation Test at age 3. Zeitschrift fu¨ r Experimentelle Psychologie, 48,
85–93.
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding
and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algo-
rithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.
Greenwald, A. G., Oakes, M. A., & Hoffman, H. (2003). Targets of
discrimination: Effects of race on responses to weapons holders. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 399 405.
Greenwald, A. G., Rudman, L. A., Nosek, B. A., & Zayas, V. (2006). Why
so little faith? A reply to Blanton and Jaccard’s (2006) skeptical view of
testing pure multiplicative theories. Psychological Review, 113, 170
180.
Greenwald, A. G., Uhlmann, E. L., Poehlman, T. A., & Banaji, M. R.
(2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III:
Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 97, 17– 41.
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuition-
ist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814 834.
Hambrick, D. C., & Mason, P. A. (1984). Upper echelons: The organiza-
759
RESEARCH REPORTS
tion as a reflection of its top managers. Academy of Management
Review, 9, 193–206.
Hume, D. (1983). An enquiry concerning the principles of morals. India-
napolis, IN: Hackett. (Original work published 1751)
Jaccard, J. (2001). Interaction effects in logistic regression. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organiza-
tions: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management Review, 16,
366 –395.
Kant, I. (1994). Grounding for the metaphysics of morals. In J. W.
Ellington (Trans.), Ethical philosophy (2nd ed.). Indianapolis, IN: Hack-
ett. (Original work published 1785)
Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S. C., Bargh, J. A., & Ross, L. (2004). Material
priming: The influence of mundane physical objects on situational
construal and competitive behavioral choice. Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 95, 83–96.
Kish-Gephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., & Trevin˜ o, L. K. (2010). Bad apples,
bad cases, and bad barrels: Meta-analytic evidence about sources of
unethical decisions at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 1–31.
Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development. San Francisco,
CA: Harper & Row.
Leavitt, K. N., Fong, C. T., & Greenwald, A. G. (2007, August). Revisiting
the satisfaction/performance linkage: Implicit core-job attitudes and
performance. Paper presented at the meeting of the Academy of Man-
agement, Philadelphia, PA.
Mehrabian, A. (1996). Relations among political attitudes, personality, and
psychopathology assessed with new measures of libertarianism and
conservatism. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 18, 469 491.
Moore, D. A., Tetlock, P. E., Tanlu, L., & Bazerman, M. H. (2006).
Conflicts of interest and the case of auditor independence: Moral seduc-
tion and strategic issue cycling. Academy of Management Review, 31,
10 –29.
Morrison, E. W., & Milliken, F. J. (2000). Organizational silence: A barrier
to change and development in a pluralistic world. Academy of Manage-
ment Review, 25, 706 –725.
O’Fallon, M. J., & Butterfield, K. D. (2005). A review of the empirical
ethical decision-making literature: 1996 –2003. Journal of Business Eth-
ics, 59, 375– 413.
Pfeffer, J. (1981). Management as symbolic action: The creation and
maintenance of organizational paradigms. In L. L. Cummings & B. M.
Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (pp. 1–52). Green-
wich, CT: JAI Press.
Pfeffer, J. (1997). Pitfalls on the road to measurement: The dangerous
liaison of human resources with the ideas of accounting and finance.
Human Resource Management, 36, 357–365.
Pfeffer, J., & Salancik. G. (1978). The external control of organizations: A
resource dependence perspective. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory.
New York, NY: Praeger.
Reynolds, S. J. (2006). A neurocognitive model of the ethical decision-
making process: Implications for study and practice. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 91, 737–748.
Reynolds, S. J., Schultz, F. C., & Hekman, D. R. (2006). Balancing
stakeholder interests: The constraining effects of resource divisibility
and stakeholder saliency. Journal of Business Ethics, 64, 285–301.
Ross, W. D. (1930). The right and the good. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett.
Schneider, B. (1983). Interactional psychology and organizational behav-
ior. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational
behavior (pp. 1–31). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human
information processing: Detection, search, and attention. Psychological
Review, 84, 1– 66.
