ArticlePDF Available

Looking Online for the Best Romantic Partner Reduces Decision Quality: The Moderating Role of Choice-Making Strategies

Authors:

Abstract

The Internet has become a means by which people expand their social networks and form close relationships. Wu and Chiou (2009) demonstrated that more search options triggered excessive searching, leading to poorer decision making and reduced selectivity in finding partners for online romantic relationships. Regarding the more-means-worse effect, they argued that more searching leads to worse choices by reducing users' cognitive resources, distracting them with irrelevant information, and reducing their ability to screen out inferior options. Expanding Simon's (1955) seminal theory, this research compared choice-making strategies of maximizers and satisficers on excessive searching, quality of final decisions, and selectivity. One hundred twelve adolescents with experiences of online romantic relationships participated in an experimental study. Participants were administered a scale that measured maximizing tendencies and were then assigned to receive either a small or a large number of options. Results indicated that the participants with high maximizing tendencies (i.e., maximizers) showed more pronounced searching than did those with low maximizing tendencies (i.e., satisficers). The negative effect of excessive searching on decision-making was more prominent for maximizers than for satisficers in terms of final choices and selectivity. These findings reveal that adopting maximizing strategies may increase vulnerability stemming from excessive searching when a large number of choices are available.
RAPID COMMUNICATION
Looking Online for the Best Romantic Partner
Reduces Decision Quality: The Moderating Role
of Choice-Making Strategies
Mu-Li Yang, Ph.D.,
1
and Wen-Bin Chiou, Ph.D.
2
Abstract
The Internet has become a means by which people expand their social networks and form close relationships.
Wu and Chiou (2009) demonstrated that more search options triggered excessive searching, leading to poorer
decision making and reduced selectivity in finding partners for online romantic relationships. Regarding the
more-means-worse effect, they argued that more searching leads to worse choices by reducing users’ cognitive
resources, distracting them with irrelevant information, and reducing their ability to screen out inferior options.
Expanding Simon’s (1955) seminal theory, this research compared choice-making strategies of maximizers and
satisficers on excessive searching, quality of final decisions, and selectivity. One hundred twelve adolescents
with experiences of online romantic relationships participated in an experimental study. Participants were
administered a scale that measured maximizing tendencies and were then assigned to receive either a small or a
large number of options. Results indicated that the participants with high maximizing tendencies (i.e., maxi-
mizers) showed more pronounced searching than did those with low maximizing tendencies (i.e., satisficers).
The negative effect of excessive searching on decision-making was more prominent for maximizers than for
satisficers in terms of final choices and selectivity. These findings reveal that adopting maximizing strategies
may increase vulnerability stemming from excessive searching when a large number of choices are available.
Introduction
Online social interactions have become one of the
predominant reasons for Internet use.
1
As online social
and personal relationships become more prevalent,
2,3
select-
ing and building interpersonal romantic relationships in cy-
berspace is an important issue. Previous studies have
investigated the prevalence and demographics of Internet
users,
4,5
the quality of relationships formed,
6
predictors of
relationship satisfaction,
7
and the psychological correlates of
users,
8,9
but they have rarely investigated how Internet users
select cyber friends for romantic relationships from a cogni-
tive perspective.
10
Online social networking sites provide search tools to en-
able members to find and evaluate the cyber profiles of their
friends and other people they might know. These tools ap-
pear to reduce the effort required by users to find potential
partners and to help them make good predictions about the
fit of any given individual to their romantic preferences.
However, Wu and Chiou
10
demonstrated that more options
triggered excessive searching and decreased the quality of
choices made. From a cognitive information-processing per-
spective, having access to more possible partners triggered
additional searches, thus partially undoing the effort-saving
benefit of search. Several reasons have been proposed for this
‘more-means-worse’’ effect in which more searching results
in worse choices being made. First, considering a large set of
options may increase cognitive load, leading individuals to
make mistakes.
11
In addition, when searchers’ cognitive re-
sources are reduced by more searches, they may be less likely
to ignore irrelevant information and more likely to be dis-
tracted or attracted by attributes that were not pertinent to
their original preferences.
12
Moreover, searching through
more options may lead users to accelerate processing by re-
ducing the amount of time spent on each alternative profile.
Such self-induced time pressure can lessen users’ ability to
distinguish between better and worse options.
13
Half a century ago, Simon
14,15
introduced an important
distinction between maximizing and satisficing as choice-
making strategies. To maximize is to seek the best and re-
quires an exhaustive search of all possibilities. To satisfice
is to seek ‘‘good enough,’’ searching until encountering an
1
Department of Mass Communication, Chang Jung Christian University, Tainan, Taiwan, Republic of China.
2
Institute of Education, National Sun Yat-Sen University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan, Republic of China.
CYBERPSYCHOLOGY &BEHAVIOR
Volume 13, Number 0, 2009
ªMary Ann Liebert, Inc.
DOI: 10.1089=cpb.2009.0208
1
CPB-2009-0208-Yang_1P
Type: rapid-communication
CPB-2009-0208-Yang_1P.3D 10/09/09 2:25am Page 1
option that crosses the threshold of acceptability. Expanding
on Simon’s classic theory, Schwartz et al.
16
asked participants
about recent purchasing decisions and used a maximization
scale to measure individual differences in maximizing ten-
dencies. Compared with satisficers, maximizers were more
likely to engage in an exhaustive search of all available op-
tions. Such differences in the subjective choice-making ex-
periences of maximizers and satisficers are attributed to the
fact that maximizers create a more onerous choice-making
process for themselves. Initially, maximizers focus on in-
creasing their choice sets by exploring multiple options,
presumably because expanded choice sets allow greater op-
portunity to seek out and find the elusive best option. Yet as
the number of options proliferates, cognitive limitations
prevent decision making because it becomes impossible to
evaluate and compare all of the options.
