ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Worldwide cannabis dependence is increasing, as is the concentration of Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in street cannabis. At the same time, the concentration of the second most abundant cannabinoid in street cannabis, cannabidiol (CBD), is decreasing. These two cannabinoids have opposing effects both pharmacologically and behaviorally when administered in the laboratory. No research has yet examined how the ratio of these constituents impacts on the appetitive/reinforcing effects of cannabis in humans. A total of 94 cannabis users were tested 7 days apart, once while non-intoxicated and once while acutely under the influence of their own chosen smoked cannabis on dependence-related measures. Using an unprecedented methodology, a sample of cannabis (as well as saliva) was collected from each user and analyzed for levels of cannabinoids. On the basis of CBD : THC ratios in the cannabis, individuals from the top and bottom tertiles were directly compared on indices of the reinforcing effects of drugs, explicit liking, and implicit attentional bias to drug stimuli. When intoxicated, smokers of high CBD : THC strains showed reduced attentional bias to drug and food stimuli compared with smokers of low CBD : THC. Those smoking higher CBD : THC strains also showed lower self-rated liking of cannabis stimuli on both test days. Our findings suggest that CBD has potential as a treatment for cannabis dependence. The acute modulation of the incentive salience of drug cues by CBD may possibly generalize to a treatment for other addictive disorders.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Cannabidiol Attenuates the Appetitive Effects of
D
9
-Tetrahydrocannabinol in Humans Smoking Their
Chosen Cannabis
Celia JA Morgan*
,1
, Tom P Freeman
1
, Gra
´inne L Schafer
1
and H Valerie Curran
1
1
Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit, Research Department of Clinical, Health and Educational Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Worldwide cannabis dependence is increasing, as is the concentration of D
9
-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in street cannabis. At the
same time, the concentration of the second most abundant cannabinoid in street cannabis, cannabidiol (CBD), is decreasing. These two
cannabinoids have opposing effects both pharmacologically and behaviorally when administered in the laboratory. No research has yet
examined how the ratio of these constituents impacts on the appetitive/reinforcing effects of cannabis in humans. A total of 94 cannabis
users were tested 7 days apart, once while non-intoxicated and once while acutely under the influence of their own chosen smoked
cannabis on dependence-related measures. Using an unprecedented methodology, a sample of cannabis (as well as saliva) was collected
from each user and analyzed for levels of cannabinoids. On the basis of CBD : THC ratios in the cannabis, individuals from the top and
bottom tertiles were directly compared on indices of the reinforcing effects of drugs, explicit liking, and implicit attentional bias to drug
stimuli. When intoxicated, smokers of high CBD : THC strains showed reduced attentional bias to drug and food stimuli compared with
smokers of low CBD : THC. Those smoking higher CBD : THC strains also showed lower self-rated liking of cannabis stimuli on both test
days. Our findings suggest that CBD has potential as a treatment for cannabis dependence. The acute modulation of the incentive
salience of drug cues by CBD may possibly generalize to a treatment for other addictive disorders.
Neuropsychopharmacology (2010) 35, 1879–1885; doi:10.1038/npp.2010.58; published online 28 April 2010
Keywords: cannabis; THC; cannabidiol; attention bias; addiction; dependence
INTRODUCTION
Cannabis is the world’s most popular illicit substance.
Although cannabis dependence was a rare phenomenon
even a decade ago, data from the European Monitoring
Centre for Drugs and Drug Abuse (EMCDDA, 2006) show
that the numbers of people seeking treatment for
dependence have increased markedly since 1999. Over a
similar time period, there also seems to have been a marked
change in the constituents of the cannabis available on the
street.
Cannabis contains around 70 different chemicals, which
are unique to the plant and called cannabinoids. The main
psychoactive ingredient is D
9
-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC)
and this is thought to produce the effects that users seek
(Curran et al, 2002). When given intravenously to healthy
humans, THC produces psychotic-like and anxiogenic
effects (D’Souza et al, 2004, 2008). In contrast, cannabidiol
(CBD), another major constituent of most strains of
cannabis, seems to have anti-psychotic properties (Zuardi
et al, 2006), and is anxiolytic (Guimares et al, 1990) and
may be neuroprotective in humans (Hermann et al, 2007).
THC and CBD have been found to have opposing
neuropharmacological actionsFthe former is an partial
agonist, whereas the latter is an antagonist at CB1 and CB2
receptors (Pertwee, 2008). CBD has also been suggested to
inhibit the reuptake of the endogenous cannabinoid,
anandamide (Bitencourt et al, 2008). The relative THC/
CBD ratio of cannabis varies greatly. Levels of CBD can
range from virtually none to up to 40% (Hardwick and
King, 2008). Higher levels of THC are found in hydro-
ponically grown varieties such as ‘skunk’ and in cross-bred
strains, which are increasingly dominating the illicit drug
market.
In addition to effects on psychotic symptoms and anxiety,
THC and CBD may have opposing effects in the processes
involved in addiction. The reinforcing effects of THC have
been repeatedly shown. Synthetic THC produces condi-
tioned place preference in rats and decreases the threshold
for intercranial self-stimulation in animal studies (see
Cooper and Haney, 2009 for a review). CBD is not acutely
reinforcing in rats (Vann et al, 2008). However, CBD has
been shown to reverse the conditioned place preference
effect induced by THC in CBD : THC ratios of 1 : 1 and 1 : 10
(Vann et al, 2008), suggesting that it may modulate the
Received 13 January 2010; revised 18 March 2010; accepted 22 March
2010
*Correspondence: Dr CJA Morgan, Clinical Psychopharmacology Unit,
Research Department of Clinical, Health and Educational Psychology,
University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK,
Tel: + 44 207 679 1932, Fax: + 44 207 916 1989,
E-mail: c.morgan@ucl.ac.uk
Neuropsychopharmacology (2010) 35, 1879 1885
&
2010 Nature Publishing Group All rights reserved 0893-133X/10
$
32.00
www.neuropsychopharmacology.org
reinforcing effects of THC. CBD has also been suggested to
have a function in the modulation of addictive behavior.
Preclinical studies have shown that acute administration of
CBD can enhance extinction of both cocaine and amphe-
tamine conditioned place preference (Parker et al, 2004).
CBD has also been found to attenuate the reinstatement of
opioid seeking in rats (Ren et al, 2009).
Given the opposing neuropharmacological actions of
THC and CBD, and the capacity of CBD to modulate the
acute reinforcing effects of THC in rats, we hypothesized
that CBD may also counteract some of the reinforcing
effects of THC in humans. This study set out to test these
hypotheses by using a novel methodology, which enabled
analysis of cannabinoids in the cannabis actually smoked by
each individual user.