Singer, P. (Ed.). (1994). Ethics. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Sonenshein, S. (2007). The role of construction, intuition, and justification
in responding to ethical issues at work: The sensemaking–intuition
model. Academy of Management Review, 32, 1022–1040.
Suchman, M. C. (1995). Managing legitimacy: Strategic and institutional
approaches. Academy of Management Review, 20, 571– 610.
Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A theory
of delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22, 664 670.
Trevin˜ o, L. K., Weaver, G. W., & Reynolds, S. J. (2006). Behavioral ethics
in organizations: A review. Journal of Management, 32, 951–990.
Uhlmann, E., Poehlman, T. A., & Bargh, J. A. (2007, August). Inherited
ideology: Implicit Puritanism in American moral cognition. Paper pre-
sented at the meeting of the Academy of Management, Philadelphia, PA.
Wade-Benzoni, K. A., Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Bazerman, M. H. (1996).
Egocentric interpretations of fairness in asymmetric, environmental so-
cial dilemmas: Explaining harvesting behavior & the role of communi-
cation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67,
111–126.
Weaver, G. R., Trevin˜ o, L. K., & Cochran, P. L. (1999). Integrated and
decoupled corporate social performance: Management commitments,
external pressures, and corporate ethics practices. Academy of Manage-
ment Journal, 42, 539 –552.
Weick, K. E. (1995). Sensemaking in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Received June 2, 2008
Revision received February 15, 2010
Accepted February 22, 2010
760 RESEARCH REPORTS
... For example, Reynolds et al. (2010), who also employed the IAT, proposed that individuals form normative associations about businesses, which means that a business could be implicitly assumed to be inherently moral or immoral. They argued that if a person holds an implicit assumption that a business is inherently moral and receives a contextual cue that is compatible with this, the cue will strengthen the individual's implicit beliefs on the outcome behavior. ...
... We used the in-basket exercise adapted from Reynolds et al. (2010) in which participants are asked to imagine themselves as employees of a fictitious company and engage three tasks, two of which are not related to unethical behavior, and one is related to unethical behavior (the insurance claim exercise). ...
... In contrast, values above the product's cost (values over $100,000) are based on illegitimate standards and were therefore deemed unethical (cf. Reynolds et al., 2010). In addition, the value reported was used as a continuous indicator so that higher values reported in the reimbursement indicated higher levels of unethical behavior. ...
Article
The literature on ethical behavior has called for studies that investigate the interaction between individual and contextual factors. This study examined whether moral identity interacts with ethical culture to predict unethical behavior at work and whether implicit and explicit moral identity affects unethical behavior distinctively. Our sample consisted of 238 participants who took part in an experiment involving an in-basket exercise that measured unethical behavior. Ethical culture was manipulated via a cover letter from a fictitious company’s CEO, and moral identity was measured through a self-report scale and the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Findings indicate that implicit and explicit moral identity were negatively associated with unethical behavior and that organizational culture moderated the relationship between moral identity and unethical behavior. The results have theoretical and practical implications for understanding the interaction of predictors of unethical behavior by combining automatic and deliberate measures of moral identity with ethical culture.
... The assumption is that people sufficiently educated about AI tools and how to use them will do so aligned with what they perceive as greater benefits and lesser harms (or can be held accountable for failure to do so). However, past research suggests that contingencies to support ethical AI use (and prevent misuse) will be needed in addition to simply AI education (e.g., Cox, 2021;Fauzihardani et al., 2019;Kish-Gephart et al., 2010;Reynolds et al., 2010). ...
Article
Ethics is fundamentally concerned with claims of “right,” “wrong,” “good,” “bad,” and how we might know those claims are accurate. Artificial intelligence (AI) is a term that represents a suite of tools where nonbiological systems process data and information to generate an output considered “intelligent.” As with any suite of technologies, choosing the right tool requires the tool user to critically evaluate which tool is best for the task (if one exists). Harnessing the power of AI systems to maximize benefit and minimize harm requires basic AI literacy. AI literacy requires a basic understanding of how mathematical models function. All future behavior analysts will need to be AI literate. This will require changes to education and training programs to ensure students have a basic understanding of model building, especially as we wait for the scholarship and research to unfold that outlines AI literacy skills specific to behavior analysts.