17
Given the principle that more options might lead to more
searching and then result in worse choices,
10
we hypothe-
sized that maximizers would be more likely than satisficers to
employ excessive searching and that they would be more
vulnerable to the negative effect of excessive searching on
decision making. An experimental study was conducted to
examine whether choice-making strategies would moderate
the effects of available options on excessive searching and
decision making in the context of finding partners for online
romantic relationships.
Methods
Participants and design
Participants were 112 teenagers and late adolescents from
southern Taiwan (58 males; aged 15 to 23 years, M¼17.49,
SD ¼2.69) who were single and members of an online-friends
Web site. They were recruited from the participant pool of an
online gaming-addiction survey supported by the National
Science Council of Taiwan and were selected using a
screening questionnaire.
18
Participants were submitted to a
22 (number of available options: large or small number of
options by choice-making strategies: maximizers or satisfi-
cers) between-participants design.
Procedure
The present study modeled Wu and Chiou’s
10
study to
investigate the moderating role of choice-making strategies
on the more-means-worse effect. To disguise its actual pur-
pose, the experiment was presented as a study on ‘‘Finding
Your Best Partner for a Romantic Relationship.’’ Participants
were asked to search for their most desirable romantic part-
ners using the search tool of an online-dating Web site,
available through Yahoo Taiwan. Each participant first en-
tered the characteristics of his or her ideal partner in terms of
16 conditions (i.e., age, height, weight, educational level,
vocation, smoking habits, drinking habits, religion, geo-
graphical area, horoscope, blood type, appearance, person-
ality, interests, travel preferences, food preferences). The data
were used to compute the preference difference scores be-
tween desirable partners and final choices. Participants then
completed a scale that measured maximizing strategies.
Later, participants saw a list of recommended partners
given by the search engine. The list was characterized by brief
titles and nicknames, forcing participants to view the entire
profile of each recommended partner to assess the goodness-
of-match. Participants read the explanation of the search
engine’s ranked list of recommended partners. They were
randomly assigned to view either small or large numbers of
available profiles (i.e., the top 40 or the top 80 rankings from
the recommended list). They were asked to review the
available profiles to choose their target partner for a romantic
relationship. After the choice was made, participants were
given the background information about this experiment.
Maximizing strategies
Participants completed 11 maximization items drawn from
Schwartz et al.
16
(e.g., ‘‘When I am in the car listening to the
radio, I often check other stations to see if something better is
playing, even if I am relatively satisfied with what I’m lis-
tening to’’ and ‘‘When shopping, I have a hard time finding
clothes that I really love’’). Each item was rated on a scale
from 1, strongly disagree,to9,strongly agree. The Cronbach’s
alpha was 0.89 in the present study.
The median-split method was employed to classify par-
ticipants into maximizers or satisficers in terms of their
choice-making strategies. The median score on the maxi-
mizing tendencies scale was 5.30. Fifty-six participants were
assigned to the maximizers group, and 56 were assigned to
the satisficers group.
Number of available options
The recommended list screened by the search engine al-
ways provided hundreds of profiles. However, a preliminary
survey before the formal experiment (N¼78) indicated that
users of online-dating Web sites generally would not review
more than 100 profiles during a single search session for
online romantic relationships.
10
In addition, the study by Wu
and Chiou
10
demonstrated a linear trend in the effect of the
number of available options (i.e., the top 30 rankings, the top
60, or the top 90). Hence, this research only manipulated
small (top 40 ranking subjects) and large (top 80 rankings)
numbers of available options. Different numbers of options
were employed as an independent variable in order to ex-
pand the generalizability of the findings. As a manipulation
check, participants were asked to rate the perceived number
of available options they received on a 9-point scale from 1,
very few,to9,very many. A independent ttest showed that
participants perceived the ‘‘large number’’ condition,
M¼7.68, SD ¼0.77, to have more options than they did the
‘‘small number’’ condition, M¼2.29, SD ¼1.12; t(110) ¼29.69,
p<0.001.
Dependent measures
First, the search ratio was measured by dividing the
number of available options by the number of unique profiles
examined to determine whether providing more options
triggered more searching. Second, the goodness-of-match of
each target option was determined by measuring the differ-
ences between the scores of each participant’s most desired
characteristics and the characteristics of the selected option.
Possible scores for this preference difference ranged from 0 to
16, because we employed a dichotomous scale (0 ¼match;
1¼mismatch) for each of the 16 characteristics. Greater
preference differences represented worse choices. Finally, the
2 YANG AND CHIOU
CPB-2009-0208-Yang_1P.3D 10/09/09 2:25am Page 2
selectivity measure determined whether more attention was
devoted to better alternatives and less attention to worse al-
ternatives. The selectivity was computed for each participant
according to the method employed by Wu and Chiou,
10
that
is, the time spent reviewing an option (in minutes, recorded
to two decimal places) was regressed on the ‘‘match score’’ for
that option. The match score was computed by the sum of
match characteristics of a subject rated on a dichotomous
scale (0 ¼mismatch; 1 ¼match; possible scores ranged from 0
to 16). More positive unstandardized regression coefficients
indicated that a participant spent more time evaluating the
options with high goodness-of-match scores, implying better
selectivity.