To index relevant aspects of reinforcing effects, we aimed
to tap into not only the explicit ‘liking’ of a drug, but also
the implicit ‘wanting’ (Robinson and Berridge, 2008). One
way in which the latter has been assessed is by examining
attentional bias to drugs of abuse. It is well known that
with the progression from drug use to abuse and on
to dependence, a drug user’s attention becomes drawn to
drug-related stimuli more than earlier reinforcing ‘natural’
rewards (Robinson and Berridge, 2001) and this can be
investigated by using attentional bias tasks. Degree of
attentional bias predicts relapse in cigarette smokers
(Waters et al, 2003) and opiate-dependent individuals
(Marissen et al, 2006) and as such relates to level of
dependence. Attentional bias toward cannabis-related
stimuli has been earlier reported in cannabis users
(Field et al, 2006), but no study has investigated the
impact of smoking different strains of cannabis may have
on such processes. We, therefore, used a ‘dot-probe’
paradigm as an attentional bias task to assess implicit
wanting of both cannabis stimuli and food stimuli (as a
natural reinforcer influenced by cannabis), and ratings of
pleasantness to assess the explicit liking of the cannabis and
food stimuli.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Design and Participants
A repeated measures design compared a sample of 94
cannabis users aged between 16 and 24 years on two test
occasions approximately 7 days apart. Inclusion criteria
required that participants had English as a native language,
were not dyslexic, had no history of psychotic illnesses, and
had normal or corrected-to-normal color vision. Partici-
pants were also excluded if they gave a positive saliva
sample (above cutoffs for cannabis use in the past 4–6 h) for
THC or CBD on the non-intoxicated day. The cannabis-
using group was required to use the drug at least once a
month for at least 1 year. They were recruited by word of
mouth and ‘snowball sampling’ (Solowij et al, 1992). Data
are first reported on the overall sample; to facilitate analysis
of the impact of THC and CBD, the sample was divided into
upper and lower tertiles (each n¼32) on the basis of
individual CBD : THC ratios in the cannabis actually
smoked.
All participants provided written, witnessed, informed
consent on both occasions. This study was approved by the
UCL Graduate School Ethics committee and its aims were
supported by the UK Home Office.
Procedure
All participants were tested on two separate occasions. One
testing session occurred when cannabis users were under
the influence of the drug (intoxicated day) and the other
when drug free (drug-free day) with session order being
counterbalanced. Participants were required to abstain from
recreational drugs and alcohol for 24 h before testing
commenced. A sample of the cannabis each participant
smoked was taken on the intoxicated day and analyzed for
levels of THC and CBD (Forensic Science Service, UK).
Saliva samples were also taken for analysis of cannabinoids,
a screening analysis was performed, and then confirmation
analysis by liquid chromatography mass spectrometry. A
urine sample was collected before cannabis use on the
intoxicated day for later analysis of THC metabolite in
urine. Instant urine tests were administered on the drug-
free day to confirm abstinence from other drugs (opiates,
cocaine, amphetamine, benzodiazepines, and other related
compounds; a positive result for THC occurs if a minimum
of 50 ng/ml of THC metabolite is present in the urine
sample; however, THC remains detectable in the body for
up to 4 weeks so 24-h abstinence of cannabis users was not
verifiable). On the intoxicated day, participants smoked the
cannabis at the site of testing in front of the experimenter,
which was usually at their own home. They were asked to
smoke an amount of cannabis that was typical for them to
become ‘stoned.’ The experimenter weighed this sample
before they made the ‘joint’ and then collected 0.3 g of the
same cannabis for analysis. The experimenter noted
whether the sample was ‘skunk,’ herbal cannabis, or resin.
Participants then completed the assessments described
below beginning 1–5 min after they had finished smoking.
For cognitive and dependence-related measures, the task
versions were balanced across the two testing days and
session order. Participants also completed the Severity of
Dependence Scale (Gossop et al, 1995), a brief 5-item
questionnaire regarding their drug use, the Wechsler Adult
Reading Test (WTAR; Wechsler, 2001) to estimate their
reading ability as an analog of premorbid IQ, and self-
reported their drug use in a drug history questionnaire. The
assessments reported here formed part of a wider test
battery on which data collection is still underway. After
testing, participants were fully debriefed and compensated
for their time.
Assessments
Dot-probe task. A computer-based dot-probe paradigm was
used to assess attentional bias to both drug- and food-
related stimuli. The 10 color photographs of cannabis-
related stimuli and 10 color photographs of food-related
stimuli were used, with each image simultaneously paired
with a neutral photograph matched as closely as possible for
visual composition and complexity (see Figure 1 for an
example). A total of 80 of the 160 total trials were
critical trials of which 40 featured cannabis-related and 40
food-related stimuli, each presented twice for 250 ms and
twice for 2000 ms. These two exposure times were used to
CBD reduces the rewarding effects of THC
CJA Morgan et al
1880
Neuropsychopharmacology
tap automatic (250 ms) and controlled (2000 ms) proces-
sing. The critical (food- or drug-related) images appeared
once on the left and once on the right at each time
interval. The side at which the probe appeared was
counterbalanced across all the trials. An asterisk was used
as the probe.
A total of 10 neutral practice trial pairs were used as
training, followed by two blocks of 80 experimental trials.
There was a short break between blocks. Each trial began
with a central fixation cross shown for 1000 ms, after which
a pair of matched images would appear, one on each side of
the fixation cross, for either the long (2000 ms) or short
(250 ms) duration. Both images then disappeared revealing
the probe behind one of the two images. Participants were
required to respond to the probe as quickly as possible
by pressing a button corresponding to the relevant side of
the screen. Attentional bias was calculated as the difference
in reaction time between when the probe replaced the
neutral compared with the incentive (drug/food) stimulus
[RtneutralFRTincentive], such that a greater difference
indicated greater bias toward that stimulus.
Picture-rating task. After the dot-probe task, participants
completed a picture-rating task as a measure of explicit
liking for drug and food stimuli. They rated each picture
earlier used in the dot-probe task on a 7-point scale, ranging
from 3 (very unpleasant) to + 3 (very pleasant).
Marijuana craving questionnaire (Heishman et al, 2009).
A short 12-item questionnaire was given to assess current
craving for cannabis.
Visual analog scale. A 100 mm visual analog scale (VAS)
anchoring from ‘not at all stoned,’ to ‘extremely stoned’ was
administered.
Statistical Analysis
Data are first reported on the overall sample. Owing to trace
levels of CBD in the majority of the participants, therefore,
we subdivided the groups on the basis of CBD41% and
then excluded the middle third to compare equal group
sizes who differed in their CBD content. Using the
CBD : THC ratio groups, dependence-related data were
subjected to a 2 2 repeated measures ANOVA with ratio
(high CBD : THC; low CBD : THC) as the between subjects
factor and day (intoxicated, drug free) as the within subjects
factor. Post hoc comparisons were Bonferroni corrected
one-way ANOVAs to explore interactions, or Bonferroni
comparisons to explore main effects.
RESULTS
Demographics and Drug Use Data
Whole sample. Over the whole sample, the mean age of
participants was 21.3±1.42 years, there were 72 males and
22 females, and participants had spent a mean of
14.67±2.11 years in education with a mean WTAR score
of 42.86±6.52. Cannabis was used as a mean of 13.9±11.53
days per month.
Sub-group analyses. High CBD : THC ratio vs low
CBD : THC ratio groups. There were no differences in
demographic variables between these two cannabis smoking
groups (Table 1). There were also no differences in self-
reported use of cannabis or clinician rated dependence on
the SDS. However, for drug use variables, individuals from
the high CBD : THC ratio group drank alcohol more
frequently than the low CBD : THC group [F(1,57) ¼4.32,
p¼0.042]. There were no significant group differences for
when alcohol was last used before the non-intoxicated day.
Figure 1 An example of a cannabis/neutral and a food/neutral-matched pair of images.
CBD reduces the rewarding effects of THC
CJA Morgan et al
1881
Neuropsychopharmacology
There was significantly greater THC content [U¼286.0,
p¼0.002] and lower CBD content [U¼76.0, po0.001] in
the low CBD : THC ratio group.
Salivary levels on the intoxicated day showed only a trend
for a group difference in CBD [U¼248.5, p¼0.099], but no
differences in salivary levels of THC.