... Lilienfeld et al., 2000), recent research follows from the dual process theories that differentiate controlled and automatic determinants of judgements and behaviours (Epstein, 1994;Kahneman, 2011;Strack & Deutsch, 2004), and accompanying development of measurement techniques that intend to capture implicit social cognitions (Fazio & Olson, 2003;James & LeBreton, 2012). The trend has already impacted organizational psychology/organizational behaviour literature (Uhlmann et al., 2012) as work showing important implicit determinants of organizational behaviour has been reported in the fields of employment discrimination (Ziegert & Hanges, 2005), goal setting (Shantz & Latham, 2009;Stajkovic et al., 2006), workplace affect (Barsade et al., 2009), and moral behaviour (Reynolds et al., 2010). Interestingly, recent review of the use of implicit measures in leadership research (Chong et al., 2017) showed that the use of implicit measures in that field is "quite rare" which lead the authors to conclude that "the failure of leadership research to adopt implicit measures . . . ...
... Ethics perceptions are shaped by the broader context, with the context influencing the salience of situational factors and shaping the ethical meanings individuals attribute to events and values (Johns, 2006;Reynolds et al., 2010). Within an organizational setting, shared perceptions of what is ethical crystalize into what is known as an EWC. ...
Article
Full-text available
Workplace ethics perceptions drive ethical behaviors, but our understanding of how context shapes the nature of this relationship is limited. Consequently, this article uses contingency theory to explore how perceptions of ethical priorities in the workplace—ethical work climate (EWC)—are differentially associated with ethical behavior based on the broader context. Specifically, we meta-analytically test theoretically relevant cultural values (i.e., collectivism, power distance) and work context factors (i.e., consequence of errors, job autonomy) as moderators of the connection between EWC perceptions and ethical behavior. Our findings reveal that while higher power-distance societal contexts strengthen the connection between EWC perceptions and ethical behavior, work contexts with higher autonomy weaken the connection. We found mixed support for collectivism as a contextual moderator, with results supporting theoretical expectations with one cultural conceptualization but not another. Contrary to expectations, the consequences of errors in work contexts did not moderate the association between EWC perceptions and ethical behavior. Taken together, this meta-analytic investigation highlights the meaningful role of the broader organizational context as a potential boundary condition for the influence of EWC perceptions on ethical behavior.
... Interpersonal factors, such as social norms, team governance, perceived justice, trust and leader-member exchange (LMX) can contribute to DB. Studies involving organizational elements also show the relationships with DB, covering organizational climate, organizational support, participative decision-making and so on (Ishaque, 2020;Marcus et al., 2016;Reynolds et al., 2010). This progress may beg the question of how to improve effective DB management under heterogeneous contexts. ...
Article
Purpose-The deviant behavior (DB) of construction workers has always been a troubling event in project management. Although scholars continue to search for the main causes of this behavior to curb it at the source, the authors know less about the role and contribution of the team. This study aims to uncover the mechanisms and conditions under which collective moral judgment focus on self (CMJS) effectively enhances DB. Design/methodology/approach-Adopting Chinese construction enterprises as samples, a hierarchical linear model (HLM) is used to test the results of the hypothesis. Moderated mediating effects are used to analyze the potential mechanisms and boundary conditions of DB. Findings-The results of the HLM analysis show that CMJS could directly and significantly induce DB, and moral disengagement (MD) plays a mediator role in this association. In addition, the positive relationship between MD and DB is stronger when performance-avoidance goal orientation (PaGO) or overqualification (Overq) is higher. Research limitations/implications-The conditions and mechanisms that influence DB are not unique. Future study could examine the explanatory and weighting mechanisms of DB from other perspectives or to construct a framework and summarize the factors that may influence DB. Practical implications-This study provides a rich theoretical basis for the prevention and correction of construction workers' DB in Chinese construction firms from the perspective of CMJS. In addition, objective moral judgments contribute to guiding employees' moral cognitive processes and positive work. Originality/value-This study extends existing research on DB and advances the practical outcomes of construction project governance. It not only illustrates that CMJS has a direct impact on DB but also clarifies the mechanisms and conditions that predispose to the generation of DB, filling the research gap on construction workers' DB from cross-level mechanisms and also enriching the theoretical system for preventing this behavior.
