ArticlePDF Available

Social vocalizations can release oxytocin in humans

The Royal Society
Proceedings of the Royal Society B
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Vocalizations are important components of social behaviour in many vertebrate species, including our own. Less well-understood are the hormonal mechanisms involved in response to vocal cues, and how these systems may influence the course of behavioural evolution. The neurohormone oxytocin (OT) partly governs a number of biological and social processes critical to fitness, such as attachment between mothers and their young, and suppression of the stress response after contact with trusted conspecfics. Rodent studies suggest that OT's release is contingent upon direct tactile contact with such individuals, but we hypothesized that vocalizations might be capable of producing the same effect. To test our hypothesis, we chose human mother-daughter dyads and applied a social stressor to the children, following which we randomly assigned participants into complete contact, speech-only or no-contact conditions. Children receiving a full complement of comfort including physical, vocal and non-verbal contact showed the highest levels of OT and the swiftest return to baseline of a biological marker of stress (salivary cortisol), but a strikingly similar hormonal profile emerged in children comforted solely by their mother's voice. Our results suggest that vocalizations may be as important as touch to the neuroendocrine regulation of social bonding in our species.
Content may be subject to copyright.
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0567
, 2661-2666 first published online 12 May 2010277 2010 Proc. R. Soc. B
Leslie J. Seltzer, Toni E. Ziegler and Seth D. Pollak
Social vocalizations can release oxytocin in humans
References
http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/277/1694/2661.full.html#related-urls
Article cited in:
http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/277/1694/2661.full.html#ref-list-1
This article cites 41 articles, 9 of which can be accessed free
Subject collections
(1956 articles)evolution (191 articles)developmental biology (1440 articles)behaviour
Articles on similar topics can be found in the following collections
Email alerting service hereright-hand corner of the article or click
Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top
http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/subscriptions go to: Proc. R. Soc. BTo subscribe to
This journal is © 2010 The Royal Society
on September 1, 2010rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
Social vocalizations can release oxytocin
in humans
Leslie J. Seltzer1,*, Toni E. Ziegler2and Seth D. Pollak1
1
Departments of Psychology, Anthropology, and Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin-Madison,
Madison, WI 53705, USA
2
Wisconsin National Primate Research Center, Madison, WI 53705, USA
Vocalizations are important components of social behaviour in many vertebrate species, including our
own. Less well-understood are the hormonal mechanisms involved in response to vocal cues, and how
these systems may influence the course of behavioural evolution. The neurohormone oxytocin (OT)
partly governs a number of biological and social processes critical to fitness, such as attachment between
mothers and their young, and suppression of the stress response after contact with trusted conspecfics.
Rodent studies suggest that OT’s release is contingent upon direct tactile contact with such individuals,
but we hypothesized that vocalizations might be capable of producing the same effect. To test our hypoth-
esis, we chose human mother daughter dyads and applied a social stressor to the children, following
which we randomly assigned participants into complete contact, speech-only or no-contact conditions.
Children receiving a full complement of comfort including physical, vocal and non-verbal contact
showed the highest levels of OTand the swiftest return to baseline of a biological marker of stress (salivary
cortisol), but a strikingly similar hormonal profile emerged in children comforted solely by their mother’s
voice. Our results suggest that vocalizations may be as important as touch to the neuroendocrine
regulation of social bonding in our species.
Keywords: oxytocin; stress; vocalizations; female; children; social bonding
1. INTRODUCTION
The strength and quality of relationships between
individuals are critical to fitness in many animals. While
the behaviours that may facilitate the formation and main-
tenance of these relationships are readily observable, such
as grooming in primates or social allofeeding in birds,
their biochemical underpinnings are less evident. Since
natural selection operates upon inter-individual variation
in behavioural phenotype, an understanding of the proxi-
mate mechanisms responsible for eliciting or perpetuating
social behaviour is critical to the study of evolution. One
of the ways in which this can be examined is through an
analysis of the hormones involved in behavioural regulation.
(a)Oxytocin, stress and social support
The neuropeptide oxytocin (OT) plays complex roles in the
central nervous system in establishing maternal/infant and
other types of attachments in a species- and sex-specific
manner (Carter 1998), particularly the development of
trust, pair bonding and recognition of familiar individuals
inrodentssuchasprairievoles(Insel 1997;Lim & Young
2006;Bales et al. 2007;Grippo et al. 2009). This makes
OT a candidate for the study of the neurological bases of
human social behaviour.
There is considerable debate as to how OT operates
to promote relationships and/or regulate stress in the face
of differential social contact in endogenous systems. One
such model holds that physical touch is critical to social
bonding as mediated by OT, as is the familiarity of the indi-
vidual providing contact, especially in the case of touch
between mothers and their offspring (Uvna¨s-Moberg
1996,1997). It appears that this type of tactile contact in
the context of complex social interactions can impact tness
as well; in vervets, for example, individuals recently groomed
by a conspecific respond more quickly to an alarm call from
that individual than they do to others (Seyfarth & Cheney
1984), and touch between chimpanzees appears to reduce
tension and bolster relationships between individuals who
have recently engaged in agonistic interactions (deWaal
2000;Arnold & Whiten 2001). Individuals with strong
social networks as evidenced by extensive grooming, particu-
larly with their own kin, also appear to have more offspring
than those with weaker ties to others (Silk 2007). While a
relationship between OT and these non-human primate
behaviours has not yet been established, such behaviours
are associated with increases in OT in other mammals
such as rats (Uvna¨ s-Moberg 1998), human adults
(Grewen et al. 2005) and children (Wismer Fries et al. 2005).
This construct, however, may belie the complexity of
how the OT system works. For example, OT also seems
to be related to amelioration of social stress, either directly
or through intermediate hormonal factors; indeed, touch in
rodent species tends to take place after stressful inter-
actions, possibly because OT is released in response to
activation of the hypothalamic– pituitary–adrenal axis
(Jezova
´et al. 1996;Campbell 2008). This in turn may
facilitate comfort-seeking and affiliative bonding, particu-
larly in females who may opt to ‘tend and befriend’
during stressful times rather than engage in fight or flight
responses in order to protect their offspring and themselves
(Ta y l o r et al. 2000). It remains to be seen whether or not
OT is released by stress itself in addition to tactile
contact—or potentially by other types of social interactions.
*Author for correspondence (lseltzer@wisc.edu).