No significant difference was found between the two
groups of urinary levels of THC acid from the samples taken
on the intoxicated day. From the instant drug test results on
the non-intoxicated, day, w
2
analysis found no significant
group differences in positive results for THC metabolite.
The w
2
analysis also found a significant difference in the
type of cannabis smoked between the groups [w
2
(4) ¼43.79,
po0.001] reflecting that all the low CBD : THC ratio group
had smoked ‘skunk’ varieties (see Table 2).
Dependence-related measures. Dot-probe task Reaction
times o100 ms or 41000 ms were excluded from the
analysis in line with earlier dot-probe studies (Duka and
Townshend, 2004) and this excluded two participants, one
from each CBD : THC ratio group. A 2 222 repeated
measures ANOVA with the additional within subjects
factors of stimulus type (food, drug) and picture duration
(short, long) found a significant day CBD : THC ra-
tio duration interaction [F(1,57) ¼6.31, p¼0.015] and a
trend for a day type interaction [F(1,57) ¼3.31, p¼
0.073]. Post-hoc exploration of the three-way interaction
showed that the significant day ratio group interaction
was attributable to greater bias to both types of stimuli
in the low CBD : THC ratio group at the short picture
presentation interval on the intoxicated day [F(1,57) ¼5.63,
p¼0.021], but no difference on the non-intoxicated day
(see Figure 2a).
Picture-rating task.A223 repeated measures
ANOVA of ratings of pleasantness of the pictures presented
in the dot-probe task, with the additional factor of stimulus
type (food, drug, neutral) yielded a significant CBD : THC
ratio stimulus type interaction [F(2,118) ¼4.29, p¼
0.016], as well as main effects of stimulus type[F(2,118) ¼
46.52, po0.001] and CBD : THC ratio [F(1,59) ¼7.61,
p¼0.008], but not day. Exploration of the interaction,
depicted in Figure 2b, shows significantly lower ratings of
pleasantness for drug stimuli in the high CBD : THC ratio
group [F(1,59) ¼12.44, p¼0.001], a trend for lower ratings
of pleasantness for food stimuli in the high CBD : THC ratio
group [F(1,59) ¼2.81, p¼0.099], but no group differences
in ratings of neutral stimuli.
MCQ (Table 3) There were no group differences in
craving as assessed by the Marijuana Craving Scale across
the 2 days.
VAS (Table 3) There were no group differences in
‘stoned’ ratings on either day and both groups had similarly
higher ratings on the intoxicated compared with the drug-
free day [F(1,59) ¼299.53, po0.001].
DISCUSSION
The main findings of this study were of reduced attentional
bias to drug and food stimuli in intoxicated individuals
smoking cannabis with a high CBD : THC ratio. We also
found evidence of an overall reduction in ratings of liking of
drug stimuli in high CBD : THC cannabis smokers.
Attentional Bias to Drug Stimuli
Attentional bias to drug stimuli in users when they were
drug free was observed in both CBD : THC groups at the
short, but not the long stimulus exposure interval. This
differentiation most likely reflects automatic processing at
the short interval and accords with ‘incentive sensitization’
processes described in Robinson and Berridge’s (1993,
2003) model of addiction, which is thought to be an
automatic process. The presence of an attentional bias at
this short time interval is consistent with some other studies
(eg Morgan et al, 2008). In drug users, incentive sensitiza-
tion is thought to accumulate over time, whereby drugs of
abuse come to grab attention or act as ‘motivational
magnets,’ eventually more so than natural reinforcers in
Table 1 Demographic and CBD and THC Data Across the Two
User Groups in the Sample
Low CBD : THC
ratio (N¼30),
mean (SD)
High CBD : THC
ratio (N¼31),
mean (SD)
Age 21.19±1.53 21.6±1.22
Number of years in education 14.55±1.85 15±1.78
Age at which cannabis first tried 15.34±2.36 14.77±1.98
How often cannabis is used (days
per month)
13.33±10.93 14.55±12.3
Time to smoke 1/8th ounce of
cannabis (days)
11.43±12.90 25.00±35.60
SDS total 3.06±2.7 2.8±2.28
Total WTAR score 42.78±4.99 44.17±6.53
Number of units used per session 10±4.6 8.44±4.43
How often is alcohol drunk (days
per month)
8.6±5.88 12.27±7.4*
Number of days since last alcohol
use
5.067±10.929 10.138±38.80
Salivary THC intoxicated (ng/ml) 21.20±42.7 15.97±28.81
Salivary CBD intoxicated (ng/ml) 0.14±0.51 2.48±7.17
CBD content (% of sample) 0.14±5.41 2.64±2.54*
THC content (% of sample) 11.92±5.41 7.74±4.20*
CBD : THC ratio (CBD/THC) 0.01±0.01 0.35±0.31*
Urinary THC acid : creatinine ratio 90.78±187.88 49.54±109.27
Abbreviations: SDS, severity of dependence scale; WTAR, Wechsler test of
adult reading. *po0.05.
Table 2 Types of Cannabis Collected, Number of Samples in
Each Group, and Corresponding Means (±SD) of CBD/THC
Ratios in Each Sample
Low CBD : THC
ratio
High CBD : THC
ratio
CBD/THC,
mean±SD
Skunk 32 6 0.02±0.02
Herbal 0 11 0.24±0.35
Resin 0 15 0.53±0.22
CBD reduces the rewarding effects of THC
CJA Morgan et al
1882
Neuropsychopharmacology
the environment (Berridge et al, 2009). The present findings
are consistent with those of earlier studies showing that
cannabis, such as other recreational drugs, elicits atten-
tional bias in its users (Field et al, 2006).
When intoxicated with their own chosen cannabis, only
the low CBD : THC group showed an attentional bias to drug
stimuli. In contrast, the high CBD : THC group showed no
evidence of any bias. Thus, even when intoxicated, cannabis
stimuli grabbed the attention of the low CBD : THC
smokers. One might expect that having smoked cannabis,
both groups would reach a level of satiety and so attentional
bias would reduce as motivational state is thought to
modulate the magnitude of conditioned responses on this
task (Duka and Townshend, 2004). However, some research
suggests that endocannabinoids may modulate afferent
satiety signals (Rodriguez et al, 2001), related to cannabis’
capacity to stimulate appetite, which could explain this
finding in the low CBD : THC group.
Higher levels of CBD seemed to remove the attentional
bias to drug stimuli at the short picture presentation
interval. Owing to the short presentation time (250 ms), this
Figure 2 (a) Attentional bias to food and drug stimuli across day, CBD : THC ratio group, and picture presentation interval. (b) Pleasantness rating across
stimulus type and CBD : THC ratio group across both days.
Table 3 Means (±SD) on Self-Ratings of Marijuana Craving and
‘stoned’ of Each CBD : THC Group Across Test Days
Low CBD : THC ratio,
n¼30, mean±SD
High CBD : THC ratio,
n¼31, mean±SD
Intoxicated Drug free Intoxicated Drug free
MCQ 40.52±11.94 41±12.35 37.88±10.48 36.28±11.55
Ratings of
‘stoned’
6.63±2.0 1.34±1.41 6.45±1.94 1.33±1.06
CBD reduces the rewarding effects of THC
CJA Morgan et al
1883
Neuropsychopharmacology
is very unlikely to be a conscious mechanism of attention
aversion. Instead, it may reflect automatic or non-conscious
processing, of which the individual is unaware. This is
commensurate with a lack of CBD : THC group differences
in explicit processing engaged both at the longer stimulus
exposure time in the attentional bias and in self-ratings of
craving and dependence on questionnaire measures. At
longer durations, drug users may use conscious attentional
aversion mechanisms, as drug stimuli may provoke
undesired craving. Greater automatic attentional bias to
drug-related stimuli has been shown to predict relapse in
cigarette smokers (Waters et al, 2003) and opiate users
(Marissen et al, 2006), respectively. Our present findings
may, therefore, also shed new light on the increasing
incidence of cannabis dependence, as the CBD content of
street cannabis has been declining over the past 20 years
(Hardwick and King, 2008). Recent research has also shown
training attentional bias away from alcohol stimuli to be
effective in reducing alcohol consumption, and this effect
was still evident at a 3-month follow-up (Fadardi and Cox,
2009), therefore, CBD might be a potentially beneficial
adjunct in this training.