Results
Participants’ performance in terms of the three dependent
measures (searching ratio, the preference difference for the
chosen option, and selectivity; see
T1 cTable 1) were submitted to
a22 (number of available options: small or large number of
options by choice-making strategies: maximizers or satisfi-
cers) between-participants model. ANOVAs were conducted
on the three dependent variables separately. Regarding dif-
ferences in the searching ratio of maximizers and satisficers, a
robust main effect of choice-making strategies was observed,
F(1, 108) ¼820.02, p<0.001, Z
2
¼0.88. Participants with high
maximizing tendencies exhibited a higher searching ratio,
M¼0.86, SD ¼0.04, than did participants with low maxi-
mizing tendencies, M¼0.65, SD ¼0.05, regardless of the
number of available options, F(1, 108) ¼1.71, p>0.05. This
finding was congruent with the salient characteristics of
maximizers in making decisions
16,17
and also indicated that
the measure of maximizing tendencies was valid. In addition,
a main effect of number of available options was observed,
F(1, 108) ¼820.02, p<0.001, Z
2
¼0.88, indicating that more
profiles were examined as more options were provided; this
also echoed the findings by Wu and Chiou.
10
As to the preference difference score of the chosen subject,
the main effect of number of available options indicated that
more options led to worse choices, F(1, 108) ¼244.74,
p<0.001, Z
2
¼0.69, which also replicated the previous finding
by Wu and Chiou.
10
More importantly, a significant interac-
tion between choice-making strategies and number of avail-
able options was observed, F(1, 108) ¼50.98, p<0.01,
Z
2
¼0.32. Further analyses indicated that the preference dif-
ference scores were more affected by the number of available
options for maximizers, M
large
(10.71) >M
small
(5.64); F(1,
54) ¼277.20, p<0.001, Z
2
¼0.84, than for satisficers, M
large
(6.65) >M
small
(4.75); F(1, 54) ¼33.99, p<0.001, Z
2
¼0.39.
Selectivity in the context of this experiment refers to whe-
ther greater attention is allocated to better alternatives and is
indicated by more positive individual regression coefficients.
In accordance with the finding by Wu and Chiou
10
, partici-
pants’ selectivity was affected by the number of available
options, F(1, 108) ¼46.19, p<0.001, Z
2
¼0.30. This finding
indicated that more available options led to reduced selec-
tivity, revealing that participants’ selectivity was better under
the condition of a small number of available options,
M¼1.01, SD ¼0.15, and worse with a large number of op-
tions, M¼0.83, SD ¼0.18. A similar pattern was observed in
the interaction between choice-making strategies and number
of available options, F(1, 108) ¼14.63, p<0.001, Z
2
¼0.12.
Further analyses indicated that the effect of number of
available options on selectivity was more pronounced for
maximizers, M
large
(0.70) <M
small
(0.97); F(1, 54) ¼63.03,
p<0.001, Z
2
¼0.54, whereas the effect was only marginally
significant for satisficers, M
large
(0.96) >M
small
(1.04); F(1,
54) ¼4.00, p<0.05, Z
2
¼0.07.
Discussion
Search tools on social networking Web sites may allow
users to find better preference matches from large datasets.
However, our findings support earlier research by Wu and
Chiou
10
and indicate that more options trigger additional
searches, thus partially undoing the effort-saving benefit of
search tools.
19
Furthermore, the more-means-worse effect
was more prominent for maximizers than for satisficers. To
maximize is to seek the best and requires an exhaustive search
of all possibilities. When maximizers’ cognitive resources are
reduced by more searches, they may be less likely to ignore
irrelevant information and more likely to be distracted by or
attracted to attributes that are not pertinent to their original
preferences.
12
Identifying the best from a large dataset of
considerations becomes increasingly difficult, compelling
maximizers to rely on external rather than internal standards
to evaluate and select outcomes.
20
Furthermore, the inevita-
bility of trade-offs among attractive options intensifies the
sting of passing up one attractive alternative when choosing
a more attractive one. The above reasons explain, from a
Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations of the Measures
Maximizers Satisficers
Small number of
options (40)
Large number of
options (80)
Small number of
options (40)
Large number of
options (80)
Measures Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Searching ratio 0.84 0.03 0.89 0.03 0.63 0.06 0.66 0.04
Preference difference of the chosen option 5.64 1.37 10.71 0.85 4.75 1.32 6.64 1.10
Selectivity 0.97 0.15 0.70 0.10 1.04 0.14 0.96 0.15
n¼28 for each experimental condition.
Search ratio ranged from 0 to 1.00. Preference difference scores for the chosen option ranged from 0 to 16 by the sum of the mismatch
characteristics of the selected subject.
The selectivity measure was obtained by each participant’s time spent inspecting a subject’s profile regressed on the match score of that
subject. More positive regression coefficients represent greater selectivity.
CHOICE-MAKING STRATEGIES AND SEARCH BIAS 3
CPB-2009-0208-Yang_1P.3D 10/09/09 2:25am Page 3
cognitive-processing perspective, why maximizers suffer
from worse decision making than that of satisficers.
In considering the limitations of this study and possible
future directions, it is noteworthy that maximizing tendencies
were treated as a global individual difference measure. It may
well be that maximizing search strategies are used for specific
kinds of decision-making tasks and are not a general tendency
for all decision-making tasks. Investigations of other decision-
making tasks (e.g., a job-searching process) may determine
whether maximizing tendencies are global or specific. Other
factors that might also trigger more searching and lead to
worse choices have not been examined. For example, accuracy
motivation can lead to more systematic processing and de-
crease susceptibility to biases.
13
Future studies may examine
whether accuracy motivation would produce even more ex-
cessive searching in seeking out romantic partners for online
relationships. Finally, Schwartz et al.
16
demonstrated that
maximizing tendencies were positively correlated with regret,
depression, and decision difficulty. It would be interesting to
examine whether maximizers are less satisfied than satisficers
and experience greater negative affect with the partners they
select through excessive searching.