Proc. R. Soc. B (2010) 277, 2661–2666
doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0567
Published online 12 May 2010
Received 17 March 2010
Accepted 20 April 2010 2661 This journal is q2010 The Royal Society
on September 1, 2010rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
(b)Vocalizations and touch—neuroendocrine
similarities?
Although it has been conjectured that human vocalizations
in the form of female speech can release OT (Brizendine
2006), this is yet to be demonstrated. Like touch, however,
vocalizations are used by a number of species to communi-
cate aggression, proceptivity, anxiety and a number of other
emotional states. Little is known ofthe role of OT in the pro-
duction of vocalizations, although new studies reveal that
they are likely to be important. In the soniferous fishes, a
counterpart to OT (isotocin) regulates the expression of
vocal communication of the sexes differently, via strongly
steroid-mediated developmental differences in the brain
(Goodson et al.2003). The neuroanatomy of oxytocinergic
neurons have also been explored in the moustached bat
(Pteronotus parnellii), showing numerous terminations in
and around auditory brain regions (Kanwal & Rao 2002).
In ‘singing’ mice (Scotinomys xerampelinus), OT receptors
are distributed densely in regions of the brain that govern
social memory, such as the hippocampus and amygdala
(Campbell et al.2009). Finally, infant OT knockout mice
show both profound social deficits (Kramer et al.2003)
and produce fewer social vocalizations than their peers
(Takayanagi et al.2005), while also demonstrating both
increased frequency of ultrasonic stress vocalizations and
higher levels of circulating cortisol (Liu et al.2008).
With this in mind, we hypothesized that endogenous OT
might change in response to vocal stimuli after a stressful
event, even in the absence of any other type of social contact.
Partly because humans are the only vertebrate capable of
producing a continuous and precisely timed amount of
comforting vocalizations upon request, we selected
mother– daughter pairs as our test subjects and investigated
whether exposure to socially supportive speech (defined as
the combination of prosodic and linguistic vocal cues)
could produce the same physiological effects as direct
physical contact as assayed via peripheral measures of OT.
The validity of peripheral measurement of OT as it
pertains to such behavioural variables is a contentious
issue requiring further empirical work to resolve. In par-
ticular, some studies suggest that the central and
peripheral OT systems operate independently of one
another to the extent that serum, salivary or urinary OT
measurement cannot reflect central processes in the way
that invasive methods such as cerebrospinal draws can
(Amico et al. 1990;Engelmann et al. 1999;Landgraf &
Neumann 2004;Neumann 2007). Other studies, how-
ever, have begun to reveal that a relationship may exist
between peripheral measures of OT and centrally
mediated behaviour, given changes in peripheral OT
with exposure to different behavioural paradigms in a
number of species, including humans (Carmichael et al.
1987;Modahl et al. 1998;Cushing & Carter 2000;Cushing
et al.2001;Wismer Fries et al. 2005;Seltzer & Ziegler
2007). Such methods allow for non-invasive, real-time
analysis of the ways in which different types of contact
may influence neuroendocrine function, potentially making
them more desirable options for use with human subjects.
2. MATERIAL AND METHODS
(a)Subjects
Our subjects consisted of 61 female children, aged 7– 12 years
(M¼9.4 years, s.d. ¼1.61 years) and their mothers. Only
pre-menarchal children were included to minimize
oestrogen-mediated changes and potential serum contami-
nation of urine associated with menstrual cycling. All
children had reached adrenarche, resulting in relative control
of inter-individual variation in cortisol levels. We selected
post-adrenarche, prepubescent females as test subjects
because: (i) we wished to test whether or not OT was released
after a stressful event in females, with and without tactile and/
or verbal contact from a trusted parent; (ii) OT-mediated
phenomena are best studied in females, anchoring our work
in the existing literature; (iii) we wished to examine whether
or not the role of OT in decreasing stress, if any, was present
before oestrogen cycling commenced with puberty; (iv) we
posited that female children would be more accepting of
warm physical touch and verbal contact with their mothers
than male children owing to cultural norms; and finally
(v) we posited that younger children might not be able to
understand instructions well enough to complete our stress
paradigm successfully. All children with a documented history
of abuse, neglect or prior institutionalization were eliminated
for purposes of this study, but are the subjects of future work.
(b)Experimental protocols
To control for potential circadian fluctuations in hormone con-
centrations, each experimental session began at 16.00 h. After
obtaining consent and assent, children underwent the Trier
Social Stress Test for Children, a procedure which
involves completing a series of timed public speaking and math
performance tasks aloud in front of an audience (Kirschbaum
et al. 1993) and that has been specially modified for use
with children (Gunnar et al. 2009). After experiencing this
stressor, children were randomly assigned to one of the three
experimental conditions. In the direct contact condition (n¼
19) children were reunited with their mothers, who comforted
their child with all sensory stimuli including physical contact. A
second group of children (n¼20) received a telephone call
from their mothers from a different location. By virtue of
physical distance, contact was limited to speech. Moreover,
children in this condition were not allowed to make visual con-
tact with their mothers throughout the course of the
experiment and were provided with a phone in order to control
for the possible influence of non-auditory social cues. A third
group of children (n¼22) participated in a control condition
in which they watched a neutral film for 75 min, but did not
have any contact with their mother of any type until the com-
pletion of the experiment. Children in the first two conditions
had 15 min of contact with their mothers (either total contact
or verbal), after which they watched the same film as the chil-
dren in the control group for 60 min. To index children’s stress
responses, salivary cortisol was collected at arrival, baseline
(30 min after the novelty of arriving at the laboratory but
prior to the stressor), immediately after the stressor but
before experimental contact, and then at 15, 30, 45 and
60 min post-maternal contact, if any. OT was assayed from
urine samples collected at the following time points matched
with salivary cortisol: arrival, baseline, post-stressor 30 min
and post-stressor 60 min. This study was approved by the
Human Subjects Committee/Institutional Review Board at
the University of Wisconsin-Madison.
(c)Hormonal assays
(i) Saliva
Saliva samples were frozen on dry ice after collection and
assayed using a microtitre plate coated with monoclonal
2662 L. J. Seltzer et al. Social vocalizations
Proc. R. Soc. B (2010)
on September 1, 2010rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
cortisol antibodies. Twenty-five microlitres of each sample
was tested duplicate. Bound cortisol peroxidase was
measured by the reaction of the peroxidase enzyme on the
substrate tetramethylbenzidine. Optical density was read
using ASSAY ZAP data reduction software at 450 nm using a
four-parameter sigmoid minus curve fit. Standard concen-
trations range from 3.00 to 0.012 mg/dl, with intra- and
inter-assay coefficients at 3.35 and 3.75, respectively.