Chapter
This chapter engages with contemporary moral psychology via the work of Jonathan Haidt, with the intention being to strike a dialogue and exchange between contemporary sociological and psychological accounts of morality. It uses Haidt’s work to explore key arguments in moral psychology regarding moral judgement, as well as Haidt’s specific arguments about innateness and moral foundations. It is argued that Haidt—and contemporary moral psychology more generally—provides crucial insights into moral judgements and into diversity in how the moral domain is understood. Haidt’s key positions of social intuitionism and moral foundation theory are expounded in sections one and two. Section three argues that there are limitations to the extent to which moral action can be understood as being intuitively driven, especially with respect to complex (but relatively ordinary) moral decisions in everyday life contexts. It is then argued that important dimensions of social relationships and power need to be given greater credence in Haidt’s account. This relative absence causes limitations for explaining moral change in the moral foundations framework. The conclusion explores potential issues that emerge from moral accounts that claim to be purely descriptive and normatively neutral.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: This study was conducted with the aim of developing, validation, reliability, and normalization of moral behavior scale among adolescents. Research Methodology: The study was carried out in two phases. In the first phase (qualitative part), the existing theoretical foundations of moral behaviors were studied and categorized by the use of the content analysis method. In the second phase (the quantitative part), based on the findings of content analysis, a 35-items scale was developed to measure two identified factors called negative and positive behaviors. The research method was descriptive-correlational, and the statistical population included all adolescents of Hamedan in the academic year of 2021-2022. The sample size included 1093 people who were randomly selected through multi-stage cluster sampling method, and they completed the researcher-made scale. To examine the validity of the scale, exploratory factor analysis, content validity method, and concurrent validity, and to investigate the reliability of the scale Cronbach alpha were used. Findings: The results of exploratory factor analysis specified 2 factors named negative and positive behaviors. The estimated Cronbach alpha reliability coefficients for the whole scale was 0.862, and subscales of negative and positive behaviors were 0.918, 0.920 respectively. Normative information of this scale is also reported. Conclusion: According to the results, moral behavior scale has suitable validity and reliability, and can be used in various organizations.
Article
Individuals tend to hold a dim view of for-profit corporations, believing that profit-seeking comes at the expense of ethicality. In the present research, we show that this belief is not universal; rather, people associate ethicality with an organization's size. Across nine experiments (N = 4,796), people stereotyped large companies as less ethical than small companies. This size-ethicality stereotype emerged spontaneously (Study 1), implicitly (Study 2), and across industries (Study 3). Moreover, we find this stereotype can be partly explained by perceptions of profit-seeking behavior (Supplementary Studies A and B), and that people construe profit-seeking and its relationship to ethicality differently when considering large and small companies (Study 4). People attribute greater profit-maximizing motives (relative to profit-satisficing motives) to large companies, and these attributions shape their subsequent judgments of ethicality (Study 5; Supplementary Studies C and D).
Article
Full-text available
Social behavior is ordinarily treated as being under conscious (if not always thoughtful) control. However, considerable evidence now supports the view that social behavior often operates in an implicit or unconscious fashion. The identifying feature of implicit cognition is that past experience influences judgment in a fashion not introspectively known by the actor. The present conclusion—that attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes have important implicit modes of operation—extends both the construct validity and predictive usefulness of these major theoretical constructs of social psychology. Methodologically, this review calls for increased use of indirect measures—which are imperative in studies of implicit cognition. The theorized ordinariness of implicit stereotyping is consistent with recent findings of discrimination by people who explicitly disavow prejudice. The finding that implicit cognitive effects are often reduced by focusing judges’ attention on their judgment task provides a basis for evaluating applications (such as affirmative action) aimed at reducing such unintended discrimination.