The search tools of online-dating sites provide users with a
convenient way to search for potential romantic partners with
whom to build and develop online relationships. However, a
large number of available options may induce more searches,
and this may in turn lead to worse choices and poor selec-
tivity. Moreover, adopting a maximizing strategy appears to
expand the detrimental effect of excessive searching on in-
formation processing and decision making. Maximizers
should pay more attention to the negative consequences that
stem from search tools’ providing too much choice and
should use the search tools constructively to tailor their
decision-making criteria. When more searching is carried out
on online-dating sites, maximizers may neither get what they
want nor want what they get.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank the National Science
Council of the Republic of China for financially supporting
this research (contract No. NSC 98-2511-S-110-001-MY2).
Disclosure Statement
No competing financial interests exist.
References
1. Cummings JN, Butler B, Kraut R. The quality of online social
relationships. Communications of the ACM 2002; 45:103–08.
2. Parks MR, Floyd K. Making friends in cyberspace. Journal of
Communication 1996; 46:80–97.
3. Wildermuth SM. Love on the line: participants’ descriptions
of computer-mediated close relationships. Communication
Quarterly 2001; 49:89–95.
4. Nice ML, Katzev R. Internet romance: the frequency and
nature of romantic on-line relationships. CyberPsychology &
Behavior 1998; 1:217–23.
5. Wolak J, Mitchell KJ, Finkelhor D. Close online relationships
in a national sample of adolescents. Adolescence 2002; 37:
441–55.
6. Baker A. What makes an online relationship successful?
Clues from couples who met in cyberspace. CyberPsychol-
ogy & Behavior 2002; 5:363–75.
7. Anderson TL. (2000) Predicting communication satisfaction
and relationship satisfaction in online romantic relation-
ships. Doctoral dissertation, University of Oklahoma.
8. Stritzke WGK, Nguyen A, Durkin K. Shyness and computer-
mediated communication: a self-presentational theory per-
spective. Media Psychology 2004; 6:1–22.
9. Anderson TL. Relationships among Internet attitudes, In-
ternet use, romantic beliefs, and perceptions of online ro-
mantic relationships. CyberPsychology & Behavior 2005;
8:521–31.
10. Wu PL, Chiou WB. More options lead to more searching and
worse choices in finding partners for romantic relationships
online: an experimental study. CyberPsychology & Behavior
2009; 3:315–8.
11. Wilson TD, Schooler JW. Thinking too much: introspection
can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal
of Personality & Social Psychology 1991; 60:181–92.
12. Martin LL, Seta JJ, Crelia RA. Assimilation and contrast as a
function of people’s willingness and ability to expend effort
in forming an impression. Journal of Personality & Social
Psychology 1990; 59:27–37.
13. Payne JW, Bettman JR, Johnson EJ. Adaptive strategy se-
lection in decision making. Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology 1988; 14:534–42.
14. Simon HA. A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly
Journal of Economics 1955; 59:99–118.
15. Simon HA. Rational choice and the structure of the envi-
ronment. Psychological Review 1956; 63:129–38.
16. Schwartz B, Ward A, Monterosso J, et al. Maximizing versus
satisficing: happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of Per-
sonality & Social Psychology 2002; 83:1178–97.
17. Iyengar SS, Lepper MR. When choice is demotivating: Can
one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality
& Social Psychology 2000; 79:995–1006.
18. Chiou WB. Induced attitude change on online gaming
among adolescents: an application of the less-leads-to-more
effect. CyberPsychology & Behavior 2008; 2:212–6.
19. Ha
¨ubl G, Trifts V. Consumer decision making in online
shopping environments: the effects of interactive decision
aids. Marketing Science 2000; 19:4–21.
20. Lyubomirsky S, Ross L. Hedonic consequences of social
comparison: a contrast of happy and unhappy people.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 1997; 73:1141–57.
Address correspondence to:
Dr. Wen-Bin Chiou
Institute of Education
National Sun Yat-Sen University
70 Lien-Hai Rd.
Kaohsiung City, Taiwan 80424
Republic of China
E-mail: wbchiou@mail.nsysu.edu.tw
4 YANG AND CHIOU
CPB-2009-0208-Yang_1P.3D 10/09/09 2:25am Page 4
... Few studies have examined the differences in how maximizers and satisficers create and maintain their social relationships. Relative to satisficers, maximizers have been found to invest more time and resources in seeking and learning about their potential romantic partners (Yang & Chiou, 2010). Further, they tend to be dissatisfied with their partners and are less committed to them (Mikkelson et al., 2016;Mikkelson & Pauley, 2013). ...
... A personality trait is generally assumed to be a determinant of behavior. Moreover, maximizers have shown a tendency to invest their resources in finding a romantic partner in experimental settings (Yang & Chiou, 2010). However, the long-term stability of the maximizing tendency remains unexplored. ...
... If so, what criteria are used to find that person? Existing findings on the quality of maximizers' decision-making processes are inconsistent (Iyengar et al., 2006;Jain, Bearden, & Filipowicz, 2013;Parker et al., 2007;Yang & Chiou, 2010). Additionally, "the best spouse" is not well-defined. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined whether maximizing tendency predicts Japanese adults' likelihood of getting married and their age at first marriage. Registered members of a survey company panel (n = 428) completed the Japanese version of the Relational Maximization Scale, which was used to assess maximizing tendency consisting of two factors. The first factor, “high standards,” was not related to either of the two variables under investigation. The second factor, “alternative search,” was positively related to age at first marriage but not to the likelihood of getting married. The results thus suggest that those who seek the best possible marriage partner tend to get married at a later age, but that they are as likely to get married as those who do not. The finding that maximizing tendency did not significantly predict the likelihood of marriage suggests that getting married remains a strong social norm in Japan.