(ii) Urine
Urine samples were snap-frozen on dry ice and subsequently
stored at 2808C. After controlled thawing, urinary samples
were subjected to solid-phase extraction using 1 ml SepPak
C18 cartridges (Waters, cat no. WAT023590). Each column
was pretreated with 1 ml of methanol and then 1 ml of
water before application of 1 ml of urine. A 10 per cent
acetonitrile (ACN) plus 1 per cent trifluroacetic acid (TFA)
wash (1 ml) was then applied, after which the elutant was col-
lected in a clean tube via application of a final 1 ml application
of 80/20 per cent ACN solution with 1 per cent TFA to the
column. Samples were then dried down in a water bath with
air stream and reconstituted in the assay-appropriate buffer
supplied in the 96-well enzyme linked immunosorbent assay
kit used (Assay Designs. Cat no. 901-153; cat no. 901-017
for AVP). Intra- and intercoefficients of variation were deter-
mined by a human urine pool, and oxytocin standards were
used to determine recoveries from the extraction method
(intra-assay/inter-assay coefficient of variation ¼24.2/10.5,
recoveries 92.1 +5.23%, n¼8). Each plate was read on a
Molecular Devices Spectramax 340PC 384 at 405 nm. Data
were analysed by weighted least-squares regression analysis
and reduced by log-logit transformation to yield peptide con-
centrations. Creatinine was also collected with each urine
sample and analysed to correct for variation in water content
(simple creatinine value divided by peptide concentration) to
arrive at pg OT per milligram creatinine (Ziegler et al.
1995). Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare
the experimental groups with one another at each time
point, with post hoc analysis performed via Tukey’s HSD test.
3. RESULTS
(a)Cortisol
Children in all three conditions exhibited an increase in sali-
vary cortisol from baseline to peak (t
68
¼23.4, p,0.01),
indicating that the social stressor was effective. However,
treatment conditions differed following the stressor
(F
2,52
¼3.99, p,0.02). Both direct, interpersonal contact
and vocal contact alone were effective at reducing children’s
cortisol levels after 1 h, although the condition involving
touch resulted in a more rapid decrease and lower levels of
peak cortisol. Across experimental time, reduction in cortisol
in children engaging in speech only was intermediate
between the other two groups, but by the end of the study
children hearing their mother’s voice and those interacting
with their mothers directly were statistically indistinguishable
(comparable to baseline levels across participants). Children
receiving no social contact exhibited higher levels of cortisol
than the other two groups, even an hour after the stressor was
complete (F
2,52
¼4.475, p,0.02; figure 1).
(b)Oxytocin
As predicted by extant studies, urinar y OT was released in
children following normative comforting by their mothers
involving direct physical contact. We also observed, how-
ever, that girls released conspicuously similar levels of OT
in response to speech with their mothers, even in the
absence of all other types of somatosensory contact.
Both physical and speech-only contact affected
children’s OT levels within 15 min post-stressor and this
effect was maintained as long as 1 h post-stressor
(F
2,42
¼4.54, p¼0.02 and F
2,42
¼3.73, p¼0.02,
respectively). By contrast, OT levels did not change
salivary cortisol (mg/dl)
arrival post-
stressor
15 m
post-
stressor
30 m
post-
stressor
45 m
post-
stressor
1 h
post-
stressor
0 m
baseline
0
0.05
0.10
0.15
0.20
Figure 1. Salivary cortisol levels across experimental time, by maternal contact type. The green arrow represents the onset of
the stressor, and the pink arrow the onset of maternal contact (if any). Both speech and direct contact facilitate a more rapid
return to baseline values than simply resting alone. Diamonds and black line, no contact; triangles and red line, direct contact;
squares and blue line, speech-only.
Social vocalizations L. J. Seltzer et al. 2663
Proc. R. Soc. B (2010)
on September 1, 2010rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
overall for children who rested alone and received no form
of maternal comforting following the stressor (figure 2).
4. CONCLUSION
This work paves the way for understanding the proximate
mechanisms responsible for the formation or mainten-
ance of social ties in humans, and also for arriving at a
model for how OT operates with respect to stress and
social contact. Simply put, our work emphasizes a
model in which social contact of at least two types (tactile
and vocal) can release OT in female children after a
socially stressful event. The implications of these findings
are twofold: those that are relevant to the evolution of
behaviour and those that are relevant to human society
and early development.
First, this work reveals that vocal cues in humans are
similar to tactile contact as seen in other mammals with
respect to the release of OT. It is, however, important to
view these results in the proper evolutionary context.
OT is a uniquely mammalian hormone, probably having
evolved along with the smooth muscle contractions
associated with parturition and lactation approximately
200 million years ago (Chauvet et al. 1985;Acher et al.
1995). Since the anatomical apparatus necessary for pro-
duction of vocal cues is at least 400 million years old
(Bass et al. 2008), it may be the case that maternal
offspring touch as a facilitator of OT release in mammals
represents an exaptation from these earlier social signal-
ling systems rather than the other way around.
Alternatively, this may be simply another example of
how this single peptide, which has remained essentially
unchanged throughout the course of mammalian
evolution, can differently influence behaviour owing to
hypervariable OT receptor status within the brains of
different species (Donaldson & Young 2008).
Since each vertebrate clade contains famously vocal
members, language in the sense of human’s unique ability
to use recursive grammar may be unlikely to stand alone
in its ability to release OT. It is at least as likely that pro-
sodic cues are responsible for the observed similarities in
OT release between touch and human speech, and that
non-linguistic social vocalizations facilitate attachment
via the release of OT or related peptides in many other
species. Nonetheless, two grammatically identical
instances of human language differ in meaning depending
on tonality, who is speaking, who is listening and the
nuances of the relationship between them. Focusing on
language alone is the focus of a future study, and it is
our hope that students of vertebrate vocalizations will
choose to focus future work on changes in peripheral
OT release in response to vocal cues in other species.