THC and CBD Effects on Explicit Liking
Cannabis users in this study who smoked high CBD
cannabis rated their explicit liking for the drug stimuli as
less than the low CBD group. This subjective measure of
‘liking’ can be thought of as reflecting hedonic processes
involved in drug abuse. The endocannabinoid system is
known to be involved in mediating ‘liking’ reactions and
microinjection of anandamide into the nucleus accumbens
doubles the level of ‘liking’ of sucrose taste in rats (Mahler
et al, 2007). Given that the high CBD cannabis users are
smoking as much cannabis as the low CBD group, that they
explicitly ‘like’ the drug less may seem counterintuitive.
However, this may relate to the notion that it is implicit
drug ‘wanting’ and not explicit ‘liking’ that mediates drug
seeking behavior, which is particularly evident in drug
addicts who will continue wanting the drug, in the absence
of any explicit liking (Robinson and Berridge, 2003). When
drug free, there was no difference in implicit attentional
bias across the groups, which is tentative support for the
suggestion that it is this process and not explicit ‘liking’ that
mediates cannabis use.
Attentional Bias to Food Stimuli
We expected that acute cannabis would increase bias to
food stimuli, in line with the drug’s well-documented
abilities to promote eating (Chopra and Chopra, 1957). The
findings in the low CBD : THC group, at the short time
interval, were consistent with this. Earlier work has shown
that the appetite stimulating, or hyperphagic, actions of
THC are mediated predominantly by CB1 receptors (Chopra
and Chopra, 1957). Our findings are thus compatible with
suggestions based on animal research that CB1 agonists
increase the incentive value, or salience, of food (Kirkham,
2009). That higher CBD : THC ratios in the cannabis
markedly attenuated acute bias to food stimuli may be
explained by the antagonistic, or even inverse agonistic,
properties of CBD at the CB1 receptor (Pertwee, 2008).
Earlier research has shown that the CB1 antagonist,
rimonabant, reduces desire to eat in humans consistent
with its earlier use in the treatment of obesity (Christensen
et al, 2007). However, rimonabant was recently withdrawn
from clinical use because of reports of depression and
anxiety after treatment (Taylor, 2009). As CBD possesses a
different mechanism of CB1 antagonism to rimonobant, and
a much better side-effect profile, these preliminary findings
may suggest a clinical use for CBD in the treatment of
obesity. However, clearly to establish this, studies would
need to control for many factors not assessed here, such as
food satiety and body weight.
Limitations
This study was subject to some limitations. Estimates of
THC levels in urine at baseline were not taken on the
intoxicated test day and these may have varied between
subjects, which may possibly have influenced results on the
drug-free day. In addition we did not breathalyze subjects
on the testing days; however, none showed any visible signs
of acute alcohol intoxication, as rated by the experimenter.
There were no differences between the groups in levels of
salivary THC on the intoxicated day, which is interesting as
levels of THC in the cannabis were significantly lower in the
high CBD : THC group. However, salivary estimates of
metabolites of cannabis are not possible; therefore, it is
possible that salivary THC and CBD levels are as a result of
contamination of the oral cavity and may be inaccurate
measures of true cannabis consumption.
Conclusions
When people are given a choice between marijuana
cigarettes with different THC concentrations, those with
higher THC content are preferred over those containing
lower THC concentrations (Chait and Zacny, 1992; Kelly
et al, 1997). The constituents of street cannabis have
changed over the past decade or so with high THC, low CBD
strains such as skunk and sinsemilla now dominating the
market (Hardwick and King, 2008). This change was
thought to be in part to be driven by user preference for
lower CBD strains, because of CBD’s potential to modulate
the psychotomimetic effects of the drug and reduce the
‘stoned’ feeling (Zuardi et al., 2006). However, the findings
of this study suggest instead that one reason may be CBD’s
capacity to modulate both the ‘wanting’ and the ‘liking’ of
THC without affecting the ‘stoned’ feeling. Our findings
suggest that lower CBD in cannabis may result in greater
salience of drug cues when intoxicated, potentially invoking
more associative learning around drug cues in users of high
THC/low CBD cannabis, which could speculatively result in
a higher chance of later addiction. The research reported
here also contributes to the growing body which suggests a
range of potential therapeutic uses of CBD, including the
ability to acutely modulate the reinforcing properties of
drugs.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work was supported by a grant to HVC and CJAM
from the Medical Research Council (UK). We thank the
CBD reduces the rewarding effects of THC
CJA Morgan et al
1884
Neuropsychopharmacology
Home Office and the Forensic Science Service for their
support of the study. We also thank all the participants who
donated their time and cannabis.
DISCLOSURE
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
REFERENCES
Berridge KC, Robinson TE, Aldridge JW (2009). Dissecting
components of reward: ‘liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr
Opin Pharmacol 9: 65–73.
Bitencourt RM, Pamplona FA, Takahashi RN (2008). Facilitation of
contextual fear memory extinction and anti-anxiogenic effects of
AM404 and cannabidiol in conditioned rats. Eur Neuropsycho-
pharmacol 18: 849–859.
Chait LD, Zacny JP (1992). Reinforcing and subjective effects of
oral delta 9-THC and smoked marijuana in humans. Psycho-
pharmacology (Berl) 107: 255–262.
Chopra IC, Chopra RN (1957). The use of cannabis drugs in India.
Bull Narc 9: 4–29.
Christensen R, Kristensen PK, Bartels EM, Bliddal H, Astrup A
(2007). Efficacy and safety of the weight-loss drug rimonabant: a
meta-analysis of randomised trials. Lancet 370: 1706–1713.
Cooper ZD, Haney M (2009). Actions of delta-9-tetrahydrocanna-
binol in cannabis: relation to use, abuse, dependence. Int Rev
Psychiatry 21: 104–112.
Curran HV, Brignell C, Fletcher S, Middleton P, Henry J (2002).
Cognitive and subjective dose-response effects of acute oral
Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in infrequent cannabis
users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 164: 61–70.
D’Souza DC, Perry E, MacDougall L, Ammerman Y, Cooper T,
Wu YT et al (2004). The psychotomimetic effects of intravenous
delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in healthy individuals: implica-
tions for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology 29: 1558–1572.
D’Souza DC, Ranganathan M, Braley G, Gueorguieva R, Zimolo Z,
Cooper T et al (2008). Blunted psychotomimetic and amnestic
effects of Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in frequent users of
cannabis. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 2505–2516.
Duka T, Townshend JM (2004). The priming effect of alcohol pre-
load on attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli. Psychophar-
macology (Berl) 176: 353–361.
EMCDDA (2006). Annual Report 2006: The State of the Drugs
Problem in Europe. European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and
Drug Addiction: Lisbon. Pamphlet.
Fadardi JS, Cox WM (2009). Reversing the sequence: reducing
alcohol consumption by overcoming alcohol attentional bias.