... partner choices made. 25 Consequently, Herbert Schnieider's theory of maximizing and satisfying as decision-making strategies appears to have renewed importance in the context of online dating. 26,27 The initiator advantage ...
... Settling for second best How the plethora of potential dating partners can lead to poorer partner selection outcomes [20][21][22][23][24][25][26][27] The initiator advantage The effects of being first party to initiate an online connection [28][29][30][31] Conversion rates Factors that lead to a higher likelihood of meeting in person 30,[32][33][34] Love at first byte Using social networking sites for online dating [35][36][37][38] Maintenance Public displays of affection How publicly announcing ones' relationship is associated with various relationship outcomes [40][41][42][43] Staying in-sync Perceived similarities or differences in outcomes 44 The green-eyed beast Technology-use behaviors associated with jealousy 37,45,46 Technoference When the use of technology interferes with relationships 6,47,48 Cybersexuality How technology is shaping sexual behaviors [49][50][51][52][53] Dissolution Getting over it Postbreakup behaviors that can help or harm post-traumatic growth 57,58 Cyberstalking Using technology to harass or stalk others [59][60][61][62][63][64][65] Other relevant literature on cyberintimacy that was included in the review [1][2][3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10][11][12][13][14][15][16][17][18][19]39,[54][55][56][66][67][68][69][70][71][72] CYBERINTIMACY: A SCOPING REVIEW 661 ...
Article
With a swipe, click, or delete, people are now able to seek and sustain romance in unprecedented ways. The emerging research shows that cyberintimacy, or the phenomenon of technology-mediated communication between partners and potential romantic interests, significantly impacts the way we form, maintain, and even extinguish romantic relationships. Hence, this scoping review aims to (a) delineate how our use of technology-mediated communication is associated with specific outcomes (e.g., satisfaction and quality) throughout the romantic relationship lifecycle, and (b) identify emerging themes in the research. Adhering to Arksey and O'Mayley's six-step framework methodology, we performed a systematic database search, literature screening, and qualitative synthesis of the findings. Seventy-two studies were selected from a pool of 4,062 articles derived from a systematic search of six academic databases. The findings show that cyberintimacy has a profound impact on outcomes in three crucial stages of the relationship lifecycle-from the way people find and field partners online through the process of initiation; to the means by which partners use technology to perform relationship maintenance; and finally, the ways in which people cope with the process of dissolution by using technology to reclaim a sense of agency.
... Seven studies only focused on online dating, and one considered both online dating and other types of online behaviours. Other authors (Yang & Chiou, 2010;Wu & Chiou, 2009) studied how people choosing a potential romantic partner online found they had more search options -offered by SNSs -which generally led to making worse choices distracting people with irrelevant information and reducing selectivity in finding a partner (Wu & Chiou, 2009). However, Yang and Chiou (2010) found that this effect was stronger for maximizer people (e.g. ...
... Other authors (Yang & Chiou, 2010;Wu & Chiou, 2009) studied how people choosing a potential romantic partner online found they had more search options -offered by SNSs -which generally led to making worse choices distracting people with irrelevant information and reducing selectivity in finding a partner (Wu & Chiou, 2009). However, Yang and Chiou (2010) found that this effect was stronger for maximizer people (e.g. «to seek the best and exhaustive search of all possibilities», p. 207) than for satisfiers (e.g. ...
Article
Full-text available
Social Network Sites (SNS) have changed the landscape for inter-personal communication and relationship maintenance (Hertlein & Stevenson, 2010). SNS is in fact a type of social media outlet, a web-based-service that allows maintenance of social relationships within one's publicly visible social network (Ellison, 2007). Connecting two billion users every day (Facebook, 2017), Facebook is one of today's most popular SNSs; its expansion has increased the scientific community's interest, giving social scientists the opportunity to observe behaviour, to test hypotheses , and to recruit participants in a naturalistic and new setting. In the first wide literature review of Facebook, Wilson, Gosling and Graham (2012) established five areas of research: characteristics of users, motivations, identity presentation strategies, privacy and information disclosure, and the role of Facebook on social interaction. However, the authors did not systematically distinguish the role of Facebook on friendship and on romantic social interactions. A recent attempt to analyse the role of SNSs on romantic relationships was pursued systematically by Rus and Tiemensma's (2017). With the aim of identifying theory-based approaches and methodological constructs (i.e., attachment style or romantic jealousy) used for studying the association between SNS use and romantic relationships , authors considered twenty-six peer-reviewed articles published between 2000 Empirical studies reveal Social Network Sites (SNS) have multiple applications related to intimate relationships. Through SNS, people can meet partners, maintain relationships, and portray commitment. Existing evidence includes both positive and negative outcomes for offline Romantic Relationships (oRRs). Nevertheless, unification and coherent frameworks on the relationship between SNSs' use and quality of oRR and factors influencing these relationships have not been provided. This was the aim of this systematic literature review that analysed a corpus of 65 peer-reviewed articles. Results from the study uncovered positive and negative outcomes of SNS on the quality of oRR in relation to online behaviour that is at least partially guided-i.e., moderated-by individual differences of the people involved, especially by the individual's attachment style. Imperato, C., & Mancini, T. (2019). «Couples in trouble» because of Social Networks? A systematic review. Psicologia sociale, 14(2), 165-204. https://doi.org/10.1482/94265