Second, vocal cues may be a viable alternative to phys-
ical contact for servicing human relationships. The work of
Harry Harlow in the 1970s demonstrates that social iso-
lation, especially early in development, is detrimental to
health and behavioural outcomes in primates, and work
with children who have been institutionalized rather than
reared by their biological parents (or early adoptive
ones), abused or neglected show that this work is probably
translational. Indeed, humans lacking social support from
family and friends have poorer health outcomes than
their better-connected peers (Couzin 2009). Vocal cues
may be able to provide some of the same relief from
these outcomes as direct interpersonal interaction includ-
ing touch. This alternative means of buffering stress
while facilitating social bonds in middle childhood may
underscore the relationship between typical development
and hormonally mediated affiliative bonding in our species.
This study was approved by the Human Subjects
Committee/Institutional Review Board at the University of
Wisconsin-Madison.
We would like to thank Bret Larget for his kind assistance, as
well as John Hawks, Karen Strier, Chuck Snowdon and two
anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments. This
research was supported by the US National Institutes of
Health (MH61285).
REFERENCES
Acher, R., Chauvet, J. & Chauvet, M. T. 1995 Man and
the chimera: selective versus neutral OT evolution. In
0
50
100
150
200
250
urinary oxytocin (pg/mg creatinine)
arrival post-stressor
1 h
post-stressor
15 m
baseline
Figure 2. Maternal speech releases oxytocin in girls, in much the same way as direct interpersonal interaction including
comforting touch. Diamonds and black line, no contact; triangles and red line, direct contact; squares and blue line, speech-only.
2664 L. J. Seltzer et al. Social vocalizations
Proc. R. Soc. B (2010)
on September 1, 2010rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
OT: cellular and molecular approaches to medicine and research
(eds R. Ivell & J. A. Russell), pp. 615–627. New York, NY:
Plenum Press.
Amico, J. A., Challinor, S. M. & Cameron, J. L. 1990 Pattern
of oxytocin concentrations in the plasma and cerebrosp-
inal fluid of lactating rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta):
evidence for functionally independent oxytocinergic path-
ways in primates. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 71,
1531–1535. (doi:10.1210/jcem-71-6-1531)
Arnold, W. & Whiten, A. 2001 Post-conflict behaviour of
wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the
Budongo Forest, Uganda. Behaviour 138, 649– 690.
(doi:10.1163/156853901316924520)
Bales, K. L., Van Westerhuyzen, J. A., Lewis-Reese, A. D.,
Grotte, N. D., Lanter, J. A. & Carter, C. S. 2007
Oxytocin has dose-dependent developmental effects on
pair-bonding and alloparental care in female prairie
voles. Horm. Behav. 52, 274– 279. (doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.
2007.05.004)
Bass, A. H., Gilland, E. H. & Baker, R. 2008 Evolutionary
origins for social vocalization in a vertebrate hindbrain–
spinal compartment. Science 321, 417 421. (doi:10.
1126/science.1157632)
Brizendine, L. 2006 The female brain. New York/Cambridge:
Morgan Road Books/Harvard University Press.
Campbell, A. 2008 Attachment, aggression and affiliation:
the role of oxytocin in female social behavior. Biol.
Psychol. 77, 1–10. (doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.09.
001)
Campbell, P., Ophir, A. G. & Phelps, S. M. 2009 Central
vasopressin and oxytocin receptor distributions in two
species of singing mice. J. Comp. Neurol. 516, 321–333.
(doi:10.1002/cne.22116)
Carmichael, M. S., Humbert, R., Dixen, J., Palmisano, G.,
Greenleaf, W. & Davidson, J. M. 1987 Plasma OT
increases in the human sexual response. J. Clin. Endocrinol.
Metab. 64,2731.(doi:10.1210/jcem-64-1-27)
Carter, C. S. 1998 Neuroendocrine perspectives on social
attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23,
779– 818. (doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00055-9)
Carter, C. S., Williams, J. R., Witt, D. M. & Insel, T. R. 1992
Oxytocin and social bonding. Ann. NY Acad.
Sci. 652, 204–211. (doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.
tb34356.x)
Chauvet, J., Hurpet, D., Michel, G., Chauvet, M. T.,
Carrick, F. N. & Acher, R. 1985 The neurohypophysial
hormones of the egg-laying mammals: identification of
arginine vasopressin in the platypus (Ornithorhynchus ana-
tinus). Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 127, 277– 282.
(doi:10.1016/S0006-291X(85)80155-8)
Couzin, J. 2009 Social science: friendship as a health factor.
Science 323, 454–457. (doi:10.1126/science.323.5913.
454)
Cushing, B. S. & Carter, C. S. 2000 Peripheral pulses of OT
increase partner preferences in female, but not male,
prairie voles. Horm. Behav. 37, 49– 56. (doi:10.1006/
hbeh.1999.1558)
Cushing, B. S., Martin, J. O., Young, L. J. & Carter, C. S.
2001 The effects of peptides on partner preference
formation are predicted by habitat in prairie
voles. Horm. Behav. 39, 48– 58. (doi:10.1006/hbeh.
2000.1633)
deWaal, F. B. M. 2000 Primates: a natural heritage of conflict
resolution. Science 289, 586–590. (doi:10.1126/science.
289.5479.586)
Donaldson, Z. R. & Young, L. J. 2008 Oxytocin, vasopressin,
and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science 322, 900– 904.
(doi:10.1126/science.1158668)
Engelmann, M., Ebner, K., Landgraf, R., Holsboer, F. &
Wotjak, C. T. 1999 Emotional stress triggers
intrahypothalamic but not peripheral release of oxytocin
in male rats. J. Neuroendocrinol. 11, 867– 872. (doi:10.
1046/j.1365-2826.1999.00403.x)
Goodson, J. L., Evans, A. K. & Bass, A. H. 2003 Putative
isotocin distributions in sonic fish: relation to vasotocin
and vocal-acoustic circuitry. J. Comp. Neurol. 462, 1– 14.
(doi:10.1002/cne.10679)
Grewen, K. M., Girdler, S. S., Amico, J. & Light, K. C.
2005 Effects of partner support on resting oxytocin,
cortisol, norepinephrine, and blood pressure
before and after warm partner contact. Psychosom.
Med. 67, 531–538. (doi:10.1097/01.psy.0000170341.
88395.47)
Grippo, A. J., Trahanas, D. M., Zimmerman II, R. R.,
Porges, S. W. & Carter, C. S. 2009 Oxytocin
protects against negative behavioral and autonomic
consequences of long-term social isolation. Psychoneuro-
endocrinology 34, 1542 1553. (doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.