Drug Alcohol Depend 101: 137–145.
Field M, Eastwood B, Bradley BP, Mogg K (2006). Selective
processing of cannabis cues in regular cannabis users. Drug
Alcohol Depend 85: 75–82.
Gossop M, Darke S, Griffiths P, Hando J, Powis B, Hall W et al
(1995). The severity of dependence scale (SDS): psychometric
properties of the SDS in English and Australian samples
of heroin, cocaine and amphetamine users. Addiction 90:
607–614.
Guimares FS, Chiaretti TM, Graeff FG (1990). Antianxiety effects of
cannabidiol in the elevated plus-maze. Psychopharmacology 100:
558–559.
Hardwick S, King LA (2008). Home Office Cannabis Potency Study.
Home Office Scientific Development Branch: St Albans. Report.
Heishman SJ, Evans RJ, Singleton EG, Levin KH, Copersino ML,
Gorelick DA (2009). Reliability and validity of a short form of
the Marijuana Craving Questionnaire. Drug Alcohol Depend 102:
35–40.
Hermann D, Sartorius A, Welzel H, Walter S, Skopp G, Ende G
et al (2007). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex N-acetylaspartate/
total creatine (NAA/tCr) loss in male recreational cannabis
users. Biol Psychiatry 61: 1281–1289.
Kelly TH, Foltin RW, Emurian CS, Fischman MW (1997). Are
choice and self-administration of marijuana related to delta 9-
THC content? Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 5: 74–82.
Kirkham TC (2009). Cannabinoids and appetite: food craving and
food pleasure. Int Rev Psychiatry 21: 163–171.
Mahler SV, Smith KS, Berridge KC (2007). Endocannabinoid
hedonic hotspot for sensory pleasure: anandamide in nucleus
accumbens shell enhances ‘liking’ of a sweet reward. Neuro-
psychopharmacology 32: 2267–2278.
Marissen MA, Franken IH, Waters AJ, Blanken P, van den BW,
Hendriks VM (2006). Attentional bias predicts heroin relapse
following treatment. Addiction 101: 1306–1312.
Morgan CJA, Rees H, Curran HV (2008). Attentional bias to
incentive stimuli in frequent ketamine users. Pschol Med 38:
1331–1340.
Parker LA, Burton P, Sorge RE, Yakiwchuk C, Mechoulam R
(2004). Effect of low doses of delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol and
cannabidiol on the extinction of cocaine-induced and amphe-
tamine-induced conditioned place preference learning in rats.
Psychopharmacology 175: 360–366.
Pertwee RG (2008). The diverse CB1 and CB2 receptor pharmacol-
ogy of three plant cannabinoids: delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol,
cannabidiol and delta9-tetrahydrocannabivarin. Br J Pharmacol
153: 199–215.
Ren Y, Whittard J, Higuera-Matas A, Morris CV, Hurd YL (2009).
Cannabidiol, a nonpsychotropic component of cannabis, in-
hibits cue-induced heroin seeking and normalizes discrete
mesolimbic neuronal disturbances. J Neurosci 29: 14764–14769.
Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993). The neural basis of drug
craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res
Brain Res Rev 18: 247–291.
Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2001). Incentive-sensitization and
addiction. Addiction 96: 103–114.
Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2003). Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol 54:
25–53.
Robinson TE, Berridge KC (2008). Review. The incentive
sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philos
Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 363: 3137–3146.
Rodriguez dF, Navarro M, Gomez R, Escuredo L, Nava F, Fu J. et al
(2001). An anorexic lipid mediator regulated by feeding. Nature
414: 209–212.
Solowij N, Hall W, Lee N (1992). Recreational MDMA use in
Sydney: a profile of ‘Ecstasy’ users and their experience with the
drug. Br J Addict 87: 1161–1172.
Taylor D (2009). Withdrawal of RimonabantFwalking the tight-
rope of 21st century pharmaceutical regulation? Curr Drug Saf
4: 2–4.
Vann RE, Gamage TF, Warner JA, Marshall EM, Taylor NL, Martin
BR, Wiley JL (2008). Divergent effects of cannabidiol on the
discriminative stimulus and place conditioning effects of Delta(9)-
tetrahydrocannabinol. Drug Alcohol Depend 94: 191–198.
Waters AJ, Shiffman S, Sayette MA, Paty JA, Gwaltney CJ,
Balabanis MH (2003). Attentional bias predicts outcome in
smoking cessation. Health Psychol 22: 378–387.
Wechsler D (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. Psychological
Corportation: San Antonio, TX.
Zuardi AW, Crippa JA, Hallak JE, Moreira FA, Guimaraes FS
(2006). Cannabidiol, a Cannabis sativa constituent, as an
antipsychotic drug. Braz J Med Biol Res 39: 421–429.
CBD reduces the rewarding effects of THC
CJA Morgan et al
1885
Neuropsychopharmacology
... There is only one study looking directly at the acute effects of cannabis on attentional bias using a visual probe paradigm. Morgan et al. (2010) performed a naturalistic study with adolescents and young adults aged 16-24 who were allowed to smoke a typical amount of their own, usual cannabis and then perform a visual probe task. Participants were grouped by high CBD content (> 1%) and low CBD content (< 1%) with results indicating increased attentional bias to food and drug-related stimuli at short exposure times (250 ms) for low CBD participants but no attentional bias in high CBD participants. ...
... A visual probe task was used to assess attentional bias, adapted from a previous study (Morgan et al. 2010). Each trial began with a 500-ms visual fixation point. ...
... No changes in attentional bias were detected at 500-ms exposure times. This result is similar to Morgan et al. (2010) whose low-CBD-to-THC group elicited a positive attentional bias to drug-related stimuli at 250-ms presentation times; however, they did not have a placebo control. Our participants had an initial attentional bias away from cannabis cues under placebo conditions that was eliminated by acute THC intoxication. ...
Article
Full-text available
Rationale Attentional bias to drug-related stimuli is hypothesised to contribute towards addiction. However, the acute effects of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) on attentional bias to cannabis cues, the differential response in adults and adolescents, and the moderating effect of cannabidiol (CBD) are unknown. Objectives Our study investigated (1) the acute effects of vaporised cannabis on attentional bias to cannabis-related images in adults and adolescents and (2) the moderating influences of age and CBD. Methods We conducted a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over study where three weight-adjusted vaporised cannabis preparations: ‘THC’ (8 mg THC for a 75-kg person), ‘THC + CBD’ (8 mg THC and 24 mg CBD for a 75-kg person) and PLA (matched placebo). Cannabis was administered on 3 separate days to 48 participants, who used cannabis 0.5–3 days/week: 24 adolescents (12 females, aged 16–17) and 24 adults (12 females, aged 26–29). Participants completed a visual probe task with cannabis cues. Our primary outcome was attentional bias to cannabis stimuli, measured using the differential reaction time to a cannabis vs. neutral probe, on 200-ms trials. Results In contrast to hypotheses, attention was directed away from cannabis cues on placebo, and there was a main effect of the drug (F(2,92) = 3.865, p = 0.024, η²p = 0.077), indicating THC administration eliminated this bias. There was no significant impact of CBD nor an age-by-drug interaction. Conclusions Acute THC intoxication eliminated attentional bias away from cannabis cues. There was no evidence of differential response in adolescents compared to adults and no evidence that a moderate vaporised dose of CBD altered the impact of cannabis on attentional bias. Trial registration This study was listed with the US National Library of Medicine and registered on ClinicalTrials.gov, URL: Do Adolescents and Adults Differ in Their Acute Response to Cannabis?—Full Text View—ClinicalTrials.gov, registration number: NCT04851392.