... Maximizers with higher levels of such maximizing tendencies are individuals who strive to make the best choice through an exhaustive search and comparison of alternatives (Schwartz et al., 2002;Cheek and Schwartz, 2016). They also tend to put great effort into the decision process by comparing more options (Schwartz et al., 2002;Chowdhury et al., 2009;Dar-Nimrod et al., 2009;Polman, 2010;Yang and Chiou, 2010) and spending more time (Schwartz et al., 2002;Chowdhury et al., 2009;Misuraca and Teuscher, 2013). On the other hand, satisficers seek to choose options that are "good enough" and stop searching once they identify choices that satisfy their standards (Schwartz et al., 2002;Cheek and Schwartz, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Maximization, the tendency to make the best choices by thoroughly searching and comparing alternatives, has long been considered a negative correlate of well-being. However, recently, it was proposed that having a maximizing tendency can lead to better coping efforts in some stressful situations and thus could be more adaptive. The objective of the present research was to demonstrate positive features of maximization and identify the coping strategies that mediate the relationship between maximization and well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Methods A sample of 3,493 participants responded to an online survey from January 20 to October 11 of 2020. The 13-item Maximization Scale assessed individuals’ maximizing tendency in terms of the maximization index and its three subdimensions (i.e., high standards, alternative search, and decision difficulty). The use of four coping strategies (i.e., preventive measures, cognitive appraisal, self-distraction, and social connection) during the COVID-19 pandemic was assessed. Hedonic and eudaimonic aspects of well-being were measured. We developed a mediation model and examined both whether there was an indirect link between maximization and well-being through the coping strategies and whether there was a direct link between maximization and well-being. Results Path analysis revealed negative direct associations between maximization measures (i.e., an index and three subdimensions) and well-being. In addition, significant indirect paths were found with varying directions depending on maximization dimensions and coping strategy types. There were positive indirect associations between the maximization index and well-being via preventive measures, between high standards and well-being through preventive measures and cognitive appraisal, and between alternative search and well-being through self-distraction and social connection. Negative indirect associations were found between decision difficulty and well-being through cognitive appraisal, self-distraction, and social connection. Discussion The current study confirmed the existence of inconsistent mediation effects between maximization and well-being via coping and highlighted coping efforts as one of the positive aspects of maximization. Discussion addressed the double-edged effect of maximization on well-being and its beneficial nature in times of distress. Future studies should examine other potential situations and moderators that can delineate maximization’s various characteristics with a longitudinal design and samples from diverse backgrounds.
... Consistent with this definition, maximizers search through more options before making a choice (Chowdury et al., 2009;Dar-Nimrod et al., 2009;Polman, 2010;Yang & Chiou, 2010). In fact, maximizers are willing to sacrifice resources (e.g., time and money) to view more options before making a choice (Dar-Nimrod et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Normative standards refer to ideals to which people, products, and organizations are held. The present research (N = 2,224) investigates a novel construct—the breadth of normative standards, or the number of criteria that normative standards need to meet. Using archival and primary data in both organizational and consumer contexts, Studies 1–2 found that Indians’ and Singaporeans’ normative standards in several domains (e.g., a good job, a good body wash) needed to satisfy more criteria than those of Americans and the British. Using incentive-compatible designs, Studies 3–5 identified two downstream consequences of broader normative standards; decision-makers with broader standards pay greater attention to detail when evaluating others’ work, and people with broader standards search for more options, even at a cost, before making a choice. This research complements past work on norms as prevalent behaviors, values, and attitudes by examining norms as standards, and documents consequences of the breadth of normative standards for employees and organizations.
... However, the extent to which individuals experience choice overload in a negative way depends on their decision-making style; specifically, their inclinations to maximize versus satisfice. Maximizers, or those who tend to search exhaustively for the best possible option, spend more time and effort making decisions than do satisficers, who only search minimally to find an adequate option (e.g., Chowdury et al., 2009;Dar-Nimrod et al., 2009;Polman, 2010;Schwartz et al., 2002;Yang & Chiou, 2010). Despite this additional time and effort, maximizers are generally less satisfied with their choices than satisficers (Chowdhury et al., 2009;Dahling & Thompson, 2012;Iyengar et al., 2006;Leach & Patall, 2013;Sparks et al., 2012;Schwartz et al., 2002), and report more negative affect and regret as a result of their choice (Chowdhury et al., 2009;Dar-Nimrod et al., 2009;Spunt et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
When selecting from too many options (i.e., choice overload), maximizers (people who search exhaustively to make decisions that are optimal) report more negative post‐decisional evaluations of their choices than do satisficers (people who search minimally to make decisions that are sufficient). Although ample evidence exists for differences in responses after‐the‐fact, little is known about possible divergences in maximizers’ and satisficers’ experiences during choice overload. Thus, using the biopsychosocial model of challenge/threat, we examined 128 participants’ cardiovascular responses as they actively made a selection from many options. Specifically, we focused on cardiovascular responses assessing the degree to which individuals (a) viewed their decisions as valuable/important and (b) viewed themselves as capable (vs. incapable) of making a good choice. Although we found no differences in terms of the value individuals placed on their decisions (i.e., cardiovascular responses of task engagement), satisficers—compared to maximizers—exhibited cardiovascular responses consistent with feeling less capable of making their choice (i.e., greater relative threat). The current work provides a novel investigation of the nature of differences in maximizers’/satisficers’ momentary choice overload experiences, suggesting insight into why they engage in such distinct search behaviors.
... profiles, maximizers examine more profiles than do satisficers (Yang & Chiou, 2010; see also Schwartz, 2004). Maximization, however, should predict more than merely laboring over a choice. ...