2009.05.017)
Gunnar, M. R., Wewerka, S., Frenn, K., Long, J. D. &
Griggs, C. 2009 Developmental changes in hypothala-
mus–pituitary– adrenal activity over the transition to
adolescence: normative changes and associations with
puberty. Dev. Psychopathol. 21, 69–85. (doi:10.1017/
S0954579409000054)
Insel, T. R. 1997 A neurological basis of social attachment.
Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 726– 735.
Insel, T. R., Winslow, J. T., Wang, Z. & Young, L. J.
1998 Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neuroendocrine
basis of pair bond formation. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 449,
215–224.
Jezova
´, D., Jura
´nkova
´, E., Mosna
´rova
´, A., Kriska, M. & Skul-
te
´tyova
´, I. 1996 Neuroendocrine response during stress
with relation to gender differences. Acta Neurobiol. 56,
779–785.
Kanwal, J. S. & Rao, P. D. 2002 Oxytocin within auditory
nuclei: a neuromodulatory function in sensory proces-
sing? Neuroreport 13, 2193–2197. (doi:10.1097/
00001756-200212030-00006)
Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M. & Hellhammer, D. H. 1993
The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’: a tool for investigating
psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory
setting. Neuropsychobiology 28, 76–78. (doi:10.1159/
000119004)
Kramer, K. M., Cushing, B. S. & Carter, C. S. 2003 Devel-
opmental effects of oxytocin on stress response: single
versus repeated exposure. Physiol. Behav. 79, 775–782.
(doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00175-6)
Landgraf, R. & Neumann, I. D. 2004 Vasopressin and oxyto-
cin release within the brain: a dynamic concept of
multiple and variable modes of neuropeptide communi-
cation. Frontiers Neuroendocrinol. 25, 150–176. (doi:10.
1016/j.yfrne.2004.05.001)
Lim, M. M. & Young, L. J. 2006 Neuropeptidergic regu-
lation of affiliative behavior and social bonding in
animals. Horm. Behav. 50, 506– 517. (doi:10.1016/j.
yhbeh.2006.06.028)
Liu, H. X., Lopatina, O., Higashida, C., Tsuji, T., Kato, I.,
Takasawa, S., Okamoto, H., Yokoyama, S. & Higashida, H.
2008 Locomotor activity, ultrasonic vocalization and oxyto-
cin levels in infant CD38 knockout mice. Neurosci. Lett. 19,
67–70. (doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2008.09.084)
Modahl, C., Green, L., Fein, D., Morris, M., Waterhouse, L.,
Feinstein, C. & Levin, H. 1998 Plasma oxytocin levels in
autistic children. Biol. Psychiatry 43, 270 277. (doi:10.
1016/S0006-3223(97)00439-3)
Neumann, I. D. 2007 Stimuli and consequences of
dendritic release of oxytocin within the brain.
Biochem. Soc. Trans. 35, 1252–1257. (doi:10.1042/BS
T0351252)
Social vocalizations L. J. Seltzer et al. 2665
Proc. R. Soc. B (2010)
on September 1, 2010rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
Seltzer, L. J. & Ziegler, T. E. 2007 Non-invasive measure-
ment of small peptides in the common marmoset
(Callithrix jacchus): a radiolabeled clearance study and
endogenous excretion under varying social conditions.
Horm. Behav. 51, 436–442. (doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.
12.012)
Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. L. 1984 Grooming, alliances
and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature 308,
541–543. (doi:10.1038/308541a0)
Silk, J. B. 2007 Social components of fitness in primate
groups. Science 317, 1347–1351. (doi:10.1126/science.
1140734)
Takayanagi, Y. et al. 2005 Pervasive social deficits, but
normal parturition, in oxytocin receptor-deficient mice.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 16 096–16 101. (doi:10.
1073/pnas.0505312102)
Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L.,
Gurung, R. A. & Updegraff, J. A. 2000 Biobehavioral
responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not
fight-or-flight. Psychol. Rev. 107, 411– 429. (doi:10.
1037/0033-295X.107.3.411)
Uvna¨ s-Moberg, K. 1996 Neuroendocrinology of the mother-
child interaction. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 7, 126–131.
(doi:10.1016/1043-2760(96)00036-7)
Uvna¨ s-Moberg, K. 1997 Physiological and endocrine effects
of social contact. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 807, 146–163.
(doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb51917.x)
Uvna¨ s-Moberg, K. 1998 Oxytocin may mediate the bene-
fits of positive social interaction and emotions.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 819 835. (doi:10.1016/
S0306-4530(98)00056-0)
Wismer Fries, A. B., Ziegler, T. E., Kurian, J. R., Jacoris,
S. & Pollak, S. D. 2005 Early experience in humans is
associated with changes in neuropeptides critical
for regulating social behavior. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci.
USA 102, 17 237 –17 240. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0504
767102)
Ziegler, T. E., Scheffler, G. & Snowdon, C. T. 1995 The
relationship of cortisol levels to social environment and
reproductive functioning in female cotton-top tamarins,
Saguinus oepdipus.Horm. Behav. 29, 407– 424. (doi:10.
1006/hbeh.1995.1028)
2666 L. J. Seltzer et al. Social vocalizations
Proc. R. Soc. B (2010)
on September 1, 2010rspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDownloaded from
... Preliminary support for such a care-related oxytocin effect was found in two studies. First, Seltzer et al. (2010) exposed children between 7 and 12 years old to the Trier Social Stress Test for Children (TSST-C; Kirschbaum et al., 1993) to induce stress. Afterwards, children were assigned to one of three conditions: 1) a reunion with their mother who comforted them verbally and physically, 2) comfort by their mother provided over the phone, or 3) watching a neutral movie clip alone as a control condition. ...
... To increase confidence in the causal effect of support on children's oxytocin levels, a within-subject design is necessary. Since contemporary theory and research suggest that oxytocin responds to attachment-related situations, our first research aim was to replicate the studies of Seltzer et al. (2010) and of Brockington et al. (2021) with a within-subject design, to minimize effects related to interperson variability. Furthermore, within-subject designs are more powerful than betweensubject designs . ...
... In sum, the current study had two research aims. The first aim was to replicate Seltzer's et al. (2010), Brockington et al. (2021), and Vandevivere et al. (2018) observations that care after exposure to stress has an immediate, positive effect on children's oxytocin and secure state attachment levels. Adding to the literature, we tested these effects using a within-subject design. ...