... This has since been reinforced by a randomised double-blind placebo-controlled trial showing that 400 mg and 800 mg of oral CBD can reduce cannabis use in individuals with CUD [16]. In terms of attentional bias toward cannabis stimuli, this has been explored previously in a naturalistic study by Morgan et al. [17]. Regular cannabis users donated their own cannabis for analysis and participants were stratified according to the CBD:THC ratio of their cannabis, either low (1:100) or high (1:3). ...
... The participants then completed tasks indexing attentional bias and explicit liking of cannabis stimuli both sober and when intoxicated with their own cannabis. Attentional bias and explicit liking of cannabis stimuli were maintained in regular users when intoxicated, but this effect was less pronounced with administration of cannabis containing higher CBD:THC ratios [17]. ...
... The effect of acute THC administration on attentional bias in infrequent cannabis users is under-researched. As in Morgan et al. [17], co-administration of CBD may mitigate against increases in bias toward cannabis stimuli. Reducing bias at this early stage could provide preliminary support for controlling the CBD content of cannabis to reduce the incidence of CUD among people who use cannabis infrequently and could offer a potential strategy for universal harm reduction [18]. ...
Article
Aims To test how attentional bias and explicit liking are influenced by delta‐9‐tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) and whether these effects are moderated by cannabidiol (CBD). Design Double‐blind, randomised, within‐subjects cross‐over study. Setting NIHR Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Facility at King's College Hospital, London, United Kingdom. Participants/Cases Forty‐six infrequent cannabis users (cannabis use <1 per week). Intervention(s) Across four sessions, participants inhaled vaporised cannabis containing 10 mg of THC and either 0 mg (0:1 CBD:THC), 10 mg (1:1), 20 mg (2:1) or 30 mg (3:1) of CBD, administered in a randomised order and counter‐balanced across participants (a total of 24 order groups). Measurements Participants completed two tasks: (1) Attentional Bias (AB), comparing reaction times toward visual probes presented behind 28 target stimuli (cannabis/food) compared with probes behind corresponding non‐target (neutral) stimuli. Participants responding more quickly to probes behind target than non‐target stimuli would indicate greater attentional bias to cannabis/food; (2) Picture Rating (PR), where all AB stimuli were rated on a 7‐point pleasantness scale, measuring explicit liking. Findings During the AB task, participants were more biased toward cannabis stimuli in the 0:1 condition compared with baseline (mean difference = 12.2, 95% confidence intervals [CIs] = 1.20–23.3, d = 0.41, P = 0.03). No other significant AB or PR differences were found between cannabis and food stimuli between baseline and 0:1 condition ( P > 0.05). No significant CBD effect was found on AB or PR task performance at any dose ( P > 0.05). There was additionally no cumulative effect of THC exposure on AB or PR outcomes ( P > 0.05). Conclusions A double‐blind, randomised, cross‐over study among infrequent cannabis users found that inhaled delta‐9‐tetrahydrocannabinol increased attentional bias toward cannabis in the absence of explicit liking, a marker of liability toward cannabis use disorder. At the concentrations normally found in legal and illegal cannabis, cannabidiol had no influence on this effect.
... The plant produces many other phytocannabinoids, of which cannabidiol (CBD). Scientific interest in this non-psychoactive molecule has expanded in recent times given its potential therapeutic applications, including substance use disorders (Britch et al., 2021;Crippa et al., 2018;Freeman et al., 2020;Grotenhermen & Müller-Vahl, 2016;Morgan et al., 2010;Russo & Guy, 2006;Sholler et al., 2020;Whiting et al., 2015). In European countries, a market for cannabis with high-CBD and lower-THC content has recently emerged. ...
... The decrease was associated with daily LT use, which was mostly reported to reduce THC withdrawal symptoms. CBD has indeed proven to be a protective factor to cannabis dependence by modulating the effects of THC (Morgan et al., 2010;Niesink & van Laar, 2013). ...
... Such categorizations also provide no information regarding how these relationships may vary based on exogenous cannabinoid content, specifically Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) and cannabidiol (CBD) levels in cannabis products. However, this information would be useful as there is emerging evidence that the relative ratios of CBD and THC in cannabis products have differential effects on both biological and behavioral processes relevant to health (Bhattacharyya et al., 2010;Morgan et al., 2010;Englund et al., 2013;Gibson et al., 2022). ...
... Of note, none of the findings appeared to be dependent on cannabinoid content as there were no significant differences between non-users, users assigned to use THC-dominant products, and users assigned to use products with CBD on any outcome. This finding is surprising given that THC and CBD can differentially affect biological systems and behavioral processes relevant to health (Bhattacharyya et al., 2010;Morgan et al., 2010;Englund et al., 2013;Gibson et al., 2022). However, the lack of findings here may be a function of the observational nature of the study. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The increasing availability of legal-market cannabis products has raised many questions about potential harms and benefits of increased use. In particular, concerns have been raised about the possible negative impact of cannabis use on behavioral determinants of obesity and chronic disease, including diet and exercise. However, previous research is mixed and has largely relied on cross-sectional survey data and coarse measurements of cannabis use, underscoring the need for more rigorous research designs. Purpose The present study utilized longitudinal daily diary data to assess whether exercise and diet patterns differed between cannabis users and non-users and, within cannabis users, whether legal-market cannabis use, diet, and exercise covaried within individuals across time and based on cannabinoid content. Methods A sample of 98 participants (77 cannabis users, 21 non-users) completed a baseline appointment and a 30-day daily diary study assessing their daily cannabis use, diet, and exercise. Cannabis users were quasi-randomly assigned to use either a THC-dominant flower product ( n = 36) or a CBD-containing flower product ( n = 41) ad libitum over the course of the daily diary study. Participants were between the ages of 21 and 41 ( M = 29.28) and were majority male (61.2%). Results At baseline, there were no differences in BMI or exercise behavior between users and non-users. Likelihood of exercising and exercise minutes per day over the 30-day period also did not differ between users and non-users, nor did these outcomes differ on cannabis use vs. non-use days among cannabis using participants. In contrast, there was some evidence for a relationship between cannabis use and dietary measures. At baseline, non-users scored higher on the Healthy Eating Index than users. Daily data also indicated that users consumed marginally more salty snacks and fast food per day relative to non-users, and users consumed more fruits/vegetables and marginally more salty snacks on cannabis use days vs. non-use days. Interestingly, among users, no associations were dependent on the cannabinoid content of their assigned product. Conclusion Findings suggest little association between cannabis use and exercise but underscore the need for further research on how cannabis use may impact dietary patterns. Future research should examine the impact of cannabis on non-behavioral pathways to obesity and chronic disease (e.g., metabolism).
... THC is the primary psychoactive component of cannabis, with euphoric and cognitive-altering properties [29]. In contrast, CBD may modulate the effects of THC [30,31] with potential therapeutic properties including antipsychotic effects [32] and helping treat cannabis addiction [33]. However, these adaptive properties may only be apparent with high doses of CBD (e.g. ...