Article
People differ in their tendencies to labor over decisions and to make choices that maximize their outcomes—a difference known as maximization. Here, we used two independent, 3-year longitudinal studies of newlywed couples to demonstrate that this individual difference in decision making has important implications for romantic relationships. Consistent with the idea that maximizers are more likely to compare their current romantic partners to potential alternative partners’ readily observable qualities, such as their physical attractiveness and status, results demonstrated that intimates’ maximization moderated the implications of these sex-differentiated variables for marital satisfaction. Specifically, maximizing men who had attractive (vs. unattractive) wives were more satisfied at the start of their marriages. Likewise, maximizing women who had high (vs. low) status husbands experienced less steep declines in satisfaction over time. These findings demonstrate that maximization has important implications for long-term romantic relationships by accentuating the effects of readily observable partner qualities on relationship outcomes. © 2019 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
... This negative association may occur for a variety of reasons. Borrowing the distinction of maximizing versus satisficing as choice strategies in dating (e.g., Yang & Chiou, 2010) Figure 2. Beliefs about overall change in mate preferences across four age-groups for total sample and men versus women. The values inside the bars represent the mean for each group. ...
Article
Full-text available
Few studies have considered how mate preferences may differ for individuals at different ages of emerging adulthood and beyond or considered at the same time other individual difference variables beyond sex and age of participant. This survey study of mate preferences (N = 738) found that participants’ age (from 18 to 40) was associated with lower mate selectivity. This negative association between age and mate selectivity was greater for men than for women. Age continued to be associated with mate selectivity controlling for shyness and perception of available partners—and both of these variables were uniquely associated with mate selectivity (negatively and positively, respectively). This study also examined participants’ beliefs about how the importance of various traits in a partner had changed over time. Overall, the participants perceived no change or a slight increase in their preferences over time, although the perceived change depended on the particular trait domain, sex of the participant, and the other individual difference variables.
Article
Full-text available
The Internet has fundamentally altered how we communicate and access information and who we can interact with. However, the implications of Internet access for partnership formation are theoretically ambiguous. We examine their association using data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY97) and Current Population Survey (CPS) in the United States. We find that the relationship between Internet access and partnership states (in the NLSY97) or partnership status (in the CPS) is age-dependent. While negative at the youngest adult ages, the association becomes positive as individuals reach their mid- to late 20s, for both same-sex and different-sex partnerships. The results suggest that Internet access is positively associated with union formation when individuals enter the stage in the young adult life course when they feel ready to commit to a long-term partnership. Our study contributes to a growing literature that highlights the implications of digital technologies for demographic processes. Supplementary material for this article is available at: https://doi.org/10.1080/00324728.2021.1999485
Article
Full-text available
The role of effort and accuracy in the adaptive use of decision processes is examined. A computer simulation using the concept of elementary information processes identified heuristic choice strategies that approximate the accuracy of normative procedures while saving substantial effort. However, no single heuristic did well across all task and context conditions. Of particular interest was the finding that under time constraints, several heuristics were more accurate than a truncated normative procedure. Using a process-tracing technique that monitors information acquisition behaviors, two experiments tested how closely the efficient processing patterns for a given decision problem identified by the simulation correspond to the actual processing behavior exhibited by subjects. People appear highly adaptive in responding to changes in the structure of the available alternatives and to the presence of time pressure. In general, actual behavior corresponded to the general patterns of efficient processing identified by the simulation. Finally, learning of effort and accuracy trade-offs are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This study compared shy and nonshy Internet users in online and offline contexts on the Revised Cheek and Buss Shyness Scale (RCBSS; Cheek, 1983) and other measures intended to gauge 4 underlying aspects of shyness: rejection sensitivity, initiating relationships, self-disclosure, and providing emotional support and advice. University students (N = 134; 76% female) participated in a Web-based survey that investigated the impact of computer-mediated communications (CMC) on shyness level. Results show that individuals classified as shy or nonshy on the basis of their scores on the RCBSS in the offline context were also significantly different on offline measures of rejection sensitivity, initiating relationships, and self-disclosure. However, they were not significantly different on these same 3 domains in the online context. The results are interpreted as support for a self-presentation theory account that the absence of visual and auditory cues online reduces shy individuals' experience of detecting negative or inhibitory feedback cues from others. We discuss positive and negative aspects of use of CMC by shy individuals.
Article
Full-text available
Set/reset (L. L. Martin; see record 1987-01092-001) hypothesis that contrast demands more cognitive effort than does assimilation was examined. In Exp. 1, the impressions of distracted Ss showed assimilation toward blatantly primed concepts, whereas the impressions of nondistracted Ss showed contrast. In Exp. 2, Ss told that their ratings would be lumped into a group average showed assimilation, whereas Ss told that their ratings would be examined individually showed contrast. In Exp. 3, the impressions of Ss low in need for cognition showed assimilation, whereas the impressions of Ss high in need for cognition showed contrast. Exp. 1 also showed that the results were not due to differences in recall of the target information, and Exp. 3 showed that the results were not due to differences in recall of the priming stimuli. Together, the results suggest that the processes involved in contrast demand more cognitive effort than do the processes involved in assimilation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
The role of effort and accuracy in the adaptive use of decision processes is examined. A computer simulation using the concept of elementary information processes identified heuristic choice strategies that approximate the accuracy of normative procedures while saving substantial effort. However, no single heuristic did well across all task and context conditions. Of particular interest was the finding that under time constraints, several heuristics were more accurate than a truncated normative procedure. Using a process-tracing technique that monitors information acquisition behaviors, two experiments tested how closely the efficient processing patterns for a given decision problem identified by the simulation correspond to the actual processing behavior exhibited by subjects. People appear highly adaptive in responding to changes in the structure of the available alternatives and to the presence of time pressure. In general, actual behavior corresponded to the general patterns of efficient processing identified by the simulation. Finally, learning of effort and accuracy trade-offs are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
One thousand university students were e-mailed a survey about relationships they had established on the Internet. Of the 248 who returned the survey, 88 (36%) indicated they had formed a friendship with another individual in an on-line setting. Nineteen (22%) described it as a close romantic relationship. These respondents were e-mailed a second survey concerning various aspects of their romantic relationship. The frequency of romantic on-line relationships in the university population investigated was determined to be .0766, or 76 per 1,000 students. The 12 respondents who returned the second survey rated their on-line relationship as equal to or superior to those they had established off-line on measures of strength, satisfaction, and ease of communication. The findings were discussed in terms of those features of computer-mediated communication that can facilitate the development of close relationships, as well as their implications for contemporary theories of relationship formation.