... Oxytocin not just works in an endocrine fashion but also acts as an important neuromodulator within many regions in the central and peripheral nervous systems, and at the receptor and synaptic levels the peptide can interact with both the dopaminergic and opioid systems (Paletta et al. 2022;Putnam and Chang 2022). Its role in attachment and bonding is clear (e.g., Fujiwara et al. 2019;Kim et al. 2014;Seltzer, Ziegler, and Pollak 2010;Strathearn et al. 2009), and in adults measured oxytocin levels are higher with communal music making (Grape et al. 2003;Keeler et al. 2015;Kreutz 2014;Ooishi et al. 2017). ...
... This unique social context may reduce social demands while preserving some advantages of social proximityincluding a state of calm or equanimity, supported by bodily synchronization and emotional attunement (92). Additionally, oxytocin is released in response to many social behaviors that are intentionally renounced during retreat-including affiliative vocalizations, eye contact, social touch, and sexual behavior (93)(94)(95). As such, the withdrawal from these normative forms of social interaction may have contributed to the observed decreases in oxytocin while on retreat. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Given its putative roles in mediating prosocial behavior, attachment bonds, and stress physiology, oxytocin modulation has been hypothesized to be a biological correlate of the salubrious effects of meditation practice. Here we investigated the effects of a month-long silent meditation retreat on changes in oxytocin, and the related hormone and vasopressin, in relation to psychosocial changes in attachment style, anxiety, personality measures, and feelings of social connectedness with fellow meditators. Methods Plasma oxytocin and vasopressin and self-report questionnaires were measured in retreat participants (n = 28) at the beginning of, and 3 weeks into, a residential meditation retreat. Control participants (n = 34), who were similar in age, gender, and meditation experience, were also assessed across a 3-week interval. Linear mixed effects models were used to assess outcomes. Results The retreat group showed a small but significant decrease in oxytocin compared to controls who showed no change. In the retreat group, higher openness to experience at Time 1 predicted greater reductions in oxytocin during the retreat, and lower oxytocin at Time 2 was related to stronger feelings of personal connection with fellow meditators. The changes in oxytocin were not related to attachment style or anxiety. Vasopressin decreased over time across both groups, suggesting no specific effect of retreat. Conclusion These preliminary findings suggest that meditation training in the context of a silent residential retreat may reduce circulating levels of oxytocin. We interpret this finding from multiple theoretical perspectives, discussing key measurement limitations and proposing future study designs that may help to differentiate the effects of different meditation practices and contexts on oxytocin signaling.
... Positive attachments continue to buffer stress beyond infancy and throughout middle childhood as well. When 7-12-year-old girls were administered the TSST-C (child version) [25], those who were able to speak to their mother returned to baseline cortisol faster than those whose mothers were unavailable after the TSST-C was administered [26]. These studies point to how parent-child relationships influence the development of stress regulation throughout childhood, as well as through early adolescence. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: The self-medication hypothesis (SMH) suggests that individuals consume alcohol to alleviate stressful emotions. Still, the underlying mechanisms between stress and heavy episodic drinking remain to be explored. Impaired control over drinking (IC) reflects a failure of self-regulation specific to the drinking context, with individuals exceeding self-prescribed limits. Parenting styles experienced during childhood have a lasting influence on the stress response, which may contribute to IC. Method: We examined the indirect influences of parenting styles (e.g., permissive, authoritarian, and authoritative) on heavy episodic drinking and alcohol-related problems through the mediating mechanisms of stress and IC. We fit a latent measurement model with 938 (473 men; 465 women) university students, utilizing bootstrap confidence intervals, in Mplus 8.0. Results: Higher levels of authoritative parenting (mother and father) were indirectly linked to fewer alcohol-related problems and less heavy episodic drinking through less stress and IC. Maternal permissiveness was indirectly linked to more alcohol-related problems and heavy episodic drinking through more stress and, in turn, more IC. Impaired control appeared to be a mediator for stress and alcohol-related problems. Conclusions: Maternal permissiveness contributes to the use of alcohol to alleviate stress. Thus, reducing stress may reduce problematic heavy drinking and alcohol problems among emerging adults with high IC who may also have experienced permissive parenting. Stress may exacerbate behavioral dysregulation of drinking within self-prescribed limits.
... Oxytocin has been measured in different types of samples [8][9][10][11]. The use of saliva has the advantages of being non-invasive, easy to collect and non-stressful [12]. In addition, saliva has a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 been demonstrated to be more sensitive to serum in some situations. ...
Article
Full-text available
The objective of this research was to develop and validate two immunoassays for oxytocin measurement in human saliva, one using a monoclonal and the other a polyclonal antibody against oxytocin, whose affinity for oxytocin was tested by an antibody mapping epitope analysis. These assays were analytically validated and used to compare oxytocin concentrations with those obtained with a commercial kit before and after the extraction or reduction/alkylation (R/A) treatments to saliva samples. The assays were also used to evaluate changes in salivary oxytocin concentrations following a physical effort and an induced psychological stress, which have previously been described as situations that cause an increase in salivary oxytocin. Both assays showed to be precise and accurate in the validation studies, and the antibodies used showed a defined binding region in case of the monoclonal antibody, whereas the polyclonal antibody showed binding events through all the oxytocin sequence. Although the monoclonal and polyclonal assays showed a positive correlation, they give results in a different range of magnitude. Both assays showed significant increases in oxytocin concentrations when applied after the physical effort and the psychological stress. This study shows that a variability in the reported values of oxytocin can occur depending on the assay and indicates that the use of different types of antibodies can give a different range of values when measuring oxytocin in saliva.