Article
Full-text available
Research on moral judgment has been dominated by rationalist models, in which moral judgment is thought to be caused by moral reasoning. The author gives 4 reasons for considering the hypothesis that moral reasoning does not cause moral judgment; rather, moral reasoning is usually a post hoc construction, generated after a judgment has been reached. The social intuitionist model is presented as an alternative to rationalist models. The model is a social model in that it deemphasizes the private reasoning done by individuals and emphasizes instead the importance of social and cultural influences. The model is an intuitionist model in that it states that moral judgment is generally the result of quick, automatic evaluations (intuitions). The model is more consistent than rationalist models with recent findings in social, cultural, evolutionary, and biological psychology, as well as in anthropology and primatology.
Article
Full-text available
Presents an integrative theoretical framework to explain and to predict psychological changes achieved by different modes of treatment. This theory states that psychological procedures, whatever their form, alter the level and strength of self-efficacy. It is hypothesized that expectations of personal efficacy determine whether coping behavior will be initiated, how much effort will be expended, and how long it will be sustained in the face of obstacles and aversive experiences. Persistence in activities that are subjectively threatening but in fact relatively safe produces, through experiences of mastery, further enhancement of self-efficacy and corresponding reductions in defensive behavior. In the proposed model, expectations of personal efficacy are derived from 4 principal sources of information: performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and physiological states. Factors influencing the cognitive processing of efficacy information arise from enactive, vicarious, exhortative, and emotive sources. The differential power of diverse therapeutic procedures is analyzed in terms of the postulated cognitive mechanism of operation. Findings are reported from microanalyses of enactive, vicarious, and emotive modes of treatment that support the hypothesized relationship between perceived self-efficacy and behavioral changes. (21/2 p ref)
Article
Full-text available
Individuals working in groups often egocentrically believe they have contributed more of the total work than is logically possible. Actively considering others’ contributions effectively reduces these egocentric assessments, but this research suggests that undoing egocentric biases in groups may have some unexpected costs. Four experiments demonstrate that members who contributed much to the group outcome are actually less satisfied and less interested in future collaborations after considering others’ contributions compared with those who contributed little. This was especially true in cooperative groups. Egocentric biases in responsibility allocation can create conflict, but this research suggests that undoing these biases can have some unfortunate consequences. Some members who look beyond their own perspective may not like what they see.
Chapter
This interdisciplinary work is a collection of major essays on reasoning: deductive, inductive, abductive, belief revision, defeasible (non-monotonic), cross cultural, conversational, and argumentative. They are each oriented toward contemporary empirical studies. The book focuses on foundational issues, including paradoxes, fallacies, and debates about the nature of rationality, the traditional modes of reasoning, as well as counterfactual and causal reasoning. It also includes chapters on the interface between reasoning and other forms of thought. In general, this last set of essays represents growth points in reasoning research, drawing connections to pragmatics, cross-cultural studies, emotion and evolution.
Book
Cambridge Core - Philosophy Texts - Kant: The Metaphysics of Morals - edited by Lara Denis
Chapter
The purpose of this chapter is to outline the development of the idea of "stakeholder management" as it has come to be applied in strategic management. We begin by developing a brief history of the concept. We then suggest that traditionally the stakeholder approach to strategic management has several related characteristics that serve as distinguishing features. We review recent work on stakeholder theory and suggest how stakeholder management has affected the practice of management. We end by suggesting further research questions.
Article
This analysis considers the impact of the top managers in an organization on the organization's outcomes, specifically strategic choices and performance levels. The focus is not on the chief executive alone, but rather on the entire top management team. Using a macro view, these organizational outcomes are perceived to be related to the values and cognitive bases of those high-power individuals in the organization. In developing the model, emphasis is on the background characteristics of the top managers as opposed to the psychological dimensions. A series of propositions that should be tested to support the upper echelons theory are presented. The topics of these propositions include age, functional track, other career experiences, education, socioeconomic roots, financial position, and group characteristics. The creation of this model is just the beginning of the work that is necessary to evaluate and understand the upper echelons theory. Further input is needed from areas such as the executive recruiting industry. Additionally, clinical and statistical studies are both necessary to fully develop this theory. (SRD)