Article
The importance of computer-mediated communication (CMC) for comparing the group that communicates face-to-face (FtF) in cyberspace is discussed. In online relationship, network convergence would imply not only that participants were introduced one another's online contacts, but also to people in their real-life social network. The growth of CMC poses new challenges for understanding of social relationships both in cyberspace and in general. The goal of such understanding is to provide an empirical reference point for evaluating conflicting visions of social life in cyberspace by exploring the prevalence and development of personal relationships.
Article
Introduction, 99. — I. Some general features of rational choice, 100.— II. The essential simplifications, 103. — III. Existence and uniqueness of solutions, 111. — IV. Further comments on dynamics, 113. — V. Conclusion, 114. — Appendix, 115.
Article
Based on traditional theories of interpersonal relationship development and on the hyperpersonal communication theory, this study examined predictors of relationship satisfaction for individuals involved in online romantic relationships. One hundred-fourteen individuals (N ¼ 114) involved in online romantic relationships, and who had only engaged in computer-mediated communication (CMC) with their partners, com-pleted an online questionnaire about their relationships. Intimacy, trust, and communi-cation satisfaction were found to be the strongest predictors of relationship satisfaction for individuals involved in online romances. Additionally, perceptions of relationship variables differed depending on relationship length and time spent communicating. Implications for interpersonal and hyperpersonal communication theories, and future investigation of online relationships, are discussed. More people are becoming involved in computer-mediated romantic relationships. These individuals inhabit an interesting relational niche because they engage in rela-tionships that are perceived by some scholars as either nontraditional or understu-died (Emmers-Sommer, 2005). Given these atypical relational circumstances, such individuals might also lack information about online romantic relationships and social support networks from which to gain confirmation about their relationship.
Article
Despite the explosive growth of electronic commerce and the rapidly increasing number of consumers who use interactive media (such as the World Wide Web) for prepurchase information search and online shopping, very little is known about how consumers make purchase decisions in such settings. A unique characteristic of online shopping environments is that they allow vendors to create retail interfaces with highly interactive features. One desirable form of interactivity from a consumer perspective is the implementation of sophisticated tools to assist shoppers in their purchase decisions by customizing the electronic shopping environment to their individual preferences. The availability of such tools, which we refer to as interactive decision aids for consumers, may lead to a transformation of the way in which shoppers search for product information and make purchase decisions. The primary objective of this paper is to investigate the nature of the effects that interactive decision aids may have on consumer decision making in online shopping environments. While making purchase decisions, consumers are often unable to evaluate all available alternatives in great depth and, thus, tend to use two-stage processes to reach their decisions. At the first stage, consumers typically screen a large set of available products and identify a subset of the most promising alternatives. Subsequently, they evaluate the latter in more depth, perform relative comparisons across products on important attributes, and make a purchase decision. Given the different tasks to be performed in such a two-stage process, interactive tools that provide support to consumers in the following respects are particularly valuable: (1) the initial screening of available products to determine which ones are worth considering further, and (2) the in-depth comparison of selected products before making the actual purchase decision. This paper examines the effects of two decision aids, each designed to assist consumers in performing one of the above tasks, on purchase decision making in an online store. The first interactive tool, a recommendation agent (RA), allows consumers to more efficiently screen the (potentially very large) set of alternatives available in an online shopping environment. Based on self-explicated information about a consumer's own utility function (attribute importance weights and minimum acceptable attribute levels), the RA generates a personalized list of recommended alternatives. The second decision aid, a comparison matrix (CM), is designed to help consumers make in-depth comparisons among selected alternatives. The CM allows consumers to organize attribute information about multiple products in an alternatives × attributes matrix and to have alternatives sorted by any attribute. Based on theoretical and empirical work in marketing, judgment and decision making, psychology, and decision support systems, we develop a set of hypotheses pertaining to the effects of these two decision aids on various aspects of consumer decision making. In particular, we focus on how use of the RA and CM affects consumers' search for product information, the size and quality of their consideration sets, and the quality of their purchase decisions in an online shopping environment. A controlled experiment using a simulated online store was conducted to test the hypotheses. The results indicate that both interactive decision aids have a substantial impact on consumer decision making. As predicted, use of the RA reduces consumers' search effort for product information, decreases the size but increases the quality of their consideration sets, and improves the quality of their purchase decisions. Use of the CM also leads to a decrease in the size but an increase in the quality of consumers' consideration sets, and has a favorable effect on some indicators of decision quality. In sum, our findings suggest that interactive tools designed to assist consumers in the initial screening of available alternatives and to facilitate in-depth comparisons among selected alternatives in an online shopping environment may have strong favorable effects on both the quality and the efficiency of purchase decisions—shoppers can make much better decisions while expending substantially less effort. This suggests that interactive decision aids have the potential to drastically transform the way in which consumers search for product information and make purchase decisions.
Article