... Peripheral measures of OT have thus traditionally been used as a surrogate including blood (serum, plasma) as well as alternative biological matrices such as saliva and urine. These peripheral measures have been used in a wide range of studies which include obesity and metabolism [7], neuropsychiatric disorders [8], and social behaviors [9]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Oxytocin (OT) is a peptide hormone synthesized in the hypothalamus and released into systemic circulation or other areas of the brain. Its physiological roles include action as a hormone with stimulation of uterine contractions and that as a neuromodulator with involvement in social behaviors and regulation of mood. Its small size and low levels within biological matrices make it challenging to accurately measure. The goal of this study was to demonstrate the specificity of the antibody, sensitivity, and reproducibility of the Phoenix Pharmaceuticals (PP) OT radioimmunoassay (RIA) for use in human urine, serum, and saliva. Specificity of the antibody was assessed by high pressure liquid chromatography with ultraviolet (HPLC-UV) separation and assay of the fractions. Immunoreactivity was evaluated using the percent OT bound, and the fraction retention times were compared to the retention time of an intact OT standard to determine which fractions contained OT in the extracted samples. Reproducibility was assessed by running replicates of pools of each biomatrix over several assays. Sensitivity was assessed by repeated measurement of physiologically relevant low-concentration specimens. In all tested specimens the greatest reactivity in assay corresponded to the same fraction(s) as the OT standard. Only minimal reactivity was found in the other fractions, suggesting that in an unfractionated sample the antibody reacts mostly with intact OT. Reproducibility was acceptable for all specimens and the coefficient of variation (CV) ranged from 3.72 to 8.04% and 5.89–12.8%, for intra and inter-assay, respectively. The limits of quantitation (LOQ) were sufficient for measurement of normal values in urine (0.643 & 1.43 pg/mL), serum (1.90 pg/mL), and saliva pools (0.485 & 4.42 pg/mL). In conclusion, the PP OT RIA is specific and sensitive enough for reproducible measurement of intact OT in human peripheral biological matrices.
Article
Human society is organized in structured social networks upon which large-scale cooperation among genetically unrelated individuals is favored and persists. Such large-scale cooperation is crucial for the success of the human species but also one of the most puzzling challenges. Recent work in social and behavioral neuroscience has linked human cooperation to oxytocin, an evolutionarily ancient and structurally preserved hypothalamic neuropeptide. This review aims to elucidate how oxytocin promotes nonkin cooperation in social networks by reviewing its effects at three distinct levels: individual cooperation, the formation of interpersonal relationships, and the establishment of heterogeneous network structures. We propose oxytocin as a proximate mechanism for fostering large-scale cooperation in human societies. Specifically, oxytocin plays an important role in facilitating network-wide cooperation in human societies by 1) increasing individual cooperation, mitigating noncooperation motives, and facilitating the enforcement of cooperative norms; 2) fostering interpersonal bonding and synchronization; and 3) facilitating the formation of heterogeneous network structures.
Article
The Concept “I will gladly pay you on Tuesday for a hamburger today.” Saying this makes a promise under the right circumstances. If the vendor accepts, a contract arises from the exchange. Extending beyond this fragment of standard contract law, there are more basic elements of moral and communal survival at stake. Promising introduces the derived concept of obligation. Failure to live up to one’s obligation is a source of being ashamed of oneself and being the subject of shaming by others (Moffett, 2018; 228-30). Once a promise is accepted and service or good provided, a contract exists and is often enforceable by law. But, even without the law, failing to live up to one’s obligations subjects the promiser to sanctions such as shaming and even punishment. “Dad, you promised!” Is an oft heard challenge when a promise is broken to a child. Very young children intuit that in promising, a person becomes obligated to one or more others (Mercier, 2020; p.92). Failing to live up to a voluntarily created obligation on oneself is expected to result in guilt if that same person aborts the obligation others came to rely upon. Legitimate excuses are license to abort an otherwise obligatory commitment. But to be legitimate, the excuses must be recognized by most others as plausibly discounting the afore mentioned obligation.
Article
Humans exhibit what appears to be a unique vocal property: octave equivalence, whereby adult male voices are, on average, an octave lower in pitch than those of adult females and children. The evolutionary significance of this seems largely to have escaped notice. While sexual selection might explain why male voices are generally lower, it cannot explain why they should be so much lower than what would be expected for body size or why the average difference should be exactly one octave. Nor does a generalized dimorphism convey why precisely tuned octaves feature so commonly in human vocal interaction. The octave features strongly in the organization of music. A consequence of this characteristic of human pitch perception and production is the capacity to share and respond to vocal pitches (and their instrumental equivalents) as if they are “the same” irrespective of the difference in range, a phenomenon known as octave equivalence. We investigate the nature of octave equivalence from an adaptive perspective and propose a hypothesis for its evolution based on the importance of chorusing for social bonding and pitch matching in intergenerational exchange.
Article
Full-text available
Since de Waal & van Roosmalen (1979) rst documented the occurrence of reconciliation between former opponents in captive chimpanzees, the study of the post-conn ict behaviour of primates has provided valuable information about some of the details of primate social organisation. The vast majority of these studies have been carried out on captive subjects and it has been assumed that these ndings are representative of wild primates. We set out to investigate whether this was true for the Sonso community of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Budongo Forest, Uganda, using controlled procedures comparable with those used in captive studies. We found that these chimpanzees were much less likely to reconcile than their captive counterparts. Only one dimension of relationship quality had an effect on the likelihood of reconciliation. Individuals which were highly compatible, in terms of time spent af liating, reconciled con icts more often than those with weak relationships. Captive chimpanzees have also been shown to 'console' one another (de Waal & van Roosmalen, 1979; de Waal & Aureli, 1996), where uninvolved bystanders initiate af liative contacts with victims of aggression. This study did not con rm that consolatory behaviour was characteristic of wild chimpanzee post-conn ict behaviour. Nor did these chimpanzees use explicit gestures during post-con ict interactions as they have been shown to do in two out of three captive studies. We conclude that the post-con ict behaviour of 1) Corresponding author; e-mail address: ka11@st-andrews.ac.uk 2) We thank Prof. Vernon Reynolds, the staff of the Budongo Forest Project, and J. Kakura, in particular, for his assistance in the eld. We are grateful to Filippo Aureli and one anonymous reviewer for detailed comments on an earlier version of this paper.
Article
Full-text available
Cortisol levels were examined in 17 cotton-top tamarin monkeys (Saguinus oedipus) to determine: (1) if first-morning void urine sampling could be used as a noninvasive method for monitoring cortisol excretion. (2) if capture and bleeding were associated with changes in urinary cortisol levels, (3) the relative cortisol levels in reproductively active and reproductively suppressed females, and (4) the relationship between cortisol levels and changes in social condition in cotton-up tamarins. Mean urinary cortisol levels during ovarian cycling did not differ between captured and bled females and undisturbed females. Mean cortisol levels were significantly lower in females who were housed in their natal groups and reproductively suppressed than in the same females when they were removed and placed adjacent to a novel male and ovarian cycling began. For all females, mean cortisol levels were higher during the periovulatory period than during the nonperiovulatory period of the ovulatory cycle, with mean cortisol levels higher in newly cycling females than in long-term cycling females. No differences were found in mean cortisol levels between long-term cycling females and prepubertal females and postpubertal natal females. Cortisol levels from long-term cycling females were much lower than during the first two to three ovarian cycles occurring in newly cycling female tamarins, during the last part of pregnancy, and during the first 6 weeks postpartum. These data provide evidence that (1) suppression of ovarian cycling in postpubertal females remaining in their natal family is not associated with stress-induced changes in cortisol levels. (2) cortisol levels are not strictly tied to reproductive condition, and (3) social change may be reflected in cortisol elevation.