Article
Full-text available
Adolescence is a time of rapid neurodevelopment and the endocannabinoid system is particularly prone to change during this time. Cannabis is a commonly used drug with a particularly high prevalence of use among adolescents. The two predominant phytocannabinoids are Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) and cannabidiol (CBD), which affect the endocannabinoid system. It is unknown whether this period of rapid development makes adolescents more or less vulnerable to the effects of cannabis on brain-network connectivity, and whether CBD may attenuate the effects of THC. Using fMRI, we explored the impact of vaporized cannabis (placebo, THC: 8 mg/75 kg, THC + CBD: 8 mg/75 kg THC & 24 mg/75 kg CBD) on resting-state networks in groups of semi-regular cannabis users (usage frequency between 0.5 and 3 days/week), consisting of 22 adolescents (16–17 years) and 24 young adults (26–29 years) matched for cannabis use frequency. Cannabis caused reductions in within-network connectivity in the default mode (F[2,88] = 3.97, P = 0.022, η² = 0.018), executive control (F[2,88] = 18.62, P < 0.001, η² = 0.123), salience (F[2,88] = 12.12, P < 0.001, η² = 0.076), hippocampal (F[2,88] = 14.65, P < 0.001, η² = 0.087), and limbic striatal (F[2,88] = 16.19, P < 0.001, η² = 0.102) networks compared to placebo. Whole-brain analysis showed cannabis significantly disrupted functional connectivity with cortical regions and the executive control, salience, hippocampal, and limbic striatal networks compared to placebo. CBD did not counteract THC’s effects and further reduced connectivity both within networks and the whole brain. While age-related differences were observed, there were no interactions between age group and cannabis treatment in any brain network. Overall, these results challenge the assumption that CBD can make cannabis safer, as CBD did not attenuate THC effects (and in some cases potentiated them); furthermore, they show that cannabis causes similar disruption to resting-state connectivity in the adolescent and adult brain.
Chapter
In the last two decades, the endocannabinoid system has emerged as a crucial modulator of motivation and emotional processing. Due to its widespread neuroanatomical distribution and characteristic retrograde signaling nature, cannabinoid type I receptors and their endogenous ligands finely orchestrate somatic and axon terminal activity of dopamine neurons. Owing to these unique features, this signaling system is a promising pharmacological target to ameliorate dopamine-mediated drug-seeking behaviors while circumventing the adverse side effects of, for instance, dopaminergic antagonists. Despite considerable preclinical efforts, an agreement on the efficacy of endocannabinoid-targeting compounds for treating drug substance use disorders in humans has not been reached. In the following chapter, we will summarize preclinical and clinical evidence addressing the therapeutic potential of cannabinoids and endocannabinoid-targeting compounds in substance use disorders. To bridge the gap between animal and clinical research, we capitalize on studies evaluating the impact of endocannabinoid-targeting compounds in relevant settings, such as the management of drug relapse. Finally, we discuss the therapeutic potential of novel cannabinoid compounds that hold promise for treating substance use disorders.
Article
The relationship between cannabis use and mental health is complex, as studies often report seemingly contradictory findings regarding whether cannabis use results in more positive or negative treatment outcomes. With an increasing number of individuals using cannabis for both recreational (i.e., non-medical) and medical purposes, it is critical to gain a deeper understanding of the ways in which cannabis may be helpful or harmful for those diagnosed with psychiatric disorders. Although cannabis is composed of hundreds of compounds, studies assessing the effects of “cannabis” most often report the impact of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (d9-THC), the primary intoxicating constituent of the plant. While d9-THC has documented therapeutic properties, negative clinical outcomes commonly associated with cannabis are generally related to d9-THC exposure. In contrast, non-intoxicating cannabinoids such as cannabidiol (CBD) show promise as potential treatment options for psychiatric symptoms. In this article, findings from studies and reviews examining the relationship between mental health conditions (mood, anxiety, psychosis, and post-traumatic stress disorder [PTSD]) and cannabis use are summarized to highlight critical variables that are often overlooked, including those associated with cannabis use patterns (e.g., frequency of use, amount used, cannabinoid exposure, product choice, and route of administration). Further, this article explores individual factors (e.g., age, sex, genetics/family history) that likely impact cannabis-related outcomes. Research to date suggests that youth and those with a family history or genetic liability for psychiatric disorders are at higher risk for negative outcomes, while more research is needed to fully understand unique effects related to sex and older age.
Article
There is emerging evidence that the endocannabinoid system (ECS) plays a significant role in the pathophysiology of many psychiatric disorders, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Increasing evidence suggests that a number of neurobiological correlates between endogenous cannabinoid function and cognitive dysfunction are seen in ADHD, making the ECS a possible target for therapeutic interventions. Cannabis use and cannabis use disorder are more prevalent in individuals with ADHD, compared to the general population, and there is growing popular perception that cannabis is therapeutic for ADHD. However, the relationship between cannabis use and ADHD symptomology is poorly understood. Further understanding of the role of the ECS in ADHD pathophysiology and the molecular alterations that may be a target for treatment is needed. To further the science on this emerging area of research, this scoping review describes the preclinical and clinical evidence seeking to understand the relationship between the ECS and ADHD.
Article
Full-text available
Americans spent $4.17 billion on cannabidiol (CBD) in 2021 and survey research indicated 60% of adults had consumed CBD, often to address pain, poor sleep, stress, anxiety, and general well-being. One survey found 90% of medical professionals reported patients mentioned CBD or asked about its efficacy, safety, or legality, and most shared they were not familiar enough with CBD to answer patients’ inquiries. Many mental health professionals may also be at a loss when discussing CBD in the practice setting. CBD advertising claims are often exaggerated, its legality varies across jurisdictions, and CBD research is typically conducted using animal models or intended for professionals with neurology expertise. Locating trustworthy and easily digestible CBD research is a challenge, and so the ability to share CBD knowledge with clients is even more limited. To address these needs, we provide a primer developed specifically for nonmedically trained mental health professionals in which we report on CBD’s history and contemporary context, takeaways from updated CBD human participant research, and its potential risks and benefits. Research does not show consistent, strong support for CBD as treatment for anxiety, although some favor its therapeutic use for trauma-related, substance use, and psychotic disorders. In light of dubious findings, we recommend mental health professionals encourage clients practice awareness to understand risks, such as medication interactions and consequences of mislabeling, and the strength of evidence supporting CBD as therapeutic for various conditions. We also encourage rigorous human subjects research with large, generalizable sample sizes.
Article
Full-text available
Co-occurring cannabis use is common among those with opioid use disorder (OUD), but the extent to which it is harmful may be due to its preparation and concentration of various cannabinoids. The current study aimed to examine the prevalence of, and long-term associations with, the use of varying cannabis products among a naturalistic longitudinal cohort of people with heroin dependence. A total of 615 people, most of whom were entering treatment, were recruited to the Australian Treatment Outcome Study (ATOS) in 2001–2002. This analysis focuses on the 401 participants followed up at 18–20 years post baseline. Structured interviews assessed the use of cannabis products, as well as demographic and health covariates. High-potency/indoor-grown cannabis was the most common type ever used (68.8%), and in the past 12 months (80.4%), followed by low potency/outdoor grown (22.4%; 14.4%), and less so for other types of cannabis. After controlling for covariates, older age at baseline was associated with lower odds of high-potency cannabis being used as the primary type in the past 12 months. In contrast to studies of non-opioid dependent populations, common use of high-potency cannabis was not associated with more severe health outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
The effects of Δ 9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) content on choice of marijuana, number of marijuana cigarettes smoked, and ratings of marijuana's effects were examined in 6 adult male marijuana smokers during a residential study consisting of four 3-day blocks of 2 sample days and 1 choice day. Days were divided into 6.5-hr work and social-access periods, beginning at 1000 and 1700. On sample days, marijuana cigarettes containing different THC concentrations (0.0% vs. 3.5% and 2.0% vs. 3.5% THC) were smoked at least once during each period. On choice days, independent choices between previously sampled marijuana cigarettes were made during each period. A maximum of 8 cigarettes could be smoked per day, and drug ratings were obtained after each period. Only choice behavior was sensitive to changes in THC content, whereas only the number of smoked marijuana cigarettes was related to context (i.e., work and social-access period).