Article
Full-text available
The macroevolutionary events leading to neural innovations for social communication, such as vocalization, are essentially unexplored. Many fish vocalize during female courtship and territorial defense, as do amphibians, birds, and mammals. Here, we map the neural circuitry for vocalization in larval fish and show that the vocal network develops in a segment-like region across the most caudal hindbrain and rostral spinal cord. Taxonomic analysis demonstrates a highly conserved pattern between fish and all major lineages of vocal tetrapods. We propose that the vocal basis for acoustic communication among vertebrates evolved from an ancestrally shared developmental compartment already present in the early fishes.
Article
Full-text available
Social impairments are central to the syndrome of autism. The neuropeptide oxytocin (OT) has been implicated in the regulation of social behavior in animals but has not yet been examined in autistic subjects. To determine whether autistic children have abnormalities in OT, midday plasma samples from 29 autistic and 30 age-matched normal children, all prepubertal, were analyzed by radioimmunoassay for levels of OT. Despite individual variability and overlapping group distributions, the autistic group had significantly lower plasma OT levels than the normal group. OT increased with age in the normal but not the autistic children. Elevated OT was associated with higher scores on social and developmental measures for the normal children, but was associated with lower scores for the autistic children. These relationships were strongest in a subset of autistic children identified as aloof. Although making inferences to central OT functioning from peripheral measurement is difficult, the data suggest that OT abnormalities may exist in autism, and that more direct investigation of central nervous system OT function is warranted.
Article
During breastfeeding or suckling, maternal oxytocin levels are raised by somatosensory stimulation. Oxytocin may, however, also be released by nonnoxious stimuli such as touch, warm temperature etc. in plasma and in cerebrospinal fluid. Consequently, oxytocin may be involved in physiological and behavioral effects induced by social interaction in a more general context. In both male and female rats oxytocin exerts potent physiological antistress effects. If daily oxytocin injections are repeated over a 5-day period, blood pressure is decreased by 10–20 mmHg, the withdrawal latency to heat stimuli is prolonged, cortisol levels are decreased and insulin and cholecystokinin levels are increased. These effects last from 1 to several weeks after the last injection. After repeated oxytocin treatment weight gain may be promoted and the healing rate of wounds increased. Most behavioral and physiological effects induced by oxytocin can be blocked by oxytocin antagonists. In contrast, the antistress effects can not, suggesting that unidentified oxytocin receptors may exist. The prolonged latency in the tail-flick test can be temporarily reversed by administration of naloxone, suggesting that endogenous opioid activity has been increased by the oxytocin injections. In contrast, the long-term lowering of blood pressure and of cortisol levels as well as the sedative effects of oxytocin have been found to be related to an increased activity of central α2-adrenoceptors. Positive social interactions have been related to health-promoting effects. Oxytocin released in response to social stimuli may be part of a neuroendocrine substrate which underlies the benefits of positive social experiences. Such processes may in addition explain the health-promoting effects of certain alternative therapies. Because of the special properties of oxytocin, including the fact that it can become conditioned to psychological state or imagery, oxytocin may also mediate the benefits attributed to therapies such as hypnosis or meditation. © 1998 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
Article
Previous experiments have shown that an exposure to defined stressors activates not only the ‘classical’ endocrine stress response but also the intrahypothalamic and peripheral release of oxytocin. In the present study we investigated the effects of an acute social defeat experience on the release of oxytocin within the hypothalamic supraoptic nucleus, just outside of the supraoptic nucleus toward the midline within the anterior ventro-lateral part of the hypothalamus, and into plasma of adult male rats. Our results demonstrate that emotional stress triggers the release of oxytocin into the extracellular fluid of both the supraoptic nucleus and the anterior ventro-lateral part of the hypothalamus (up to ≈320% and 170%, respectively). Interestingly, oxytocin release within the latter brain area, which is likely to originate from axons forming the hypothalamo-neurohypophysial tract, was higher in absolute terms than that within the supraoptic nucleus itself, both under basal conditions and in response to social defeat. In contrast to intrahypothalamic release patterns, plasma oxytocin levels remained virtually unchanged upon stressor exposure. This demonstrates that the release of oxytocin within the hypothalamus is triggered by emotional stress. Furthermore, it indicates that under physiological conditions the release of oxytocin from the dendrites and somata upon axon terminals in the neurohypophysis is differentially regulated. Although not yet studied in detail, it may be hypothesized that the spatial and temporal release pattern of oxytocin is controlled by integrative neuronal networks at different brain levels (including hypothalamus and posterior pituitary) to ensure the appropriate involvement of this peptide in the stress response of the animal.
Article
Positive social interactions and social support may protect against various forms of mental and physical illness, although the mechanisms for these effects are not well identified. The socially monogamous prairie vole, which – like humans – forms social bonds and displays high levels of parasympathetic activity, has provided a useful model for investigating neurobiological systems that mediate the consequences of sociality. In the present study, adult female prairie voles were exposed to social isolation or continued pairing with a female sibling (control conditions) for 4 weeks. During weeks 3 and 4 of this period, animals were administered oxytocin (20 μg/50 μl, SC) or saline vehicle (50 μl, SC) daily for a total of 14 days. In Experiment 1, autonomic parameters were recorded during and following isolation or pairing. Isolation (vs. pairing) significantly increased basal heart rate (HR) and reduced HR variability and vagal regulation of the heart; these changes in isolated animals were prevented with oxytocin administration. In Experiment 2, behaviors relevant to depression [sucrose intake and swimming in the forced swim test (FST)] were measured as a function of isolation. Isolation reduced sucrose intake and increased immobility in the FST; these behaviors also were prevented by oxytocin. Administration of oxytocin did not significantly alter cardiac, autonomic or behavioral responses of paired animals. These findings support the hypothesis that oxytocinergic mechanisms can protect against behavioral and cardiac dysfunction in response to chronic social stressors, and can provide insight into social influences on behavior and autonomic function in humans.