Article
Full-text available
The aims of the research were to (a) compare the alcohol attentional bias (AAB) of social, hazardous, and harmful drinkers and (b) assess the effects of alcohol attention-control training on the AAB and alcohol consumption of hazardous and harmful drinkers. Participants were social drinkers (N = 40), hazardous drinkers (N = 89), and harmful drinkers (N = 92). Paper-and-pencil measures were used to collect information about participants’ socio-demographic characteristics, health status, motivational structure, drinking-related locus of control and situational self-confidence, readiness to change, affect, and alcohol consumption. Computerized classic, alcohol- and concerns-Stroop tests were administered. All participants were tested individually, with the order of tests counterbalanced across participants. After the baseline assessment, the hazardous and harmful drinkers were trained with the Alcohol Attention-Control Training Program (AACTP) for two and four sessions, respectively. Both samples completed a post-training assessment, and the harmful drinkers also completed 3-month follow-up. Results indicated that (a) the harmful drinkers had larger AAB than the hazardous and the social drinkers; (b) the attentional training reduced the hazardous and harmful drinkers’ AAB; and (c) the harmful drinkers showed post-training reductions in alcohol consumption and improvements on the other drinking-related indices. The harmful drinkers’ improvements were maintained at the 3-month follow-up.
Article
The question of addiction concerns the process by which drug-taking behavior, in certain individuals, evolves into compulsive patterns of drug-seeking and drug-taking behavior that take place at the expense of most other activities, and the inability to cease drug-taking, that is, the problem of relapse. In this paper we summarize one view of this process, the “incentive-sensitization” view, which we e rst proposed in 1993. Four major tenets of the incentive-sensitization view are discussed. These are: (1) potentially addictive drugs share the ability to alter brain organization; (2) the brain systems that are altered include those normally involved in the process of incentive motivation and reward; (3) the critical neuroadaptations for addiction render these brain reward systems hypersensitive (“sensitized”) to drugs and drug-associated stimuli; and (4) the brain systems that are sensitized do not mediate the pleasurable or euphoric effects of drugs (drug “liking”), but instead they mediate a subcomponent of reward we have termed incentive salience (drug “wanting”).
Article
This paper presents a biopsychological theory of drug addiction, the 'Incentive-Sensitization Theory'. The theory addresses three fundamental questions. The first is: why do addicts crave drugs? That is, what is the psychological and neurobiological basis of drug craving? The second is: why does drug craving persist even after long periods of abstinence? The third is whether 'wanting' drugs (drug craving) is attributable to 'liking' drugs (to the subjective pleasurable effects of drugs)? The theory posits the following. (1) Addictive drugs share the ability to enhance mesotelencephalic dopamine neurotransmission. (2) One psychological function of this neural system is to attribute 'incentive salience' to the perception and mental representation of events associated with activation of the system. Incentive salience is a psychological process that transforms the perception of stimuli, imbuing them with salience, making them attractive, 'wanted', incentive stimuli. (3) In some individuals the repeated use of addictive drugs produces incremental neuroadaptations in this neural system, rendering it increasingly and perhaps permanently, hypersensitive ('sensitized') to drugs and drug-associated stimuli. The sensitization of dopamine systems is gated by associative learning, which causes excessive incentive salience to be attributed to the act of drug taking and to stimuli associated with drug taking. It is specifically the sensitization of incentive salience, therefore, that transforms ordinary 'wanting' into excessive drug craving. (4) It is further proposed that sensitization of the neural systems responsible for incentive salience ('for wanting') can occur independently of changes in neural systems that mediate the subjective pleasurable effects of drugs (drug 'liking') and of neural systems that mediate withdrawal. Thus, sensitization of incentive salience can produce addictive behavior (compulsive drug seeking and drug taking) even if the expectation of drug pleasure or the aversive properties of withdrawal are diminished and even in the face of strong disincentives, including the loss of reputation, job, home and family. We review evidence for this view of addiction and discuss its implications for understanding the psychology and neurobiology of addiction.
Article
The Severity of Dependence Scale (SDS) was devised to provide a short, easily administered scale which can be used to measure the degree of dependence experienced by users of different types of drugs. The SDS contains five items, all of which are explicitly concerned with psychological components of dependence. These items are specifically concerned with impaired control over drug taking and with preoccupation and anxieties about drug use. The SDS was given to five samples of drug users in London and Sydney. The samples comprised users of heroin and users of cocaine in London, and users of amphetamines and methadone maintenance patients in Sydney. The SDS satisfies a number of criteria which indicate its suitability as a measure of dependence. All SDS items load significantly with a single factor, and the total SDS score was extremely highly correlated with the single factor score. The SDS score is related to behavioural patterns of drug taking that are, in themselves, indicators of dependence, such as dose, frequency of use, duration of use, daily use and degree of contact with other drug users; it also shows criterion validity in that drug users who have sought treatment at specialist and non-specialist agencies for drug problems have higher SDS scores than non-treatment samples. The psychometric properties of the scale were good in all five samples, despite being applied to primary users of different classes of drug, wing different recruitment procedures in different cities in different countries.
Article
The reinforcing and subjective effects of oral delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) and smoked marijuana were studied in two groups of regular marijuana users. One group (N=10) was tested with smoked marijuana and the other (N=11) with oral THC. Reinforcing effects were measured with a discrete-trial choice procedure which allowed subjects to choose between the self-administration of active drug or placebo on two independent occasions. Subjective effects and heart rate were measured before and after drug administration. Smoked active marijuana was chosen over placebo on both choice occasions by all subjects. Similarly, oral THC was chosen over placebo on both occasions by all but one subject. Both active drug treatments produced qualitatively and quantitatively similar subjective effects, and both significantly increased heart rate, although the time course of effects differed substantially between the two treatments. The results demonstrate that both smoked marijuana and oral THC can serve as positive reinforcers in human subjects under laboratory conditions. The experimental paradigm used here should prove useful for identifying factors that influence the self-administration of marijuana and other cannabinoids by humans.
Article
The effects of Δ–9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) content on choice of marijuana, number of marijuana cigarettes smoked, and ratings of marijuana's effects were examined in 6 adult male marijuana smokers during a residential study consisting of four 3-day blocks of 2 sample days and 1 choice day. Days were divided into 6.5-hr work and social-access periods, beginning at 1000 and 1700. On sample days, marijuana cigarettes containing different THC concentrations (0.0% vs. 3.5% and 2.0% vs. 3.5% THC) were smoked at least once during each period. On choice days, independent choices between previously sampled marijuana cigarettes were made during each period. A maximum of 8 cigarettes could be smoked per day, and drug ratings were obtained after each period. Only choice behavior was sensitive to changes in THC content, whereas only the number of smoked marijuana cigarettes was related to context (i.e., work and social-access period).
Article
This paper presents a biopsychological theory of drug addiction, the ‘Incentive-Sensitization Theory’. The theory addresses three fundamental questions. The first is: why do addicts crave drugs? That is, what is the psychological and neurobiological basis of drug craving? The second is: why does drug craving persist even after long periods of abstinence? The third is whether ‘wanting’ drugs (drug craving) is attributable to ‘liking’ drugs (to the subjective pleasurable effects of drugs)? The theory posits the following.